Abstract
Background: the relationship between cognition and sexual activity in healthy older adults is under-researched. A limited amount of research in this area has shown that sexual activity is associated with better cognition in older men. The current study explores the possible mediating factors in this association in men and women, and attempts to provide an explanation in terms of physiological influences on cognitive function.
Methods: using newly available data from Wave 6 of the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing, the current study explored associations between sexual activity and cognition in adults aged 50–89 (n = 6,833). Two different tests of cognitive function were analysed: number sequencing, which broadly relates to executive function, and word recall, which broadly relates to memory.
Results: after adjusting for age, education, wealth, physical activity, depression, cohabiting, self-rated health, loneliness and quality of life, there were significant associations between sexual activity and number sequencing and recall in men. However, in women there was a significant association between sexual activity and recall, but not number sequencing.
Conclusions: possible mediators of these associations (e.g. neurotransmitters) are discussed. The cross-sectional nature of the analysis is limiting, but provides a promising avenue for future explorations and longitudinal studies. The findings have implications for the promotion of sexual counselling in healthcare settings, where maintaining a healthy sex life in older age could be instrumental in improving cognitive function and well-being.
Keywords: cognition, sexual activity, ageing, gender differences, English Longitudinal Study of Ageing (ELSA), older people
Cognitive function has been associated with a number of physical, psychological and emotional factors in older adults, such as cognitive lifestyle [1], psychological factors of quality of life [2], loneliness [3] and mood [4] and physical activity [5, 6].
Sexual activity is equivalent to mild to moderate physical activity in the range of 3–5 METS (metabolic equivalents) [7], but very little research has focussed specifically on possible associations between sexual activity and cognitive function. The limited amount of existing research focuses on of the impact of cognitive impairment or dementia on sexual relationships in older adult couples [8–10]. A recent systematic review found that those experiencing cognitive decline and dementia engaged in fewer sexual activities than their cognitively intact, non-demented counterparts [11].
There is a gender bias in research examining links between sexual activity and cognition in older age. For example, healthy older Italian men with a continued interest in sex, and those who were sexually active, had better overall cognitive functioning [12, 13]. One explanation for the relative lack of research with women could be that females are more likely to be widowed—and hence lose their sexual partner—at an earlier age than men [14]. In addition to this gender bias, there has been little exploration of potential mediators in the relationship between sexual activity and cognitive function, particularly in women. Indeed, there are gender differences in cognitive function across the lifespan [15], which are arguably due to fundamental effects of prenatal sex hormones on brain development and structure [see Ref. 16 for review]. However, the observed gender differences in cognition scores are usually small, ranging from 0.3 to 0.9 standard deviation units [17].
The current study proposes to investigate the association between sexual activity and cognitive function in older adults in more detail, including a thorough investigation of mediating variables such as age, gender and loneliness. The English Longitudinal Study of Ageing (ELSA) is a nationally representative panel survey that gathers data on health, lifestyle and socioeconomic variables in adults over the age of 50 years. Wave 6 of ELSA is the first wave to include questions pertaining to sexual relationships [18], and as such is ideal for the study of associations between sexual activity and cognitive function in an ageing population.
Method
Participants
Data were drawn from survey responses to Wave 6 version 1 of ELSA (n = 10,601). Respondents under the age of 50 years or over the age of 90 years were excluded (n = 436). (In ELSA, respondents who were aged 90 and above were coded with an arbitrary age of 99 years to promote anonymity, which would be an inaccurate representation of age within the oldest portion of the sample. Those under the age of 50 years (mostly comprising younger partners of invited respondents) were outside the remit of our focus on an ageing population.) Respondents who did not answer the question pertaining to sexual activity over the past 12 months (n = 3,332) were also removed from the analysis. The remaining sample (n = 6,833) comprised 3,060 men and 3,773 women between the ages of 50 and 89 years.
Design
Independent variable
The independent variable in this study was whether or not the respondent had engaged in any form of sexual activity over the previous 12 months (ELSA Wave 6 contains a rich dataset of variables pertaining to sexual activity, function and problems. We thank an anonymous reviewer for drawing our attention to these. However, for this exploratory analysis, we chose to use this variable to distinguish older adults who were sexually active from those who were not.). In this case, sexual activity could include intercourse, masturbation, petting or fondling.
Dependant variables
The dependant variables were scores from cognitive tests of recall and a number sequencing. In the recall task, respondents heard a list of 10 everyday words and were asked to recall them straight away (immediate recall) and after a short delay (delayed recall; maximum score 20).
The number sequencing task required completion of a number sequence, by first determining the number pattern, and then deciding which number was missing, e.g. 1, 2, __, 4, where the correct answer would be ‘3’. Raw scores were converted into standardised scores and made available in the ELSA Wave 6 dataset (range 409–584).
Covariates
Model 1: Age was measured in full years (range 50–89), and education was divided into three standard categories of none, intermediate and degree/higher. Net financial wealth was divided into quintiles (1st quintile = poorest, 5th quintile = wealthiest).
Model 2: Physical activity was predefined in ELSA as sedentary, low, moderate or high. Cohabiting was classified as the respondent living with spouse/partner or not, and self-rated health was categorised as good (comprising ‘excellent’, ‘very good’ and ‘good’) or poor (comprising ‘fair’ and ‘poor’).
Model 3: Depression was indicated by scoring 4 or more on the 8-item CES-D depression scale [19, 20] or a reported diagnosis of depression. Quality of life was measured by total score on the CASP19 questionnaire [21], and loneliness was calculated from total score on the three-item loneliness scale [22].
Data analysis
All data analysis was conducted using IBM SPSS Version 22.0 (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, USA). Analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) was used to assess differences in cognitive function scores in sexually active and sexually inactive participants, with separate models for men and women, and for number sequencing and recall scores. Model 1 included standard adjustments for the mediating effects of age, education and wealth on cognitive function scores. Model 2 included all Model 1 covariates, plus adjustments for physical activity, cohabiting and self-rated health. Model 3 included all previous variables, plus depression, quality of life and loneliness. (Interactions between the independent variable (sexual activity) and each covariate were assessed separately, where any statistically significant (P < 0.05) interactions were included in the final models.)
Results
Table 1 shows sexually active men and women to have significantly higher scores on the number sequencing and recall tests than sexually inactive men and women (all P < 0.001). Additionally, sexually active men and women were more likely to have a higher level of education, be younger, wealthier, more physically active, not depressed, less lonely and have a better quality of life (all P ≤ 0.001).
Table 1.
Men | Sexual activity in past 12 months? |
P | |
---|---|---|---|
Yes (n = 2,349) | No (n = 711) | ||
Age (years) | 64.4 (7.8) | 72.9 (8.5) | <0.001 |
Cognitive function | |||
Number sequencing | 543.3 (25.6) | 529.7 (30.5) | <0.001 |
Recall | 11.1 (3.1) | 9.0 (3.2) | <0.001 |
Education | |||
Degree/higher | 51.1 | 32.1 | <0.001 |
Intermediate | 35.8 | 42.8 | 0.001 |
None | 13.1 | 25.0 | <0.001 |
Wealth (quintiles) | |||
1st (poorest) | 11.3 | 19.0 | <0.001 |
2nd | 16.0 | 21.5 | 0.001 |
3rd | 20.1 | 22.1 | 0.274 |
4th | 25.1 | 21.4 | 0.053 |
5th (wealthiest) | 27.5 | 16.0 | <0.001 |
Physical activity | |||
Sedentary/low | 18.0 | 34.0 | <0.001 |
Moderate | 52.1 | 49.3 | 0.201 |
High | 29.9 | 16.8 | <0.001 |
Cohabiting | 84.6 | 73.9 | <0.001 |
Good health | 79.2 | 56.5 | <0.001 |
Depression | 11.3 | 16.1 | 0.001 |
Loneliness score | 3.9 (1.4) | 4.2 (1.6) | <0.001 |
Quality of life (CASP19) | 41.8 (8.4) | 37.7 (9.5) | <0.001 |
Women | Yes (n = 2,148) | No (n = 1,625) | |
Age (years) | 62.4 (7.5) | 69.6 (8.9) | <0.001 |
Cognitive function | |||
Number sequencing | 536.3 (24.2) | 527.3 (28.1) | <0.001 |
Recall | 12.2 (3.2) | 10.7 (3.5) | <0.001 |
Education | |||
Degree/higher | 36.1 | 23.5 | <0.001 |
Intermediate | 47.0 | 44.1 | 0.076 |
None | 16.9 | 32.4 | <0.001 |
Wealth (quintiles) | |||
1st (poorest) | 10.4 | 23.7 | <0.001 |
2nd | 16.6 | 22.9 | <0.001 |
3rd | 19.8 | 22.9 | 0.025 |
4th | 24.8 | 17.3 | <0.001 |
5th (wealthiest) | 28.5 | 13.1 | <0.001 |
Physical activity | |||
Sedentary/low | 20.2 | 37.2 | <0.001 |
Moderate | 56.1 | 48.6 | <0.001 |
High | 23.6 | 14.2 | <0.001 |
Cohabiting | 85.6 | 47.0 | <0.001 |
Good health | 81.6 | 66.8 | <0.001 |
Depression | 17.2 | 22.6 | <0.001 |
Loneliness score | 4.1 (1.5) | 4.5 (1.7) | <0.001 |
Quality of life (CASP19) | 42.6 (8.4) | 39.0 (9.2) | <0.001 |
ANOVA for continuous variables, presented as mean (SD). χ2 for categorical variables, presented as %.
Table 2 shows the results of the ANCOVA for Models 1, 2 and 3, for men and women separately. After full adjustments in Model 3, sexually active men had significantly higher number sequencing (F1, 2,128 = 5.444, P = 0.020; 20.7% variance explained) and recall (F1, 2,164 = 13.810, P < 0.001; 24.2% variance explained) scores than sexually inactive men. For females, the significant association between sexual activity and unadjusted cognitive function scores (see Table 1) was attenuated following adjustments. There was no significant association between sexual activity and number sequencing in any of the models for women (all P > 0.10; see Table 2). However, there was a significant association between sexual activity and recall scores in Model 3, where sexually active women scored higher than sexually inactive women (F1, 2,610 = 9.064, P = 0.003; 20.9% variance explained).
Table 2.
Sexual activity in past 12 months? |
Model results |
|||
---|---|---|---|---|
Yes | No | R² | P | |
Men | ||||
Number sequencinga | ||||
Model 1 | 536.7 (0.6) | 532.1 (1.1) | 0.184 | <0.001 |
Model 2 | 534.3 (0.8) | 530.8 (1.2) | 0.204 | 0.008 |
Model 3 | 536.4 (1.2) | 531.4 (1.9) | 0.207 | 0.020 |
Recallb | ||||
Model 1 | 10.4 (0.1) | 9.7 (0.1) | 0.220 | <0.001 |
Model 2 | 10.2 (0.1) | 9.7 (0.1) | 0.240 | 0.001 |
Model 3 | 10.3 (0.1) | 9.5 (0.2) | 0.242 | <0.001 |
Women | ||||
Number sequencingc | ||||
Model 1 | 530.9 (0.6) | 529.5 (0.7) | 0.153 | 0.137 |
Model 2 | 530.0 (0.8) | 528.7 (0.8) | 0.165 | 0.216 |
Model 3 | 531.4 (0.9) | 530.7 (0.9) | 0.155 | 0.523 |
Recalld | ||||
Model 1 | 11.5 (0.1) | 11.2 (0.1) | 0.193 | 0.016 |
Model 2 | 11.5 (0.1) | 11.1 (0.1) | 0.212 | 0.008 |
Model 3 | 11.7 (0.1) | 11.2 (0.1) | 0.209 | 0.003 |
Scores are presented as adjusted mean (SE). All variables, models and statistical analyses are described in the Method section.
aMen, Number sequencing models: Model 1: age, education, wealth; Model 2: Model 1 plus physical activity; Model 3: Model 2 plus depression, quality of life, loneliness, sexual activity × depression, sexual activity × quality of life.
bMen, Recall models: Model 1: age, education, wealth; Model 2: Model 1 plus physical activity; Model 3: Model 2 plus depression, quality of life, loneliness, sexual activity × depression.
cWomen, Number sequencing models: Model 1: age, education, wealth; Model 2: Model 1 plus physical activity; Model 3: Model 2 plus depression, quality of life and loneliness.
dWomen, Recall models: Model 1: age, education, wealth, sexual activity × age; Model 2: Model 1 plus physical activity, sexual activity × physical activity, sexual activity × cohabiting; Model 3: Model 2 plus depression, quality of life and loneliness.
Discussion
Previous research has shown that older men who are sexually active also have increased levels of general cognitive function [12, 13]. The current study supports these findings in two different cognitive tasks, namely number sequencing and recall. This association remains after adjusting for confounding variables such quality of life, loneliness, depression and physical activity [2–5]. This indicates an additional benefit of sexual activity on cognition in older men.
For females in the current study, after adjustments for age, education and wealth (Model 1), there was no significant difference in scores on a number sequencing task between sexually active and inactive women. Thus, initial differences were accounted for by age, education and wealth, rather than sexual activity per se. There was however, a significant association between sexual activity and recall scores in women, even after full adjustments (Model 3). These results show an additional benefit of sexual activity, on memory function specifically, in women.
Gender differences in cognitive function [15, 17] may be underpinned by sex differences in brain development and structure [16]. Therefore, it is possible that physiological correlates of sexual activity (e.g. testosterone, oxytocin) have different effects on the brain and hence cognitive function in men and women. Though speculative and an area for future research, the relationship between enhanced cognitive performance and higher levels of sexual activity may be explained via alterations in neurotransmission. For example, the potential cognitive enhancing effects of dopamine, particularly at D1 and D4 receptors [23], the relationship between dopamine and sexual behaviour [24], and other biological processes such as enhanced oxytocin release [25], are all possible mediators in the observed association. Future neuroimaging and psychobiological studies could examine the influence of neurotransmitters in this association in men and women.
Strengths and limitations
This is the first study of this kind to explore the association between sexual activity and cognitive function in a large cohort of older, community-dwelling adults. We have shown associations between sexual activity and two different types of cognitive function in men and for memory specifically in women. It should be noted that the magnitude of differences in scores was small, although this is in line with general findings in the literature [17]. As this was a cross-sectional analysis, we can only speculate as to a causal relationship at this time, where prospective studies would assist with the question of causality.
Implications and conclusions
This study provides a starting point for understanding gender differences in the associations between sexual activity and cognition. The findings have important implications for the inclusion of sexual health discussions during routine health checks for over 50s and for the provision of sexual counselling in this age group. This could also provide a modest benefit to cognitive function in older adults.
Key points.
There is limited research on the possible effects of sexual activity on cognition in later life.
Sexual activity is associated with higher scores on tests of memory and executive function, in adults aged 50–89.
Maintaining healthy sexual relationships in later life may be associated with better cognitive function.
Conflicts of interest
None declared.
References
- 1.Marioni RE, van den Hout A, Valenzuela MJ, Brayne C, Matthews FE; MRC Cognitive Function and Ageing Study. Active cognitive lifestyle associates with cognitive recovery and a reduced risk of cognitive decline. J Alzheimers Dis 2012; 28: 223–30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Llewellyn DJ, Lang IA, Langa KM, Huppert FA. Cognitive function and psychological well-being: findings from a population-based cohort. Age Ageing 2008; 37: 685–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Holwerda TJ, Deeg DJH, Beekman ATF et al. . Feelings of loneliness, but not social isolation, predict dementia onset: results from the Amsterdam Study of the Elderly (AMSTEL). J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2014; 85: 135–42. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Wilson RS, Barnes LL, Mendes de Leon CF et al. . Depressive symptoms, cognitive decline, and risk of AD in older persons. Neurology 2002; 59: 364–70. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Blondell SJ, Hammersley-Mather R, Veerman JL. Does physical activity prevent cognitive decline and dementia? A systematic review and meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. BMC Public Health 2014; 14: 510. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Angevaren M, Aufdemkampe G, Verhaar HJJ, Aleman A, Vanhees L. Physical activity and enhanced fitness to improve cognitive function in older people without known cognitive impairment. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 2008; 3: CD005381. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Levine GN, Steinke EE, Bakaeen FG et al. . Sexual activity and cardiovascular disease: a scientific statement from the American Heart Association. Circulation 2012; 125: 1058–72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Momtaz YA, Hamid TA, Ibrahim R. The impact of mild cognitive impairment on sexual activity. A J Alzheimers Dis Other Demen 2013; 28: 759–62. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Ballard CG, Solis M, Gahir M et al. . Sexual relationships in married dementia sufferers. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 1997; 12: 447–51. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Derouesné C, Guigot J, CHermat V et al. . Sexual behavioral changes in Alzheimer disease. Alz Dis Assoc Disord 1996; 10: 86–92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Hartmans C, Comijs H, Jonker C. Cognitive functioning and its influence on sexual behaviour in normal aging and dementia. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2013; 29: 441–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Dello Buono M, Zaghi PC, Padoani W et al. . Sexual feelings and sexual life in an Italian sample of 335 elderly 65 to 106-year-olds. Arch Gerontol Geriatr 1998; 26: 155–62. [Google Scholar]
- 13.Padoani W, Dello Buono M, Marietta P et al. . Influence of cognitive status on the sexual life of 352 elderly Italians aged 65–105 years. Gerontology 2000; 46: 258–65. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Lindau ST, Schumm LP, Laumann EO et al. . A study of sexuality and health among older adults in the United States. New Eng J Med 2007; 357: 762–74. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Munro CA, Winicki JM, Schretlen DJ et al. . Sex differences in cognition in healthy elderly individuals. Neuropsychol Dev Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn 2012; 19: 759–68. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Zaidi ZF. Gender differences in human brain: a review. Open Anat J 2010; 2: 37–55. [Google Scholar]
- 17.Andreano JM, Cahill L. Sex influences on the neurobiology of learning and memory. Learn Mem 2009; 16: 248–66. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Lee DM, Nazroo J, O'Connor DB, Blake M, Pendleton N. Sexual health and well-being among older men and women in England: findings from the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing. Arch Sex Behav 2015; 000: 1–12; doi:10.1007/s10508-014-0465-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Radloff LS. The CES-D scale: a self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Appl Psychol Meas 1977; 1: 385–401. [Google Scholar]
- 20.Steffick DE. Documentation of affective functioning measures in the Health and Retirement Study. HRS Health Working Group; Report Number: DR-005, 2000. [Google Scholar]
- 21.Hyde M, Wiggins RD, Higgs P, Blane DB. A measure of quality of life in early old age: the theory, development and properties of a needs satisfaction model (CASP-19). Aging Ment Health 2003; 7: 186–94. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Hughes ME, Waite LJ, Hawkley LC, Cacioppo JT. A short scale for measuring loneliness in large lurveys: results from two population-based studies. Res Aging 2004; 26: 655–72. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Furth KE, Mastwal S, Wang KH, Buonanno A, Vullhorst D. Dopamine, cognitive function, and gamma oscillations: role of D4 receptors. Front Cell Neurosci 2013; 7: 1–19. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Melis MR, Argiolas A. Dopamine and sexual behavior. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 1995; 19: 19–38. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Guastella AJ, Einfeld SL, Gray KM et al. . Intranasal oxytocin improves emotion recognition for youth with autism spectrum disorders. Biol Psychiat 2010; 67: 692–4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]