Abstract
Background: Energy drink use is associated with increased risk behavior among adolescents and college students. This study examined this relationship in a nationwide sample of young adults and also examined relations between energy drink use and delay discounting.
Methods: Participants were 874 U.S. adults 18–28 years of age with past 30-day consumption of caffeine and alcohol. Participants completed an online survey of energy drink use, drug use, sexual activity, alcohol misuse (alcohol use disorders identification test [AUDIT]), sensation seeking (four-item Brief Sensation Seeking Scale [BSSS-4]), and delay discounting of monetary rewards and condom use.
Results: Over one-third of participants (n = 303) reported consuming energy drinks at least once per week. Weekly energy drink users were more likely than less-than-weekly energy drink users to report a recent history of risk behaviors, including cigarette smoking (56% vs. 28%, p < 0.0001), illicit stimulant use (22% vs. 6%, p < 0.0001), and unprotected sex (63% vs. 45%, p < 0.0001). Covariate-adjusted analyses found that weekly energy drink users did not have significantly higher BSSS-4 scores (3.5 vs. 3.1, p = 0.098), but they had higher mean AUDIT scores (8.0 vs. 4.8, p < 0.0001), and they more steeply discounted delayed monetary rewards. Although weekly energy drink users did not show steeper discounting of delayed condom use, they showed a lower likelihood of using a condom when one was immediately available.
Conclusions: This study extends findings that energy drink use is associated with risk behavior, and it is the first study to show that energy drink use is associated with monetary delay discounting.
Introduction
The popularity of energy drinks is growing at a remarkable rate. According to industry reports, annual sales of energy drinks and shots rose 60% in the United States between 2008 and 2012, and sales are expected to continue to increase at a similar rate, exceeding $21 billion per year in 2017.1 Energy drink use has become particularly common among teenagers and young adults. Although recent nationwide data on energy drink consumption are scarce, some researchers estimate that over 18% of young adults consume energy drinks at least weekly,2 and, among college students, this percentage may be as high as 39%.3
Emerging evidence points to a relationship between energy drink use and various risk behaviors, including alcohol abuse,2–10 cigarette smoking,2,6–9,11–13 illicit drug use,2,6–9,11–13 nonmedical use of prescription drugs,5,6,8,11,14 fighting,8 and sexual risk behavior.8 One of the primary aims of this study was to replicate and extend these findings. Although previous studies typically surveyed high school and university students,3,4,7–11,14 this study collected data from a nationwide sample of young adults using Amazon Mechanical Turk (MTurk), a website that allows researchers to rapidly and affordably collect data from a population of over 500,000 MTurk account holders.15–18 MTurk is a popular platform for participant recruitment and data collection among behavioral and social scientists, and several studies conducted with MTurk have replicated empirical and descriptive results obtained through traditional laboratory and survey methods.19–22
Another aim of this study was to examine associations between energy drink use and delay discounting.23–25 As the delay to a reward increases, its perceived value typically decreases, and many risk behaviors are associated with increased delay discounting. For example, individuals with a history of risk behavior often discount delayed monetary rewards more so than individuals without such a history.26–28 Importantly, the concept of delay discounting has been extended from monetary rewards to other domains, including condom use. The Sexual Delay Discounting Task was developed to examine hypothetical choices between immediate unprotected sex and delayed sex with a condom.29 This task shows good test–retest reliability,30 and the outcomes are associated with self-reported sexual risk behavior.29,31–33 As with monetary delay discounting, individuals with a history of risk behavior (e.g., substance abuse) show steeper delay discounting of condom-protected sex.31–34 Collectively, studies that examined monetary and sexual delay discounting outcomes suggest that delay discounting is a fundamental behavioral process related to risk behavior. To our knowledge, however, no studies have investigated the relationship between energy drink use and monetary or sexual delay discounting.
Methods
Participants
Participants were recruited via MTurk (www.mturk.com). Researchers created an MTurk “Requester” account and posted the study on a searchable database of “Human Intelligence Tasks” (HITs). Keywords for the HIT were as follows: survey, demographics, psychology, and questionnaire. The HIT could be viewed only by individuals registered as “Workers” on MTurk who resided in the United States and had an approval rating from former Requesters that was ≥95%.35 The HIT was titled, “Behavioral health & decision-making study,” and its stated purpose was to learn how individuals “make decisions related to diet, sex, money, and drugs.” The HIT was launched on Tuesday, July 29, 2014 at 10:47 AM EST and closed on Sunday, August 3, 2014 at 9:40 PM EST.
Participants who selected the HIT were informed that they would receive up to $3.00 for successful completion of the HIT: $1.50 for completing the HIT and a $1.50 bonus for paying attention during the survey and answering questions carefully. Participants were required to pass a brief qualification survey before they could access the main survey. After informed consent was obtained, the qualification survey assessed the following undisclosed inclusion criteria: participants had to be 18–28 years of age, reside in the United States, and correctly answer two attention check questions (i.e., “trick” questions”35). Individuals who reported no caffeine or alcohol use in the past month were excluded from participation. In addition, individuals who indicated that they did not want to answer questions about sexual, criminal, and drug use history were excluded from participation.
Of the 2794 participants who took the qualification survey, 1650 participants were excluded and not permitted to take the main survey because they reported that they were 29 years of age or older. Among the remaining participants, 1014 met all other inclusion criteria and took the main survey. The main survey took an average of 32 minutes and 14 seconds to complete. Data from participants who did not complete the main survey or failed to pass additional attention check questions contained within the main survey were excluded from analysis. The final study sample included 874 participants. Study procedures were approved by the Johns Hopkins University Institutional Review Board.
Measures
The survey was hosted by Qualtrics (Provo, UT) and contained questions about demographics, energy drink consumption, drug use, sexual risk behavior,* alcohol misuse, sensation seeking, and delay discounting.
Alcohol misuse was assessed with the 10-item alcohol use disorders identification test (AUDIT).36 AUDIT scores range from 0 to 40, with higher scores indicating greater alcohol misuse.
A four-item version of the Brief Sensation Seeking Scale (BSSS-4),37,38 was used to assess propensity for sensation seeking. BSSS-4 scores range from 1 to 5 with higher scores indicating greater sensation seeking.
A 27-item monetary choice questionnaire39 was used to assess delay discounting of hypothetical monetary rewards. Discounting rates obtained with this commonly used brief task are well correlated with those obtained with a more comprehensive discounting procedure.40 The monetary choice questionnaire contains nine questions about each of three delayed reward magnitudes: small ($25, $30, or $35), medium ($50, $55, or $60), and large ($75, $80, or $85). Participants were presented with a series of choices between a smaller amount of money today and a larger amount of money after a variable delay (e.g., “Would you prefer $20 today or $55 in 7 days?”; see Kirby et al.39 for a complete list of questions).
The Sexual Delay Discounting Task29 was used to assess delay discounting of condom use in casual sex situations. A detailed description of the task, including an illustration, has been previously published.32 Briefly, participants were presented with an array of 60 photographs of racially and ethnically diverse, clothed individuals (30 men and 30 women) and instructed to check a box next to the photograph of each individual with whom they would be willing to have sex, assuming they were not in a committed relationship, they liked the individual's personality, and there was no risk of pregnancy. Alternatively, participants could select the following option: “I would not have sex with any of the people above, even if I liked their personalities and was not in a committed relationship.” Participants who selected this option and participants who did not select at least two photographs could not complete the remainder of the Sexual Delay Discounting Task.
Participants were asked to identify from among their selections the individual who they (1) most wanted to have sex with (“most sex”), (2) least wanted to have sex with (“least sex”), (3) believed was most likely to have a sexually transmitted infection (“most STI”), and (4) believed was least likely to have an STI (“least STI”). The remaining questions in the Sexual Delay Discounting Task pertained only to the photographs that represented these four partner conditions. In the presence of the photograph and the corresponding description of the partner condition (e.g., “This is the person you would MOST want to have sex with.”), participants used a visual analog scale to rate the likelihood that they would use a condom to have sex with the partner if a condom was readily and immediately available (i.e., the “zero-delay trial”). The leftmost position on the scale specified “0” along with the text “I will definitely have sex with this person without a condom.” The rightmost position on the scale specified “100” along with the text “I will definitely have sex with this person with a condom.” Participants then used similar visual analog scales to rate the likelihood that they would have immediate sex without a condom versus waiting to have sex with a condom at each of the following delays presented in ascending order: 1 hour, 3 hours, 6 hours, 1 day, 1 week, 1 month, and 3 months. Due to a programming error, the 1 month delay scenario was not presented to some participants; thus, data from this delay scenario were excluded for all participants in data analyses.
Data analysis
Energy drink use
Participants were asked, “During a typical week, on how many days do you drink energy drinks?” (The following products were listed as examples: Red Bull, 5-hour ENERGY Shot, Monster Energy, Rockstar, NOS, Amp, Full Throttle, and Xyience.) From a drop-down menu, participants could choose one of eight response options, ranging from “0” to “7.” Participants were dichotomized as “less-than-weekly energy drink users” if they reported drinking energy drinks on 0 days during a typical week (n = 571) or “weekly energy drink users” if they reported drinking energy drinks on at least 1 day during a typical week (n = 303). Selection of these two response categories was informed by previous research2–4 and the distribution of responses to this question (the majority of weekly energy drink users [n = 168] consumed energy drinks on 1 day per week, and very few weekly energy drink users [n = 47] consumed energy drinks on 4 or more days per week).
Monetary delay discounting
Delay discounting data were analyzed under the assumption of a hyperbolic model of decay: V = A/(1 + kD), wherein, V represents subjective reward value, A is the objective or nondiscounted reward value, D is the delay to reward, and k is a free parameter that corresponds with the rate of delay discounting.41 The monetary choice questionnaire was designed such that a participant's discounting parameter, k, could be estimated based on the pattern of responding across the items.39 We identified the point at which each participant switched from a smaller-sooner preference to a larger-later preference within each delayed reward magnitude and calculated the geometric mean of the k values for the last smaller-sooner choice item and the first larger-later choice item (see Kirby et al.39 for a complete list of questionnaire items and corresponding k values). When choices were nonsystematic (i.e., more than one switch point), then k values were not estimated for that participant (k values were not estimated for 21% of the final sample; n = 185). Values of k ranged from 0.00016 (all larger-later choices) to 0.25 (all smaller-sooner choices). Because the distribution of k values was non-normal, we applied a log10 transformation to all values before further analysis.
Responses on the monetary choice questionnaire were also analyzed in terms of the proportion of larger–later choices overall and at each reward magnitude for all participants. Although the proportion of larger–later choices is highly correlated with k values, it does not assume the hyperbolic model of delay discounting.42
Sexual delay discounting
Eighty-eight percent of participants (n = 767) completed the Sexual Delay Discounting Task. For these participants, likelihood of condom use was plotted as a function of delay to condom availability (in hours) and was summarized using an area under the curve (AUC) measure,43 which ranged from 0 to 1 (corresponding to 0–100% likelihood of having condom-protected sex at each delay). Because individual differences occur in the likelihood of using an immediately available condom, a standardized AUC measure was calculated for each partner condition to isolate the effect of delay on the likelihood of condom use. For the standardized AUC measures, the likelihood value for each delay in each partner condition was divided by the likelihood value of the zero-delay trial in that condition. Thus, data from participants who reported zero likelihood of using an immediately available condom (n = 123 in the “least STI” partner condition; n = 9 in the “most STI” partner condition; n = 46 in the “least sex” partner condition; n = 120 in the “most sex” partner condition) were excluded from the standardized AUC analysis because the effect of delay on the value of condom-protected sex was undefined. Lower standardized AUC values indicate a lower likelihood of waiting to engage in condom-protected sex (i.e., steeper delay discounting), and higher standardized AUC values indicate a greater likelihood of waiting to engage in condom-protected sex (i.e., shallower delay discounting).
Statistical analyses
Statistical analyses were conducted with SPSS® 22 (Armonk, NY) and SAS® 9.3 (Cary, NC). Demographic characteristics were compared between groups using independent t-tests and chi-square tests. To test the internal validity of the monetary choice task, discounting for the three magnitude ranges within the task were compared within-subjects using repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA). Subsequent analyses of the task used composite measures across magnitudes: mean log10 k and overall proportion of larger-later responses. To test the internal validity of the Sexual Delay Discounting Task, discounting between the two pairs of partner conditions (i.e., “most sex” vs. “least sex” and “most STI” vs. “least STI”) were compared within-subjects using repeated measures ANOVA.
Logistic regression was used to examine the relationship between energy drink consumption and self-reported risk behavior (e.g., cigarette smoking, illicit drug use, unprotected sex) while adjusting for the following demographic variables: age, sex, race, employment, education, income, and marital status. In addition, AUDIT scores, BSSS-4 scores, monetary delay discounting measures (mean log10 k values and proportion of larger-later choices), and sexual delay discounting measures (zero-delay likelihood of condom use and standardized AUC) were analyzed with a one-factor model (weekly energy drink use) covarying for the same demographic variables listed above plus past year cigarette smoking and past year use of any illicit drugs or nonmedical use of prescription drugs with a compound symmetry covariance structure in SAS Proc Mixed. These additional covariates were included in the analysis because previous research has shown that cigarette smoking and illicit drug use are related to delay discounting,24,27,32–34 sensation seeking,44,45 and alcohol misuse.46 Further, AUDIT score was added as a covariate in the analysis of BSSS-4 scores, monetary delay discounting measures, and sexual delay discounting measures to adjust for the potential influence that alcohol misuse may have on these outcomes.
Results
Demographic characteristics are displayed in Table 1. Weekly energy drink users and less-than-weekly energy drink users were similar across most characteristics. The mean age of weekly energy drink users (M = 24.1, SD = 2.6) was significantly higher than that of less-than-weekly energy drink users (M = 23.7, SD = 2.7, t = −2.09, p = 0.037); however, it is unlikely that this difference of 0.4 years was clinically meaningful. A significantly lower percentage of weekly energy drink users were female (51% vs. 68%, χ2 = 22.7, p < 0.0001), students (21% vs. 32%, χ2 = 9.95, p = 0.002), and unemployed (11% vs. 20%, χ2 = 11.5, p = 0.001), and a significantly higher percentage of weekly energy drink users were employed full-time (50% vs. 33%, χ2 = 24.2, p < 0.0001) and divorced (5% vs. 1%, χ2 = 14.4, p < 0.001).
Table 1.
Demographic characteristic | Less than weekly energy drink use (n = 571) | Weekly energy drink use (n = 303) | Total sample (n = 874) |
---|---|---|---|
Mean years of age (SD)a | 23.7 (2.7) | 24.1 (2.6) | 23.9 (2.7) |
Sex (% female)b | 68 | 51 | 62 |
Ethnicity (% non-Hispanic) | 92 | 92 | 92 |
Race (%) | |||
White or Caucasian | 79 | 83 | 80 |
Black or African American | 6 | 4 | 5 |
Asian | 7 | 7 | 7 |
Native Hawaiian or other Pacific Islander | <1 | 0 | <1 |
American Indian or Alaska Native | <1 | 1 | <1 |
Other | 2 | <1 | 1 |
More than one race | 5 | 6 | 5 |
Annual household income (%) | |||
Under $25,000 | 31 | 29 | 30 |
$25,000–$34,999 | 18 | 17 | 18 |
$35,000–$49,999 | 17 | 19 | 18 |
$50,000–$74,999 | 16 | 20 | 17 |
$75,000–$99,999 | 9 | 7 | 8 |
$100,000–$124,999 | 5 | 6 | 5 |
$125,000–$150,000 | 2 | 1 | 2 |
Over $150,000 | 3 | 1 | 2 |
Highest level of education (%) | |||
No high school diploma | 1 | 1 | 1 |
High school diploma or equivalent | 9 | 8 | 9 |
Some college, no degree | 34 | 40 | 36 |
Trade, technical, vocational training | 2 | 2 | 2 |
Associate's degree | 8 | 11 | 9 |
Bachelor's degree | 35 | 32 | 34 |
Master's degree | 8 | 7 | 8 |
Professional/doctorate degree | 2 | <1 | 1 |
Employment status (%) | |||
Employed full-timeb | 33 | 50 | 39 |
Employed part-time | 15 | 17 | 16 |
Studentc | 32 | 21 | 28 |
Unemployedb | 20 | 11 | 17 |
Marital status (%) | |||
Married | 19 | 19 | 19 |
Widowed | 0 | 0 | 0 |
Divorcedb | 1 | 5 | 3 |
Never married | 80 | 75 | 78 |
p ≤ 0.05 represent significant group differences between weekly energy drink users and less-than-weekly energy drink users based on results of t-tests for continuous variables and chi-square tests for categorical variables.
p ≤ 0.001 represent significant group differences between weekly energy drink users and less-than-weekly energy drink users based on results of t-tests for continuous variables and chi-square tests for categorical variables.
p ≤ 0.01 represent significant group differences between weekly energy drink users and less-than-weekly energy drink users based on results of t-tests for continuous variables and chi-square tests for categorical variables.
Outcomes related to past year drug use, sexual risk behavior, and other risk behavior are displayed in Table 2. After adjusting for demographic characteristics, weekly energy drink users were significantly more likely than less-than-weekly energy drink users to report all risk-related outcomes with the exception of one outcome (i.e., past year engagement in an activity that led to the physical injury of oneself or others).
Table 2.
Percentage | Adjusted analysesa | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
Behavior | Less-than-weekly energy drink use (n = 571) | Weekly energy drink use (n = 303) | W (p) | Odds ratio (95% CI) |
Past year drug use | ||||
Cigarettes | 28 | 56 | 56.0 (<0.0001) | 3.27 (2.40–4.45) |
Cannabis | 38 | 56 | 23.0 (<0.0001) | 2.06 (1.53–2.76) |
Sedatives (e.g., Valium, Xanax) | 13 | 24 | 16.2 (<0.0001) | 2.17 (1.48–3.16) |
Prescription stimulants (e.g., Ritalin, Adderall) | 9 | 20 | 23.2 (<0.0001) | 2.81 (1.85–4.29) |
Illicit stimulants (e.g., cocaine, crystal meth) | 6 | 22 | 37.9 (<0.0001) | 4.11 (2.62–6.44) |
Prescription opioids (e.g., Vicodin, OxyContin) | 12 | 27 | 30.6 (<0.0001) | 2.87 (1.97–4.17) |
Illicit opioids (e.g., heroin, opium) | 2 | 6 | 8.5 (0.004) | 3.51 (1.51–8.20) |
Other (e.g., ecstasy, LSD, bath salts) | 9 | 19 | 17.9 (<0.0001) | 2.47 (1.63–3.76) |
Past year sexual risk behavior | ||||
Unprotected sex (with someone other than spouse) | 45 | 63 | 34.2 (<0.0001) | 2.53 (1.82–3.51) |
Taken advantage of someone sexually or been taken advantage of by someone sexually | 4 | 14 | 13.3 (<0.001) | 2.89 (1.64–5.12) |
Sex with someone who was drunk or high | 41 | 66 | 51.5 (<0.0001) | 3.04 (2.24–4.12) |
Sex while drunk or high | 43 | 69 | 54.7 (<0.0001) | 3.20 (2.35–4.35) |
Sex with someone not known very well | 15 | 31 | 26.1 (<0.0001) | 2.52 (1.77–3.60) |
Sex that was later regretted | 15 | 22 | 9.7 (0.002) | 1.81 (1.25–2.63) |
Past year other risk behavior | ||||
Physical fight | 4 | 14 | 21.0 (<0.0001) | 3.60 (2.08–6.24) |
Drove/rode in vehicle without wearing safety belt | 43 | 53 | 11.0 (0.001) | 1.64 (1.22–2.87) |
Drove while intoxicated | 14 | 30 | 23.7 (<0.0001) | 2.40 (1.69–3.41) |
Rode with intoxicated driver | 21 | 37 | 22.4 (<0.0001) | 2.16 (1.57–2.96) |
Activity that led to arrest of self or other | 2 | 6 | 8.4 (0.004) | 3.43 (1.49–7.88) |
Activity that led to injury of self or other | 8 | 12 | 2.1 (0.15) | 1.43 (0.88–2.31) |
Dangerous/risky activity on dare | 9 | 19 | 15.4 (<0.0001) | 2.38 (1.55–3.67) |
Played extreme sport | 16 | 26 | 9.6 (0.002) | 1.77 (1.23–2.54) |
Adjusted for age, sex, race, employment status, education, income, and marital status. All p-values in bold represent significant group differences between weekly energy drink users and less-than-weekly energy drink users at α = 0.05.
AUDIT scores, BSSS-4 scores, and delay discounting outcomes are displayed in Table 3. Weekly energy drink users had significantly higher AUDIT scores (M = 8.0, SD = 6.4) than less-than-weekly energy drink users (M = 4.8, SD = 4.3, p < 0.0001), and they had significantly higher BSSS-4 scores (M = 3.5, SD = 0.8 vs. M = 3.1, SD = 0.8, p < 0.0001). As shown in Table 3, between-group differences in mean AUDIT scores remained significant after adjusting for demographic characteristics and past year drug use. However, between-group differences in BSSS-4 scores were no long significant after adjusting for AUDIT score and other covariates.
Table 3.
Mean (SD) | F (p) | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
Assessment | Less-than-weekly energy drink use | Weekly energy drink use | Unadjusted analyses | Adjusted analysesa |
AUDIT score | 4.8 (4.3) | 8.0 (6.4) | 76.5 (<0.0001) | 24.1 (<0.0001) |
BSSS-4 score | 3.1 (0.8) | 3.5 (0.8) | 30.6 (<0.0001) | 2.8 (0.098) |
Monetary delay discounting | ||||
Proportion of larger later choices | 0.46 (0.19) | 0.40 (0.18) | 20.7 (<0.0001) | 8.9 (0.003) |
Mean log10k | −2.04 (0.69) | −1.79 (0.66) | 20.1 (<0.0001) | 9.6 (0.002) |
Sexual delay discounting | ||||
Least likely to have STI partner condition | ||||
Likelihood of using condom when immediately available | 0.71 (0.38) | 0.57 (0.41) | 22.6 (<0.0001) | 10.9 (0.001) |
Standardized AUC | 0.50 (0.40) | 0.46 (0.42) | 1.0 (0.31) | 0.3 (0.59) |
Most likely to have STI partner condition | ||||
Likelihood of using condom when immediately available | 0.92 (0.19) | 0.90 (0.22) | 2.5 (0.12) | 0.7 (0.4) |
Standardized AUC | 0.78 (0.33) | 0.74 (0.35) | 1.4 (0.23) | 1.1 (0.29) |
Least want to have sex with partner condition | ||||
Likelihood of using condom when immediately available | 0.83 (0.29) | 0.78 (0.30) | 4.6 (0.03) | 2.1 (0.15) |
Standardized AUC | 0.71 (0.37) | 0.65 (0.35) | 4.0 (0.05) | 0.2 (0.68) |
Most want to have sex with partner condition | ||||
Likelihood of using condom when immediately available | 0.71 (0.38) | 0.57 (0.42) | 21.4 (<0.0001) | 7.9 (0.005) |
Standardized AUC | 0.46 (0.40) | 0.40 (0.42) | 2.5 (0.12) | 0.6 (0.45) |
In the adjusted analyses, all assessment outcomes were analyzed with a one-factor model (weekly energy drink use) covarying for age, sex, race, employment status, education, income, marital status, past year cigarette smoking, and past year use of any illicit drugs or nonmedical use of prescription drugs. For BSSS-4, monetary delay discounting, and sexual delay discounting outcomes, AUDIT score was also included as a covariate. All p-values in bold represent significant group differences between weekly energy drink users and less-than-weekly energy drink users at α = 0.05.
AUC, area under the curve; AUDIT, alcohol use disorders identification test; BSSS-4, four-item Brief Sensation Seeking Scale; STI, sexually transmitted infection.
In testing the internal validity of the monetary choice questionnaire, a significant main effect of magnitude was found in which larger rewards were discounted less steeply than smaller rewards (log10 k, F = 664.32, p < 0.0001; proportion larger later responses, F = 731.55, p < 0.0001), replicating a well-established finding of monetary delay discounting.47 Also replicated was the finding that overall proportion of larger-later choices was highly correlated with mean log10 k (r = −0.99, p < 0.0001).42 Weekly energy drink users had higher mean log10 k values than less-than-weekly energy drink users (M = −1.79, SD = 0.66 vs. M = −2.04, SD = 0.69, p < 0.0001), indicating steeper discounting of delayed monetary rewards, and they chose a significantly lower proportion of larger-later rewards than less-than-weekly energy drink users (M = 0.40, SD = 0.18 vs. M = 0.46, SD = 0.19, p < 0.0001). As shown in Table 3, these differences remained significant following the covariate adjusted analyses.
In testing the internal validity of the Sexual Delay Discounting Task, a significant effect of partner condition on standardized AUC was found (“most sex” AUC<“least sex” AUC, F = 372.18, p < 0.0001; “most STI” AUC>“least STI,” F = 439.48, p < 0.0001), which replicated a finding that there is typically steeper delay discounting of condom-protected sex when partners are perceived as more desirable and less likely to have an STI.29–33 Weekly energy drink users were less likely than less-than-weekly energy drink users to use a condom if one was immediately available (i.e., during the zero-delay trial) in the “least STI” partner condition (M = 0.57, SD = 0.41 vs. M = 0.71, SD = 0.38, p < 0.0001) and in the “most sex” partner condition (M = 0.57, SD = 0.42 vs. M = 0.71, SD = 0.38, p < 0.0001). Weekly energy drink users also had a lower mean standardized AUC than less-than-weekly energy drink users in the “least sex” partner condition (M = 0.65, SD = 0.35 vs. M = 0.71, SD = 0.37, p = 0.05); however, as shown in Table 3, this finding was no longer significant following the covariate adjusted analyses. Thus, the only between-group differences in the Sexual Delay Discounting Task data that remained significant following the adjusted analyses were the mean likelihoods of using an immediately available condom when partners were perceived as less likely to have an STI (“least STI”) or more desirable (“most sex”).
Discussion
The results of this study replicate and extend findings that self-reported patterns of energy drink consumption are associated with retrospective reports of risk behavior. Participants who consumed energy drinks at least once per week were more likely than less-than-weekly energy drink users to report past year cigarette smoking, nonmedical use of prescription drugs, illicit drug use, unprotected sex, and other risk behavior (Table 2). In addition, weekly energy drink users were more likely to report alcohol misuse as indicated by significantly higher AUDIT scores (Table 3). Notably, AUDIT scores ≥8 are highly correlated with hazardous alcohol use,36,48 and significantly more weekly energy drink users had AUDIT scores in this range (45% vs. 21%, χ2 = 54.5, p < 0.0001). Although weekly energy drink users demonstrated a greater propensity for sensation seeking than less-than-weekly energy drink users as evidenced by significantly higher BSSS-4 scores, this difference was no longer significant after adjusting for AUDIT score and other covariates.
To our knowledge, this study is the first to demonstrate that weekly energy drink use is associated with steeper delay discounting of monetary rewards. That is, relative to less-than-weekly energy drink users, weekly energy drink users chose significantly fewer larger-later reward options on the monetary choice questionnaire, and they had significantly higher mean log10 k values after adjusting for demographic characteristics, past year cigarette smoking, AUDIT score, and past year illicit drug use or nonmedical use of prescription drugs (Table 3).
Analyses of the Sexual Delay Discounting Task data revealed no evidence that weekly energy drink users more steeply discounted delayed condom use after adjusting for covariates. However, data from the task show that, when a condom is readily and immediately available (i.e., during the zero-delay trials), weekly energy drink users were significantly less likely to use a condom with partners they perceived as more desirable and less likely to have an STI (Table 3). Taken together, results of this study suggest that energy drink use is associated with a broad pattern of impulsivity that can be assessed via self-report and performance on decision-making tasks.
Future studies should evaluate the extent to which these findings generalize from MTurk users to the general population. Although the prevalence of weekly energy drink use observed in the current sample (35%) was similar to that observed in some college samples (39%),3 it was higher than that observed in a nationwide sample of young adults in 2008 and 2009 (19%).2 Notably, participants in this study were slightly younger than participants in that previous study (i.e., 18–28 vs. 20–34 years of age), and data from this study were collected 5–6 years after data were collected in the previous study. Nonetheless, the primary aim of this study was not to estimate the prevalence of weekly energy drink use in the United States; rather, the aims were to examine relations between energy drink use, risk behavior, and delay discounting. Indeed, many of the relationships observed among current study outcomes replicated findings from previous studies that used more traditional survey methods. For example, energy drink users were more likely to be male,2,5,8,11 employed,13 have higher AUDIT scores,3 and they were more likely to engage in various risk behaviors.2–11,13,14 In addition, results from the delay discounting tasks replicated results from studies that found that larger monetary rewards are discounted less steeply than smaller rewards47 and delay discounting of condom-protected sex is steeper when partners are perceived as more desirable and less likely to have an STI.29–33
The results of this study should be interpreted within the context of several limitations. First, all outcomes were based on self-report. Consequently, over- or under-reporting may have occurred in response to retrospective report questions. Second, questions on the monetary choice questionnaire and Sexual Delay Discounting Task were hypothetical. Importantly, however, similar rates of delay discounting have been observed with both hypothetical and real monetary rewards.24,49,50 In addition, previous studies have shown associations between performance on the Sexual Delay Discounting Task and self-report of real-world sexual risk behavior.29,31,33 Third, the sample was relatively racially and ethnically homogeneous. Over 90% of the sample was non-Hispanic and 80% was White. Future studies should extend the results of this study to a more racially and ethnically diverse sample. Fourth, to increase the likelihood that the study sample would include participants with exposure to energy drinks and alcohol, the study included only participants who reported consuming caffeine and alcohol at least once during the past month. Future studies should include caffeine and alcohol abstainers for comparison. Finally, this study was not designed to examine the independent relationships between impulsivity and alcohol use, impulsivity and energy drink use, and impulsivity and alcohol mixed with energy drink use. To our knowledge, these relationships have not been prospectively examined within the same study. However, some research has shown that a history of consuming alcohol mixed with energy drinks is associated with many of the outcomes examined in this study, including various risk behaviors51 and monetary delay discounting.52 Notably, few participants in this study reported consuming alcohol mixed with energy drinks on a weekly basis (9%; n = 76), but many more participants reported past year consumption of alcohol mixed with energy drinks (34%; n = 299). After adjusting for the latter variable in subsequent analyses, differences between energy drink users and less-than-weekly energy drink users remained significant across all but two risk-related outcomes (“drove/rode in vehicle without wearing safety belt” and “played an extreme sport”; data not shown). In addition, differences observed in AUDIT scores, monetary choice questionnaire outcomes, and outcomes from the Sexual Delay Discounting Task remained significant after adjusting for this additional covariate, suggesting that frequent energy drink use was associated with risk behavior and impulsivity regardless of whether participants consumed alcohol mixed with energy drinks. Nevertheless, future studies are needed to prospectively investigate the combined and independent effects of alcohol and energy drink use on impulsivity and risk behavior.
The nature of the relationship between energy drink use and risk behavior remains unclear. Some researchers have proposed that energy drink use is one of many activities associated with a larger pattern of impulsive or risky behavior.4,8 If this theory is correct, then marketing strategies that advertise the stimulant effects of energy drinks may promote energy drink consumption among individuals who are predisposed to or already engaging in risk behavior. However, the key to the relationship between energy drink use and risk behavior might also lie in the main psychoactive ingredient in energy drinks—caffeine. Some research has shown that caffeine dependence and heavy caffeine use from other sources (e.g., coffee) are associated with dependence on alcohol and illicit drugs.53 Other recent research has shown that frequent soft drink consumption9 and coffee consumption54 are positively and independently associated with drug use. These studies further show that soft drink and coffee consumers who also frequently consume energy drinks are even more likely to use drugs than individuals who consume only soft drinks or coffee. Future research should attempt to further clarify associations between caffeine use, energy drink consumption, and impulsivity by prospectively examining the acute and chronic effects of caffeine and energy drink consumption on measures of impulsivity, such as monetary delay discounting.
The results of this study add to a growing literature pointing to frequent energy drink use as a marker for risk behavior in teenagers and young adults. Given that rates of illicit drug use and STI transmission are highest in this age group,55,56 further investigation of this behavioral marker is warranted to determine whether it has potential to aid parents, educators, and clinicians in identifying individuals who are most likely to engage in risk behavior.
Acknowledgments
Research and article preparation was supported by National Institutes of Health grants R01DA003890, T32DA007209, R01DA027615, R01AA021446, R01DA032363, R01DA035277, and R21DA032717. The sponsor had no further role in the study design; the collection, analysis, or interpretation of the data; the writing of the article; or in the decision to submit the article for publication. The authors thank Paul Nuzzo and Linda Felch for their assistance with data analysis.
Author Disclosure Statement
No competing financial interests exist.
Sexual risk questions were adapted from Miller.8 These and all other survey questions were the same across participants with one exception—a question about past year unprotected sex contained different languages for “never-married” versus “ever-married” participants. Never-married participants were asked, “During the past year, how many times have you had sexual intercourse without using a condom?”; whereas, ever-married participants were asked, “During the past year, how many times have you had sexual intercourse without using a condom with someone other than your current or former spouse?”
References
- 1.Packaged Facts. Energy Drinks and Shots: U.S. Market Trends; 2013. Available at www.packagedfacts.com/Energy-Drinks-Shots-7124908/ (accessed September14, 2015)
- 2.Larson N, Laska MN, Story M, Neumark-Sztainer D. Sports and energy drink consumption are linked to health-risk behaviours among young adults. Public Health Nutr. 2015;18:1–10 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Skewes MC, Decou CR, Gonzalez VM. Energy drink use, problem drinking and drinking motives in a diverse sample of Alaskan college students. Int J Circumpolar Health. 2013:72 eCollection [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Arria AM, Caldeira KM, Kasperski SJ, Vincent KB, Griffiths RR, O'Grady KE. Energy drink consumption and increased risk for alcohol dependence. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2011;35:365–375 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Gallucci AR, Martin RJ, Morgan GB. The consumption of energy drinks among a sample of college students and college student athletes. J Community Health. 2015. [Epub ahead of print]: DOI: 10.1007/s10900-015-0075-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Hamilton HA, Boak A, Ilie G, Mann RE. Energy drink consumption and associations with demographic characteristics, drug use and injury among adolescents. Can J Public Health. 2013;104:e496–e501 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Hull L, Dillon PM, O'Connell MM, Chitnavis P, Svikis DS. P.6.016 is use of caffeinated energy drinks associated with higher levels of tobacco, alcohol and other drug use in American students? Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2011;21:S168–S169 [Google Scholar]
- 8.Miller KE. Energy drinks, race, and problem behaviors among college students. J Adolesc Health. 2008;43:490–497 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Terry-McElrath YM, O'Malley PM, Johnston LD. Energy drinks, soft drinks, and substance use among United States secondary school students. J Addict Med. 2014;8:6–13 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Velazquez CE, Poulos NS, Latimer LA, Pasch KE. Associations between energy drink consumption and alcohol use behaviors among college students. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2012;123:167–172 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Arria AM, Caldeira KM, Kasperski SJ, et al. . Increased alcohol consumption, nonmedical prescription drug use, and illicit drug use are associated with energy drink consumption among college students. J Addict Med. 2010;4:74–80 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Azagba S, Langille D, Asbridge M. An emerging adolescent health risk: caffeinated energy drink consumption patterns among high school students. Prev Med. 2014;62:54–59 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Trapp GS, Allen KL, O'Sullivan T, Robinson M, Jacoby P, Oddy WH. Energy drink consumption among young Australian adults: associations with alcohol and illicit drug use. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2014;134:30–37 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Woolsey CL, Williams RD, Jr., Jacobson BH, et al. . Increased energy drink use as a predictor of illicit prescription stimulant use. Subst Abus. 2014;35:96–103 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Berinsky AJ, Huber GA, Lenz GS. Evaluating online labor markets for experimental research: Amazon.com's Mechanical Turk. Polit Anal. 2012;20:351–368
- 16.Buhrmester M, Kwang T, Gosling SD. Amazon's Mechanical Turk a new source of inexpensive, yet high-quality, data? Perspect Psychol Sci. 2011;6:3–5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Paolacci G, Chandler J, Ipeirotis PG. Running experiments on Amazon Mechanical Turk. Judgm Decis Making. 2010;5:411–419 [Google Scholar]
- 18.The Economist. The Roar of the Crowd; 2012. Available at www.economist.com/node/21555876 (accessed September14, 2015)
- 19.Crump MJ, McDonnell JV, Gureckis TM. Evaluating Amazon's Mechanical Turk as a tool for experimental behavioral research. PLoS One. 2013;8:e57410. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Jarmolowicz DP, Bickel WK, Carter AE, Franck CT, Mueller ET. Using crowdsourcing to examine relations between delay and probability discounting. Behav Processes. 2012;91:308–312 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Johnson PS, Herrmann ES, Johnson MW. Opportunity costs of reward delays and the discounting of hypothetical money and cigarettes. J Exp Anal Behav. 2015;103:87–107 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Sprouse J. A validation of Amazon Mechanical Turk for the collection of acceptability judgments in linguistic theory. Behav Res Methods. 2011;43:155–167 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Ainslie G. Specious reward: a behavioral theory of impulsiveness and impulse control. Psychol Bull. 1975;82:463. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Bickel WK, Odum AL, Madden GJ. Impulsivity and cigarette smoking: delay discounting in current, never, and ex-smokers. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 1999;146:447–454 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Rachlin H, Green L. Commitment, choice and self-control. J Exp Anal Behav. 1972;17:15–22 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Daugherty JR, Brase GL. Taking time to be healthy: predicting health behaviors with delay discounting and time perspective. Pers Indiv Differ. 2010;48:202–207 [Google Scholar]
- 27.MacKillop J, Amlung MT, Few LR, Ray LA, Sweet LH, Munafò MR. Delayed reward discounting and addictive behavior: a meta-analysis. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2011;216:305–321 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Petry NM. Delay discounting of money and alcohol in actively using alcoholics, currently abstinent alcoholics, and controls. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2001;154:243–250 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Johnson MW, Bruner NR. The sexual discounting task: HIV risk behavior and the discounting of delayed sexual rewards in cocaine dependence. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2012;123:15–21 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Johnson MW, Bruner NR. Test–retest reliability and gender differences in the sexual discounting task among cocaine-dependent individuals. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2013;21:277–286 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Dariotis JK, Johnson MW. Sexual discounting among high-risk youth ages 18–24: implications for sexual and substance use risk behaviors. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2015;23:49–58 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Herrmann ES, Hand DJ, Johnson MW, Badger GJ, Heil SH. Examining delay discounting of condom-protected sex among opioid-dependent women and non-drug-using control women. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2014;144:53–60 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Herrmann E, Johnson P, Johnson M. Examining delay discounting of condom-protected sex among men who have sex with men using crowdsourcing technology. AIDS Behav. 2015;19:1655–1665 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Johnson MW, Johnson PS, Herrmann ES, Sweeney MM. Delay and probability discounting of sexual and monetary outcomes in individuals with cocaine use disorders and matched controls. PLoS One. 2015;10:e0128641. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Peer E, Vosgerau J, Acquisti A. Reputation as a sufficient condition for data quality on Amazon Mechanical Turk. Behav Res Methods. 2014;46:1023–1031 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Saunders JB, Aasland OG, Babor TF, de la Fuente JR, Grant M. Development of the alcohol use disorders identification test (AUDIT): WHO collaborative project on early detection of persons with harmful alcohol consumption. Addiction. 1993;88:791–804 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Stephenson MT, Hoyle RH, Palmgreen P, Slater MD. Brief measures of sensation seeking for screening and large-scale surveys. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2003;72:279–286 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Vallone D, Allen JA, Clayton RR, Xiao H. How reliable and valid is the Brief Sensation Seeking Scale (BSSS-4) for youth of various racial/ethnic groups? Addiction. 2007;102:71–78 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Kirby KN, Petry NM, Bickel WK. Heroin addicts have higher discount rates for delayed rewards than non-drug-using controls. J Exp Psychol Gen. 1999;128:78–87 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Epstein LH, Richards JB, Saad FG, Paluch RA, Roemmich JN, Lerman C. Comparison between two measures of delay discounting in smokers. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2003;11:131–138 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Mazur JE. An adjusting procedure for studying delayed reinforcement. In: Quantitative Analyses of Behavior: Vol 5, The Effect of Delay and of Intervening Events on Reinforcement Value. Commons M.L., Mazur J.E., Nevin J.A. and Rachlin H. (Eds). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1987: pp. 55–73 [Google Scholar]
- 42.Myerson J, Baumann AA, Green L. Discounting of delayed rewards: a theoretical interpretation of the Kirby questionnaire. Behav Processes. 2014;107:99–105 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Myerson J, Green L, Warusawitharana M. Area under the curve as a measure of discounting. J Exp Anal Behav. 2001;76:235–243 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Newcomb MD, McGee L. Influence of sensation seeking on general deviance and specific problem behaviors from adolescence to young adulthood. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1991;61:614–628 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Zuckerman M, Ball S, Black J. Influences of sensation seeking, gender, risk appraisal, and situational motivation on smoking. Addict Behav. 1990;15:209–220 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Soeken KL, Bausell RB. Alcohol use and its relationship to other addictive and preventive behaviors. Addict Behav. 1989;14:459–464 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Green L, Myerson J, Oliveira L, Chang SE. Delay discounting of monetary rewards over a wide range of amounts. J Exp Anal Behav. 2013;100:269–281 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Conigrave KM, Hall WD, Saunders JB. The AUDIT questionnaire: choosing a cut-off score. Addiction. 1995;90:1349–1356 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Johnson MW, Bickel WK. Within-subject comparison of real and hypothetical money rewards in delay discounting. J Exp Anal Behav. 2002;77:129–146 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Lagorio CH, Madden GJ. Delay discounting of real and hypothetical rewards III: steady-state assessments, forced-choice trials, and all real rewards. Behav Processes. 2005;69:173–187 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Peacock A, Pennay A, Droste N, Bruno R, Lubman DI. “High” risk? A systematic review of the acute outcomes of mixing alcohol with energy drinks. Addiction. 2014;109:1612–1633 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Amlung M, Few LR, Howland J, Rohsenow DJ, Metrik J, MacKillop J. Impulsivity and alcohol demand in relation to combined alcohol and caffeine use. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2013;21:467–474 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Kendler KS, Myers J, O Gardner C. Caffeine intake, toxicity and dependence and lifetime risk for psychiatric and substance use disorders: an epidemiologic and co-twin control analysis. Psychol Med. 2006;36:1717–1725 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Svikis D, Dillon P, Thacker L, et al. . Coffee and energy drink use in college freshmen: is trouble brewing? Drug Alcohol Depend. 2015;146:e112–e113 [Google Scholar]
- 55.Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration. Results from the 2011 National Survey on Drug Use and Health: Summary of National Findings; 2012. Available at http://archive.samhsa.gov/data/NSDUH/2k11Results/NSDUHresults2011.pdf (accessed September14, 2015) [PubMed]
- 56.Weinstock H, Berman S, Cates W. Sexually transmitted diseases among American youth: incidence and prevalence estimates, 2000. Perspect Sex Reprod Health. 2004;36:6–10 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]