Abstract
Novelty seeking (NS), defined as a tendency to pursue novel and intense emotional sensations and experiences, is one of the most relevant individual factors predicting drug use among humans. High novelty seeking (HNS) individuals present an increased risk of drug use compared to low novelty seekers. The NS endophenotype may explain some of the differences observed among individuals exposed to drugs of abuse in adolescence. However, there is little research about the particular response of adolescents to drugs of abuse in function of this endophenotype, and the data that do exist are inconclusive. The present work reviews the literature regarding the influence of NS on psychostimulant reward, with particular focus on adolescent subjects. First, the different animal models of NS and the importance of this endophenotype in adolescence are discussed. Later, studies that have used the most common animal models of reward (self-administration, conditioned place preference paradigms) to evaluate how the NS trait influences the rewarding effects of psychostimulants are reviewed. Finally, possible explanations for the enhanced risk of developing substance dependence among HNS individuals are discussed. In conclusion, the studies referred to in this review show that the HNS trait is associated with: (1) increased initial sensitivity to the rewarding effects of psychostimulants, (2) a higher level of drug craving when the subject is exposed to the environmental cues associated with the drug, and (3) enhanced long-term vulnerability to relapse to drug consumption after prolonged abstinence.
Keywords: Animal models, conditioned place preference, novelty seeking, psychostimulants, rewarding effects, self-administration.
INTRODUCTION
Predisposition to drug addiction is caused by the interaction of many different factors, among which the most documented are a vulnerable phenotype or personality, the drug itself and environment [1]. Individual differences in vulnerability to addiction are thought to exist before the first experience of a drug, and may relate, at least in part, to individual differences in sensitivity to drug reward [2]. Therefore, the study of endophenotypes is a way of clarifying the underlying factors that contribute to making an individual vulnerable to drug addiction [3]. Endophenotypes have been defined as quantitative traits related with a disorder and are heritably determined, mainly state-independent (i.e., manifested in periods of health or illness), co-segregate with a disorder within the family, and are more prevalent among non-affected family members than in the general population [4]. In this way, endophenotypes are considered to be traits that are half-way between genotype and phenotype, since both are characterized by predisposing genes and clinical symptoms of a complex disorder [3]. Hence, many authors have used endophenotype to identify markers of susceptibility to drug dependence as the scientific basis of new preventive strategies for individuals at risk and to create successful therapies that mitigate neuropsychiatric diseases [3, 5-8].
Novelty seeking (NS), or sensation seeking, is a personality trait defined as a tendency to pursue novel and intense emotional sensations and experiences [9]. It is employed to refer to individuals who like taking risks with the purpose of achieving stimulation and seeking new environments and situations that make their experiences more intense [10]. The first author who defined the term of novelty or sensation seeking was Zuckerman, who developed a sensation seeking scale together with Link [11], and the most widely used scale for measuring this parameter, known as Zuckerman’s Sensation Seeking Scale [10]. Other authors have extended the work of Zuckerman, such as Cloninger, whose Cloninger’s Tridimensional Personality Questionnaire (TPQ) is employed to measure the NS trait in humans [12]. Since then, further research has improved understanding of this phenotype [2, 8, 10, 13-17]. NS is a multifaceted behavioural construct that includes thrill-seeking, novelty-preference, risk-taking and harm avoidance, and these personality characteristics are present in individuals at risk of developing a drug use disorder [8].
The endophenotype of NS is one of the most relevant individual factors that predict drug use among humans [2]. There is considerable evidence that high novelty seekers present an increased risk of drug use compared to low novelty seekers [3, 8, 18-22]. Numerous scientists have highlighted the important contribution of this personality trait to the aetiology and ontogenesis of addiction, which has justified considering the novelty/sensation-seeking trait as a significant neurobiological marker of individuals predisposed to addiction [1, 16, 17]. Additionally, it is known that the NS trait is mediated by the mesolimbic dopaminergic system in a similar way to drug seeking behaviour [23].
Adolescence is the stage of life in whichit is most likely that drug consumption is initiated [17, 24, 25], since high-risk behaviours and increased exploration are typical in this period [26]. Risk-taking behaviour seems to be manifested in adolescence due to enhanced sensation seeking related to changes in functions of dopaminergic activity between childhood and adolescence [27]. Neurobehavioral disinhibition in the frontal cortex circuits observed in human imaging studies suggests that adolescents are more sensitive to reward and evaluate good experiences more positively than they do adverse ones negatively [28, 29]. The adolescent brain operates in a promotivational state due to a limited inhibitory capacity, poor regulatory control, and dopaminergic hyperactivity in the nucleus accumbens (NAcc) when processing appetitive stimuli and amygdala hyperactivity [30]. Therefore, the harm-avoidance system is not effective for avoiding drug consumption when adolescents are exposed to drugs. In consequence, NS behaviour is exhibited before drug consumption [2], and the risk of drug use in high novelty seeking (HNS) adolescentsis increased [31-34]. This early drug-taking has been associated with increased use and dependence in adulthood [35-37]. However, not all users progress to dependence, and this transitionis thought to depend on certain characteristics, of which NS is likely to be one.
A greater sensitivity of HNS young adults to a drug’s effects has also been reported [34, 38-40]. Some human studies have demonstrated that high novelty seekers display greater sensitivity to the physiological and psychological effects of psychostimulants in comparison with low novelty seekers [34, 41-43]. Some authors have suggested that high novelty seekers have a tendency to develop addictive behaviour [16], usually at initiation of consumption [33, 44-46], as a consequence of differential reward sensitivities between individuals [17]. However, there is lack of a consensus, as other studies challenge this relationship [47, 48] and the connection between sensation/novelty seeking and addiction risk [22, 49]. Longitudinal studies point to a direct path leading from NS to initiationof drug use [2, 50], but this traithas been shown to be absent in non-abuser siblings of individuals addicted to psychostimulants and to be present in regular consumers who control their consumption of the drug [22]. Individuals with high levels of NS are more likely to experiment with drugs; nevertheless, despite continuous use, they have a relatively low risk of developing dependence when there is no family history of addiction [49]. Moreover, it is important to note that drug use itself enhancesthe NS trait [22].
Therefore, it is currently under debate whether NS truly represents an endophenotype of addiction risk. Studies in humans have not established whether the NS trait is causally related to drug abuse or a consequence of this behaviour. It is important to continue exploring this issue in studies with animal models that allow a better assessment of the causal relationship between NS and vulnerability to the development of addiction [8, 17]. The animal model of novelty exploration or response in rodents has been used to study sensation seeker behaviour vulnerability. Studies show that animal models evaluate similar behavioural patterns to the human novelty/sensation seeking trait [51], in addition to physiological consequences and gene expression patterns [17]. Moreover, they allow the ethical problems surrounding research with humans to be avoided. In this sense, these modelsarea good way to evaluate the vulnerability of human sensation seekers to developing drug addiction.
In the present work, we review the literature on the influence of NS endophenotype on psychostimulant reward, with particular focus on adolescent subjects. First, we discussanimal models of NS (e.g., the inescapable vs. free-choice novelty models) and address differences in the way they measure this endophenotype, which can influence the results obtained regarding the relationship between drugs of abuse and NS in experimental animals. Afterwards, we emphasize the importance of this endophenotype in adolescence, a developmental period with enhanced vulnerability to the effects of drugs of abuse. We then review the studies that have used the most common animal models of reward [intravenous self -preference (CPP)] to evaluate how the NS trait influences the rewarding effects of psychostimulants. Finally, we discuss possible explanations for the enhanced risk of HNS individuals to developing substance dependence, and we draw conclusions about whether NS truly represents an endophenotype of addiction risk.
ANIMAL MODELS OF NOVELTY SEEKING
Novelty-seeking in rodents can be defined as the enhanced specific exploration of novel situations, unknown objects or stimuli. It is a complex behaviour that involves the detection of changes in the environment (cognition), and is related to stress responsiveness [52]. Rodents show an innate preference for novelty [53], and their behaviour in novel environments is conditioned by the interaction of several factors, including activity, motivation to explore and fear/anxiety [54, 55].
Different procedures have been used to screen rats and mice in order to assign them to subgroups according to high or low expression of a given measure. Nowadays, two different approaches are employed to model the NS trait: (1) novelty-induced locomotor reactivity to a new inescapable environment; and (2) preference for novel objects or environments; i.e., a propensity to explore a new object/environment in a free-choice procedure [17, 56-59]. The first approach, and most assessed paradigm, is known as Novelty Responding, which classifies animals as high or low responders according to their locomotor activity in a new inescapable environment. Although commonly considered animal models of the NS trait, some researchers affirm that this is inaccurate, as it is not clear whether the novelty-induced locomotor activity displayed by rodents in an inescapable environment is an escape behaviour or an exploratory one. Thus, the animal models of preference for novel objects/environments in a free-choice procedure are considered a better measure of NS, since rodents can choose to either approach or avoid novelty in the test [2].
Procedures of Response to Inescapable Novelty
The novel open-field environment was the first test described by Piazza and collaborators [56] to assess the locomotor response of rodents to a novel environment. The median [60], quartile split [61] of distance or activity, and number of rears [62] recorded over 10 min [63], 30 min [64], 1h [63, 65] or 2h [56] are used to categorize the animals as high or low responders [17]. The test consists of placing the animal in a new environment from which it cannot escape, and is therefore considered an inescapable procedure [2]. The novelty response can also be measured by rearing behaviour in an open field, which distinguishes between high and low rearing-activity or vertical-activation (HRA or LRA). This is a stable trait that reflects both emotionality and exploratory activity [66, 67]. Stronger rearing has been observed in HRA in a novel object test, which shows reactivity to novelty and exploratory activity in response to novel stimuli [68].
Activity in a novel environment has also been measured using an exploration box test [66, 69]. This test consists of placing three unknown objects and one familiar food (i.e. food pellet) in the same location on five consecutive days. The behavioural measures recorded during this task are locomotor movement and rearing activity for the exploration of the three unfamiliar objects. These measures are used to classify animals in terms of high or low exploratory activity (HE or LE), and indicate exploration behaviour as a whole and with respect to a particular stimulus. Thus, this task has been considered an indicator of inquisitive and inspective exploration [66, 69].
In addition to a novel open-field environment, the wheel-running procedure has been used to classify animals as high or low responders according to their activity in the running wheel. Larson and Carroll [70] assessed both the motor response to novelty (day 1) and locomotor activity on the second day. High responding (HR) rats presented more avidity for wheel-running than low responding (LR) rats, but both showed a similar locomotor activity on day 2. In this way, the procedure aims to distinguish between the response to novel stimuli and the animal’s inherent motor activity.
Novelty Seeking Free-Choice Procedures
The novel environment test is the most popular method used to classify animals according to their high or low exploratory behaviour. Animals are first counterbalanced by placing them in one of the two compartments of a CPP or black/white box in order to habituate them to the environment [71-74] during one [62, 75] or more sessions [74]. The door is then opened so that the animals can explore both compartments freely; typically for a 20 min period (test session) [62, 74, 75]. The following behaviours are generally scored: number of crossings between compartments; latency of first time the animal enters the novel compartment, and percentage of time spent in the novel compartment. Animals are then categorized as high or low novelty seekers depending on their novelty-preference score (percentage) in the novel and familiar environment.
Another method similar to the novel environment test is the spontaneous alternation task, in which a Y-maze [62] or T-maze [76] is used. Animals are placed in the stem of the apparatus and are allowed to enter any arm. In a second trial, the animals are free to make the same or an alternative choice. Two paradigms have been used to test spontaneous alternation: the forced trial procedure and free trial procedure. In both cases, animals are confined for 30 sec to either the chosen or forced compartment and then allowed to explore both arms freely in a test phase. Rodents show a higher preference for the noveltycompartment when the election is forced rather than chosen freely by the animal, because the last procedure depends on the bias of the rodent for one side of the apparatus or the other [76-78].
The Hole-board test is considered a useful tool to evaluate novelty preference in rodents [79, 80]. The apparatus consists of a square box, in the floor of which there are equidistant holes and sensors inside the holes that detect the number of head dips performed by the animals. This measure, and the latency time to the first head dip, are used to classify animals as high or low novelty seekers [62, 81, 82]. The emotional response of animals when facing an unfamiliar environment can be estimated and used sometimes to assess anxiety state too [79, 82-85]. This procedure shares certain features with the novelty responder test, such as inescapable condition, though some authors have verified that it is a good measure of NS because it is independent of locomotor activity [2, 85, 86].
The first description of the novel object preference test as a new procedure for evaluating directed exploration was provided by Nicholls and collaborators [87]. In a similar way to the novel environment test, rodents are free to choose between a novel and familiar object. In a playground maze, animals are exposed on consecutive days to several familiar objects during periods of 3 [87], 5 [88] or 10 minutes [74, 75, 89]. The quantity of familiar objects can vary: one novel and eight familiar objects [88], one novel and two familiar objects [75, 89], or one novel and one familiar object [90]. In the last trial, one familiar object is replaced by a new object and the animal’s approach to the novel or familiar object is assessed. The measures typically obtained in this test are the latency to explore and time spent exploring the novel object, total number of approaches to the novel object, and the percentage of time spent exploring novel and familiar objects. Rodents are classified as high or low novelty seekers depending on a median [88] or mean [75] split of the duration or percentage of duration spent engaged with the novel object. In the playground maze the animal’s locomotion is measured by recording the distance covered by the animals as they cross the maze.
In the novel-object place conditioning paradigm, first described by Bevins and Bardo [91], animals are conditioned to one side of the apparatus paired with a new object that is placed there daily (paired side), while no objects are placed on the other side (unpaired side). Each animal is exposed to distinct environments for 10 [91] or 15 minutes [92] and is conditioned for 8 [91] or 5 consecutive days [92]. On the test day, rodents are allowed to explore the compartments freely without any object inside the chambers for 10 min. The measure obtained in this test is the time spent in each compartment, with more time being spent in the novelty-paired compartment by high novelty seekers.
Another paradigm similar to the object preference test is object preference on the hole-board, described by Kliethermes and Crabbe [62]. The preference for a novel object or exploration is defined by the sum of the time engaged in head dipping in the holes connecting compartments that contain objects divided by the total time engaged in head dipping. The procedure is identical to the Hole-board test [79], with the exception that two objects are placed in two separate holes.
In summary, novelty-seeking free-choice procedures allow animals to choose to approach or reject novelty: i.e., rodents can freely choose the novel object or environment. However, procedures of response to inescapable novelty do not allow animals to choose freely the novel object or environment because they are confined to a determinate place. In these procedures, the measure used to categorize subjects as high or low novelty-seekers is their response to an inescapable novel environment such as locomotion activity or pressing a lever during a specified period of time. Therefore, novelty-seeking free-choice procedures are currently considered more suitable for categorizing the novelty-seeking trait.
ADOLESCENCE AND NOVELTY SEEKING
Adolescence is a time of major biological and psychological changes and a critical period of development for the central nervous system (CNS) and, more specifically, for the dopaminergic system [93]. In rodents, the term adolescence refers to postnatal day (PND) 21 to PND 60 [94]. Three different stages of adolescence have been distinguished: early adolescence (prepubescent or juvenile, PND 21 to 34), middle adolescence (periadolescent, PND 34 to 46), and late adolescence (young adult, PND 46 to 59) [95-97].
Although adolescence is related mainly with hormonal changes, this developmental period is also marked by critical neural plasticity [98]. Alterations of the dopaminergic system, especially the projections from mesolimbic areas to the prefrontal cortex (PFC), appear to play a critical role in the increased reward-seeking behaviour of adolescent individuals. The adolescent stage is characterized by an increase, a reduction and a redistribution of dopamine (DA) receptor concentration [99]. Despite lower DA release in basal conditions, dopaminergic neurons are capable of delivering much greater amounts of DA if they are stimulated with pharmacological or environmental stimuli [100]. This is probably due to the fact that extracellular DA levels are greater in adolescents [100-103] and the rate of DA increase may be faster at this age [102, 104]. These changes provoke hypersensitivity of the striatal system, which, together with immature inhibitory substrates in the PFC (the “cognitive control” system), leads to an increase in adolescent risk-taking [99, 105, 106]. Therefore, it can be hypothesized that some adaptive behaviours characteristic of adolescence, such as play behaviour or exploration of novel environments or objects, result in enhanced DA release when compared to adulthood [94]. To sum up, the adolescent brain operates in a promotivational state with limited inhibitory and regulatory control, dopaminergic hyperactivity in the NAcc when processing appetitive stimuli, and, finally, hyperactivity in the amygdala, which explains affective intensity and lability, and a weaker harm-avoidance system [107]. Consequently, the relatively early development of the NAcc and amygdala, together with an immature PFC, tilts behaviour toward risk-seeking and sensation-seeking preference [30].
Studies with animal models have confirmed these characteristics in adolescent rodents, which are prone to high risk taking behaviour and increased exploration [26, 25, 108-110], in a similar way to adolescent humans [27]. Therefore, adolescent rodents are more sensitive than their adult counterparts to the rewarding properties of stimuli such as food [111-113], social peers and novelty [92]. Accordingly, adolescents find drugs more rewarding, and for that reason self-administer higher doses of psychostimulants such as cocaine, methamphetamine and amphetamine [102, 114-120]. Moreover, they experience less aversive effects of addictive drugs and reduced withdrawal symptoms [see review 111]. Altogether, these factors favour adolescents’ initiation of drug consumption and make them more vulnerable to developing substance abuse disorders.
The NS trait is considered to be more pronounced in adolescent than in adult rodents, as occurs in humans [94], although its influence on drug consumption has generally been studied in adult rodents [16]. Juvenile rodents present greater hyperactivity and exploration in novel environments, an increased social peer interaction, and consummatory behaviours [reviewed in 26, 121]. In addition, adolescent mice have been shown to perform a higher number of explorations than adults in the novel object recognition task and novel environment test of free-choice, in which they spend more time exploring the novel object/environment [75]. In an inescapable, novel environment, adolescent rodents displayed higher novelty-induced locomotor activity than adults when measured during the initial minutes in the open field [58, 122], but not when measured over a longer interval [63, 122], probably because adolescents habituate to novelty more rapidly [58].
NOVELTY SEEKING ENDOPHENOTYPE AND THE REWARDING EFFECTS OF PSYCHOSTIMULANT DRUGS
There is considerable evidence that HNS represent an increased risk of drug use when compared to LNS. However, differences in NS do not always predict drug reward [see reviews 2, 8], probably due to variations in the procedure for evaluating NS or in the drug’s effects. With this in mind, this section reviews the studies that have assessed the influence of NS on the rewarding effects of psychostimulant drugs in the most common animal models of reward: the intravenous drug self-administration and CPP paradigms. We have distinguished between the two main types of animal models of NS, since the results of tests for inescapable and free-choice novelty environment do not always correlate [59, 64, 74, 123], probably due to the fact that they measure different aspects of NS.
Self-Administration Paradigm
Intravenous drug self-administration is a widely used animal model of human drug abuse and dependence, since it is a procedure that allows the reinforcing effects of abuse substances to be measured [124]. Tables 1 and 2 summarises the results of the main studies to have used the self-administration paradigm to evaluate the reinforcing effects of psychostimulants in animals according to their NS phenotype.
Table 1.
Results of the main studies to have used the self-administration paradigm to evaluate the reinforcing effects of psychostimulants in animals classified as high or low novelty seekers according to an inescapable NS paradigm.
Novelty-Seeking Animal Model | Animals | Age | Drug | Main Results in Self-Administration | References |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Locomotor response to novelty | Male Sprague-Dawley rats | Adult 280 to 300 g | 10 µg/inf amphetamine FR1 i.v. | Only acquired HR | Piazza et al., 1989 |
Male and female Sprague–Dawley rats | Adult male: 200–225 g or 250-300 g Adult female: 150–175 g |
0.01-0.16-0.03 mg/kg/inf amphetamine FR1 to FR5 i.v. | HR>LR | Pierre and Vezina, 1997; Klebaur et al., 2001 | |
Male and female Sprague–Dawley rats | Adult 90-100 PND 250–320 g |
0.2-0.219-0.25-0.5-1 mg/kg/inf cocaine FR1 i.v. 0.1 mg/kg/ inf cocaine FR1to FR6 i.v. |
HR>LR HR females>HR males |
Grimm and See, 1997; Piazza et al., 2000; Kabbaj et al., 2001; Mantsch et al., 2001; Davis et al., 2008; Cummings et al., 2011 | |
Male Sprague–Dawley rats | Adult 280–320 g | 0.175 mg/kg/inf cocaine FR1 i.v. | Only acquired HR | Marinelli and White, 2000 | |
Male diversity Outbred (DO) mice | Housing at 4 weeks old Adult | 1.0 mg/kg/inf cocaine FR1 i.v. | HR>LR | Dickson et al., 2015 | |
Male Sprague–Dawley rats | Adult 175–200 g | 0.1 mg/kg/inf amphetamine FR1 i.v. | HR=LR | Cain et al., 2005; Meyer et al., 2010 | |
Male Sprague–Dawley rats | Adult 10 weeks old | 0.1 ml of 0.25 mg methylphenidate per infusion FR1 i.v. | HR=LR | de la Peña et al., 2014 | |
Male prague–Dawley rats | Adult 250–320 g | 1 mg/kg/inf cocaine FR1 to FR6 i.v. 0.25 mg/kg/inf cocaine FR5 i.v. 0.3-0.8-1 mg/kg/inf cocaine FR1 i.v. |
HR=LR | Piazza et al., 2000; Mitchell et al., 2005; Beckmann et al., 2011; Belin et al. 2011 | |
Male Sprague–Dawley rats | Adult 200 g | 1 mg/kg/inf MDMA FR1 i.v. | HR=LR | Bird and Schenk, 2013 | |
Wheel running | Female Wistar rats | Adult 250–340 g | 0.4 mg/kg/inf cocaine FR1 i.v. | HR=LR | Larson and Carroll, 2005 |
Table 2.
Results of the main studies to have used the self-administration paradigm to evaluate the reinforcing effects of psychostimulants in animals classified as high or low novelty seekers according to a free-choice NS paradigm.
Novelty-Seeking Animal Model | Animals | Age | Drug | Main Results in Self-Administration | References |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Novelty place preference | Male Sprague–Dawley rats | Adult 175–200 g 70-75 PND |
0.1 mg/kg/inf amphetamine FR1 and FR5 i.v. | HNS>LNS | Cain et al., 2005; Meyer et al. 2010 |
Male Sprague–Dawley rats | Adult 250–300g | 0.3-0.8-1 mg/kg/inf cocaine FR1 i.v. | HNS>LNS | Beckmann et al., 2011; Belin et al., 2011 | |
Male and female Sprague–Dawley rats | Adult male: 200–225 g Adult female: 150–175 g |
0.03-0.16 mg/kg/inf amphetamine FR1 to FR5 i.v. | HR=LR | Klebaur et al., 2001 | |
Novel object test | Male Sprague–Dawley rats | Adult 10 weeks old | 0.1 ml of 0.25 mg methylphenidate per infusion FR1 i.v. | HNS >LNS | de la Peña et al., 2014 |
Male Sprague–Dawley rats | Adult 175–200 g | 0.01 - 0.1 mg/kg/inf amphetamine FR1 to FR5 i.v. | HR=LR | Cain et al., 2005 | |
Hole-board | Male diversity Outbred (DO) mice | Housing at 4 weeks old Adult | 1 mg/kg/infcocaine FR1 i.v. | HNS>LNS | Dickson et al., 2015 |
Procedures of Response to Novelty
The animal model of response to novelty characterized initially by Piazza and collaborators [56] has been widely used to predict the reinforcing effect of psychostimulants [see reviews 2, 8, 17, 125]. It has been reported that HR rats in a novel environment self-administer more amphetamine than LR rats [56, 57, 125, 126], whether males or females [57]. Moreover, HR rats were shown to acquire amphetamine self-administration with lower doses than LR rats, while their drug intake and motivation for the drug (breaking point) were similar after 11 days of training [125].
Similarly, HR rats acquire cocaine self-administration at lower doses [127-129] and more rapidly [130-132] than their LR counterparts. HR rats also self-administer greater amounts of cocaine and display more regular self-administration at lower doses [60, 129, 133]. Importantly, HR females have been shown to self-administer significantly more cocaine than HR male and LR male and female rats [130, 131].
Although these results suggest that the response to novelty predicts vulnerability to acquire psychostimulant self-administration, other studies have reported a lack of such a relationship for amphetamine [74, 123], methylphenidate [134], cocaine [59, 60, 64, 129, 135] and 3,4-methylenedioxyme-thamphetamine (MDMA) [136]. Differences in the operant response measured and the fixed ratio (FR) schedule used, as well as the time to access the drug [74, 125] and the doses administered [59, 60, 64, 129, 135], may have reduced inherent individual differences in subsequent operant responses to the drug in the aforementioned studies. In fact, differences between HR and LR animals in the self-administration of psychostimulants with respect to acquisition of the operant response seem to exist only at low doses.
Novelty Seeking Free-Choice Procedures
Preference for a new environment/object in a free-choice test may also predict amphetamine self-administration. HNS rats have been shown to self-administer more amphetamine [74] or methylphenidate [134] than LNS rats. Moreover, Meyer and collaborators [123], in a study performed across twelve inbred rat strains, reported a relationship between novelty preference ratio and amphetamine infusions, since HNS strains self-administered more amphetamine than LNS ones.
Likewise, free-choice categorized HNS animals show higher vulnerability and severity to cocaine addiction [59]. Rats displaying higher exploration are more predisposed to cocaine consumptionthan low novelty preference rats. HNS animals have higher addiction scores, such as persistent responding when the drug is signalled as unavailable, resistance of the self-administration response to punishment, and a higher breaking point in a progressive ratio (PR) schedule. All these differences have been observed during the acquisition of self-administration with low doses of cocaine [59, 64], but no differences were observed with higher doses [64]. In line with this, female HR rats showed a greater number of responses on the active lever than LR after a priming injection of cocaine, though no significant differences were detected between high and low responders with respect to the maintenance or extinction of cocaine self-administration [70]. However, the number of exploratory nose pokes in the hole-board test of diverse outbred mice strains of both sexes positively correlated with the acquisition of self-administration, the number of cocaine infusions during the FR1 schedule, and the breaking point in the progressive ratio [132].
Nevertheless, the novelty place preference test does not always predict amphetamine self-administration of individuals. Male and female rats, classified as high or low novelty seekers according to the novelty preference test, do not show differences in self-administration of amphetamine [57]. These apparently contradictory results, in addition to the results of the few studies to have used free-choice preference tests, suggest that these NS measures are not as predictive of drug self-administration as individual differences in inescapable novelty (high/low responders) test [2]. A possible explanation is that the inescapable and free-choice novelty tests measure different facets of NS [59, 74, 123]. Several studies have observed a lack of correlation among different tasks that evaluate NS trait [59, 63, 64, 74, 75, 123], suggesting that each NS paradigm assesses different aspects of this trait.
Conditioned Place Preference
CPP is a paradigm that evaluates the conditioned rewarding effects of addictive drugs [137-139]. It is a procedure that assesses positive and pleasant properties of stimuli in a rapid and simple way [140]. In this paradigm, contextual or environmental stimuli acquire secondary appetitive properties (conditioned rewarding effects) when paired with a primary reinforcer [138, 141]. Conditioned reward implies that animals attribute positive incentive value to the cues associated with the primary reinforcer, and thus perform free or voluntary responses to obtain access to said cues [142]. Under appropriate conditions, CPP can be sensitive to a wide range of substances, including psychostimulants [140]. Tables 3 and 4 summarizes the results of the main studies to have used the CPP paradigm to evaluate the rewarding effects of psychostimulants in animals classified as high or low novelty seekers.
Table 3.
Results of the main studies to have used the CPP paradigm to evaluate the rewarding effects of psychostimulants in animals classified as high or low novelty seekers according to an inescapable NS paradigm.
Novelty-Seeking Animal Model | Animals | Age | Drug | Rewarding Effects of Psychostimulants in CPP | References |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Locomotor response to novelty | Male Long Evans rats | PND 46 Adolescent PND 70 Adult |
Amphetamine (0.5 or 1.0 mg/kg) i.p. |
HR less sensitive to amphetamine CPP (%time) |
Mathew et al., 2010 |
Male Sprague-Dawley rats | 290-330g Adult 200-225g Adult |
Cocaine (5,10,12,15,20 mg/kg) i.p Amphetamine (0.4mg/kg) s.c |
HR=LR acquired CPP | Gong et al., 1996 Kosten and Miserendino, 1998 Dietz et al., 2007 Capriles et al., 2012 Robinet et al., 1998 | |
C57BL/6J male mice | 7-8 weeks Adult | Cocaine (4, 8, 12 mg/kg) i.p. |
LR>HR in CPP at low doses | Brabant et al., 2005 Shimosato and Watanabe, 2003 |
|
Male Slc:ddY mice | 5 weeks Adolescent |
Cocaine (5, 10, 20mg/kg) i.p. |
|||
Male and female OF1 mice | PND 35 Adolescent PND 56 Young adult |
Cocaine (1mg/kg) i.p. |
10 min: young adult HNR>LNS 1h: HR young adult male=LR young adult female acquired CPP |
Arenas et al., 2014 |
Table 4.
Results of the main studies to have used the CPP paradigm to evaluate the rewarding effects of psychostimulants in animals classified as high or low novelty seekers according to a free-choice NS paradigm.
Novelty-Seeking Animal Model | Animals | Age | Drug | Rewarding Effects of Psychostimulants in CPP | Reference |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Novel environment | Male Sprague- Dawley rats |
200-225g Adult |
Amphetamine (0.4 mg/kg) s.c. |
HR>LR higher magnitude of CPP | Robinet et al. 1998 |
Novelty test chambers | Male Wistar rats | 180-200g Adult |
Amphetamine (0.2 – 3.2mg/kg) s.c. |
HR=LR acquired CPP at high doses HR>LR stronger CPP at the lowest dose |
Pelloux et al. 2004 |
Novelty test chambers | Sprague- Dawley rats |
Adult | Amphetamine (0.75-10mg/kg) i.p. | HNS=LNS acquired CPP | Erb and Parker, 1994 |
Playground maze | Male Sprague- Dawley rats |
200-225g Adult |
Amphetamine (1–3 mg/kg) s.c. | HNS>LNS amphetamine CPP (1 mg/kg). | Klebaur and Bardo, 1999 |
Novel object recognition task Novel environment test |
Male and female OF1 mice | PND 35 Adolescent PND 56 Young adult |
Cocaine (1mg/kg) i.p. | HNS (Young adult)>LNS acquired CPP | Vidal-Infer et al., 2012 |
Hole-board | Male and female OF1 mice | PND 35 Adolescent PND 56 Young adult |
Cocaine (1mg/kg) i.p. | HNS (adolescent) = LNS (young adult female) acquired CPP | Arenas et al., 2014 |
Male OF1 mice | PND 31 Adolescent (binge) PND 67 Adult(CPP) |
Alcohol Binge (2.5g/kg) i.p. + Cocaine (CPP) (1 or 6mg/kg) i.p. MDMA (CPP) (1.25 or 2.5mg/kg) i.p. |
HNS=LNS acquisition CPP HNS reinstates cocaine CPP |
Montagud-Romero et al., 2014 | |
PND 28/33 Adolescent (binge) PND 60/67 Adult (CPP) |
MDMA or Cocaine binge i.p. + Cocaine (CPP) (1mg/kg) i.p. MDMA (CPP) (1.25mg/kg) i.p. |
HNS>LNS acquired CPP with both drugs. | Rodríguez-Arias et al., 2015 Mateos-García et al., 2015 |
Procedures of Response to Novelty
Few studies have evaluated the influence of novelty response in the CPP induced by amphetamine, probably because the first results to be reported were negative. HR and LR rats did not differ in amphetamine CPP at low [143] or high doses [144], and both adolescent and adult HR rats were less sensitive than their LR counterparts to amphetamine in the CPP [122]. Only Pelloux and collaborators [145] reported that response to forced novelty predicted the strength of amphetamine-induced CPP, although only at a low dose.
Similarly, no notable differences were observed between HR and LR rats in the CPP induced by a wide range of doses of cocaine [146-149]. However, significantly higher place preference in HR rats 30 days after post-conditioning test has been reported [148]. In adolescent and adult male mice, the locomotor response to novelty correlated negatively with the magnitude of the CPP induced by low cocaine doses, but not by that induced by higher doses [150, 151]. Recently, another study has reported that male and female HR young adult mice (according to their locomotor response to novelty) develop CPP after conditioning with a subthreshold dose of cocaine, differently to LR mice [63].
The CPP paradigm is generally characterized by a lack of dose-response effects; thus, the use of effective doses for inducing CPP may mask differences between HR and LR animals [137, 152]. Dose-response effects in the CPP paradigm can only be demonstrated by changes in the extinction of conditioned preference or in sensitivity to the reinstatement of drug-CPP [139]. The use of sub-threshold doses of cocaine may be useful to identify animals that are more sensitive to the conditioned rewarding effects of the drug. Another variable to take into consideration is the duration of the test used to classify animals as HR or LR, which may partly explain the lack of an association between the novelty response trait and drug effects in the CPP [58, 63, 122].
Novelty Seeking Free-Choice Procedures
The evaluation of novelty preference in a free-choice procedure seems to detect more differences between high and low novelty seekers. For example, the CPP induced by amphetamine [88, 143] and cocaine [63-75] has been shown to be greater in HNS than in LNS rodents, although other studies have failed to find a relationship between novelty environment preference and amphetamine-induced CPP [145].
Recently, studies in our laboratory have demonstrated that the evaluation of the novelty preference endophenotype depends on the age and/or sex of the mice and the type of test used to measure the NS trait [63, 75]. HNS mice tested in a novel environment in adulthood acquired CPP with subthreshold doses of cocaine, but no differences in CPP were observed between HNS and LNS when they were tested during adolescence [75]. Conversely, according to the hole-board test, HNS adolescent mice were more likely than LNS to develop CPP with subthreshold doses of cocaine. However, adult LNS females showed higher conditioning scores than HNS [63]. These results demonstrate that the behavioural profiles of adolescent and adult animals vary among NS paradigms, suggesting that the different NS tests evaluate different behavioural components of this endophenotype. Thus, the novel environment test can be considered to have a greater predictive capacity to identify “vulnerable-drug” individuals among adult mice, while the hole-board test is more effective for predicting drug vulnerability in adolescents [63, 75].
Finally, the influence of the NS trait in the long-term effects of adolescent exposure to drugs have been evaluated in a series of studies [82, 153, 154]. Mice were classified during adolescence as high or low novelty seekers according to the hole-board test and were subsequently exposed to chronic administration of cocaine, MDMA or alcohol. Cocaine- or MDMA-induced CPP was then evaluated in adulthood. Repeated administration of cocaine or MDMA during adolescence was shown to increase the conditioned rewarding effects of these drugs in HNS animals only [82, 154]. Ethanol binge drinking during adolescence increased sensitivity to the rewarding effects of low doses of cocaine and MDMA during adulthood in both HNS and LNS subjects. However, only high novelty seekers showed a reinstatement of cocaine CPP after a priming dose of this drug [153].
NEUROBIOLOGICAL SUBSTRATES OF NOVELTY SEEKING
The endophenotype of NS refers to individual differences in motivation for novelty [9], and the role of mesolimbic DA in motivational processes is well documented [155]. On the other hand, the involvement of the mesolimbic dopaminergic system in the rewarding effects of drugs of abuse has been widely studied [156]. Thus, differences in dopaminergic neurobiological substrates may explain, at least partially, the distinction between high and low novelty seekers and their variable sensitivity to drugs of abuse [17, 157].
There is a great deal of evidence that DA modulates NS behaviour. A free-choice approach to novelty directly activates the reward-relevant mesocorticolimbic dopamine circuitry [2]. Novel stimuli excite DA neurons, especially in the ventral striatum, and activate the brain regions that receive dopaminergic input [17]. Moreover, blockade of the DA transporter (DAT) increases NS behaviour in monkeys without affecting their capacity to select cues that are predictive of reward [158], and disruption of the DAT Dat1 gene diminishes novelty-related behaviour in mice [159]. Similarly, all animal models of NS are sensitive to manipulation of brain DA function [157]. Novelty place preference is blocked by DA antagonists [73, 160] and by depleting DA levels in NAcc with the neurotoxin 6-hydroxydopamine [161]. These findings demonstrate that increased extracellular dopamine levels underlie the positive evaluation of novel stimuli with respect to promoting exploratory behaviour, and suggest that alterations in DA reuptake contribute to excessive NS [158].
Additionally, several brain differences have been identified in HR vs. LR rats, such as higher extracellular DA in the NAcc and striatum, increased velocity of DA uptake in the NAcc, and higher mRNA levels of tyrosine hydroxylase and the DA D1 receptor. Furthermore, HR rats exhibit a reduced density of accumbal DA D2 receptors, suggesting a decreased number of release-regulating autoreceptors or a compensatory downregulation of postsynaptic receptors in response to increased presynaptic DA release [see review 2]. These differences between HR and LR rats would explain the more intense basal firing of midbrain DA neurons observed in high novelty seekers, which is likely to be due to subsensitivity to impulse-regulating D2 somatodendritic autoreceptors [128]. D4 receptors have also been implicated in the response to novelty, since D4 knockout mice behave in the same way as low responders [17], and administration of an agonist of the D4 receptors has been shown to increase exploration of a novel object in mice [162].
Differential DA signalling between sensation/novelty-seeking personalities in humans has also been reported [163, 164]. Specifically, HNS individuals exhibit higher endogenous DA levels, lower activity of isoforms of monoamine oxidase (MAO), higher activity of dihydroxyphenylalanine (DOPA) decarboxylase, and stronger dopaminergic responses to cues predictive of reward in striatal regions with respect to LNS subjects [see review 157]. Finally, a strong relationship between polymorphism at DA D4 receptor loci and individual differences in NS personality has also been reported [157, 165]. Therefore, differences in the dopaminergic system have been highlighted as a possible explanation of the relation between this trait and drug addiction [16, 17, 166].
On the other hand, the role played bythe serotoninergic (5HT) system in the NS trait has also been assessed. Specifically, serotoninergic receptors (5-HT2C) seem to modulate mesocorticolimbic DA release, thus affecting motivation and behaviour. The 5-HT2C antagonist SB242084 has been shown to increase acquisition of the cocaine-conditioned response only in LR rats (enhancing the hedonic value of cocaine), suggesting that 5-HT2C receptors influence individual differences in cocaine reinforce-related learning/memory processes [148]. More research is required to throw light on the role of the serotonin system in the influence of the NS endophenotype on the rewarding effects of drugs of abuse.
In summary, the HNS trait seems to be associated with differences in striatal DA function; specifically, higher novelty seekers may have higher endogenous striatal DA levels, stronger dopaminergic responses to reward cues, and lower availability of D2-type (D2/D3/D4) DA receptors in the striatum. Thus, a combination of high dopaminergic tone and a lower density of D2-type receptors in the striatum are potential contributors to the higher NS endophenotype, as reflected by an increased tendency to exhibit approach reactions towards novel stimuli which, on the contrary, elicit aversive reactions in others [157].
CONCLUSION
It is currently under debate whether NS truly represents an endophenotype for addiction risk. There is considerable evidence that high novelty seekers present an increased risk of drug abuse in comparison to low novelty seekers. However, whether this increased vulnerability is due to a greater sensitivity to the rewarding effects of psychostimulants or to early exposure to drugs has been the subject of little study [22, 134]. Nevertheless, the studies referred to in this revision have shown that the HNS trait is associated with increased initial sensitivity to the rewarding effects of psychostimulants, especially at low doses [63, 74, 75, 82, 123, 145, 153, 154]. In addition, HNS represents a higher risk of relapse into cocaine-seeking behaviour when the subject is re-exposed to the environmental cues associated with the drug [148, 153].
Despite the fact that subjects are more likely to become drug users during adolescence, very little research has been carried out regarding the relation between the NS trait and drugs of abuse in adolescent individuals. The NS trait could explain some of the differences observed among individuals exposed to drugs of abuse in adolescence, since we have demonstrated the influence of this endophenotype on the long-term effects of exposure during adolescence to drugs such as cocaine or MDMA [82, 153, 154].
Another important point with which to conclude this review is the fact that the different animal models of NS measure different parameters, as several studies have demonstrated [57, 63, 75, 143, 145, 167]. These different measures of the NS endophenotype may represent distinct behavioural components that are not equivalent, thus highlighting the complexity of the processes involved in the response to novelty. Furthermore, different NS tests involve varying capacities for identify vulnerable-drug individuals in adolescents vs adults. Future research will need to identify which dimensions of human sensation- and/or novelty-seeking are modelled in rodent paradigms and represent vulnerability markers for progression to and maintenance of drug addiction [157, 168].
In conclusion, a major challenge for current research into addiction is to identify the personality traits that predispose individuals to develop a drug use disorder during adolescence. HNS individuals present a greater risk of initiating drug use and developing substance dependence, as they exhibit higher sensitivity to the rewarding effects of psychostimulants and more marked changes after their consumption. Therefore, it is crucial to continue to employ animal models in order to understand better this causal relationship.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
Ministerio de Economía y Competitividad (MINECO), Dirección General de Investigación, PSI2014-51847-R, Instituto de Salud Carlos III, Red de Trastornos Adictivos (RTA) RD12/0028/0005 and Unión Europea, Fondos FEDER “una manera de hacer Europa”. Ministerio de Sanidad, Servicios Sociales e Igualdad Delegación del Gobierno para el Plan Nacional Sobre Drogas, Proyectos de Investigación sobre Drogodependencias, 2014I007. Generalitat Valenciana, Conselleria de Educación, PROMETEOII/2014/063. The European Foundation for Alcohol Research (ERAB), EA13 08.
CONFLICT OF INTEREST
The authors confirm that this article content has no conflict of interest.
LIST OF ABREVIATIONS
- 5HT
= Serotoninergic
- 5-HT2C
= Serotoninergic receptors
- CNS
= Central nervous system
- CPP
= Conditioned place preference
- DA
= Dopamine
- DAT
= Dopamine transporter
- DOPA
= Dihydroxyphenylalanine
- FR
= Fixed ratio
- HE
= High exploratory activity
- HNS
= High novelty seeking
- HR
= High responding
- HRA
= High rearing-activity or vertical-activation
- i.v.
= intravenous administration
- LE
= Low exploratory activity
- LNS
= Low novelty seeking
- LR
= Low responding
- LRA
= Low rearing-activity or vertical-activation
- MAO
= Monoamine oxidase
- MDMA
= 3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine
- NAcc
= Nucleus accumbens
- NS
= Novelty seeking
- PFC
= Prefrontal cortex
- PND
= Postnatal day
- PR
= Progressive ratio
- TPQ
= Tridimensional Personality Questionnaire
REFERENCES
- 1.Belin D., Deroche-Gamonet V. Responses to novelty and vulnerability to cocaine addiction: contribution of a multi-symptomatic animal model. Cold Spring Harb. Perspect. Med. 2012;2(11):a011940. doi: 10.1101/cshperspect.a011940. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Bardo M.T., Neisewander J.L., Kelly T.H. Individual differences and social influences on the neurobehavioral pharmacology of abused drugs. Pharmacol. Rev. 2013;65(1):255–290. doi: 10.1124/pr.111.005124. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Ersche K.D., Turton A.J., Chamberlain S.R., Müller U., Bullmore E.T., Robbins T.W. Cognitive dysfunction and anxious-impulsive personality traits are endophenotypes for drug dependence. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2012;169(9):926–936. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.11091421. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Gottesman I.I., Gould T.D. The endophenotype concept in psychiatry: etymology and strategic intentions. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2003;160(4):636–645. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.4.636. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Uhl G.R. Molecular genetics of addiction vulnerability. NeuroRx. 2006;3(3):295–301. doi: 10.1016/j.nurx.2006.05.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Wong C.C., Schumann G. Review. Genetics of addictions: strategies for addressing heterogeneity and polygenicity of substance use disorders. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 2008;363(1507):3213–3222. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2008.0104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Kreek M.J., Levran O., Reed B., Schlussman S.D., Zhou Y., Butelman E.R. Opiate addiction and cocaine addiction: underlying molecular neurobiology and genetics. J. Clin. Invest. 2012;122(10):3387–3393. doi: 10.1172/JCI60390. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Jupp B., Dalley J.W. Behavioral endophenotypes of drug addiction: etiological insights from neuroimaging studies. Neuropharmacology. 2014;76:487–497. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2013.05.041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Zukerman M. Sensation seeking: Beyond the optimal level of arousal. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1979. [Google Scholar]
- 10.Zuckerman M. Behavioral expressions and biosocial bases of sensation seeking. Cambridge university press; 1994. [Google Scholar]
- 11.Zuckerman M., Link K. Construct validity for the sensation-seeking scale. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 1968;32(4):420–426. doi: 10.1037/h0026047. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Cloninger C. The tridimensional personality questionnaire, version IV. St.Louis, MO: Department of Psychiatry, Washington University School of Medicine; 1987. a. [Google Scholar]
- 13.Kosten T.A., Ball S.A., Rounsaville B.J. A sibling study of sensation seeking and opiate addiction. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 1994;182(5):284–289. doi: 10.1097/00005053-199405000-00006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Wills T.A., Windle M., Cleary S.D. Temperament and novelty seeking in adolescent substance use: convergence of dimensions of temperament with constructs from Cloninger's theory. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1998;74:387. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.74.2.387. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Ball S. Personality traits, disorders, and substance abuse. On the psychobiology of personality: essays in honor of Marvin Zuckerman. 2004:203–222. [Google Scholar]
- 16.Nadal Alemany R. La búsqueda de sensaciones y su relación con la vulnerabilidad a la adicción y al estrés. Adicciones. 2008;20(1):59–72. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Blanchard M.M., Mendelsohn D., Stamp J.A. The HR/LR model: further evidence as an animal model of sensation seeking. Neurosci. Biobehav. R. 2009;33:1145–1154. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.05.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Piazza P.V., Deroche V., Rougé-Pont F., Le Moal M. Behavioral and biological factors associated with individual vulnerability to psychostimulant abuse. NIDA Res. Monogr. 1998;169:105–133. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Kreek M.J., Nielsen D.A., Butelman E.R., LaForge K.S. Genetic influences on impulsivity, risk taking, stress responsivity and vulnerability to drug abuse and addiction. Nat. Neurosci. 2005;8(11):1450–1457. doi: 10.1038/nn1583. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Flagel S.B., Akil H., Robinson T.E. Individual differences in the attribution of incentive salience to reward-related cues: Implications for addiction. Neuropharmacology. 2009;56(Suppl. 1):139–148. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2008.06.027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Dalley J.W., Everitt B.J., Robbins T.W. Impulsivity, compulsivity, and top-down cognitive control. Neuron. 2011;69(4):680–694. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.01.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Ersche K.D., Turton A.J., Pradhan S., Bullmore E.T., Robbins T.W. Drug addiction endophenotypes: impulsive versus sensation-seeking personality traits. Biol. Psychiatry. 2010;68(8):770–773. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.06.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Bardo M.T., Donohew R.L., Harrington N.G. Psychobiology of novelty seeking and drug seeking behavior. Behav. Brain Res. 1996;77(1-2):23–43. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(95)00203-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Kandel D.B., Logan J.A. Patterns of drug use from adolescence to young adulthood: I. Periods of risk for initiation, continued use, and discontinuation. Am. J. Public Health. 1984;74(7):660–666. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.74.7.660. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Spear L.P. Rewards, aversions and affect in adolescence: Emerging convergences across laboratory animal and human data. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2011;1:390–403. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2011.08.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Spear L.P. The adolescent brain and age-related behavioral manifestations. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2000;24(4):417–463. doi: 10.1016/S0149-7634(00)00014-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Steinberg L. Risk taking in adolescence new perspectives from brain and behavioral science. Curr. Dir. Psychol. 2007;16:55–59. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00475.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Crews F.T., Boettiger C.A. Impulsivity, frontal lobes and risk for addiction. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 2009;93(3):237–247. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2009.04.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Geier C., Luna B. The maturation of incentive processing and cognitive control. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 2009;93(3):212–221. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2009.01.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Rodríguez-Arias M., Aguilar M.A. Polydrug use in adolescence. INTECH Open Access Publisher; 2012. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Martin C.A., Kelly T.H., Rayens M.K., Brogli B.R., Brenzel A., Smith W.J., Omar H.A. Sensation seeking, puberty, and nicotine, alcohol, and marijuana use in adolescence. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry. 2002;41(12):1495–1502. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200212000-00022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Martin C.A., Kelly T.H., Rayens M.K., Brogli B., Himelreich K., Brenzel A., Bingcang C.M., Omar H. Sensation seeking and symptoms of disruptive disorder: association with nicotine, alcohol, and marijuana use in early and mid-adolescence. Psychol. Rep. 2004;94(3 Pt 1):1075–1082. doi: 10.2466/pr0.94.3.1075-1082. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Sargent J.D., Tanski S., Stoolmiller M., Hanewinkel R. Using sensation seeking to target adolescents for substance use interventions. Addiction. 2010;105(3):506–514. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2009.02782.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Stoops W.W., Lile J.A., Robbins C.G., Martin C.A., Rush C.R., Kelly T.H. The reinforcing, subject-rated, performance, and cardiovascular effects of d-amphetamine: influence of sensation-seeking status. Addict. Behav. 2007;32(6):1177–1188. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2006.08.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Clark D.B., Kirisci L., Tarter R.E. Adolescent versus adult onset and the development of substance use disorders in males. Drug Alcohol Depend. 1998;49(2):115–121. doi: 10.1016/S0376-8716(97)00154-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Kandel D.B., Yamaguchi K., Chen K. Stages of progression in drug involvement from adolescence to adulthood: further evidence for the gateway theory. J. Stud. Alcohol. 1992;53(5):447–457. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1992.53.447. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Kuhn C., Johnson M., Thomae A., Luo B., Simon S.A., Zhou G., Walker Q.D. The emergence of gonadal hormone influences on dopaminergic function during puberty. Horm. Behav. 2010;58(1):122–137. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2009.10.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Perkins K.A., Lerman C., Coddington S.B., Jetton C., Karelitz J.L., Scott J.A., Wilson A.S. Initial nicotine sensitivity in humans as a function of impulsivity. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 2008;200(4):529–544. doi: 10.1007/s00213-008-1231-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Fillmore M.T., Ostling E.W., Martin C.A., Kelly T.H. Acute effects of alcohol on inhibitory control and information processing in high and low sensation-seekers. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2009;100(1-2):91–99. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2008.09.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Kelly T.H., Delzer T.A., Martin C.A., Harrington N.G., Hays L. R., Bardo M. T. Performance and subjective effects of diazepam and d-amphetamine in high and low sensation seekers. Behav. Pharmacol. 2009;20:505–517. doi: 10.1097/FBP.0b013e3283305e8d. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Sax K.W., Strakowski S.M. Enhanced behavioral response to repeated d-amphetamine and personality traits in humans. Biol. Psychiatry. 1998;44(11):1192–1195. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(98)00168-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Hutchison K.E., Wood M.D., Swift R. Personality factors moderate subjective and psychophysiological responses to d-amphetamine in humans. Exp. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 1999;7(4):493–501. doi: 10.1037/1064-1297.7.4.493. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Kelly T.H., Robbins G., Martin C.A., Fillmore M.T., Lane S.D., Harrington N.G., Rush C.R. Individual differences in drug abuse vulnerability: d-amphetamine and sensation-seeking status. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 2006;189(1):17–25. doi: 10.1007/s00213-006-0487-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Nees F., Tzschoppe J., Patrick C.J., Vollstädt-Klein S., Steiner S., Poustka L., Banaschewski T., Barker G.J., Büchel C., Conrod P.J., Garavan H., Heinz A., Gallinat J., Lathrop M., Mann K., Artiges E., Paus T., Poline J.B., Robbins T.W., Rietschel M., Smolka M.N., Spanagel R., Struve M., Loth E., Schumann G., Flor H., IMAGEN Consortium Determinants of early alcohol use in healthy adolescents: the differential contribution of neuroimaging and psychological factors. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2012;37(4):986–995. doi: 10.1038/npp.2011.282. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Spillane N.S., Muller C.J., Noonan C., Goins R.T., Mitchell C.M., Manson S. Sensation-seeking predicts initiation of daily smoking behavior among American Indian high school students. Addict. Behav. 2012;37:1303–1306. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2012.06.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Stephenson M.T., Helme D.W. Authoritative parenting and sensation seeking as predictors of adolescent cigarette and marijuana use. J. Drug Educ. 2006;36(3):247–270. doi: 10.2190/Y223-2623-7716-2235. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Corr P.J., Kumari V. Individual differences in mood reactions to d-amphetamine: a test of three personality factors. J. Psychopharmacol. (Oxford) 2000;14(4):371–377. doi: 10.1177/026988110001400406. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.White T.L., Lott D.C., de Wit H. Personality and the subjective effects of acute amphetamine in healthy volunteers. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2006;31:1064–1074. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300939. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Ersche K.D., Jones P.S., Williams G.B., Smith D.G., Bullmore E.T., Robbins T.W. Distinctive personality traits and neural correlates associated with stimulant drug use versus familial risk of stimulant dependence. Biol. Psychiatry. 2013;74(2):137–144. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.11.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Horvath L.S., Milich R., Lynam D., Leukefeld C., Clayton R. Sensation Seeking and Substance Use: A Cross-Lagged Panel Design. Individ. Differ. Res. 2004;2:175–183. [Google Scholar]
- 51.Miczek K.A., de Wit H. Challenges for translational psychopharmacology research--some basic principles. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 2008;199(3):291–301. doi: 10.1007/s00213-008-1198-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Ballaz S.J., Akil H., Watson S.J. Previous experience affects subsequent anxiety-like responses in rats bred for novelty seeking. Behav. Neurosci. 2007;121:1113. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.121.5.1113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Dere E., Huston J.P., De Souza Silva M.A. The pharmacology, neuroanatomy and neurogenetics of one-trial object recognition in rodents. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2007;31(5):673–704. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.01.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Marquez C., Nadal R., Armario A. Responsiveness of the hypothalamic-pituitary adrenal axis to different novel environments is a consistent individual trait in adult male outbred rats. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2005;30:179–187. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2004.05.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Hefner K., Holmes A. An investigation of the behavioral actions of ethanol across adolescence in mice. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 2007;191(2):311–322. doi: 10.1007/s00213-006-0646-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Piazza P.V., Deminiere J.M., Le Moal M., Simon H. Factors that predict individual vulnerability to amphetamine self-administration. Science. 1511-1513;1989(245):2781295. doi: 10.1126/science. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Klebaur J.E., Bevins R.A., Segar T.M., Bardo M.T. Individual differences in behavioral responses to novelty and amphetamine self-administration in male and female rats. Behav. Pharmacol. 2001;12(4):267–275. doi: 10.1097/00008877-200107000-00005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Philpot R.M., Wecker L. Dependence of adolescent novelty-seeking behavior on response phenotype and effects of apparatus scaling. Behav. Neurosci. 2008;122(4):861–875. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.122.4.861. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Belin D., Berson N., Balado E., Piazza P.V., Deroche-Gamonet V. High-novelty-preference rats are predisposed to compulsive cocaine self-administration. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2011;36(3):569–579. doi: 10.1038/npp.2010.188. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Piazza P.V., Deroche-Gamonent V., Rouge-Pont F., Le Moal M. Vertical shifts in self-administration dose-response functions predict a drug-vulnerable phenotype predisposed to addiction. J. Neurosci. 2000;20:4226–4232. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-11-04226.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Belin D., Mar A.C., Dalley J.W., Robbins T.W., Everitt B.J. High impulsivity predicts the switch to compulsive cocaine-taking. Science. 2008;320(5881):1352–1355. doi: 10.1126/science.1158136. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Kliethermes C.L., Crabbe J.C. Genetic independence of mouse measures of some aspects of novelty seeking. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 2006;103:5018–5023. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0509724103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Arenas M.C., Daza-Losada M., Vidal-Infer A., Aguilar M.A., Miñarro J., Rodríguez-Arias M. Capacity of novelty-induced locomotor activity and the hole-board test to predict sensitivity to the conditioned rewarding effects of cocaine. Physiol. Behav. 2014;133:152–160. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2014.05.028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Beckmann J.S., Marusich J.A., Gipson C.D., Bardo M.T. Novelty seeking, incentive salience and acquisition of cocaine self-administration in the rat. Behav. Brain Res. 2011;216(1):159–165. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.07.022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Sell S.L., Dillon A.M., Cunningham K.A., Thomas M.L. Estrous cycle influence on individual differences in the response to novelty and cocaine in female rats. Behav. Brain Res. 2005;161(1):69–74. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2005.01.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Pawlak C.R., Ho Y.J., Schwarting R.K. Animal models of human psychopathology based on individual differences in novelty-seeking and anxiety. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2008;32(8):1544–1568. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.06.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Pawlak C.R., Schwarting R.K. Repeated nicotine treatment in rats with high versus low rearing activity: analyses of behavioural sensitisation and place preference. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 2005;178(4):440–450. doi: 10.1007/s00213-004-2024-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Pawlak C.R., Schwarting R.K. Object preference and nicotine consumption in rats with high vs. low rearing activity in a novel open field. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 2002;73(3):679–687. doi: 10.1016/S0091-3057(02)00852-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Harro J., Oreland L., Vasar E., Bradwejn J. Impaired exploratory behaviour after DSP-4 treatment in rats: implications for the increased anxiety after noradrenergic denervation. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 1995;5(4):447–455. doi: 10.1016/0924-977X(95)80003-K. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Larson E.B., Carroll M.E. Wheel running as a predictor of cocaine self-administration and reinstatement in female rats. Pharmacol. Biochem. Be. 2005;82:590–600. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2005.10.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Hughes R.N. Behaviour of male and female rats with free choice of two environments differing in novelty. Anim. Behav. 1968;16(1):92–96. doi: 10.1016/0003-3472(68)90116-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Parker L.A. Place conditioning in a three-or four-choice apparatus: role of stimulus novelty in drug-induced place conditioning. Behav. Neurosci. 1992;106:294. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.106.2.294. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Bardo M., Bowling S., Robinet P., Rowlett J., Lacy M., Mattingly B. Role of dopamine D1 and D2 receptors in noveltymaintained place preference. Exp. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 1993;1:101. doi: 10.1037/1064-1297.1.1-4.101. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Cain M.E., Saucier D.A., Bardo M.T. Novelty seeking and drug use: contribution of an animal model. Exp. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 2005;13(4):367–375. doi: 10.1037/1064-1297.13.4.367. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Vidal-Infer A., Arenas M.C., Daza-Losada M., Aguilar M.A., Miñarro J., Rodríguez-Arias M. High novelty-seeking predicts greater sensitivity to the conditioned rewarding effects of cocaine. Pharmacol. Biochem. Be. 2012;102:124–132. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2012.03.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Lalonde R. The neurobiological basis of spontaneous alternation. Neurosci. Biobehav R. 2002;26:91–104. doi: 10.1016/S0149-7634(01)00041-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Dember W.N., Fowler H. Spontaneous alteration after free and forced trials. Can. J. Psychol. 1959;13:151–154. doi: 10.1037/h0083776. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Douglas R.J., Isaacson R.L. Homogeneity of single trial response tendencies and spontaneous alternation in the T-maze. Psychol. Rep. 1965;16:87–92. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1965.16.1.87. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Boissier J.R., Simon P. [The exploration reaction in the mouse. Preliminary note]. Therapie. 1962;17:1225–1232. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Boissier J.R., Simon P., Lwoff J.M. Use of a Particular Mouse Reaction (Hole Board Method) for the Study of Psychotropic Drugs. Therapie. 1964;19:571–583. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Abreu-Villaça Y., Queiroz-Gomes Fdo.E., Dal Monte A.P., Filgueiras C.C., Manhães A.C. Individual differences in novelty-seeking behavior but not in anxiety response to a new environment can predict nicotine consumption in adolescent C57BL/6 mice. Behav. Brain Res. 2006;167(1):175–182. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2005.09.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Mateos-García A., Roger-Sánchez C., Rodriguez-Arias M., Miñarro J., Aguilar M.A., Manzanedo C., Arenas M.C. Higher sensitivity to the conditioned rewarding effects of cocaine and MDMA in High-Novelty-Seekers mice exposed to a cocaine binge during adolescence. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 2015;232(1):101–113. doi: 10.1007/s00213-014-3642-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Takeda H., Tsuji M., Matsumiya T. Changes in head-dipping behavior in the hole-board test reflect the anxiogenic and/or anxiolytic state in mice. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 1998;350(1):21–29. doi: 10.1016/S0014-2999(98)00223-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Saitoh A., Hirose N., Yamada M., Yamada M., Nozaki C., Oka T., Kamei J. Changes in emotional behavior of mice in the holeboard test after olfactory bulbectomy. J. Pharmacol. Sci. 2006;102:377–386. doi: 10.1254/jphs.FP0060837. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Calabrese E.J. An assessment of anxiolytic drug screening tests: hormetic dose responses predominate. Crit. Rev. Toxicol. 2008;38(6):489–542. doi: 10.1080/10408440802014238. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Kliethermes C.L., Kamens H.M., Crabbe J.C. Drug reward and intake in lines of mice selectively bred for divergent exploration of a hole board apparatus. Genes Brain Behav. 2007;6(7):608–618. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2006.00289.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Nicholls B., Springham A., Mellanby J. The playground maze: a new method for measuring directed exploration in the rat. J. Neurosci. Methods. 1992;43(2-3):171–180. doi: 10.1016/0165-0270(92)90026-A. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Klebaur J.E., Bardo M.T. Individual differences in novelty seeking on the playground maze predict amphetamine conditioned place preference. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 1999;63(1):131–136. doi: 10.1016/S0091-3057(98)00258-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Frick K.M., Gresack J.E. Sex differences in the behavioral response to spatial and object novelty in adult C57BL/6 mice. Behav. Neurosci. 2003;117(6):1283–1291. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.117.6.1283. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Ennaceur A., Aggleton J.P. The effects of neurotoxic lesions of the perirhinal cortex combined to fornix transection on object recognition memory in the rat. Behav. Brain Res. 1997;88(2):181–193. doi: 10.1016/S0166-4328(97)02297-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Bevins R.A., Bardo M.T. Conditioned increase in place preference by access to novel objects: antagonism by MK-801. Behav. Brain Res. 1999;99(1):53–60. doi: 10.1016/S0166-4328(98)00069-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Douglas L.A., Varlinskaya E.I., Spear L.P. Novel-object place conditioning in adolescent and adult male and female rats: effects of social isolation. Physiol. Behav. 2003;80(2-3):317–325. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2003.08.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Jetha M.K., Segalowitz S.J. Adolescent brain development: Implications for behavior. New York: Academic Press; 2012. [Google Scholar]
- 94.Laviola G., Macrì S., Morley-Fletcher S., Adriani W. Risk-taking behavior in adolescent mice: psychobiological determinants and early epigenetic influence. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2003;27(1-2):19–31. doi: 10.1016/S0149-7634(03)00006-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Kellogg C.K., Awatramani G.B., Piekut D.T. Adolescent development alters stressor-induced Fos immunoreactivity in rat brain. Neuroscience. 1998;83(3):681–689. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4522(97)00408-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Adriani W., Macrì S., Pacifici R., Laviola G. Peculiar vulnerability to nicotine oral self-administration in mice during early adolescence. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2002;27(2):212–224. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(02)00295-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Macrì S., Adriani W., Chiarotti F., Laviola G. Risk-taking during exploration of a plus-maze is greater in adolescent than in juvenile or adult mice. Anim. Behav. 2002;64:541–546. doi: 10.1006/anbe.2002.4004. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Calkins S.D. Psychobiological models of adolescent risk: implications for prevention and intervention. Dev. Psychobiol. 2010;52(3):213–215. doi: 10.1002/dev.20435. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Steinberg L. A dual systems model of adolescent risk-taking. Dev. Psychobiol. 2010;52(3):216–224. doi: 10.1002/dev.20445. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Laviola G., Pascucci T., Pieretti S. d-Amphetamine-induced behavioural sensitization and striatal dopamine release in awake freelymoving periadolescent rats. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 2001;68:115–124. doi: 10.1016/S0091-3057(00)00430-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.Walker Q.D., Kuhn C.M. Cocaine increases stimulated dopamine release more in periadolescent than adult rats. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 2008;30(5):412–418. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2008.04.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102.Badanich K.A., Adler K.J., Kirstein C.L. Adolescents differ from adults in cocaine conditioned place preference and cocaine-induced dopamine in the nucleus accumbens septi. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 2006;550(1-3):95–106. doi: 10.1016/j.ejphar.2006.08.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103.Frantz K.J., O’Dell L.E., Parsons L.H. Behavioral and neurochemical responses to cocaine in periadolescent and adult rats. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2007;32(3):625–637. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301130. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104.Camarini R., Griffin W.C., III, Yanke A.B., Rosalina dos Santos B., Olive M.F. Effects of adolescent exposure to cocaine on locomotor activity and extracellular dopamine and glutamate levels in nucleus accumbens of DBA/2J mice. Brain Res. 2008;1193:34–42. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.11.045. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105.Chambers R.A., Taylor J.R., Potenza M.N. Developmental neurocircuitry of motivation in adolescence: a critical period of addiction vulnerability. Am. J. Psychiatry. 2003;160(6):1041–1052. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.6.1041. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 106.Fareri D.S., Martin L.N., Delgado M.R. Reward-related processing in the human brain: developmental considerations. Dev. Psychopathol. 2008;20(4):1191–1211. doi: 10.1017/S0954579408000576. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 107.Ernst M., Romeo R.D., Andersen S.L. Neurobiology of the development of motivated behaviors in adolescence: a window into a neural systems model. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 2009;93(3):199–211. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2008.12.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 108.Spear L.P. The developing brain and adolescent-typical behavior patterns: an evolutionary approach. In: Walker E., Romer D., editors. Adolescent Psychopathology and the Developing Brain: Integrating Brain and Prevention Science. New York: Oxford University Press; 2007. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 109.Doremus T.L., Brunell S.C., Rajendran P., Spear L.P. Factors influencing elevated ethanol consumption in adolescent relative to adult rats. Alcohol. Clin. Exp. Res. 2005;29(10):1796–1808. doi: 10.1097/01.alc.0000183007.65998.aa. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 110.Schramm-Sapyta N.L., Walker Q.D., Caster J.M., Levin E.D., Kuhn C.M. Are adolescents more vulnerable to drug addiction than adults? Evidence from animal models. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 2009;206(1):1–21. doi: 10.1007/s00213-009-1585-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 111.Vaidya J.G., Grippo A.J., Johnson A.K., Watson D. A comparative developmental study of impulsivity in rats and humans: the role of reward sensitivity. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2004;1021:395–398. doi: 10.1196/annals.1308.051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 112.Wilmouth C.E., Spear L.P. Hedonic sensitivity in adolescent and adult rats: taste reactivity and voluntary sucrose consumption. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 2009;92:73–566. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2009.02.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 113.Friemel C.M., Spanagel R., Schneider M. Reward sensitivity for a palatable food reward peaks during pubertal developmental in rats. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2010;4:00039. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 114.Brenhouse H.C., Andersen S.L. Delayed extinction and stronger reinstatement of cocaine conditioned place preference in adolescent rats, compared to adults. Behav. Neurosci. 2008;122(2):460–465. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.122.2.460. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 115.Brenhouse H.C., Dumais K., Andersen S.L. Enhancing the salience of dullness: behavioral and pharmacological strategies to facilitate extinction of drug-cue associations in adolescent rats. Neuroscience. 2010;169:628–636. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2010.05.063. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 116.Zakharova E., Leoni G., Kichko I., Izenwasser S. Differential effects of methamphetamine and cocaine on conditioned place preference and locomotor activity in adult and adolescent male rats. Behav. Brain. Res. 2009;198:45–50. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2008.10.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 117.Aberg M., Wade D., Wall E., Izenwasser S. Effect of MDMA (ecstasy) on activity and cocaine conditioned place preference in adult and adolescent rats. Neurotoxicol. Teratol. 2007;29(1):37–46. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2006.09.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 118.Balda M.A., Anderson K.L., Itzhak Y. Adolescent and adult responsiveness to the incentive value of cocaine reward in mice: role of neuronal nitric oxide synthase (nNOS) gene. Neuropharmacology. 2006;51:341–349. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2006.03.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 119.Campbell J.O., Wood R.D., Spear L.P. Cocaine and morphine-induced place conditioning in adolescent and adult rats. Physiol. Behav. 2000;68(4):487–493. doi: 10.1016/S0031-9384(99)00225-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 120.Schramm-Sapyta N.L., Pratt A.R., Winder D.G. Effects of periadolescent versus adult cocaine exposure on cocaine conditioned place preference and motor sensitization in mice. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 2004;173(1-2):41–48. doi: 10.1007/s00213-003-1696-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 121.Doremus-Fitzwater T.L., Varlinskaya E.I., Spear L.P. Social and non-social anxiety in adolescent and adult rats after repeated restraint. Physiol. Behav. 2009;97:484–494. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2009.03.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 122.Mathews I.Z., Morrissey M.D., McCormick C.M. Individual differences in activity predict locomotor activity and conditioned place preference to amphetamine in both adolescent and adult rats. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 2010;95:63–71. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2009.12.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 123.Meyer A.C., Rahman S., Charnigo R.J., Dwoskin L.P., Crabbe J.C., Bardo M.T. Genetics of novelty seeking, amphetamine self-administration and reinstatement using inbred rats. Genes Brain Behav. 2010;9(7):790–798. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2010.00616.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 124.Thomsen M., Caine S.B. Intravenous drug self-administration in mice: practical considerations. Behav. Genet. 2007;37(1):101–118. doi: 10.1007/s10519-006-9097-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 125.Kabbaj M. Individual differences in vulnerability to drug abuse: the high responders/low responders model. CNS Neurol. Disord. Drug Targets. 2006;5(5):513–520. doi: 10.2174/187152706778559318. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 126.Pierre P.J., Vezina P. Predisposition to self-administer amphetamine: the contribution of response to novelty and prior exposure to the drug. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 1997;129(3):277–284. doi: 10.1007/s002130050191. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 127.Grimm J.W., See R.E. Cocaine self-administration in ovariectomized rats is predicted by response to novelty, attenuated by 17-beta estradiol, and associated with abnormal vaginal cytology. Physiol. Behav. 1997;61(5):755–761. doi: 10.1016/S0031-9384(96)00532-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 128.Marinelli M., White F.J. Enhanced vulnerability to cocaine self-administration is associated with elevated impulse activity of midbrain dopamine neurons. J. Neurosci. 2000;20(23):8876–8885. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-23-08876.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 129.Mantsch J.R., Ho A., Schlussman S.D., Kreek M.J. Predictable individual differences in the initiation of cocaine self-administration by rats under extended-access conditions are dose-dependent. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 2001;157(1):31–39. doi: 10.1007/s002130100744. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 130.Davis B.A., Clinton S.M., Akil H., Becker J.B. The effects of novelty-seeking phenotypes and sex differences on acquisition of cocaine self-administration in selectively bred High-Responder and Low-Responder rats. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 2008;90(3):331–338. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2008.03.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 131.Cummings J.A., Gowl B.A., Westenbroek C., Clinton S.M., Akil H., Becker J.B. Effects of a selectively bred novelty-seeking phenotype on the motivation to take cocaine in male and female rats. Biol. Sex Differ. 2011;2:3. doi: 10.1186/2042-6410-2-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 132.Dickson P.E., Ndukum J., Wilcox T., Clark J., Roy B., Zhang L., Li Y., Lin D.T., Chesler E.J. Association of novelty-related behaviors and intravenous cocaine self-administration in Diversity Outbred mice. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 2015;232(6):1011–1024. doi: 10.1007/s00213-014-3737-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 133.Kabbaj M., Norton C.S., Kollack-Walker S., Watson S.J., Robinson T.E., Akil H. Social defeat alters the acquisition of cocaine self-administration in rats: role of individual differences in cocaine-taking behavior. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 2001;158(4):382–387. doi: 10.1007/s002130100918. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 134.De la Peña I., Gonzales E.L., de la Peña J.B., Kim B.N., Han D.H., Shin C.Y., Cheong J.H. Individual differences in novelty-seeking behavior in spontaneously hypertensive rats: Enhanced sensitivity to the reinforcing effect of methylphenidate in the high novelty-preferring subpopulation. J. Neurosci. Methods. 2014 doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2014.08.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 135.Mitchell J.M., Cunningham C.L., Mark G.P. Locomotor activity predicts acquisition of self-administration behavior but not cocaine intake. Behav. Neurosci. 2005;119(2):464–472. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.119.2.464. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 136.Bird J., Schenk S. Contribution of impulsivity and novelty-seeking to the acquisition and maintenance of MDMA self-administration. Addict. Biol. 2013;18(4):654–664. doi: 10.1111/j.1369-1600.2012.00477.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 137.Bardo M.T., Bevins R.A. Conditioned place preference: what does it add to our preclinical understanding of drug reward? Psychopharmacology http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/ 11255927. 2000;153:31–43. doi: 10.1007/s002130000569. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 138.Tzschentke T.M. Measuring reward with the conditioned place preference (CPP) paradigm: update of the last decade. Addict. Biol. 2007;12:227–462. doi: 10.1111/j.1369-1600.2007.00070.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 139.Aguilar M.A., Rodríguez-Arias M., Miñarro J. Neurobiological mechanisms of the reinstatement of drug-conditioned place preference. Brain Res. Brain Res. Rev. 2009;59(2):253–277. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2008.08.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 140.Moser P., Wolinsky T., Duxon M., Porsolt R.D. How good are current approaches to nonclinical evaluation of abuse and dependence? J. Pharmacol. Exp. Ther. 2011;336(3):588–595. doi: 10.1124/jpet.110.169979. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 141.Tzschentke T.M. Measuring reward with the conditioned place preference paradigm: a comprehensive review of drug effects, recent progress and new issues. Prog. Neurobiol. 1998;56(6):613–672. doi: 10.1016/S0301-0082(98)00060-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 142.Robbins T.W. The acquisition of responding with conditioned reinforcement: effects of pipradrol, methylphenidate, d-amphetamine, and nomifensine. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 1978;58(1):79–87. doi: 10.1007/BF00426794. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 143.Robinet P.M., Rowlett J.K., Bardo M.T. Individual differences in novelty-induced activity and the rewarding effects of novelty and amphetamine in rats. Behav. Processes. 1998;44(1):1–9. doi: 10.1016/S0376-6357(98)00022-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 144.Erb S.M., Parker L.A. Individual differences in novelty-induced activity do not predict strength of amphetamine-induced place conditioning. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 1994;48(3):581–586. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(94)90317-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 145.Pelloux Y., Costentin J., Duterte-Boucher D. Differential effects of novelty exposure on place preference conditioning to amphetamine and its oral consumption. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 2004;171(3):277–285. doi: 10.1007/s00213-003-1584-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 146.Kosten T.A., Miserendino M.J. Dissociation of novelty- and cocaine-conditioned locomotor activity from cocaine place conditioning. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 1998;60(4):785–791. doi: 10.1016/S0091-3057(97)00388-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 147.Dietz D., Wang H., Kabbaj M. Corticosterone fails to produce conditioned place preference or conditioned place aversion in rats. Behav. Brain Res. 2007;181(2):287–291. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2007.04.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 148.Capriles N., Watson S., Jr, Akil H. Individual differences in the improvement of cocaine-induced place preference response by the 5-HT2C receptor antagonist SB242084 in rats. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 2012;220(4):731–740. doi: 10.1007/s00213-011-2524-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 149.Gong W., Neill D., Justice J.B., Jr Conditioned place preference and locomotor activation produced by injection of psychostimulants into ventral pallidum. Brain Res. 1996;707(1):64–74. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(95)01222-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 150.Shimosato K., Watanabe S. Concurrent evaluation of locomotor response to novelty and propensity toward cocaine conditioned place preference in mice. J. Neurosci. Methods. 2003;128(1-2):103–110. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0270(03)00153-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 151.Brabant C., Quertemont E., Tirelli E. Evidence that the relations between novelty-induced activity, locomotor stimulation and place preference induced by cocaine qualitatively depend upon the dose: a multiple regression analysis in inbred C57BL/6J mice. Behav. Brain Res. 2005;158:201–210. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2004.08.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 152.Maldonado C., Rodríguez-Arias M., Castillo A., Aguilar M.A., Miñarro J. Gamma-hydroxybutyric acid affects the acquisition and reinstatement of cocaine-induced conditioned place preference in mice. Behav. Pharmacol. 2006;17(2):119–131. doi: 10.1097/01.fbp.0000190685.84984.ec. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 153.Montagud-Romero S., Daza-Losada M., Vidal-Infer A., Maldonado C., Aguilar M.A., Miñarro J., Rodríguez-Arias M. The novelty-seeking phenotype modulates the long-lasting effects of intermittent ethanol administration during adolescence. PLoS One. 2014;9(3):e92576. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0092576. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 154.Rodríguez-Arias M., Vaccaro S., Arenas M.C., Aguilar M.A., Miñarro J. The novelty-seeking phenotype modulates the long-lasting effects of adolescent MDMA exposure. Physiol. Behav. 2015;141:190–198. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2015.01.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 155.Salamone J.D., Correa M. The mysterious motivational functions of mesolimbic dopamine. Neuron. 2012;76(3):470–485. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.10.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 156.Nutt D.J., Lingford-Hughes A., Erritzoe D., Stokes P.R. The dopamine theory of addiction: 40 years of highs and lows. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2015;16(5):305–312. doi: 10.1038/nrn3939. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 157.Norbury A., Husain M. Sensation-seeking: Dopaminergic modulation and risk for psychopathology. Behav. Brain Res. 2015;288:79–93. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2015.04.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 158.Costa V.D., Tran V.L., Turchi J., Averbeck B.B. Dopamine modulates novelty seeking behavior during decision making. Behav. Neurosci. 2014;128(5):556–566. doi: 10.1037/a0037128. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 159.Pogorelov V.M., Rodriguiz R.M., Insco M.L., Caron M.G., Wetsel W.C. Novelty seeking and stereotypic activation of behavior in mice with disruption of the Dat1 gene. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2005;30(10):1818–1831. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300724. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 160.Misslin R., Ropartz P., Jung L. Impairment of responses to novelty by apomorphine and its antagonism by neuroleptics in mice. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 1984;82(1-2):113–117. doi: 10.1007/BF00426392. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 161.Pierce R.C., Crawford C.A., Nonneman A.J., Mattingly B.A., Bardo M.T. Effect of forebrain dopamine depletion on novelty-induced place preference behavior in rats. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 1990;36(2):321–325. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(90)90411-A. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 162.Powell S.B., Paulus M.P., Hartman D.S. Godel, T., Geyer, M.ARO-10-5824 is a selective dopamine D4 receptor agonist that increases novel object exploration in C57 mice. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2003;44:473–481. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3908(02)00412-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 163.Zuckerman M. Sensation seeking: A comparative approach to a human trait. Behav. Brain Sci. 1984;7:413–434. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X00018938. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 164.Gjedde A., Kumakura Y., Cumming P., Linnet J., Møller A. Inverted-U-shaped correlation between dopamine receptor availability in striatum and sensation seeking. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2010;107(8):3870–3875. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0912319107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 165.Ebstein R.P., Novick O., Umansky R., Priel B., Osher Y., Blaine D., Belmaker R.H. Dopamine D4 receptor (D4DR) exon III polymorphism associated with the human personality trait of novelty seeking. Nat. Genet. 1996;12:78–80. doi: 10.1038/ng0196-78. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 166.Cloninger C.R. A systematic method for clinical description and classification of personality variants. A proposal. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry. 1987;44(6):573–588. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1987.01800180093014. b. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 167.Bevins R.A., Klebaur J.E., Bardo M.T. Individual differences in response to novelty, amphetamine-induced activity and drug discrimination in rats. Behav. Pharmacol. 1997;8(2-3):113–123. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 168.Deroche-Gamonet V., Piazza P.V. Psychobiology of cocaine addiction: Contribution of a multisymptomatic animal model of loss of control. Neuropharmacology. 2014;76 doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2013.07.014. Part B:437-49. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]