Abstract
Young men who have sex with men (YMSM) may be at greater risk for body dissatisfaction, compared to their heterosexual peers. However, differences within YMSM populations are understudied, precluding the identification of YMSM who are at greatest risk. This study examined body dissatisfaction in a racially/ethnically diverse sample of YMSM ages 18–19 in New York City. Using cross-sectional data from the baseline visit of a longitudinal cohort study of YMSM (N = 591), body dissatisfaction was assessed using the Male Body Attitudes Scale. Three outcomes were modeled using linear regression: (1) overall body dissatisfaction, (2) muscularity dissatisfaction, and (3) body fat dissatisfaction. Covariates in the models included race/ethnicity, sexual orientation, BMI, gay community affiliation, and internalized homonegativity. White YMSM experienced greater body dissatisfaction across the three models. Internalized homonegativity was a statistically significant predictor of dissatisfaction across the three models, though its association with body dissatisfaction was relatively small. The findings point to future avenues of research, particularly qualitative research to explore demographic and cultural nuances in body attitudes among YMSM.
Keywords: Body image, Body dissatisfaction, Gay, Bisexual, MSM, Internalized homonegativity, Sexual orientation
Introduction
Body dissatisfaction and body image concerns have historically been seen as a problem affecting women, yet there is a growing literature base documenting body dysmorphia and body dissatisfaction among men (Blashill, 2010; Brennan et al., 2013; Daniel & Bridges, 2010; Parent, 2013; Pope, Phillips, & Olivardia, 2000). While there is significant variation across studies, there is evidence that gay men and other sexual minority men may experience greater body dissatisfaction than their heterosexual peers (Morrison, Morrison, & Sager, 2004). Similar findings have been noted among young sexual minority men and young men who have sex with men (YMSM) (Calzo, Corliss, Blood, Field, & Austin, 2013; Carper, Negy, & Tantleff-Dunn, 2010; Martins, Tiggemann, & Kirkbride, 2007; Michaels, Parent, & Moradi, 2013). Specific components of body dissatisfaction for YMSM may include overall dissatisfaction, muscularity dissatisfaction, and body fat dissatisfaction (Blashill, 2010; Brennan, Craig, & Thompson, 2012; Tylka, Bergeron, & Schwartz, 2005; Yelland & Tiggemann, 2003). Body dissatisfaction can have significant behavioral and health implications for YMSM. For example, prior research with male adolescents and young adults (sexual minorities as well as heterosexuals) indicates associations between body dissatisfaction and poor mental health (e.g., depression, low self-esteem), unhealthy weight control behaviors (e.g., fasting and disordered eating), and use of health supplements with potential health risks (Field et al., 2005; Hadland, Austin, Goodenow, & Calzo, 2014; Holsen, Kraft, & Roysamb, 2001; Neumark-Sztainer, Paxton, Hannan, Haines, & Story, 2006; Olivardia, Pope, Borowiecki, & Cohane, 2004; Smith, Hawkeswood, Bodell, & Joiner, 2011; Tylka, 2011). Body dissatisfaction may also be associated with more distal behavioral outcomes, such as sexual risk behavior among MSM or lower self-efficacy for condom use across genders and sexual orientations (Blashill & Safren, 2014, 2015; Parent & Moradi, 2014; Wilton, 2009).
To date, the bulk of YMSM body dissatisfaction research has been comparative (i.e., comparing YMSM to heterosexual peers) (Calzo et al., 2013; Carper et al., 2010; Hadland et al., 2014), or has applied theory such as objectification theory (Martins et al., 2007), or has examined mechanisms of social influence (Tylka, 2011). Taken together, these studies have demonstrated greater risk among YMSM, and have also examined mechanisms underlying this increased risk for YMSM as a whole. However, they do not identify the young men within YMSM populations who are experiencing the greatest burdens of body dissatisfaction. In this paper, we seek to explore sociodemographic (race/ethnicity, sexual orientation), physical (BMI), and psychosocial factors (internalized homonegativity and gay community affiliation), associated with three components (overall body composition, muscularity, and body fat) of body dissatisfaction within a sample of YMSM.
A number of factors that are potentially associated with YMSM body dissatisfaction have been explored in the literature. For men of all sexual orientations, it is plausible that such dissatisfaction is related to the intense pressures of Western cultures in which youth and beauty are valued and highly sought after (Beren, Hayden, Wilfley, & Grilo, 1996). For YMSM, this may be compounded by intense pressures within gay cultures (Drummond, 2005; Martins et al., 2007; Williamson, 1999). Other factors potentially related to body attitudes include nalized homonegativity, gay community affiliation, body mass index (BMI), and racial and ethnic identities.
Internalized Homonegativity
Sexual minorities, and sexual minority youth in particular, are at risk of internalizing the homophobia they encounter in the social environment (Meyer, 2003; Newcomb & Mustanski, 2010; Radkowsky & Siegel, 1997; Stall, Friedman, & Catania,2008). Research has examined the associations between internalized homonegativity and body dissatisfaction in sexual minority men, particularly regarding muscularity. This link has largely been interpreted by researchers as either a psychological, social, or physical defense against the perceived weakness or effeminacy of gay men, and/or harassment associated with gender non-conformity (Brennan et al., 2012; Halkitis, Green, & Wilton, 2004; Kimmel & Mahalik, 2005; Wiseman & Moradi, 2010). Prior research has identified associations between internalized homonegativity and body dissatisfaction (Kimmel & Mahalik, 2005; Reilly & Rudd, 2006) and drive for muscularity (Brennan et al., 2012).
Gay Cultures and Community Affiliation
Images of idealized bodies, internalization of these ideals, and objectification may contribute to body attitudes or dissatisfaction among both men and women (Daniel & Bridges, 2010; Parent & Moradi, 2011; Wiseman & Moradi, 2010). For men who have sex with men (MSM), body dissatisfaction may also be specifically attributable to aspects of gay culture (Levesque & Vichesky, 2006; Morrison et al., 2004; Williamson, 1999; Wood, 2004). While men’s bodies in general are increasingly objectified in broader non-gay cultures, images of lean, muscular, and sexualized bodies have historically pervaded gay cultures and gay media (Drummond, 2005; Martins et al., 2007; Williamson, 1999). Gay men may internalize the notion of their own bodies as objects (i.e., self-objectify) in line with the high social and sexual value accorded to these bodies (Daniel & Bridges, 2010; Drummond, 2005; Martins et al., 2007; Wiseman & Moradi, 2010).
Studies examining associations between gay community affiliation and body dissatisfaction yield conflicting results. Stronger gay community affiliation may be associated with greater general body dissatisfaction, or greater drive for muscularity, specifically (Beren et al., 1996; Hunt, Gonsalkorale, & Nosek, 2012; Levesque & Vichesky, 2006). However, contrary evidence also exists; community affiliation may be unrelated to body dissatisfaction (Tiggemann, Martins, & Kirkbride, 2007), or may in fact be protective with regard to body image and attitudes (Feldman & Meyer, 2007; Levesque & Vichesky, 2006). With regard to directionality, it is also plausible that one’s body or body image impacts one’s perceived or actual gay community affiliation (Wiseman & Moradi, 2010). For YMSM, the development of their sexual identity and increasing involvement in gay communities may engender body image concerns during this maturation period (Calzo et al., 2013; Morrison et al., 2004). Integrating gay community affiliation in YMSM body dissatisfaction research may also help to capture a social context that influences body attitudes during a key developmental period.
Body Mass Index
The relationship between body mass index and body dissatisfaction in males is nuanced and findings vary across existing research, likely attributable to the (1) inability of BMI to distinguish between fat mass and muscle mass, paired with (2) the use of general dissatisfaction measures. For example, studies using a global measure of body dissatisfaction (i.e., attitudes toward the body as a whole, or multifaceted items summed into a singles core) have typically evidenced a curvilinear shape between BMI and dissatisfaction among males (Austin, Haines, & Veugelers, 2009; Calzoetal., 2012; Kostanski, Fisher, & Gullone, 2004; Muth & Cash, 1997; Presnell, Bearman, & Stice, 2004). This relationship has been interpreted, succinctly, as underweight men desiring more body mass, and overweight men desiring leanness and/or less body mass. However, this “global” approach obscures the distinction of leanness and muscular definition often desired by men. Among studies that have used muscularity and body fat measures more specifically, BMI has typically been associated with body fat attitudes, but inconsistently (and linearly, when so) associated with muscularity attitudes (Brennan et al., 2012; Daniel & Bridges, 2010; Huntet al., 2012; Tylka, 2011; Tylka & Andorka, 2012).
Demographic Differences
Few studies have explored racial and ethnic differences in sexual minority men’s body image and attitudes (Brennan et al., 2013; Siconolfi, Halkitis, Allomong, & Burton, 2009). In general populations of men (without regard to sexual orientation), differences in body image across racial and ethnic groups have been largely inconsistent, although Black men in the U.S. may tend to have better body image than White men (Ricciardelli, McCabe, Williams, & Thompson, 2007). Data regarding differences related to drive for muscularity or weight gain are sparse and also inconsistent (Ricciardelli et al., 2007). Our prior research has not found racial/ethnic differences in adult gay and bisexual men’s body image, though there were differences in disordered eating scores with White and Latino men evidencing higher scores (Siconolfi et al., 2009). Data from the Youth Risk Behavior Surveillance System (YRBSS) have also indicated that Hispanic young men were more likely than their peers to engage in weight control (Chao et al., 2008). There is a paucity of analyses examining racial/ethnic differences in body image and body dissatisfaction among both young and adult sexual minority men, likely attributable to predominately White samples in the existing research (Brennan et al., 2012, 2013).
Finally, as sexual orientation is neither binary nor monolithic, there may be differences in body image or dissatisfaction within sexual minority subgroups. However, prior studies have also relied heavily on gay-identified samples and thus prohibit analysis of potential nuances between MSM sub-populations, such as non-gay-identified MSM (Wiseman & Moradi, 2010). It is plausible that body dissatisfaction may be associated with sexual orientation, such that men with gay or “exclusively homosexual” sexual orientations or identities would be more strongly influenced by gay body image ideals than other non-gay MSM. For example, muscular ideals portrayed in gay media may be more salient for gay-identified men than non-gay MSM because MSM are less likely to be exposed to these ideals, and if exposed, may not identify as strongly with these ideals. There is a need for research on the role of both sexual orientation and race/ethnicity as they relate to men’s body image and attitudes (Blashill, 2010; Brennan et al., 2012, 2013).
Gaps in Existing Literature
There are also a number of methodological shortcomings in prior studies of sexual minority men. Gay and bisexual men’s body image is multifaceted and may encompass constructs such as muscularity and body fat, though a number of body image measures have not explicitly included these factors (Blashill, 2010; Levesque & Vichesky, 2006; McCabe & Ricciardelli, 2004). Along these lines, early research with men utilized measures that were originally developed for women, and thus neglected the specific components of body image that likely differ between men and women (e.g., muscularity, versus thinness) (Kaminski, Chapman, Haynes, & Own, 2005; McCabe & Ricciardelli, 2004; Parent, 2013). Finally, sampling limitations in current studies (i.e., small samples, predominately White samples, predominately gay-identified samples, and adult samples) preclude knowledge regarding body image and body attitudes among racially/ethnically diverse YMSM.
In summary, comparative analyses have indicated that YMSM are at risk for body dissatisfaction. Yet, YMSM are significantly understudied with regard to body image and body dissatisfaction. In these analyses, we seek to build upon existing literature by examining associations among multifaceted body dissatisfaction, race/ethnicity, sexual orientation, BMI, internalized homonegativity, and community affiliation in a diverse sample of YMSM.
Based on the existing literature, we hypothesized that (1) greater internalized homophobia, and (2) White race/ethnicity would be associated with greater body dissatisfaction. We also hypothesized that greater internalized homophobia would be associated with greater muscularity dissatisfaction. We did not generate a priori hypotheses regarding the other covariates of interest, as existing data are limited and/or conflicting.
Method
Participants
This study employs data collected during the baseline assessment of a prospective cohort study of YMSM residing in New York City (NYC). Complete study details are described elsewhere (Halkitis et al., 2013). As explained to participants, the study sought to examine the longitudinal relationships between mental health, sexual behavior, and substance use in a cohort of young sexual minority men. Briefly, active (e.g., solicitation of individuals) and passive (e.g., posters) methods were used to recruit a diverse sample of YMSM from across the five boroughs of NYC. Recruitment took place over a period of 23 months in 2009–2011. Eligibility criteria included ages 18–19, biological male sex, sexual activity (i.e., contact that could have resulted in orgasm) with another man in the prior 6 months, residence in the NYC metropolitan area, and a self-reported HIV-negative or unknown serostatus. During recruitment, 2,068 individuals screened for eligibility. A total sample of 600 men completed the baseline survey at the research center. For the analyses presented here, seven participants had missing data for a key variable (height, n = 5; internalized homonegativity, n = 2); an additional 2 participants had incomplete data. Thus, 8 YMSM were excluded from analyses, yielding an analytic sample of 591 YMSM.
Audio computer-assisted self-interview (ACASI) software was used to collect all data analyzed here. Participants provided informed consent before participation, and the study was approved by the IRB at the New York University. Participants received $35 remuneration for completing the baseline survey.
Independent Variables
Sociodemographics
Participants indicated their race and ethnicity, which we collapsed into categories of Black (non-Hispanic), Hispanic or Latino, Asian or Pacific Islander (non-Hispanic), and White (non-Hispanic) or multiracial/other (non-Hispanic) (e.g., multiple racial identities, Native American, unsure, etc.). Participants also indicated their sexual identity on a 7-point Kinsey scale, ranging from “exclusively homosexual” to “exclusively heterosexual” (Kinsey, Pomeroy, & Martin, 1948). For the present analysis, we dichotomized this measure as “exclusively homo-sexual” and “not exclusively homosexual.” No participants identified as “exclusively heterosexual.”
Body Mass Index (BMI)
BMI was calculated as a continuous variable after data collection using the standard formula of kilograms over height squared (Keys, Fidanza, Karvonen, & Kimura, 1972). Participants reported their height (feet, inches) and weight (pounds) separately.
Psychosocial Factors
We assessed local gay community affiliation using an item developed by O’Donnell et al. (2002). Participants responded to the statement “I feel part of the gay community in New York City” on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from “strongly disagree” to “strongly agree.” Because responses at either “strongly” anchor were sparse, we collapsed responses in to “disagree,” “neutral,” and “agree.” Finally, we measured internalized homonegativity using four items from Thiedeetal. (2003). Participants indicated their agreement with statements (e.g., “Sometimes I wish I was not gay/bisexual.”) on a 5-point Likert scale, ranging from “strongly disagree” to “strongly agree.” For this analysis, a total score was created by summing the four responses (α=0.87); a higher score indicates greater internalized homonegativity.
Dependent Variables
Body Dissatisfaction
We assessed body dissatisfaction using the Male Body Attitudes Scale (MBAS) (Tylka et al., 2005). The MBAS consists of 24 items in total across three multi-item subscales: muscularity (e.g., “I wish my arms were stronger.”), body fat (e.g., “I think my body should be leaner.”), and height (e.g., “I wish I were taller.”). The MBAS yields a total score, muscularity score, body fat score, and height score, each of which is calculated by averaging an individual’s responses to corresponding items. Participants indicated the frequency of a given attitude on a 6-point scale ranging from “never” to “always.” A higher score indicates greater dissatisfaction. The MBAS has strong internal consistency (α’s = 0.91 to 0.82) and strongtest–retest reliability (r’s = 0.94 to 0.81) (Tylka et al., 2005). Though all items were included in the total body dissatisfaction score, we did not examine the height subscale separately due to concerns about its two-item factor structure and its potentially limited relevance to gay men (see Blashill, 2010; Blashill & Vander Wal, 2009a).
Because this was the first known use of the MBAS with a diverse sample of YMSM, we conducted an exploratory factor analysis. Results indicated three factors, nearly identical to the original measure. Thus, for comparability across studies, we maintained the existing structure of the MBAS. Additionally, a new, three-item fourth factor emerged (items 4, 17, and 18), but was ignored because it lacked a coherent, theoretical construct and thus appeared to be spurious. The overall (α= 0.92), muscularity (α= 0.88) and body fat (α= 0.92) Cronbach’s alphas were high in this sample, indicating a good reliability of assessment. The overall body attitude score consisted of all 24 scale items, including overall attitudes (e.g., “I feel dissatisfied with my overall body build.”) as well as the muscularity and body fat items. The muscularity subscale consisted of 10 items relevant to muscularity and definition (e.g., “I think my arms should be larger (i.e., more muscular).”). The body fat subscale consisted of eight items (e.g., “Have you felt excessively large and rounded (i.e., fat)?”) relevant to body fat and leanness. Total scores were computed on a range of 1–6.
Analytic Plan
We used exploratory analyses to characterize the participants and variables of interest. We tested bivariate associations between each dependent variable and each independent variable using either Pearson correlations (continuous) or ANOVAs (categorical, with Bonferroni corrections for multiple comparisons). One multiple regression model was constructed for each of the body dissatisfaction scales (overall body dissatisfaction, muscularity dissatisfaction, and body fat dissatisfaction). Because race/ethnicity emerged as a significant predictor of body dissatisfaction in the overall sample, we also conducted a secondary set of stratified analyses to model within racial/ethnic groups with sufficient sample size (Hispanic/Latino, Black, multiracial/other, and White) using the same predictors from the main regressions. For the purposes of conceptually guided modeling, as well as to allow comparisons of findings across the three models, each multivariable model shared the same set of independent variables which were entered simultaneously. For categorical variables, we set reference groups (White for race/ethnicity, and “neutral” for community affiliation). Theory, along with exploratory incremental F tests and curve estimation, indicated quadratic relationships between BMI and overall body dissatisfaction, and between BMI and body fat dissatisfaction. Thus, a quadratic BMI polynomial term was included in the overall body dissatisfaction and body fat dissatisfaction models. To reduce multicollinearity associated with linear and quadratic BMI, and to facilitate interpretation, BMI was centered at the mean. Variables were checked for multicollinearity using the variance inflation factor (VIF). Regression diagnostics indicated that normality assumptions for ordinary least squares (OLS) regression had been violated (residuals were heteroskedastic), attributable to a few extreme outliers with high leverage and/or influence. In order to correct for heteroskedasticity of residuals, we used robust regression with iteratively re-weighted least squares (IRLS). All analyses were conducted using Stata, Version 12 (StataCorp, 2011).
Results
Sample
The sample was diverse regarding race/ethnicity, sexual orientation, and gay community affiliation (Table 1). Participants had a mean BMI of 23.05 (SD = 4.35). Mean scores for both overall body dissatisfaction and muscularity dissatisfaction were similar (M = 2.99 each, SDs =0.96 and 1.09, respectively). The mean body fat dissatisfaction score (M =3.06, SD =1.39) was slightly higher than the overall body and muscularity dissatisfaction mean scores, albeit with more variance. Sample characteristics are detailed in Table 1.
Table 1.
% | n | |
---|---|---|
Race/ethnicity | ||
Hispanic/Latino | 38.07 | 225 |
Black | 14.72 | 87 |
Asian/Pacific Islander | 4.91 | 29 |
Multiracial/other | 13.03 | 77 |
White | 29.27 | 173 |
Sexual orientationa | ||
Exclusively homosexual/gay | 41.46 | 245 |
Not exclusively homosexual/gay | 58.54 | 346 |
Community affiliation | ||
Neutral | 35.19 | 208 |
Agree | 42.47 | 251 |
Disagree | 22.34 | 132 |
| ||
M | SD | |
| ||
Body mass index (BMI) (range = 16.03–48.81) | 23.05 | 4.35 |
Internalized homonegativity (range = 4–20) | 8.51 | 4.22 |
MBAS score (range = 1.0–5.92) | 2.99 | 0.96 |
MBAS muscularity subscale (range = 1–6.0) | 2.99 | 1.09 |
MBAS body fat subscale (range = 1–6.0) | 3.06 | 1.39 |
Assessed using the Kinsey Scale (Kinsey, Pomeroy, & Martin, 1948). No participants identified as“0,”or “Exclusively heterosexual”
Bivariate Associations
There were differences by race/ethnicity in overall body dissatisfaction (F(4, 586) =2.56, p = .04), with White men (M = 3.12, SE =0.07; 95 % CI 2.99–3.26) reporting higher scores than Black men (M =2.75, SE =0.10; 95 % CI 2.55–2.94, p =.02). Community affiliation was also associated with overall body dissatisfaction scores (F(2, 588) =5.04, p = .01), as men who felt neutral about their affiliation (M = 3.09, SE = 0.06; 95 % CI 2.97–3.22) had higher scores than those who felt affiliated (M = 2.85, SE = 0.06; 95 % CI 2.73, 2.97; p = .02). Additionally, men who did not feel affiliated had higher scores (M = 3.11, SE = 0.08;95 %CI 2.96–3.27) than those who were affiliated (p = .03). Higher BMI was moderately correlated with higher total body scores (r = 0.27, p<.001), as was greater internalized homonegativity (r = 0.28, p<.001). Sexual orientation was not associated with overall body dissatisfaction scores.
There were also differences by race/ethnicity in muscularity dissatisfaction (F(4, 586) = 4.34, p =.001), as White men (M = 3.24, SE =0.08;95 %CI 3.08–3.40) had higher scores than both Hispanic/Latino men (M =2.90, SE = 0.07;95 %CI 2.76–3.04; p =.02) and Black men (M =2.76, SE =0.12; 95 % CI 2.54–2.99; p = .01). Sexual orientation was marginally associated with muscularity scores (F(1, 589) = 3.89, p = .049), and men who were not exclusively homosexual/gay had higher scores (M = 3.07, SE = 0.06; 95 % CI 2.95–3.18) than men who were exclusively homosexual/gay (M = 2.89, SE = 0.07; 95 % CI 2.75, 3.02; p = .049). Higher BMI weakly negatively correlated with muscularity dissatisfaction (r = −0.08, p = .04), but internalized homonegativity was moderately positively correlated (r = 0.25, p<.001). Community affiliation was not associated with muscularity dissatisfaction.
Finally, BMI was strongly correlated (r = 0.46, p<.001) with body fat dissatisfaction, and internalized homonegativity was weakly correlated (r = 0.16, p = .001) with this subscale.
Main Multivariable Models
For the model assessing overall body dissatisfaction, (F(10, 580) = 14.59, p<.001, R2 = 19.1 %), race/ethnicity overall was associated with dissatisfaction (p<.001), and Hispanic/Latino (B = −0.31; 95 %CI −0.49 to −0.12; p = .001), Black (B = −0.62; 95 % CI −0.86 to −0.38; p<.001), and multiracial/other men (B = −0.35; 95 % CI −0.60 to −0.11; p = .005) had lower dissatisfaction scores as compared to White men. BMI was associated with dissatisfaction (B = 0.10; 95 % CI 0.07 −0.12; p<.001) as was the BMI quadratic term (B = −0.004, 95 % CI −0.006 to −0.002) indicating a concave curvilinear effect. Greater internalized homonegativity was also associated with greater dissatisfaction (B = 0.06; 95 % CI, 0.05 −0.08; p<.001). The model is detailed in Table 2.
Table 2.
B | Adjusted MLR (R2 = 19.1 %)
|
||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
SE | t | p | 95 % CI | ||
Race/ethnicitya | <.001 | ||||
Hispanic/Latino | −0.31* | 0.09 | −3.25 | .001 | −0.49, −0.12 |
Black | −0.62* | 0.12 | −5.15 | <.001 | −0.86, −0.38 |
Asian/Pacific Islander | −0.13 | 0.18 | −0.70 | ns | −0.48, −0.23 |
Multiracial/other | −0.35* | 0.12 | −2.82 | .005 | −0.60, −0.11 |
Sexual orientation | |||||
Exclusively homosexual/gay | 0.05 | 0.08 | 0.57 | ns | −0.11, 0.20 |
Community affiliationb | ns | ||||
Agree | −0.10 | 0.08 | −1.15 | ns | −0.27, 0.07 |
Disagree | −0.11 | 0.10 | −1.11 | ns | −0.31, 0.09 |
Body mass index (BMIc)c | 0.10* | 0.12 | 8.06 | <.001 | 0.07, 0.12 |
BMIc2 | −0.004* | 0.001 | −3.55 | <.001 | −0.006, −0.002 |
Internalized homonegativity | 0.06* | 0.01 | 6.86 | <.001 | 0.05, 0.08 |
Constant | −1.40 | 0.76 | −1.84 | .07 | −2.90, 0.10 |
p<.05
Reference group is White race/ethnicity
Reference group is Neutral
Centered at mean (23.064)
For the model assessing muscularity dissatisfaction, (F(9, 581) = 7.54, p<.001, R2 = 9.7 %), race/ethnicity overall was associated with muscularity dissatisfaction (p = .002), and Hispanic/Latino (B = −0.33; 95 % CI = −0.55 to −0.11; p = .004), Black (B = −0.52; 95 % CI −0.81 to −0.23; p<.001), and multiracial/other (B = −0.45; 95 % CI −0.74 to −0.15; p = .003) men had less dissatisfaction as compared to White men. Conversely, greater internalized homonegativity (B = 0.07; 95 % CI 0.05 to 0.09; p<.001) was associated with increased dissatisfaction. The model is detailed in Table 3.
Table 3.
B | Adjusted MLR (R2 = 9.7 %)
|
||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
SE | t | p | 95 % CI | ||
Race/ethnicitya | .002 | ||||
Hispanic/Latino | −0.33* | 0.11 | −2.89 | .004 | −0.56, −0.11 |
Black | −0.52* | 0.15 | −3.54 | <.001 | −0.81, −0.23 |
Asian/Pacific Islander | −0.06 | 0.22 | −0.28 | ns | −0.49, 0.37 |
Multiracial/other | −0.45* | 0.15 | −2.96 | .003 | −0.74, −0.15 |
Sexual orientation | |||||
Exclusively homosexual/gay | −0.05 | 0.10 | −0.48 | ns | −0.23, 0.14 |
Community affiliationb | ns | ||||
Agree | −0.13 | 0.10 | −1.21 | ns | −0.33, 0.08 |
Disagree | −0.19 | 0.12 | −1.53 | ns | −0.43, 0.05 |
Body mass index (BMIc)c | −0.01 | 0.01 | −1.31 | ns | −0.03, 0.01 |
Internalized homonegativity | 0.07* | 0.01 | 6.11 | <.001 | 0.05, 0.09 |
Constant | 3.06 | 0.27 | 10.97 | <.001 | 2.51, 3.61 |
p<.05
Reference group is White race/ethnicity
Reference group is Neutral
Centered at mean (23.064)
For the model assessing body fat dissatisfaction, (F(10, 580) = 27.52, p<.001, R2 = 31.5 %), race/ethnicity overall was associated with dissatisfaction (p<.001), and Hispanic/Latino (B = −0.34; 95 % CI −0.60 to −0.09; p = .007) and Black (B = −0.82; 95 %CI −1.14 to −0.50; p<.001)men had less body fat dissatisfaction as compared to White men. BMI (B = 0.24; 95 % CI 0.20 −0.27; p<.001) was associated with dissatisfaction as was the BMI quadratic term (B = −0.01, 95 % CI −0.01 to −0.007, p<.001) indicating a concave curvilinear effect. Greater internalized homonegativity (B = 0.05; 95 % CI 0.03 −0.08; p<.001) was associated with greater dissatisfaction. The model is detailed in Table 4.
Table 4.
B | Adjusted MLR (R2 = 31.5 %)
|
||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
SE | t | p | 95 % CI | ||
Race/ethnicitya | <.001 | ||||
Hispanic/Latino | −0.34* | 0.13 | −2.68 | .007 | −0.56, −0.09 |
Black | −0.82* | 0.16 | −5.00 | <.001 | −1.14, −0.50 |
Asian/Pacific Islander | −0.10 | 0.24 | −0.41 | ns | −0.58, 0.38 |
Multiracial/other | −0.28 | 0.17 | −1.67 | ns | −0.61, 0.05 |
Sexual orientation | |||||
Exclusively homosexual/gay | 0.15 | 0.11 | 1.44 | ns | −0.56, 0.36 |
Community affiliationb | ns | ||||
Agree | −0.10 | 0.12 | −0.86 | ns | −0.33, −0.13 |
Disagree | −0.11 | 0.14 | −0.80 | ns | −0.38, 0.16 |
Body mass index (BMIc)c | 0.24* | 0.02 | 14.90 | <.001 | 0.21, 0.27 |
BMIc2 | −0.01* | 0.001 | −7.07 | <.001 | −0.01, −0.007 |
Internalized homonegativity | 0.05* | 0.01 | 4.23 | <.001 | 0.03, 0.08 |
Constant | −7.92 | 1.04 | −7.65 | <.001 | −9.95, −5.89 |
p<.05
Reference group is White race/ethnicity
Reference group is neutral
Centered at mean (23.064)
Within-Group Multivariable Models
Because race/ethnicity was associated with body dissatisfaction, we also conducted exploratory analyses stratified by race/ethnicity to determine if covariates of body dissatisfaction varied, qualitatively, across racial/ethnic groups. Among Hispanic/Latino YMSM, for the model assessing overall body dissatisfaction (F(6, 218) =7.10, p<.001, R2 =15.0 %), higher internalized homonegativity (B =0.06, 95 % CI 0.03 −0.10, p< .001) was associated with greater body dissatisfaction, as was BMI (B = 0.09, 95 % CI 0.05 −0.13, p<.001) and the BMI quadratic term (B = −0.005, 95 % CI −0.008 to −0.002, p = .003) indicating a concave curvilinear effect. For the model assessing muscularity dissatisfaction (F(5,219) =2.96, p =.01, R2 =5.67 %), only greater internalized homonegativity (B = 0.06, 95 % CI 0.02 to 0.10, p<.01) was associated with increased muscularity dissatisfaction. For the model assessing body fat dissatisfaction (F(6, 218) = 17.48, p<.001, R2 =31.4 %), internalized homonegativity (B =0.06, 95 % CI 0.01–0.11, p = .02) as well as BMI (B =0.24, 95 % CI 0.19–0.29, p<.001) and the BMI quadratic term (B = −0.01, 95 % CI −0.02 to −0.008), which indicated a concave curvilinear effect, were associated with dissatisfaction.
Among Black YMSM, for the model assessing overall body dissatisfaction (F(6, 80) = 4.32, p<.001, R2 = 23.3 %), internalized homonegativity (B = 0.05, 95 % CI 0.002–0.09, p = .04) was associated with greater body dissatisfaction. The model for muscularity dissatisfaction had marginal fit (F(5, 81) = 2.01, p = .09, R2 = 10.47 %), and only greater internalized homonegativity (B = 0.06, 95 % CI 0.001– to 0.11, p = .05) indicated a statistically marginally significant association with increased muscularity dissatisfaction. With regard to body fat dissatisfaction (F(6, 80) = 8.25, p<.001, R2 = 36.1 %), only the linear BMI term (B = 0.21, 95 % CI 0.12–0.31, p<.001) was associated with increased dissatisfaction.
Among YMSM of multiracial/other ethnic identity, for the model assessing overall body dissatisfaction (F(6, 70) =4.43, p<.001, R2 =28.2 %), internalized homonegativity (B =0.07, 95 % CI 0.02–0.11, p =.01), the linear BMI term (B =0.09, 95 % CI 0.02–0.15, p =.01), and “exclusively homosexual” sexual orientation (B =0.48, 95 % CI 0.01–0.95, p =.04) were associated with body dissatisfaction. For the model assessing muscularity dissatisfaction (F(5, 71) = 2.37, p = .04, R2 = 14.5 %), only greater internalized homonegativity (B =0.09, 95 % CI 0.03–0.15, p = .006) was associated with increased muscularity dissatisfaction. For the model assessing body fat dissatisfaction (F(6, 70) = 5.77, p<.001, R2 =33.5 %), the linear BMI term (B = 0.16, 95 % CI 0.07–0.25, p =.001) and “exclusively homosexual” orientation (B =0.75, 95 % CI 0.13–1.4, p =.02) were associated with increased body fat dissatisfaction.
Among White YMSM, for the model assessing overall body dissatisfaction (F(6, 166) =7.72, p<.001, R2 =21.2 %), the linear BMI term (B =0.13, 95 % CI 0.09–0.18, p<.001) and internalized homonegativity (B = 0.06, 95 % CI 0.03–0.09, p =.001) were associated with body dissatisfaction. The model assessing muscularity dissatisfaction did not fit adequately (F(5, 167) =1.70, p=0.13, R2 =4.78 %). For the model assessing body fat dissatisfaction, (F(6, 166) = 14.81, p<.001, R2 = 34.8 %), internalized homonegativity (B =0.05,95 %CI0.003 to 0.10, p =.04), the linear BMI term (B =0.28, 95 % CI 0.22 to 0.35, p<.001), and the BMI quadratic term (B =−0.01, 95 % CI −0.02 to −0.001), indicating a concave curvilinear effect, were associated with dissatisfaction.
Discussion
In the overall sample of YMSM ages 18–19, men reported more body fat dissatisfaction as compared to muscularity dissatisfaction and general body dissatisfaction. The typical participant body weight was within the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention’s (CDC’s) “healthy” BMI range; however, there was great variation, ranging from moderately under-weight to obese BMIs (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2015). Notably, there was diversity in race/ethnicity, sexual orientation, and level of gay community affiliation. This diversity enables analyses within a sample of YMSM, as compared to the majority of existing research that has either compared monolithic categories (i.e., sexual minorities versus heterosexuals) or research that has tested theory and pathways of body dissatisfaction. Instead, we sought to identify YMSM at greatest risk for body dissatisfaction.
The mean dissatisfaction scores were in line with existing research that has used the MBAS with general samples or primarily heterosexual samples (Griffiths, Angus, Murray, & Touyz, 2014; Griffiths, Murray, & Touyz, 2015; Kelly, Cotter, Tanofsky-Kraff, & Mazzeo, 2014; Tylka et al., 2005), as well as samples of adult gay men (Blashill, 2010; Blashill & Vander Wal, 2009b; Tylka & Andorka, 2012). The overall dissatisfaction score in the present study was slightly lower than prior findings (M =2.99 here, versus 3.05–3.46 elsewhere) as was the muscularity dissatisfaction score (M =2.99 versus 3.14–3.5 elsewhere).
Though many factors were associated with each facet of body dissatisfaction at the bivariate level, most variables were not associated with the outcome after adjustment in the final model. Additionally, the influence of covariates was generally small, and the proportion of variance explained the variables in the muscularity models was generally low for the overall sample model (~10 %) as well as the within-group models. Across all three models, race/ethnicity was associated with dissatisfaction, with Hispanic/Latino, Black, and multiracial/other YMSM evidencing less negative attitudes than White YMSM (though the multiracial/other difference did not hold in the body fat model).
Significant covariates were somewhat consistent across the racial/ethnic sub-models when analyses were constrained within the given racial/ethnic group. For overall body dissatisfaction, BMI was a significant predictor of greater dissatisfaction across all racial/ethnic groups, except for Black YMSM. Internalized homonegativity was a significant predictor across all groups. For muscularity dissatisfaction, internalized homonegativity was a significant predictor for Hispanic/Latino, Black, and multiracial/other YMSM. Regarding body fat dissatisfaction, linear BMI was a salient predictor across all groups. For White and Hispanic/Latino YMSM, the quadratic BMI term was also significant, as was internalized homonegativity. Finally, “exclusively homosexual” orientation was a significant predictor of both body fat dissatisfaction and overall body dissatisfaction for men of multiracial/other identity, only.
White YMSM evidenced higher levels of body dissatisfaction. In prior research, race/ethnicity was not associated with overall body dissatisfaction (Siconolfi et al., 2009) or drive for muscularity (Brennan et al., 2012) in adult populations of gay and bisexual men. The present findings may be attributable to predominance of White men’s sexualized bodies in gay media (Brennan et al., 2013), which might lead White YMSM to adhere to more stringent body ideals. In considering differences in muscularity ideal among very young boys, Harrison & Bond (2007) posit that Bandura’s social cognitive theory (Bandura, 2001) may explain the differences they found in drive for muscularity among White and Black male youth. That is, because characters in these magazines are disproportionately White, the bodies and characters depicted may be more salient for boys of White race/ethnicity (Harrison & Bond, 2007). Returning to the present study, it is also possible that this finding reflects differences in racialized bodies. For example, gay and bisexual men of color experience racialized and exoticized aspects of body ideals, and also may enact resistance against these ideals (Brennan et al., 2013). It is also possible that if men of color have internalized relevant racialized body ideals, they are not reflected in the body parts or attitudes captured by the MBAS. The MBAS is a relatively new, multifaceted measure heretofore not utilized in a sample of YMSM that is diverse with regard to race/ethnicity as well as sexual orientation.
Higher BMI was associated with both overall dissatisfaction as well as body fat dissatisfaction, with quadratic terms indicating a concave curvilinear influence on the shape of the association between BMI and dissatisfaction. Evidence of a curvilinear shape has been found in prior research with young men (Austin et al., 2009; Calzo et al., 2012; Kostanski et al., 2004; Presnell et al., 2004). However, the curvilinear shape (concave) found here was the inverse of prior findings (i.e., convex). In the present study, the U-shape opened downward with peak dissatisfaction in the middle; i.e., men with very low and very high BMI tended to have lower dissatisfaction, while “average” men with middle BMIs had higher dissatisfaction. In prior research, the U-shape has opened upward, with higher dissatisfaction in the low and high ends of the BMI spectrum. Two factors may explain this inverse finding. First, the MBAS assesses dissatisfaction with specific body parts, rather than only global measures of dissatisfaction used in existing research. Thus, the specificity of the MBAS may be more apt to disentangle the nuances that underlie the curvilinear shape of global body dissatisfaction (i.e., thin men who desire bulk and muscularity, and obese men who desire leanness or muscular definition). Second, it is plausible that this relationship simply reflects the body attitudes of a young, urban population who have many more progressive “body positive” attitudes. We do caution, however, that the influence of the quadratic terms in the present studies was relatively weak, and implies are latively flat and wide parabolic shape. The lack of an association between BMI and muscularity dissatisfaction is likely attributable to its inability to differentiate weight associated with muscle versus body fat (Daniel & Bridges, 2010). Thus, a muscular man with low muscularity dissatisfaction and an obese man with high muscularity dissatisfaction may share the same BMI. The use of three body dissatisfaction facets (body fat, muscularity, and overall body dissatisfaction) allowed us to examine the role of BMI in dissatisfaction with more nuance than some prior research.
Internalized homonegativity emerged as a consistent factor across the three main models. However, its effect was rather small in the multivariate models, despite a moderate association found in bivariate analyses. Prior research indicated links between internalized homonegativity with body dissatisfaction (Kimmel & Mahalik, 2005) as well as drive for muscularity (Brennan et al., 2012). Internalized homonegativity indicates a potential challenge to intervention efforts. YMSM may perceive body image as a “gay” or feminine problem (Hargreaves & Tiggemann, 2006) and thus those who experience body dissatisfaction and its associated distress maybe less likely to seek or accept support for body image distress. Importantly, internalized homonegativity is a component of Meyer’s minority stress model (Meyer, 2003). Body dissatisfaction and associated distress may contribute to sexual minority stressors. Additionally, body dissatisfaction may be positioned as an outcome (rather than a component) of the model; the minority stress model in corporates both internal (e.g., internalized homonegativity) and external (e.g., discrimination) stress processes, and thus may be a useful framework for understanding how one experiences their body within the social environment.
Notably, sexual orientation was associated only with muscularity dissatisfaction at the bivariate level, and was not significant in the main multivariable models. Two hypotheses may be proposed. First, there may not be significant differences in body dissatisfaction by sexual orientation in this population when other factors (e.g., internalized homonegativity) are controlled for. Second, it is also plausible that many of these young men are still forging sexual identities, and thus, internalized homonegativity may be more salient at this point in their lives than when they are older (Reilly & Rudd, 2006). Along these lines, gay community affiliation was not a significant predictor once entered in the multivariable models. The influence of gay community affiliation on body image may become more salient with age as some men increasingly engage with gay cultures and communities (Calzo et al., 2013; Morrison et al., 2004). Prior research has identified associations between community affiliation and body dissatisfaction (Beren et al., 1996) and drive for muscularity (Hunt et al., 2012; Levesque & Vichesky, 2006). The present contrast in findings may also be attributable to differences in measurement, as the present study assessed gay community affiliation using a single, subjective measure of local community attachment. Other studies have used multi-item measures of specific activity in gay communities, such as frequenting gay venues (Hunt et al., 2012; Tiggemann et al., 2007). As such, the present measure may be capturing a different concept of community affiliation. We also note the potential for geographical and cultural differences and norms across gay communities.
Again, we note that a significant proportion of the variance in body dissatisfaction was not explained by these models, indicating that other factors may be more salient in explaining body dissatisfaction in YMSM. Future research may be better able to explain YMSM dissatisfaction with the inclusion of these variables. At the individual level, relevant factors include psychological factors including depression (Brennan et al., 2012), self-esteem (Olivardia et al., 2004) social sensitivity (Blashill & Vander Wal, 2009b), perfectionism (Dakanalis et al., 2014), and emotional functioning (Dakanalis et al., 2014; Griffiths et al., 2014). Atthenexus of individual and social influences, potential factors include media influence and salience (Carper et al., 2010; Drummond, 2005), objectification and self-objectification (Daniel & Bridges, 2010; Martins et al., 2007; Michaels et al., 2013; Parent & Moradi, 2011; Wiseman & Moradi, 2010), masculinity or gender attitudes (Blashill, 2011; Griffiths et al., 2015), and peer and partner influences (Adams, Turner, & Bucks, 2005; McArdle & Hill, 2011; Tylka, 2011; Tylka & Andorka, 2012; Wood, 2004).
These findings have health implications, but first we must acknowledge the spectrum of body attitudes and associated behaviors and psychological health. That is, young men’s concerns about their body may range from “normative intensities,” (i.e., those associated with normative and potentially healthy fitness behaviors) to truly deleterious levels of body dissatisfaction or dysmorphia (psychological distress and/or potentially harmful behaviors) (Parent, 2013). For YMSM with higher levels of dissatisfaction, psychological stress related to body dissatisfaction may present an additional burden in addition to the disproportionate stress they may experience as sexual minorities (Meyer, 2003). Indeed, prior analyses of this sample have indicated existing mental health needs (Storholm et al., 2013). Second, YMSM with body dissatisfaction may engage in unhealthy weight control behaviors, use of health supplements with potentially deleterious health effects (Field et al., 2005; Hadland et al., 2014; Holsen et al., 2001; Neumark-Sztainer et al., 2006; Olivardia et al., 2004; Parent,2013; Smith et al., 2011; Tylka, 2011), and use of steroids (Blashill & Safren, 2014; Parent, 2013). Finally, it is possible that body image is a component of health syndemics (Halkitis, Wolitski, & Millett, 2013; Stall et al., 2008) affecting YMSM. Thus, in assessment and intervention, it is important to consider body image as a component of holistic of YMSM health.
Limitations
Several limitations must be considered. First, participants self-reported all data, and thus, it is subject to recall bias as well as social desirability bias. However, the use of ACASI may help to reduce these biases (Gribble, Miller, Rogers, & Turner, 1999; Kurth et al., 2004; Tourangeau & Smith, 1996). We note the difficulty in interpreting findings regarding the “multiracial/other” category of YMSM, as this was a collapsed classification. Regarding self-report, BMI may be conservatively biased, as individuals tend to over-report height and under-report weight, yielding a lower BMI (Brener, McManus, Galuska, Lowry, & Wechsler, 2003; Gorber, Tremblay, Moher, & Gorber, 2007). As an overall measure, BMI is also unable to distinguish between weight attributable to fat or muscle, and thus, higher BMIs do not necessarily indicate an overweight (or, conversely, muscular) participant. With regard to the MBAS, not all aspects of men’s body image (e.g., penis size; Grov, Parsons, & Bimbi, 2010) are assessed. Other measures have limitations as well. First, the gay community affiliation measure is a single item that only captures local, NYC community affiliation; men may have affiliations with other geographic or social gay communities. There is room for subjective interpretation of “gay community” in responding to the questionnaire item, as “gay community” was not defined and is not monolithic (Frost & Meyer, 2012). Second, the measure of internalized homonegativity is not a validated measure, though it did demonstrate high reliability in this sample (α= 0.87). Finally, we did not assess more proximal and specific sources of influence on body satisfaction, such as peers or sexual partners (Tylka & Andorka, 2012). All data are cross-sectional, though this analysis is not intended to establish causality. External validity may be limited by the non-probability sampling of participants, and findings may not be generalizable to YMSM outside of NYC.
Conclusions
White race/ethnicity and internalized homonegativity are associated with multifaceted body dissatisfaction in this sample of YMSM. Within racial/ethnic subgroups, internalized homonegativity remains a common predictor of body dissatisfaction. BMI was also associated with overall body dissatisfaction, as well as body fat scores. The former two factors point to novel areas for future qualitative work in order to explore and delineate mechanisms. For example, how do YMSM of diverse racial/ethnic backgrounds experience and view their bodies (Brennan et al., 2013)? If YMSM of minority race/ethnicity are at lower risk for body dissatisfaction, what are the relevant protective factors at play? Additionally, what is the role and directionality of internalized homonegativity as it relates to body dissatisfaction, specifically in younger men? Future research might also explore traits and strategies of resistance and resilience that are protective against body dissatisfaction (Brennan et al., 2013; Williamson, 1999). Finally, it is critical to frame body dissatisfaction as a socially influenced health issue faced by YMSM, rather than a trait inherent to this population. For men who do experience body dissatisfaction and distress, we must look beyond the individual-level factors to account for the surrounding social and cultural context that influences body image (Williamson, 1999; Wood, 2004).
Acknowledgments
The authors wish to thank Cui Yang for reviewing an early draft of this article. We also thank the study participants of Project 18. The project was funded by the National Institute on Drug Abuse (NIDA) (R01DA025537). The content does not necessarily reflect the opinions or views of NIDA or the National Institutes of Health (NIH).
References
- Adams G, Turner H, Bucks R. The experience of body dissatisfaction in men. Body Image. 2005;2(3):271–283. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2005.05.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Austin SB, Haines J, Veugelers PJ. Body satisfaction and body weight: Gender differences and sociodemographic determinants. BMC Public Health. 2009;9(1):313. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-9-313. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bandura A. Social cognitive theory of mass communications. In: Bryant J, Zillman D, editors. Media effects: Advances in theory and research. 2. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum; 2001. pp. 121–153. [Google Scholar]
- Beren SE, Hayden HA, Wilfley DE, Grilo CM. The influence of sexual orientation on body dissatisfaction in adult men and women. International Journal of Eating Disorders. 1996;20(2):135–141. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1098-108x(199609)20:2<135:aid-eat3>3.3.co;2-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blashill AJ. Elements of male body image: Prediction of depression, eating pathology and social sensitivity among gay men. Body Image. 2010;7(4):310–316. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2010.07.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blashill AJ. Gender roles, eating pathology, and body dissatisfaction in men: A meta-analysis. Body Image. 2011;8(1):1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2010.09.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blashill AJ, Safren SA. Sexual orientation and anabolic-androgenic steroids in us adolescent boys. Pediatrics. 2014;133(3):469–475. doi: 10.1542/peds.2013-2768d. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blashill AJ, Safren SA. Body dissatisfaction and condom use self-efficacy: A meta-analysis. Body Image. 2015;12:73–77. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2014.10.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blashill AJ, Vander Wal JS. The Male Body Attitudes Scale: A confirmatory factor analysis with a sample of gay men. Body Image. 2009a;6(4):322–325. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2009.07.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blashill AJ, Vander Wal JS. Mediation of gender role conflict and eating pathology in gay men. Psychology of Men & Masculinity. 2009b;10(3):204–217. [Google Scholar]
- Brener ND, McManus T, Galuska DA, Lowry R, Wechsler H. Reliability and validity of self-reported height and weight among high school students. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2003;32(4):281–287. doi: 10.1016/s1054-139x(02)00708-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brennan DJ, Asakura K, George C, Newman PA, Giwa S, Hart TA, Betancourt G. Never reflected anywhere: Body image among ethnoracialized gay and bisexual men. Body Image. 2013;10(3):389–398. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2013.03.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brennan DJ, Craig SL, Thompson DEA. Factors associated with a drive for muscularity among gay and bisexual men. Culture, Health & Sexuality. 2012;14(1):1–15. doi: 10.1080/13691058.2011.619578. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Calzo JP, Corliss HL, Blood EA, Field AE, Austin SB. Development of muscularity and weight concerns in heterosexual and sexual minority males. Health Psychology. 2013;32(1):42–51. doi: 10.1037/a0028964. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Calzo JP, Sonneville KR, Haines J, Blood EA, Field AE, Austin SB. The development of associations among body mass index, body dissatisfaction, and weight and shape concern in adolescent boys and girls. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2012;51(5):517–523. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2012.02.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carper TLM, Negy C, Tantleff-Dunn S. Relations among media influence, body image, eating concerns, and sexual orientation in men: A preliminary investigation. Body Image. 2010;7(4):301–309. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2010.07.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. About BMI for children and teens. 2015 Retrieved from http://www.cdc.gov/healthyweight/assessing/bmi/childrens_bmi/about_childrens_bmi.html.
- Chao YM, Pisetsky EM, Dierker LC, Dohm FA, Rosselli F, May AM, Striegel-Moore RH. Ethnic differences in weight control practices among U.S. adolescents from 1995 to 2005. International Journal of Eating Disorders. 2008;41(2):124–133. doi: 10.1002/eat.20479. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dakanalis A, Favagrossa L, Clerici M, Prunas A, Colmegna F, Zanetti MA, Riva G. Body dissatisfaction and eating disorder symptomatology: A latent structural equation modeling analysis of moderating variables in 18-to 28-year-old males. The Journal of Psychology. 2014 doi: 10.1080/00223980.2013.842141. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Daniel S, Bridges SK. The drive for muscularity in men: Media influences and objectification theory. Body Image. 2010;7(1):32–38. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2009.08.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Drummond MJN. Men’s bodies: Listening to the voices of young gay men. Men and Masculinities. 2005;7(3):270–290. doi: 10.1177/1097184x04271357. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Feldman MB, Meyer IH. Eating disorders in diverse lesbian, gay, and bisexual populations. International Journal of Eating Disorders. 2007;40(3):218–226. doi: 10.1002/eat.20360. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Field AE, Austin SB, Camargo CA, Taylor CB, Striegel-Moore RH, Loud KJ, Colditz GA. Exposure to the mass media, body shape concerns, and use of supplements to improve weight and shape among male and female adolescents. Pediatrics. 2005;116(2):e214–220. doi: 10.1542/peds.2004-2022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Frost DM, Meyer IH. Measuring community connectedness among diverse sexual minority populations. Journal of Sex Research. 2012;49(1):36–49. doi: 10.1080/00224499.2011.565427. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gorber SC, Tremblay M, Moher D, Gorber B. A comparison of direct vs. Self-report measures for assessing height, weight and body mass index: A systematic review. Obesity Reviews. 2007;8(4):307–326. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-789x.2007.00347.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gribble JN, Miller HG, Rogers SM, Turner CF. Interview mode and measurement of sexual behaviors: Methodological issues. Journal of Sex Research. 1999;36(1):16–24. doi: 10.1080/00224499909551963. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Griffiths S, Angus D, Murray SB, Touyz S. Unique associations between young adult men’s emotional functioning and their body dissatisfaction and disordered eating. Body Image. 2014;11(2):175–178. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2013.12.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Griffiths S, Murray SB, Touyz S. Extending the masculinity hypothesis: An investigation of gender role conformity, body dissatisfaction, and disordered eating in young heterosexual men. Psychology of Men & Masculinity. 2015;16(1):108–114. [Google Scholar]
- Grov C, Parsons JT, Bimbi DS. The association between penis size and sexual health among men who have sex with men. Archives of Sexual Behavior. 2010;39(3):788–797. doi: 10.1007/s10508-008-9439-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hadland SE, Austin SB, Goodenow CS, Calzo JP. Weight misperception and unhealthy weight control behaviors among sexual minorities in the general adolescent population. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2014;54(3):296–303. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2013.08.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Halkitis PN, Green KA, Wilton L. Masculinity, body image, and sexual behavior in HIV-seropositive gay men: A two-phase formative behavioral investigation using the internet. International Journal of Men’s Health. 2004;3(1):27–42. doi: 10.3149/jmh.0301.27. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Halkitis PN, Moeller RW, Siconolfi DE, Storholm ED, Solomon TM, Bub KL. Measurement model exploring a syndemic in emerging adult gay and bisexual men. AIDS and Behavior. 2013;17(2):662–673. doi: 10.1007/s10461-012-0273-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Halkitis PN, Wolitski RJ, Millett GA. A holistic approach to addressing HIV infection disparities in gay, bisexual, and other men who have sex with men. American Psychologist. 2013;68(4):261–273. doi: 10.1037/a0032746. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hargreaves DA, Tiggemann M. ‘Body image is for girls’: A qualitative study of boys’ body image. Journal of Health Psychology. 2006;11(4):567–576. doi: 10.1177/1359105306065017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harrison K, Bond BJ. Gaming magazines and the drive for muscularity in preadolescent boys: A longitudinal examination. Body Image. 2007;4(3):269–277. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2007.03.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holsen I, Kraft P, Roysamb E. The relationship between body image and depressed mood in adolescence: A 5-year longitudinal panel study. Journal of Health Psychology. 2001;6(6):613–627. doi: 10.1177/135910530100600601. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hunt CJ, Gonsalkorale K, Nosek BA. Links between psychosocial variables and body dissatisfaction in homosexual men: Differential relations with the drive for muscularity and the drive for thinness. International Journal of Men’s Health. 2012;11(2):127–136. doi: 10.3149/jmh.1102.127. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Kaminski PL, Chapman BP, Haynes SD, Own L. Body image, eating behaviors, and attitudes toward exercise among gay and straight men. Eating Behaviors. 2005;6(3):179–187. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2004.11.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kelly NR, Cotter EE, Tanofsky-Kraff M, Mazzeo SE. Racial variations in binge eating, body image concerns, and compulsive exercise among men. Psychology of Men & Masculinity. 2014 doi: 10.1037/a0037585. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Keys A, Fidanza F, Karvonen MJ, Kimura N. Indices of relative weight and obesity. Journal of Chronic Diseases. 1972;25(6):329–343. doi: 10.1159/000281293. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kimmel SB, Mahalik JR. Body image concerns of gay men: The roles of minority stress and conformity to masculine norms. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2005;73(6):1185–1190. doi: 10.1037/0022-006x.73.6.1185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kinsey AC, Pomeroy WB, Martin CE. Sexual behavior in the human male. Philadelphia: W. B. Saunders Co; 1948. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kostanski M, Fisher A, Gullone E. Current conceptualisation of body image dissatisfaction: Have we got it wrong? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2004;45(7):1317–1325. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00315.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kurth AE, Martin DP, Golden MR, Weiss NS, Heagerty PJ, Spielberg F, Holmes KK. A comparison between audio computer-assisted self-interviews and clinician interviews for obtaining the sexual history. Sexually Transmitted Diseases. 2004;31(12):719–726. doi: 10.1097/01.olq.0000145855.36181.13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levesque MJ, Vichesky DR. Raising the bar on the body beautiful: An analysis of the body image concerns of homosexual men. Body Image. 2006;3(1):45–55. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2005.10.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martins Y, Tiggemann M, Kirkbride A. Those speedos become them: The role of self-objectification in gay and heterosexual men’s body image. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 2007;33(5):634–647. doi: 10.1177/0146167206297403. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McArdle KA, Hill MS. Understanding body dissatisfaction in gay and heterosexual men: The roles of self-esteem, media, and peer influence. Men and Masculinities. 2011;11(5):511–532. doi: 10.1177/0146167206297403. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- McCabe MP, Ricciardelli LA. Body image dissatisfaction among males across the lifespan. Journal of Psychosomatic Research. 2004;56(6):675–685. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3999(03)00129-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meyer IH. Prejudice, social stress, and mental health in lesbian, gay, and bisexual populations: Conceptual issues and research evidence. Psychological Bulletin. 2003;129(5):674–697. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.129.5.674. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Michaels MS, Parent MC, Moradi B. Does exposure to muscularity-idealizing images have self-objectification consequences for heterosexual and sexual minority men? Psychology of Men & Masculinity. 2013;14(2):175–183. [Google Scholar]
- Morrison MA, Morrison TG, Sager CL. Does body satisfaction differ between gay men and lesbian women and heterosexual men and women? Body Image. 2004;1(2):127–138. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2004.01.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Muth JL, Cash TF. Body-image attitudes: What difference does gender make? Journal of Applied Social Psychology. 1997 doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.1997.tb01607.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Neumark-Sztainer D, Paxton SJ, Hannan PJ, Haines J, Story M. Does body satisfaction matter? Five-year longitudinal associations between body satisfaction and health behaviors in adolescent females and males. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2006;39(2):244–251. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2005.12.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Newcomb ME, Mustanski B. Internalized homophobia and internalizing mental health problems: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review. 2010;30(8):1019–1029. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2010.07.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O’Donnell L, Agronick G, San Doval A, Duran R, Myint-U A, Stueve A. Ethnic and gay community attachments and sexual risk behaviors among urban Latino young men who have sex with men. AIDS Education and Prevention. 2002;14(6):457–471. doi: 10.1521/aeap.14.8.457.24109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Olivardia R, Pope HGJ, Borowiecki JJI, Cohane GH. Biceps and body image: The relationship between muscularity and self-esteem, depression, and eating disorder symptoms. Psychology of Men & Masculinity. 2004;5(2):112–120. doi: 10.1037/1524-9220.5.2.112. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Parent MC. Clinical considerations in etiology, assessment, and treatment of men’s muscularity-focused body image disturbance. Psychology of Men & Masculinity. 2013;14(1):88–100. [Google Scholar]
- Parent MC, Moradi B. His biceps become him: A test of objectification theory’s application to drive for muscularity and propensity for steroid use in college men. Journal of Counseling Psychology. 2011;58(2):246–256. doi: 10.1037/a0021398. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Parent MC, Moradi B. Self-objectification and condom use self-efficacy in women university students. Archives of Sexual Behavior. 2014:1–11. doi: 10.1007/s10508-014-0384-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pope HGJ, Phillips KA, Olivardia R. The Adonis complex: The secret crisis of male body obsession. New York: Free Press; 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Presnell K, Bearman SK, Stice E. Risk factors for body dissatisfaction in adolescent boys and girls: A prospective study. International Journal of Eating Disorders. 2004;36(4):389–401. doi: 10.1002/eat.20045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Radkowsky M, Siegel LJ. The gay adolescent: Stressors, adaptations, and psychosocial interventions. Clinical Psychology Review. 1997;17(2):191–216. doi: 10.1016/s0272-7358(97)00007-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reilly A, Rudd NA. Is internalized homonegativity related to body image? Family and Consumer Sciences Research Journal. 2006 doi: 10.1177/1077727x06289430. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Ricciardelli LA, McCabe MP, Williams RJ, Thompson JK. The role of ethnicity and culture in body image and disordered eating among males. Clinical Psychology Review. 2007;27(5):582–606. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2007.01.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Siconolfi D, Halkitis PN, Allomong TW, Burton CL. Body dissatisfaction and eating disorders in a sample of gay and bisexual men. International Journal of Men’s Health. 2009;8(3):254–264. doi: 10.3149/jmh.0803.254. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Smith AR, Hawkeswood SE, Bodell LP, Joiner TE. Muscularity versus leanness: An examination of body ideals and predictors of disordered eating in heterosexual and gay college students. Body Image. 2011;8(3):232–236. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2011.03.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stall R, Friedman M, Catania JA. Interacting epidemics and gay men’s health: A theory of syndemic production among urban gay men. In: Wolitski RJ, Stall R, Valdiserri RO, editors. Unequal opportunity: Health disparities affecting gay and bisexual men in the United States. New York: Oxford University Press; 2008. pp. 251–274. [Google Scholar]
- StataCorp. Stata statistical software: Release 12. College Station, TX: StataCorp LP; 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Storholm ED, Siconolfi DE, Halkitis PN, Moeller RW, Eddy JA, Bare MG. Sociodemographic factors contribute to mental health disparities and access to services among young men who have sex with men in New York City. Journal of Gay & Lesbian Mental Health. 2013;17(3):294–313. doi: 10.1080/19359705.2012.763080. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thiede H, Valleroy LA, MacKellar DA, Celentano DD, Ford WL, Hagan H, Torian LV. Regional patterns and correlates of substance use among young men who have sex with men in 7 US urban areas. American Journal of Public Health. 2003;93(11):1915–1921. doi: 10.2105/ajph.93.11.1915. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tiggemann M, Martins Y, Kirkbride A. Oh to be lean and muscular: Body image ideals in gay and heterosexual men. Psychology of Men & Masculinity. 2007;8(1):15–24. doi: 10.1037/1524-9220.8.1.15. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Tourangeau R, Smith TW. Asking sensitive questions: The impact of data collection mode, question format, and question context. Public Opinion Quarterly. 1996;60(2):275–304. [Google Scholar]
- Tylka TL. Refinement of the tripartite influence model for men: Dual body image pathways to body change behaviors. Body Image. 2011;8(3):199–207. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2011.04.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tylka TL, Andorka MJ. Support for an expanded tripartite influence model with gay men. Body Image. 2012;9(1):57–67. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2011.09.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tylka TL, Bergeron D, Schwartz JP. Development and psychometric evaluation of the male body attitudes scale (MBAS) Body Image. 2005;2(2):161–175. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2005.03.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Williamson I. Why are gay men a high risk group for eating disturbance? European Eating Disorders Review. 1999;7:1–4. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1099-0968(199903)7:1<1:aid-erv275>3.3.co;2-l. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Wilton L. A preliminary study of body image and HIV sexual risk behavior in black gay and bisexual men: Implications for HIV prevention. Journal of Gay & Lesbian Social Services. 2009;21(4):309–325. [Google Scholar]
- Wiseman MC, Moradi B. Body image and eating disorder symptoms in sexual minority men: A test and extension of objectification theory. Journal of Counseling Psychology. 2010;57(2):154–166. doi: 10.1037/a0018937. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wood MJ. The gay male gaze. Journal of Gay & Lesbian Social Services. 2004;17(2):43–62. doi: 10.1300/j041v17n02_03. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Yelland C, Tiggemann M. Muscularity and the gay ideal: Body dissatisfaction and disordered eating in homosexual men. Eating Behaviors. 2003;4(2):107–116. doi: 10.1016/s1471-0153(03)00014-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]