Skip to main content
In Silico Pharmacology logoLink to In Silico Pharmacology
. 2016 Mar 16;4:4. doi: 10.1186/s40203-016-0017-6

A momentary biomarker for depressive mood

Jinhyuk Kim 1,2, Toru Nakamura 1, Yoshiharu Yamamoto 1,
PMCID: PMC4792818  PMID: 26979449

Abstract

Many biomarkers from genetic, neuroimaging, and biological/biochemical measures have been recently developed in order to make a shift toward the objective evaluation of psychiatric disorders. However, they have so far been less successful in capturing dynamical changes or transitions in pathological states, such as those occurring during the course of clinical treatments or pathogenic processes of disorders. A momentary biomarker is now required for objective monitoring of such dynamical changes. The development of ecological momentary assessment (EMA) allows the assessment of dynamical aspects of diurnal/daily clinical conditions and subjective symptoms. Furthermore, a variety of validation studies on momentary symptoms assessed by EMA using behavioral/physiological/biochemical measures have demonstrated the possibility of evaluating momentary symptoms from such external objective measures. In this review, we introduce physical activity as a candidate biobehavioral biomarker for psychiatric disorders. We also mention its potential as a momentary biomarker for depressive mood. Finally, we address the continuous monitoring of the pathogenic processes and pathological states of depressive disorders based on physical activity, as well as its application in pharmacological animal studies.

Keywords: Physical activity, Depressive mood, Major depressive disorders, Ecological momentary assessment, Psychobehavioral biomarker

Review

Seeking a momentary biomarker for psychiatric disorders

Diagnosis of psychiatric disorders largely relies on the description of patients’ subjective symptoms (i.e., physical symptoms and psychological states), causing difficulty in objective evaluation of pathological states of patients. Therefore, there is a recent tendency to incorporate “biomarkers” into psychiatry to improve the current diagnostic system (Singh and Rose 2009). A variety of physiological or biological biomarkers, such as genetic (Lacerda-Pinheiro et al. 2014; Geaghan and Cairns 2015; Banigan et al. 2013), neuroimaging (Wolfers et al. 2015; Arnone et al. 2009; Vita et al. 2009; Wood et al. 2011; Schnack et al. 2014; Diler et al. 2013; Arribas et al. 2010), and biochemical measures (Pagan et al. 2014; Yang et al. 2013), have been identified through association studies with clinical properties of disorders. Some of them have been able to distinguish patients from healthy subjects (Banigan et al. 2013; Arribas et al. 2010; Pagan et al. 2014; Yang et al. 2013) or categorize patients according to their specific psychiatric disorders (Schnack et al. 2014; Nakamura et al. 2007; Diler et al. 2013; Arribas et al. 2010; Banigan et al. 2013) with a certain level of the accuracy. However, they have so far been less successful in capturing dynamical changes in subjective symptoms or transitions of clinical conditions, e.g., those following a clinical treatment (Boksa 2013), at sufficient time-resolutions.

The dynamical or “momentary” aspects of clinical conditions and symptoms are considered important for the diagnosis of the psychiatric disorders and also provide more complete information about the disorders in question. This is widely accepted, especially so in the field of psychosomatic/behavioral medicine (Stone and Shiffman 1994; Conner and Barrett 2012; Trull and Ebner-Priemer 2009; Trull and Ebner-Priemer 2013; Shiffman et al. 2008; Moskowitz and Young 2006), and thus methodological developments to assess momentary information on symptoms, and in the analytical methods for such data (e.g., multilevel modeling), have been implemented over the last two decades.

Among these, ecological momentary assessment (EMA) is a data collection technique capable of repeated real-time assessments of behaviors, psychological states, and physiological reactions in individuals’ daily life (Stone and Shiffman 1994; Kim et al. 2013a). Because of its momentary nature, EMA can enhance the ecological validity of measurements and also avoid the retrospective recall effects which plague in traditional retrospective self-report methods. With the increasing evidence on the usefulness of this technique, EMA is now generally regarded as the ‘gold standard’ to assess the dynamical aspects of subjective symptoms.

Many studies have examined the validity of EMA by investigating covariate properties between momentary self-reported symptoms and other external measures (i.e., biomarkers) using various populations, one of these being patients with psychiatric disorders. For example, cardiovascular reactivity (Kamarck et al. 2005; Kamarck et al. 1998; Smith et al. 2012; Grossman et al. 2008) and cortisol-related reactivity (Robles et al. 2011; van Eck et al. 1996; Smyth et al. 1998; Steptoe et al. 2007; Bitsika et al. 2015) were reported to be associated with levels of psychological stress, and changes in pulmonary functions tested by a spirometer were associated with daily positive/negative affect, as well as the symptom of shortness of breath in asthma patients. Health-related behaviors, such as eating (Lavender et al. 2013; Crosby et al. 2009), smoking (Chandra et al. 2011; Shiffman et al. 2007), and alcohol consumption (Muraven et al. 2005; Jahng et al. 2011), exhibited associations with variation in physical symptoms and psychological states, e.g., craving, positive/negative affect, and anxiety. Furthermore, associations between physical activity measured by self-report and daily fluctuations in psychological states have been reported (Dunton et al. 2009; Wichers et al. 2012). These studies provide strong evidence that various biological/physiological measures are associated with momentary symptoms, possibly in a concurrent fashion. In addition, the existence of such external measures for subjective symptoms indicates the possibility of the practical use of the biomarkers for monitoring momentary symptoms in a continuous fashion simply by measuring other physiological/biological data (i.e., without the need for self-reports).

Behavioral abnormalities as an objective biomarker

Micro-fluctuations in physical activity contain rich information on the dynamics of our bodily movements in daily life. These data can be continuously obtained in an unobtrusive manner through the use of a wrist-watch-type activity monitor, referred to as an actigraph. The detailed analysis of actigraph data has shown its potential as a psychobehavioral biomarker for psychiatric disorders, and particularly for depressive disorders (Teicher et al. 1995; Teicher et al. 1997; Burton et al. 2013; Indic et al. 2012; Walther et al. 2012; Volkers et al. 2003; Berle et al. 2010). For example, in major depressive disorder (MDD), various types of behavioral alterations were observed; decreased levels of physical activity during daytime (Faurholt-Jepsen et al. 2012; St-Amand et al. 2013; Teicher et al. 1995; Burton et al. 2013); sleep disturbances (Joffe et al. 2009; St-Amand et al. 2013); disruption of the circadian rhythm (Teicher et al. 1997; Robillard et al. 2013; Teicher et al. 1995) as well as improvements over the course of clinical treatment (Baune et al. 2007; Burton et al. 2013; Teicher et al. 1995).

Recent research has shown the existence of robust statistical laws organizing daily life behaviors, specifically how resting and active periods derived from physical activity data are interwoven into daily life (Nakamura et al. 2007). In addition, this research found a significant alteration in the statistical law of resting period durations in patients with MDD, as these patients exhibited more intermittent behavioral patterns than healthy subjects characterized by reduced mean activity levels associated with occasional bursts of physical activity counts (Nakamura et al. 2007; Nakamura et al. 2008). Furthermore, alterations of intermittent properties of physical activity have been reported in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder (Sano et al. 2012; Nakamura et al. 2016). These findings suggest that the intermittency of physical activity is a useful measure for evaluating behavioral abnormalities associated with psychiatric disorders, and that its characterization is likely to provide an objective biomarker for these disorders. Interestingly, it has also been shown that the statistical laws found in human behaviors are shared by mice, and similar alterations in resting period durations to patients with MDD have also been confirmed in mice with mutant circadian clock genes (Nakamura et al. 2008).

Physical activity provides a momentary biomarker for depressive mood

More recently, using multilevel modeling approaches (Stone et al. 2007; Schwartz and Stone 1998), Kim et al. probed the psychobehavioral correlates in temporal diurnal fluctuations in momentary depressive mood assessed by EMA and behavioral dynamics (Kim et al. 2013b; Kim et al. 2015). Their results showed that an increased intermittency of physical activity appeared concurrently with the worsening of depressive mood in healthy subjects across a wide range of populations (adolescents, undergraduates, and adult office workers) (Kim et al. 2013b), as well as in patients with MDD (Kim et al. 2015) (Fig. 1-a). Furthermore, the validity of the psychobehavioral correlates across healthy subjects and patients with MDD were confirmed, indicating that the same psychobehavioral correlates are shared by both groups, though only the mean levels of depressive mood scores were significantly higher in the MDD group (Kim et al. 2015).

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

A momentary biomarker for depressive mood. a The temporal associations of depressive mood and local statistics of physical activity. Estimated values of the univariate multilevel model coefficient for the associations are shown in a colored matrix form consisting of 25 rows (different location) and 12 columns (different size) in patients with MDD (n = 14). Each grid cell indicates specific location and size of a time frame used for calculating the local statistics of physical activity surrounding each EMA recording of depressive mood. A color in each cell represents the value of the model coefficient (γ10) of the predictors: local mean (left matrix) or skewness (right one) of physical activity which evaluate lower/higher mean activity levels and asymmetry of a distribution, respectively (i.e., intermittency of physical activity). The false discovery rate with the q value of .05 was used as the multiple comparison adjustment. Only the significant cases were shown by colors. Note that the univariate model used for the analysis is as follows; Depressvie mood scoreij = γ00 + γ10 (Local statistics of locomotor activityij) + ζ0i + εij [see (Kim et al. 2013b) for details]. b A estimation of momentary depressive mood from physical activity in a patient with MDD [modified from (Kim et al. 2015)]. The parameters of the best-fitting multilevel model describing the temporal associations of depressive mood and local statistics of physical activity were optimized individually using data collected at one week in the early part of the measurement. Subsequently, the momentary depressive scores in another week in the later part of the measurement were estimated using personalized parameters and local statistics of physical activity. In this patient, the correlation coefficient between self-reported (i.e., EMA recordings) and estimated depressive mood scores was considerably high [r = 0.80 (p = 0.002)]. Note that the multilevel model we used for estimation is as follows: Depressive mood scoreij = γ00 + γ10 (Meanij) + γ20 (Skewnessij) + γ30 (Meanij × Skewnessij) + ζ0i + ζ1i (Meanij) + εij. c Challenges in continuous monitoring of depressive disorders and pharmacological animal studies

Importance of physical activity and further challenges in psychiatric research

Physical activity provides a behavioral biomarker for momentary depressive mood (Kim et al. 2015). Due to the continuous nature of the measurement of physical activity, this behavioral biomarker will enable assessment of diurnal changes of depressive mood with higher resolution than the use of self-reports, contributing to the development of “continuous” monitoring of pathological states in psychiatric disorders (Fig. 1-b and -c). This continuous monitoring has the potential to provide rich information on dynamical aspects of momentary mood and largely contribute to the development of early detection methods of psychiatric disorders (Nakamura et al. 2016) as well as the novel objective evaluation of their treatments.

One further application of the monitoring of symptoms might be in animal research (Fig. 1-c). Animal studies have played a crucial role in psychiatric research (Konopka and Roberts 2015; Cosgrove et al. 2015; Bolkan et al. 2015); however, critical problems exist in the evaluation of animal symptoms, such as “depressive mood.” Diagnosis of humans mainly relies on verbal communication, making it impossible to diagnose animals using the current human diagnostic system. Therefore, animal symptoms are often evaluated based on behavioral assessments which can be impractical to directly apply to humans (Nestler and Hyman 2010). The probing depressive mood based on physical activity may partly solve the above problems and provide a new approach to bridge research about human disorders with animal models.

In order to enhance the accuracy of mood estimation, multidimensional approaches integrating a variety of biomarkers from behavioral, clinical, mathematical, molecular, and imaging studies will be required (Singh and Rose 2009; Kennedy et al. 2012). Due to the complex nature of psychiatric disorders, the presentation of symptoms, process of development, and response to specific medications and treatments for each disease and even each individual are highly diverse. While physical activity is a robust measure, a single biomarker is not likely to be enough to cover this diversity; the combination of physical activity with other behavioral/physiological/biological biomarkers is essential (Kennedy et al. 2012; McGorry et al. 2014). In addition, the development of mathematical methodologies, especially the methods for repeated data with individual variations [e.g., multilevel modeling (Stone et al. 2007; Schwartz and Stone 1998) or machine learning approaches (Bishop 1995)], may become necessary.

One further challenge would be a mathematical modeling approach to psychiatric disorders. The recent development of measurement technologies (e.g., the state-of-the-art wearable devices) has enabled us to obtain high-quality, multidimensional, and intensive longitudinal data (ILD) (Walls and Schafer 2006). This ILD might open possible avenues to reconstruct/infer the dynamical systems underlying the transitions in observed physiological/biological phenomena in psychiatric disorders (e.g., shifts in physical activity data or momentary depressive mood scores). This approach might also provide a novel framework for the early detection of pathological transitions of disease states (Nakamura et al. 2016).

Conclusion

Physical activity has potential as an objective biobehavioral biomarker for psychiatric disorders. The presence of psychobehavioral correlates between momentary depressive mood and intermittent dynamics of physical activity allow the continuous monitoring of pathogenic processes and pathological states in depressive disorders. It is possible that this approach will also be informative in pharmacological animal studies, and future research should explore these directions.

Acknowledgment

The authors would like to thank Drs. T. Sasaki, K. Yoshiuchi, and H. Kikuchi for their considerable contributions to the studies making essential parts of this review. The authors also would like to thank Dr. J. C. Foo for critical reading of the manuscript. This study is partly supported by Grants-in-Aid for Scientific Research [#26242069, #15 K12678] (to Y.Y.) and [#26882016] (to J.K.) from the Ministry of Education, Culture, Sports, Science and Technology, PRESTO (to T.N.) from the Japan Science and Technology Agency.

Footnotes

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

JK, TN, and YY wrote the manuscript. All authors reviewed and approved the final manuscript.

References

  1. Arnone D, Cavanagh J, Gerber D, Lawrie SM, Ebmeier KP, McIntosh AM. Magnetic resonance imaging studies in bipolar disorder and schizophrenia: meta-analysis. Br J Psychiatry. 2009;195(3):194–201. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.108.059717. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Arribas JI, Calhoun VD, Adali T. Automatic Bayesian classification of healthy controls, bipolar disorder, and schizophrenia using intrinsic connectivity maps from FMRI data. IEEE Trans Biomed Eng. 2010;57(12):2850–2860. doi: 10.1109/TBME.2010.2080679. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Banigan MG, Kao PF, Kozubek JA, Winslow AR, Medina J, Costa J, et al. Differential expression of exosomal microRNAs in prefrontal cortices of schizophrenia and bipolar disorder patients. PLoS One. 2013;8(1) doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0048814. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Baune BT, Caliskan S, Todder D. Effects of adjunctive antidepressant therapy with quetiapine on clinical outcome, quality of sleep and daytime motor activity in patients with treatment-resistant depression. Hum Psychopharmcol. 2007;22(1):1–9. doi: 10.1002/hup.817. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Berle JO, Hauge ER, Oedegaard KJ, Holsten F, Fasmer OB. Actigraphic registration of motor activity reveals a more structured behavioural pattern in schizophrenia than in major depression. BMC Res Notes. 2010;3:149. doi: 10.1186/1756-0500-3-149. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bishop CM. Neural networks for pattern recognition. Oxford: Clarendon; 1995. [Google Scholar]
  7. Bitsika V, Sharpley CF, Andronicos NM, Agnew LL. Hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal axis daily fluctuation, anxiety and age interact to predict cortisol concentrations in boys with an autism spectrum disorder. Physiol Behav. 2015;138:200–207. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2014.11.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Boksa P. A way forward for research on biomarkers for psychiatric disorders. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2013;38(2):75–77. doi: 10.1503/jpn.130018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bolkan SS, Poyraz FC, Kellendonk C. Using human brain imaging studies as a guide toward animal models of schizophrenia. Neuroscience. 2015 doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.05.055. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Burton C, McKinstry B, Szentagotai Tatar A, Serrano-Blanco A, Pagliari C, Wolters M. Activity monitoring in patients with depression: a systematic review. J Affect Disord. 2013;145(1):21–28. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2012.07.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Chandra S, Scharf D, Shiffman S. Within-day temporal patterns of smoking, withdrawal symptoms, and craving. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2011;117(2-3):118–125. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2010.12.027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Conner TS, Barrett LF. Trends in ambulatory self-report: the role of momentary experience in psychosomatic medicine. Psychosom Med. 2012;74(4):327–337. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e3182546f18. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Cosgrove VE, Kelsoe JR, Suppes T. Toward a Valid Animal Model of Bipolar Disorder: How the Research Domain Criteria Help Bridge the Clinical-Basic Science Divide. Biol Psychiatry. 2015 doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.09.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Crosby RD, Wonderlich SA, Engel SG, Simonich H, Smyth J, Mitchell JE. Daily mood patterns and bulimic behaviors in the natural environment. Behav Res Ther. 2009;47(3):181–188. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2008.11.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Diler RS, De Almeida JR, Ladouceur C, Birmaher B, Axelson D, Phillips M. Neural activity to intense positive versus negative stimuli can help differentiate bipolar disorder from unipolar major depressive disorder in depressed adolescents: a pilot fMRI study. Psychiatry Res. 2013;214(3):277–284. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2013.06.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Dunton GF, Atienza AA, Castro CM, King AC. Using ecological momentary assessment to examine antecedents and correlates of physical activity bouts in adults age 50+ years: a pilot study. Ann Behav Med. 2009;38(3):249–255. doi: 10.1007/s12160-009-9141-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Faurholt-Jepsen M, Brage S, Vinberg M, Christensen EM, Knorr U, Jensen HM, et al. Differences in psychomotor activity in patients suffering from unipolar and bipolar affective disorder in the remitted or mild/moderate depressive state. J Affect Disord. 2012;141(2-3):457–463. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2012.02.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Geaghan M, Cairns MJ. MicroRNA and Posttranscriptional Dysregulation in Psychiatry. Biol Psychiatry. 2015;78(4):231–239. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.12.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Grossman P, Deuring G, Garland SN, Campbell TS, Carlson LE. Patterns of objective physical functioning and perception of mood and fatigue in posttreatment breast cancer patients and healthy controls: an ambulatory psychophysiological investigation. Psychosom Med. 2008;70(7):819–828. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e31818106f1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Indic P, Murray G, Maggini C, Amore M, Meschi T, Borghi L, et al. Multi-scale motility amplitude associated with suicidal thoughts in major depression. PLoS One. 2012;7(6) doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0038761. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Jahng S, Solhan MB, Tomko RL, Wood PK, Piasecki TM, Trull TJ. Affect and alcohol use: an ecological momentary assessment study of outpatients with borderline personality disorder. J Abnorm Psychol. 2011;120(3):572–584. doi: 10.1037/a0024686. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Joffe H, Soares CN, Thurston RC, White DP, Cohen LS, Hall JE. Depression is associated with worse objectively and subjectively measured sleep, but not more frequent awakenings, in women with vasomotor symptoms. Menopause. 2009;16(4):671–679. doi: 10.1097/gme.0b013e3181957377. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Kamarck TW, Schwartz JE, Shiffman S, Muldoon MF, Sutton-Tyrrell K, Janicki DL. Psychosocial stress and cardiovascular risk: what is the role of daily experience? J Pers. 2005;73(6):1749–1774. doi: 10.1111/j.0022-3506.2005.00365.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Kamarck TW, Shiffman SM, Smithline L, Goodie JL, Paty JA, Gnys M, et al. Effects of task strain, social conflict, and emotional activation on ambulatory cardiovascular activity: daily life consequences of recurring stress in a multiethnic adult sample. Health Psychol. 1998;17(1):17–29. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.17.1.17. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Kennedy SH, Downar J, Evans KR, Feilotter H, Lam RW, MacQueen GM, et al. The Canadian Biomarker Integration Network in Depression (CAN-BIND): advances in response prediction. Curr Pharm Des. 2012;18(36):5976–5989. doi: 10.2174/138161212803523635. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Kim J, Kikuchi H, Yamamoto Y. Systematic comparison between ecological momentary assessment and day reconstruction method for fatigue and mood states in healthy adults. Br J Health Psychol. 2013a;18(1):155–67. doi:10.1111/bjhp.12000. [DOI] [PubMed]
  27. Kim J, Nakamura T, Kikuchi H, Sasaki T, Yamamoto Y. Co-variation of depressive mood and locomotor dynamics evaluated by ecological momentary assessment in healthy humans. PLoS One. 2013b;8(9), e74979. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  28. Kim J, Nakamura T, Kikuchi H, Yoshiuchi K, Sasaki T, Yamamoto Y. Covariation of depressive mood and spontaneous physical activity in major depressive disorder: toward continuous monitoring of depressive mood. IEEE J Biomed Health Inform. 2015;19(4):1347–1355. doi: 10.1109/JBHI.2015.2440764. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Konopka G, Roberts TF. Animal Models of Speech and Vocal Communication Deficits Associated with Psychiatric Disorders. Biol Psychiatry. 2015 doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.07.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Lacerda-Pinheiro SF, Pinheiro RFF, De Lima MAP, Da Silva CGL, Dos Santos MDV, Teixeira AG, et al. Are there depression and anxiety genetic markers and mutations? A systematic review. J Affect Disord. 2014;168:387–398. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2014.07.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Lavender JM, De Young KP, Wonderlich SA, Crosby RD, Engel SG, Mitchell JE, et al. Daily Patterns of Anxiety in Anorexia Nervosa: Associations With Eating Disorder Behaviors in the Natural Environment. J Abnorm Psychol. 2013 doi: 10.1037/a0031823. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. McGorry P, Keshavan M, Goldstone S, Amminger P, Allott K, Berk M, et al. Biomarkers and clinical staging in psychiatry. World Psychiatry. 2014;13(3):211–223. doi: 10.1002/wps.20144. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Moskowitz DS, Young SN. Ecological momentary assessment: what it is and why it is a method of the future in clinical psychopharmacology. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2006;31(1):13–20. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Muraven M, Collins RL, Shiffman S, Paty JA. Daily fluctuations in self-control demands and alcohol intake. Psychology of addictive behaviors: journal of the Society of Psychologists in Addictive Behaviors. 2005;19(2):140–147. doi: 10.1037/0893-164X.19.2.140. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Nakamura T, Kiyono K, Wendt H, Abry P, Yamamoto Y. Multiscale analysis of intensive longitudinal biomedical signals and its clinical applications. Proc IEEE. 2016;104(2):242–261. doi: 10.1109/JPROC.2015.2491979. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  36. Nakamura T, Kiyono K, Yoshiuchi K, Nakahara R, Struzik Z, Yamamoto Y. Universal scaling law in human behavioral organization. Phys Rev Lett. 2007;99(13):138103. doi: 10.1103/PhysRevLett.99.138103. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Nakamura T, Takumi T, Takano A, Aoyagi N, Yoshiuchi K, Struzik ZR, et al. Of mice and men--universality and breakdown of behavioral organization. PLoS One. 2008;3(4) doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0002050. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Nestler EJ, Hyman SE. Animal models of neuropsychiatric disorders. Nat Neurosci. 2010;13(10):1161–1169. doi: 10.1038/nn.2647. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Pagan C, Delorme R, Callebert J, Goubran-Botros H, Amsellem F, Drouot X, Boudebesse C, Le Dudal K, Ngo-Nguyen N, Laouamri H, Gillberg C, Leboyer M, Bourgeron T, Launay JM (2014) The serotonin-N-acetylserotonin-melatonin pathway as a biomarker for autism spectrum disorders. Transl Psychiat 4. doi: ARTN e47910.1038/tp.2014.120 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  40. Robillard R, Naismith SL, Rogers NL, Ip TK, Hermens DF, Scott EM, et al. Delayed sleep phase in young people with unipolar or bipolar affective disorders. J Affect Disord. 2013;145(2):260–263. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2012.06.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Robles TF, Shetty V, Zigler CM, Glover DA, Elashoff D, Murphy D, et al. The feasibility of ambulatory biosensor measurement of salivary alpha amylase: relationships with self-reported and naturalistic psychological stress. Biol Psychol. 2011;86(1):50–56. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.10.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Sano W, Nakamura T, Yoshiuchi K, Kitajima T, Tsuchiya A, Esaki Y, et al. Enhanced persistency of resting and active periods of locomotor activity in schizophrenia. PLoS One. 2012;7(8) doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0043539. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Schnack HG, Nieuwenhuis M, Van Haren NE, Abramovic L, Scheewe TW, Brouwer RM, et al. Can structural MRI aid in clinical classification? A machine learning study in two independent samples of patients with schizophrenia, bipolar disorder and healthy subjects. Neuroimage. 2014;84:299–306. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.08.053. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Schwartz JE, Stone AA. Strategies for analyzing ecological momentary assessment data. Health Psychol. 1998;17(1):6–16. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.17.1.6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Shiffman S, Balabanis MH, Gwaltney CJ, Paty JA, Gnys M, Kassel JD, et al. Prediction of lapse from associations between smoking and situational antecedents assessed by ecological momentary assessment. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2007;91(2-3):159–168. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2007.05.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Shiffman S, Stone AA, Hufford MR. Ecological momentary assessment. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2008;4(1):1–32. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.3.022806.091415. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Singh I, Rose N. Biomarkers in psychiatry. Nature. 2009;460(7252):202–207. doi: 10.1038/460202a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Smith TW, Birmingham W, Uchino BN. Evaluative threat and ambulatory blood pressure: cardiovascular effects of social stress in daily experience. Health Psychol. 2012;31(6):763–766. doi: 10.1037/a0026947. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Smyth J, Ockenfels MC, Porter L, Kirschbaum C, Hellhammer DH, Stone AA. Stressors and mood measured on a momentary basis are associated with salivary cortisol secretion. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 1998;23(4):353–370. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4530(98)00008-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. St-Amand J, Provencher MD, Belanger L, Morin CM. Sleep disturbances in bipolar disorder during remission. J Affect Disord. 2013;146(1):112–119. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2012.05.057. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Steptoe A, Gibson EL, Hamer M, Wardle J. Neuroendocrine and cardiovascular correlates of positive affect measured by ecological momentary assessment and by questionnaire. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2007;32(1):56–64. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2006.10.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Stone AA, Shiffman S. Ecological momentary assessment (EMA) in behavioral medicine. Ann Behav Med. 1994;16:199–202. [Google Scholar]
  53. Stone AA, Shiffman S, Atienza AA, Nebeling L. The science of real-time data capture: self-reports in health research. Oxford; New York: Oxford University Press; 2007. [Google Scholar]
  54. Teicher BA, Holden SA, Dupuis NP, Kakeji Y, Ikebe M, Emi Y, et al. Potentiation of cytotoxic therapies by TNP-470 and minocycline in mice bearing EMT-6 mammary carcinoma. Breast Cancer Res Treat. 1995;36(2):227–236. doi: 10.1007/BF00666043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Teicher MH, Glod CA, Magnus E, Harper D, Benson G, Krueger K, et al. Circadian rest-activity disturbances in seasonal affective disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1997;54(2):124–130. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1997.01830140034007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Trull TJ, Ebner-Priemer U. Ambulatory assessment. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2013;9:151–176. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050212-185510. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Trull TJ, Ebner-Priemer UW. Using experience sampling methods/ecological momentary assessment (ESM/EMA) in clinical assessment and clinical research: introduction to the special section. Psychol Assess. 2009;21(4):457–462. doi: 10.1037/a0017653. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Van Eck M, Berkhof H, Nicolson N, Sulon J. The effects of perceived stress, traits, mood states, and stressful daily events on salivary cortisol. Psychosom Med. 1996;58(5):447–458. doi: 10.1097/00006842-199609000-00007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Vita A, De Peri L, Sacchetti E. Gray matter, white matter, brain, and intracranial volumes in first-episode bipolar disorder: a meta-analysis of magnetic resonance imaging studies. Bipolar Disord. 2009;11(8):807–814. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2009.00759.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Volkers AC, Tulen JH, van den Broek WW, Bruijn JA, Passchier J, Pepplinkhuizen L. Motor activity and autonomic cardiac functioning in major depressive disorder. J Affect Disord. 2003;76(1-3):23–30. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0327(02)00066-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Walls TA, Schafer JL. Models for intensive longitudinal data. Oxford; New York: Oxford University Press; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  62. Walther S, Hugli S, Hofle O, Federspiel A, Horn H, Bracht T, et al. Frontal white matter integrity is related to psychomotor retardation in major depression. Neurobiol Dis. 2012;47(1):13–19. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2012.03.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Wichers M, Peeters F, Rutten BP, Jacobs N, Derom C, Thiery E, et al. A time-lagged momentary assessment study on daily life physical activity and affect. Health Psychol. 2012;31(2):135–144. doi: 10.1037/a0025688. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Wolfers T, Buitelaar JK, Beckmann CF, Franke B, Marquand AF. From estimating activation locality to predicting disorder: A review of pattern recognition for neuroimaging-based psychiatric diagnostics. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2015;57:328–349. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.08.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Wood SJ, Yung AR, McGorry PD, Pantelis C. Neuroimaging and Treatment Evidence for Clinical Staging in Psychotic Disorders: From the At-Risk Mental State to Chronic Schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry. 2011;70(7):619–625. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.05.034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Yang J, Chen T, Sun L, Zhao Z, Qi X, Zhou K, et al. Potential metabolite markers of schizophrenia. Mol Psychiatry. 2013;18(1):67–78. doi: 10.1038/mp.2011.131. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from In Silico Pharmacology are provided here courtesy of Springer

RESOURCES