Abstract
Stress modulates pain perception, resulting in either stress-induced analgesia or stress-induced hyperalgesia, as reported in both animal and human studies. The responses to stress include neural, endocrine, and behavioural changes, and built-in coping strategies are in place to address stressors. Peculiar to humans are additional factors that modulate pain that are experienced in times of stress, notably psychological factors that potentially influence the directionality of pain perception.
Keywords: stress, pain, analgesia, hyperalgesia, neuroimaging
Introduction
From the time of conception, stress is present in human life (1). ‘Stress’ is defined as a state of disharmony or threatened homeostasis (2). Stress can be due to either physical or psychological stressors and it is followed initially by a response specific to the stressor (3). As the stress increases, the response takes on a more stereotypical nature, termed the ‘General Adaptation Syndrome’ (4). The sequelae of stress include neural, endocrine and behavioural responses. The neural response is activation of the sympathetic nervous system, resulting in release of epinephrine and norepinephrine (5), whereas the endocrine response involves stimulation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis (6). The behavioural responses include increases in pain threshold, changes in locomotor activity and body temperature, and catalepsy (7). The neural and endocrine responses to stress can be summed up by the stress system.
The Stress System
The stress system is made up of central and peripheral components (3). The central components are located in the hypothalamus and brainstem, and they include (a) the parvocellular neurons that secrete corticotropin-releasing hormone (CRH), (b) arginine vasopressin (AVP) neurons of the paraventricular nuclei (PVN) of the hypothalamus, (c) CRH neurons of the paragigantocellular and parabrachial nuclei of the medulla and the locus caeruleus (LC), and (d) other mostly noradrenergic (norepinephrine, NE) cell groups in the medulla and pons (LC-NE system). The peripheral components include the peripheral limbs of the HPA axis and the efferent sympathetic adrenomedullary system (2).
The central components of the stress system interact with three higher brain control areas, namely (1) the mesocortical or mesolimbic system (affect and anticipatory phenomena), (2) the amygdala and hippocampus complex (propagation and termination of stress system activity), and (3) the arcuate nucleus (the setting of pain sensation) (3).
Activation of the stress system brings about various changes in body systems, including increased secretion of hypothalamic beta-endorphin and other proopiomelanocortin-derived peptides, such as adrenocorticotrophic hormone (ACTH), which reciprocally inhibits stress system activity, resulting in analgesia (8,9).
Coping Strategies to Stress
The body has built-in mechanisms to address stress. The term ‘allostasis’ means the process of maintaining stability (homeostasis) by active means, whereas ‘allostatic load’ means the wear and tear the body and brain undergo to achieve allostasis (10). Persistence of stress over time will deplete the body’s resources, resulting in an increased allostatic load and eventually ending in disease or death.
Animals and humans display distinct coping strategies when faced with different types of stress. These coping mechanisms are mediated by neural substrates within the periaqueductal grey (PAG; 11). The PAG circuitry is organised into distinct longitudinal columns that function to coordinate somatomotor, autonomic, and behavioural reactions to different types of stress. The lateral column subserves escapable or controllable physical stressors, such as superficial pain. The dorsolateral column functions in reactions to psychological stressors, such as unconditioned stress or fear. Both of these columns are involved in active coping reactions, i.e., confrontational defensive or escape reactions. This is accompanied by hypertension, tachycardia, and analgesia. The third column is the ventrolateral column, which is activated when faced with an inescapable physical stressor, such as deep pain and opioid withdrawal, and psychological stressors in the form of conditioned stress or fear.
Involvement of the ventrolateral column results in a passive-coping reaction, such as quiescence and hyporeactivity, and is accompanied by hypotension, bradycardia, and analgesia (11). The dorsolateral and lateral PAG evoke nonopioid-mediated analgesia, whereas the ventrolateral PAG evokes opioid-mediated analgesia. Stressors, however, often combine physical and psychological elements, thus activating multiple PAG neuronal columns (12).
Parameters of Stress
Stress level can be determined using psychological as well as neuroendocrine measurements. The Symptoms of Stress Inventory (SOSI) (13) was designed to measure physical, psychological, and behavioural responses to stressful situations. The subject is asked to rate the frequency with which they experience stressrelated symptoms on a 5-point scale ranging from never to frequently during the past week. Other measures include the Perceived Stress Scale (PSS) and the Life Events Stress Scale (14). The distress level may be measured using the Likert Scale, which ranges from 1 (not at all distressed) to 10 (extremely distressed).
The neuroendocrine measurements of stress levels include systolic and diastolic blood pressure, heart rate, salivary cortisol concentration, and salivary alpha-amylase activity (15–21).
Several psychological and psychosocial stressors have been utilised in studies of stress in humans. The Trier Social Stress Test (22), a public speaking and mental arithmetic stressor, and adaptations of this test (23,24) are widely used in studies of stress in humans (25,26). It has been shown that this stressor induces a strong stress response both by subjective report and physiological measures.
The Stress Appraisal Measure (SAM) is a measure of anticipation of stress (27). Grillon et al (28) used a similar speech presentation and mental arithmetic paradigm as the social stressor. Takai et al (16) showed a video of corneal surgery to induce psychological stress. The Montreal Imaging Stress Task, an adaptation of the Trier Social Stress Test, is used in functional imaging studies to investigate the processing of psychological stress in the human brain (24). It is derived from the Trier Mental Challenge Test and consists of a series of mental arithmetic tasks together with social evaluative threat components. Negative feedback in combination with a challenging cognitive task, such as mental arithmetic or public speaking as used in the Trier Social Stress Test (22), have been shown to be highly effective in eliciting strong stress responses. The combined cognitive challenge induces a strong stress response on both the subjective and physiological levels.
Neuroimaging in Stress and Pain Studies
Imaging provides a leap forward in stress (29) and pain studies (30). The advent of functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) has made possible the study of human cognition by analysing brain activations (31). In pain studies, fMRI enables brain imaging during pain-intensity-related haemodynamic changes (32), as well as the modulation of pain by various psychological (33,34) and pharmacological interventions (35–37).
Wang et al. (38) used arterial spin-labelling perfusion MRI to measure cerebral blood flow changes associated with mild to moderate stress induced by psychological stress. Perfusion MRI is ideal for imaging a sustained behavioural state (e.g., stress) involving functions of deep brain structures because of its excellent reproducibility over long-term periods and minimal sensitivity to magnetic field inhomogeneity effects. Psychological stress induced by mental arithmetic tasks causes increased activation in the right ventral prefrontal cortex and left insula/putamen area (38). The ventromedial prefrontal cortex, along with the lateral orbitofrontal cortex and the amygdala, are associated with emotional processing (39), whereas the right prefrontal cortex is associated with negative affect (40). These activations are accompanied by increases in heart rate, salivary cortisol and perceived stress levels.
In a PET study, task-related increases in extracellular dopamine levels were observed in the ventral striatum of individuals who mentally performed arithmetic computations in the Montreal Imaging Stress Task (24). fMRI findings in the same study revealed activations in the visual association cortices, angular cortex, sensory cortex, motor cortex, thalamus and caudate nucleus during the task performance. Performing mental arithmetic in the absence of stress resulted in activation of the posterior cingulate, angular, motor and visual association cortices. The main effects of stress were later shown to be activations in the left premotor area, the medial left prefrontal cortex, and bilaterally in the area of the cingulum/white matter (41). Apart from the activations, the psychosocial stress also caused deactivation of limbic system components including the hippocampus, hypothalamus, medio-orbitofrontal cortex, and anterior cingulate cortex. The presence of stress was determined by measuring salivary cortisol. This finding corresponds to the study reported by Niddam et al. (42) in which suppressed hippocampal activity was observed in patients with chronic pain (myofascial pain syndrome) when they were stimulated in a hypersensitive myofascial trigger point, reflecting stress-related changes in relation to chronic pain as a physical and emotional stressor. These findings suggest that a reduction in limbic system activity is essential for the initiation of the stress response (41).
Psychological/cognitive tasks used as stressors in imaging studies usually serve as a distraction to divert the subject’s attention from the perception of pain. In these studies, both the task and the pain stimuli are applied at the same time, hence the effects are mainly due to attention focused on the stressor rather than the pain. Petrovic et al. (43) used cold pressor pain during an attention-demanding maze task and demonstrated decreased activity in the somatosensory association areas and the periaqueductal grey that was accompanied by lower ratings of pain and increased activation in the orbitofrontal cortex. Using the counting Stroop test as the distractor and applying noxious thermal heat, Bantick et al. (44) showed lowered activation in the pain matrix (thalamus, insula, cognitive division of the ACC) and increased activation in the affective division of the ACC and the orbitofrontal cortex. Using the colour-word Stroop test and heat pain, Valet et al. (45) demonstrated reduced activation in pain-related areas and increased activation in the cingulofrontal cortex, periaqueductal grey, and posterior thalamus.
It has been shown that anxiety exacerbates pain through activation in the hippocampus (33), which has led to suggesting ways to reduce pain by disengaging the hippocampus during potentially painful clinical procedures. A study using positron emission tomography (PET) showed that psychological stress in humans causes mesolimbic dopamine release (46). Using pain as the stressor, another PET study showed that basal ganglia dopaminergic activity is involved in pain processing, as well as emotional processing of the pain stimulus (47). Nigrostriatal D2 dopamine receptor activity was related to the sensory aspect of pain, whereas mesolimbic D2/D3 dopamine receptor activity was related to negative affect and fear. This finding outlines the regions involved in the physical and emotional responses to painrelated stress in humans.
Yilmaz et al. (48) performed an fMRI study to investigate the neural correlates of stress-induced analgesia (SIA) in humans. This study used mental arithmetic as the cognitive stressor, white noise as an additional distressing element and mechanical pressure as the pain stimulus. Subject participation was rewarded with monetary remuneration. An adequate stress response was elicited with this study design that was evidenced by physiological changes. The results showed that the analgesia (as indexed by increased pain tolerance from pre to post-stress) correlated with the BOLD response for the post versus pre-stress contrast in the rostral ACC and right SI. Correlations for the differences in pain unpleasantness perceived with the pre and poststress using the BOLD contrast were significant in the dorsal ACC.
Directionality of Stress Effects on Pain Response
A crucial factor for the occurrence of SIA and stress-induced hyperalgesia (SIH) in humans is the influence of psychological and cognitive elements on stress and pain processing, which will in turn determine the outcome of the pain response. Pain experience in humans involves sensory-discriminative, motivationalaffective, and cognitive components (49). Table 1 summarises the human studies on the effects of stress on pain behaviour.
Table 1:
Stressor | Pain test | Pain behaviour observation | Reference |
---|---|---|---|
Foot shock | Flexion reflex | SIA | 60, 61 |
Perceived coping inefficacy in a mathematical task | Cold pressor test | SIA | 62 |
Blocked by naloxone | |||
Exposure of spider phobics to spiders | Electrical stimuli | SIA | 63 |
Giving a speech about a recent anger arousing experience | Thermal pain stimuli | SIA only in women with low mean arterial pressure | 64 |
Mental arithmetic | Cold pressor test | SIH | 65 |
Fear induction by exposure to electrical shock | Radiant heat | SIA | 51 |
Anxiety induction by threat of shock | SIH | ||
Cold water immersion | Thermal CO2 laser | SIA | 66 |
20-minute Stroop test | Capsaicin | SIA in men | 59 |
SIH in women | |||
Anticipation of pain with visual cue presentation | Random and intermittent noxious stimuli | SIH | 33 |
Mental arithmetic plus noise | Electrical stimuli | SIA | 67 |
Public speaking | Cold pressor test | SIA | 68 |
Speech task | Cold pressor test | No significant difference in intensity but increased post-pain negatice affectivity | 69 |
Trier Social Stress Test | Tourniquet test (ischaemic pain) | SIA – in men only | 25 |
Cold pressor test | No SIA | ||
Thermal pain stimuli | SIA in women only (unpleasantness rating only) | ||
External control of noxious stimuli | Electrical stimuli | SIH | 70 |
Logical task using sequence of symbols with time pressure | Pin-prick | SIH – increased VAS score. | 58 |
Emotional stress – pictures and audio of physical violence | SIH – increased VAS score. | ||
Interview and serial subtraction | Cold pressor test | SIH | 71 |
Anticipation of painful stimuli with presentation | Thermal pain stimuli | SIH | 72 |
Metyrapone-induced hypocortisolism | Mechanical pain stimuli | SIH | 73 |
Mental arithmetic plus white noise | Mechanical pressure | SIA | 48 |
Induction of hypoglycaemic state | Thermal pain stimuli | SIH | 74 |
Psychosocial stress test | Thermal pain stimuli | SIH | 75 |
SIA – stress-induced analgesia.
SIH – stress-induced hyperalgesia.
Whether a human subjected to stress will exhibit analgesia or hyperalgesia is relatively subjective compared to the experience of animals and is dependent on the psychological effects that the stressor exerts on the individual’s emotions. Emotion modulates pain through an interaction of valence (pleasant-unpleasant) and arousal (calm-excited) (50). The valence-by-arousal interaction determines the directionality of the stressor on the pain response. A negatively valenced emotion with low to moderate arousal evokes anxiety and enhances pain, whereas one with high arousal, such as fear, reduces pain (51). Conversely, a positively valenced emotion always reduces pain, as long as minimal arousal is achieved.
How the stressor is perceived by the individual is in turn determined by the personality of the individual. A person prone to catastrophising may find a stressor negatively affecting his or her emotions. Studies have shown that women have a higher predilection for catastrophising (52), although other studies did not find significant differences between men and women (53,54). Animal studies have shown that oestrogen enhances pain sensitivity (55), and women smokers with low oestrogen levels exhibit lower pain perception (25). Studies examining pain and differences between the sexes revealed that women are more sensitive to threat-related stimuli and experience more negative affect than men, leading to an increased pain perception (50).
One oft-quoted early example of the interaction between pain and stress is the observation by Dr. Henry Beecher (56) of soldiers wounded in battle who needed less analgesic than civilians suffering equivalent injuries. This observation suggests that the amount of pain felt by a person is determined by how the person perceives the pain. The soldiers injured in battle were better able to address their pain because of their acceptance of injuries as being part and parcel of being in battle, and they are relieved that their injuries do not lead to death. The civilians, on the other hand, treat their injuries as a major tragedy, hence their heightened pain perception. This suggests that the amount of pain perceived by a person is governed not only by the amount of tissue injury present but also by emotional and psychological factors. The injury caused more distress to the civilians than to the soldiers, and this leads to more pain, i.e., hyperalgesia.
The motivation-decision model by Fields (57), states that analgesia may either be the avoidance of a bigger threat than pain or the anticipation of reward. In the face of menace, such as threat of a predator, attending to the dangerous situation takes precedence over attending to the pain, hence analgesia. Likewise, in situations where a reward can be gained, the motivation for reward attenuates the sensation of pain, resulting in analgesia.
In the study reported by Stoeter et al (58), subjects performed a cognitive task and were subjected to an emotional stressor before they received a pin-prick pain stimulus. Pain ratings after both stressors were increased, indicating hyperalgesia. However, brain activation during pain stimuli after cognitive stress was reduced, whereas activations after emotional stressors were increased.
The pain stimulus used to inflict pain is another factor that determines the directionality of the effect that stress exerts on the pain response. An inescapable pain stimulus, such as capsaicin, causes more distress than an acute pain stimulus, such as thermal heat pain. In the study by Logan et al (59), stress due to a 20 minutes Stroop test followed by capsaicin injection enhanced pain intensity in women only, whereas men exhibited reduced pain.
Stress-induced Analgesia
SIA is well documented in animal studies, and various manipulations have been employed to produce analgesia to pain stimulation, namely stress in the form of footshock (76), swimming (77), novel environment (78) or immobilization (79). Induction of SIA in laboratory animals showed that low levels of stress facilitated learning and responding, whereas high levels disrupted responding (80). Psychological stress and stress resulting from loud noise increased anxiety-like (81) and depressive-like behaviour (82, 83) but decreased working memory functioning (84, 83) in rats.
Animal studies of SIA are considered a model of the anecdotal reports of reduced pain sensation in humans during extreme situations (85). Extensive research on animal responses to stress and pain stimulation is not always an acceptable predictive model for SIA in humans. SIA is not easy to quantify in humans, given the obvious limitations involved in subjecting humans to stress and pain. Furthermore, the animal model is capable of distinguishing the specific pain modalities (86), whereas human pain includes overlapping aspects of specific types of pain. Clearly, the factor that separates the responses observed in animals and those in humans is the higher level cognitive processing that occurs in the human brain, and these in turn are determined by various factors, namely past experiences, learning, and memory moulded by the plasticity of the central nervous system (87).
Early human studies on SIA by Willer used the expectation of noxious stimulation, i.e., a noxious footshock given at 7 to 8 times the pain threshold as the stressor, which inhibited the nociceptive flexion reflex (60, 61). Several other studies have shown evidence of stress-induced analgesia using noxious heat (88) and physical stress (89).
In later studies, however, other means have been used to expose subjects to stress and have included manipulations of psychological (90) and cognitive stress (62). Bandura et al (62) used perceived coping inefficacy in a mathematical task to elicit SIA in a cold pressor test. The analgesia was blocked by naloxone, suggesting the opioidergic system meditated the response. For a review of the underlying mechanisms of SIA, please see Butler and Finn (91).
Stress-induced hyperalgesia
Animal studies have shown that acute stress, such as inescapable holding, (92) and chronic stress, such as repeated swim stress (93,94), actually induced hyperalgesia instead of analgesia. Chronic stress has been shown to attenuate dopaminergic activity in the nucleus accumbens, resulting in hyperalgesia (95). Rats exposed to chronic unavoidable stress exhibited a decrease in dopaminergic tone in the shell of the nucleus accumbens (96). The decrease in dopaminergic tone lasted up to 14 days after the stress exposure. Rats submitted to chronic stress also displayed hyperalgesia for up to 28 days (95). Chronic stress also caused decreased morphine sensitivity, suggesting that the opioidergic systems were modified. Quintero et al (93) demonstrated that hyperalgesia due to an inescapable subchronic stress is resulted from diminished central 5-HT activity. The underlying mechanisms for SIH are reviewed in Jennings et al (97).
A study in children with recurrent abdominal pain showed that stress reduced the pain threshold instead of causing analgesia (71). Chronic pain patients have reported enhanced pain during stress (42). Stress has been shown to exacerbate pain in gastro-oesophageal reflux patients (98). Modulation of oesophageal perception by anxiety, stress and depression cause patients to perceive low intensity oesophageal stimuli as being painful. Studies performed in fibromyalgia patients showed that stress causes an increase in musculoskeletal pain (99). Excessive stress beyond the control of the individual may be followed by dysphoria, reduced functioning (100) and eventually psychiatric (101) or somatic disease (3). Other findings show that prolonged stress causes dysfunction of the HPA axis, causing metabolic derangement that subsequently leads to obesity and other metabolic syndromes, such as Type 2 diabetes and atherosclerosis (102). Stress-reducing strategies have also been used to alleviate pain in patients undergoing surgery (103).
Conclusion
Stress itself is subjective, with different emotions contributing to its manifestations, often with conflicting effects on pain perception as evidenced by the analgesic and hyperalgesic responses. The stress system does not function alone; the genetic and psychological makeup of a person, experience and environmental factors all contribute to the response to pain. The interactions among these factors subsequently result in the sequelae of survival, disease or even death.
Acknowledgments
None.
Footnotes
Conflict of interest
None.
Funds
This work was supported by Universiti Sains Malaysia RUI grant 1001/PPSP/812130.
Authors’ Contributions
None.
References
- 1.Pike IL. Maternal stress and fetal responses: evolutionary perspectives on preterm delivery. Am J Hum Biol. 2005;17(1):55–65. doi: 10.1002/ajhb.20093. doi: doi.org/10.1002/ajhb.20093 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Elenkov IJ, Chrousos GP. Stress system--organization, physiology and immunoregulation. Neuroimmunomodulation. 2006;13(5-6):257–267. doi: 10.1159/000104853. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Tsigos C, Chrousos GP. Hypothalamic-pituitaryadrenal axis, neuroendocrine factors and stress. J Psychosom Res. 2002;53(4):865–871. doi: 10.1016/s0022-3999(02)00429-4. doi: doi.org/10.1016/S0022-3999(02)00429-4 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Selye H. A syndrome produced by diverse nocuous agents. 1936. Nature. 1936;138(32):32. doi: 10.1176/jnp.10.2.230a. doi: 10.1038/138032a0 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Makino S, Hashimoto K, Gold PW. Multiple feedback mechanisms activating corticotropin-releasing hormone system in the brain during stress. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2002;73(1):147–158. doi: 10.1016/s0091-3057(02)00791-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Brotman DJ, Golden SH, Wittstein IS. The cardiovascular toll of stress. Lancet. 2007;370(9592):1089–1100. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(07)61305-1. doi: doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(07)61305-1 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Yamada K, Nabeshima T. Stress-induced behavioral responses and multiple opioid systems in the brain. Behav Brain Res. 1995;67(2):133–45. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(94)00150-e. doi: doi.org/10.1016/0166-4328(94)00150-E . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Marinelli PW, Quirion R, Gianoulakis C. An in vivo profile of beta-endorphin release in the arcuate nucleus and nucleus accumbens following exposure to stress or alcohol. Neuroscience. 2004;127(3):777–84. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2004.05.047. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Contet C, Gavériaux-Ruff C, Matifas A, Caradec C, Champy MF, Kieffer BL. Dissociation of analgesic and hormonal responses to forced swim stress using opioid receptor knockout mice. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2006;31(8):1733–1744. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300934. doi: doi.org/10.1038/sj.npp.1300934 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.McEwen BS. Physiology and neurobiology of stress and adaptation: central role of the brain. Physiol Rev. 2007;87(3):873–904. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00041.2006. doi: doi.org/10.1152/physrev.00041.2006 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Keay KA, Bandler R. Parallel circuits mediating distinct emotional coping reactions to different types of stress. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2001;25(7-8):669–678. doi: 10.1016/s0149-7634(01)00049-5. doi: doi.org/10.1016/S0149-7634(01)00049-5 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Vaughan CW. Stressed-out endogenous cannabinoids relieve pain. Trends Pharmacol Sci. 2006;27(2):69–71. doi: 10.1016/j.tips.2005.11.011. doi: doi.org/10.1016/j.tips.2005.11.011 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Carlson LE, Ursuliak Z, Goodey E, Angen M, Speca M. The effects of a mindfulness meditation-based stress reduction program on mood and symptoms of stress in cancer outpatients: 6-month follow-up. Support Care Cancer. 2001;9(2):112–123. doi: 10.1007/s005200000206. doi: doi.org/10.1007/s005200000206 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Cohen S, Kamarck T, Mermelstein R. A global measure of perceived stress. J Health Soc Behav. 1983;24(4):385–396. doi: doi.org/10.2307/2136404 . [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Salahuddin L, Cho J, Jeong MG, Kim D. Ultra short term analysis of heart rate variability for monitoring mental stress in mobile settings. Conf Proc IEEE Eng Med Biol Soc. 2007:4656–4659. doi: 10.1109/IEMBS.2007.4353378. doi: doi.org/10.1109/IEMBS.2007.4353378 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Takai N, Yamaguchi M, Aragaki T, Eto K, Uchihashi K, Nishikawa Y. Gender-specific differences in salivary biomarker responses to acute psychological stress. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2007;1098:510–515. doi: 10.1196/annals.1384.014. doi: doi.org/10.1196/annals.1384.014 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Brydon L, Wright CE, O'Donnell K, Zachary I, Wardle J, Steptoe A. Stress-induced cytokine responses and central adiposity in young women. Int J Obes (Lond) 2008;32:443–450. doi: 10.1038/sj.ijo.0803767. doi: doi.org/10.1038/sj.ijo.0803767 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Dickerson SS, Mycek PJ, Zaldivar F. Negative social evaluation, but not mere social presence, elicits cortisol responses to a laboratory stressor task. Health Psychol. 2008;27:116–121. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.27.1.116. doi: doi.org/10.1037/0278-6133.27.1.116 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Fechir M, Breimhorst M, Kritzmann S, Geber C, Schlereth T, Baier B, Birklein F. Naloxone inhibits not only stress-induced analgesia but also sympathetic activation and baroreceptor-reflex sensitivity. Eur J Pain. 2012;16(1):82–92. doi: 10.1016/j.ejpain.2011.06.009. doi: doi.org/10.1016/j.ejpain.2011.06.009 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Slawomira JD, Wessa M, Ridder S, Lang S, Diers M, Steil R, Flor H. Enhanced stress analgesia to a cognitively demanding task in patients with posttraumatic stress disorder. J Affect Disord. 2012;136(3):1247–1251. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2011.06.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Ahmad A. Oxford (US): University of Oxford; 2012. The role of the Prefrontal Cortex in Pain Modulation [DPhil thesis] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Kirschbaum C, Pirke KM, Hellhammer DH. The 'Trier Social Stress Test'--a tool for investigating psychobiological stress responses in a laboratory setting. Neuropsychobiology. 1993;28(1-2):76–81. doi: 10.1159/000119004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Jones AK, Derbyshire SW. Reduced cortical responses to noxious heat in patients with rheumatoid arthritis. Ann Rheum Dis. 1997;56:601–607. doi: 10.1136/ard.56.10.601. doi: doi.org/10.1136/ard.56.10.601 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Dedovic K, Renwick R, Mahani NK, Engert V, Lupien SJ, Pruessner JC. The Montreal Imaging Stress Task: using functional imaging to investigate the effects of perceiving and processing psychosocial stress in the human brain. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2005;30(5):319–325. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Girdler SS, Maixner W, Naftel HA, Stewart PW, Moretz RL, Light KC. Cigarette smoking, stress-induced analgesia and pain perception in men and women. Pain. 2005;114(3):372–385. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2004.12.035. doi: doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2004.12.035 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Alexander JK, Hillier A, Smith RM, Tivarus ME, Beversdorf DQ. Beta-adrenergic modulation of cognitive flexibility during stress. J Cogn Neurosci. 2007;19(3):468–478. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.3.468. doi: doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2007.19.3.468 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Peacock EJ, Wong PT. The Stress Appraisal Measure (SAM): A multidimensional approach to cognitive appraisal. Stress Medicine. 1990;6:227–336. [Google Scholar]
- 28.Grillon C, Duncko R, Covington MF, Kopperman L, Kling MA. Acute stress potentiates anxiety in humans. Biol Psychiatry. 2007;62(10):1183–1186. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.06.007. doi: doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.06.007 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Pruessner JC, Dedovic K, Pruessner M, Lord C, Buss C, Collins L, Dagher A, Lupien SJ. Stress regulation in the central nervous system: evidence from structural and functional neuroimaging studies in human populations - 2008 Curt Richter Award Winner. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2010;35(1):179–191. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.02.016. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.02.016 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Schweinhardt P, Bushnell MC. Neuroimaging of pain: insights into normal and pathological pain mechanisms. Neurosci Lett. 2012;520(2):129–130. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2012.06.014. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2012.06.014 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Huettel SA, Song AW, McCarthy G. 1st ed. Sinauer (MA): Associates Inc Sunderland; 2003. Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging. [Google Scholar]
- 32.Porro CA. Functional imaging and pain: behavior, perception, and modulation. Neuroscientist. 2003;9(5):354–369. doi: 10.1177/1073858403253660. doi: doi.org/10.1177/1073858403253660 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Ploghaus A, Narain C, Beckmann CF, Clare S, Bantick S, Wise R, Matthews PM, Rawlins JN, Tracey I. Exacerbation of pain by anxiety is associated with activity in a hippocampal network. J Neurosci. 2001;21(24):9896–9903. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-24-09896.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Kalisch R, Wiech K, Critchley HD, Seymour B, O'Doherty JP, Oakley DA, Allen P, Dolan RJ. Anxiety reduction through detachment: subjective, physiological, and neural effects. J Cogn Neurosci. 2005;17(6):874–883. doi: 10.1162/0898929054021184. doi: doi.org/10.1162/0898929054021184 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Wise RG, Tracey I. The role of fMRI in drug discovery. J Magn Reson Imaging. 2006;23(6):862–876. doi: 10.1002/jmri.20584. doi: 10.1515/revneuro-2014-0031 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Maihöfner C, Ringler R, Herrndobler F, Koppert W. Brain imaging of analgesic and antihyperalgesic effects of cyclooxygenase inhibition in an experimental human pain model: a functional MRI study. Eur J Neurosci. 2007;26(5):1344–1356. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05733.x. doi: doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05733.x . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Ahmad AH, Abdul Aziz CB. The Brain in Pain. Malays J Med Sci. 2014;21(Spec Issue):46–54. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Wang J, Rao H, Wetmore GS, Furlan PM, Korczykowski M, Dinges DF, Detre JA. Perfusion functional MRI reveals cerebral blood flow pattern under psychological stress. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2005;102(49):17804–17809. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0503082102. doi: doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0503082102 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Gazzaniga MS, Ivry RB, Mangun GR. 2nd ed. Cambrige (USA): V MIT Press; 2002. Cognitive Neuroscience. [Google Scholar]
- 40.Davidson RJ, Irwin W, Anderle MJ, Kalin NH. The neural substrates of affective processing in depressed patients treated with venlafaxine. Am J Psychiatry. 2003;160(1):64–75. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.1.64. doi: doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.160.1.64 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Pruessner JC, Dedovic K, Khalili-Mahani N, Engert V, Pruessner M, Buss C, Renwick R, Dagher A, Meaney MJ, Lupien S. Deactivation of the Limbic System During Acute Psychosocial Stress: Evidence from Positron Emission Tomography and Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Studies. Biol Psychiatry . 2008;63(2):234–240. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.04.041. doi: doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.04.041 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Niddam DM, Chan RC, Lee SH, Yeh TC, Hsieh JC. Central representation of hyperalgesia from myofascial trigger point. Neuroimage. 2008;39(3):1299–1306. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.09.051. doi: doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.09.051 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Petrovic P, Petersson KM, Ghatan PH, Stone-Elander S, Ingvar M. Pain-related cerebral activation is altered by a distracting cognitive task. Pain. 2000;85(1-2):19–30. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3959(99)00232-8. doi: doi.org/10.1016/S0304-3959(99)00232-8 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Bantick SJ, Wise RG, Ploghaus A, Clare S, Smith SM, Tracey I. Imaging how attention modulates pain in humans using functional MRI. Brain. 2002;125(Pt 2):310–319. doi: 10.1093/brain/awf022. doi: doi.org/10.1093/brain/awf022 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Valet M, Sprenger T, Boecker H, Willoch F, Rummeny E, Conrad B, Erhard P, Tolle TR. Distraction modulates connectivity of the cingulo-frontal cortex and the midbrain during pain - an fMRI analysis. Pain. 2004;109(3):399–408. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2004.02.033. doi: doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2004.02.033 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Pruessner JC, Champagne F, Meaney MJ, Dagher A. Dopamine release in response to a psychological stress in humans and its relationship to early life maternal care: a positron emission tomography study using [11C]raclopride. J Neurosci. 2004;24(11):2825–31. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3422-03.2004. doi: doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3422-03.2004 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Scott DJ, Heitzeg MM, Koeppe RA, Stohler CS, Zubieta JK. Variations in the human pain stress experience mediated by ventral and dorsal basal ganglia dopamine activity. J Neurosci. 2006;26(42):10789–95. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2577-06.2006. doi: doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2577-06.2006 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Yilmaz P, Diers M, Diener S, Rance M, Wessa M, Flor H. Brain correlates of stress-induced analgesia. Pain. 2010;151:522–529. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2010.08.016. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2010.08.016 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Amit Z, Galina ZH. Stress-induced analgesia: adaptive pain suppression. Physiol Rev. 1986;66(4):1091–1120. doi: 10.1152/physrev.1986.66.4.1091. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Rhudy JL, Williams AE. Gender differences in pain: do emotions play a role? Gend Med. 2005;2(4):208–226. doi: 10.1016/s1550-8579(05)80051-8. doi: doi.org/10.1016/S1550-8579(05)80051-8 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Rhudy JL, Meagher MW. Fear and anxiety: divergent effects on human pain thresholds. Pain. 2000;84(1):65–75. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3959(99)00183-9. doi: doi.org/10.1016/S0304-3959(99)00183-9 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Edwards RR, Haythornthwaite JA, Sullivan MJ, Fillingim RB. Catastrophizing as a mediator of sex differences in pain: differential effects for daily pain versus laboratory-induced pain. Pain. 2004;111(3):335–341. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2004.07.012. doi: doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2004.07.012 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Robinson ME, Dannecker EA, George SZ, Otis J, Atchison JW, Fillingim RB. Sex differences in the associations among psychological factors and pain report: a novel psychophysical study of patients with chronic low back pain. J Pain. 2005;6(7):463–470. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2005.02.007. doi: doi.org/10.1016/j.jpain.2005.02.007 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Hooten WM, Townsend CO, Decker PA. Gender differences among patients with fibromyalgia undergoing multidisciplinary pain rehabilitation. Pain Med. 2007;8(8):624–632. doi: 10.1111/j.1526-4637.2006.00202.x. doi: doi.org/10.1111/j.1526-4637.2006.00202.x . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Ratka A, Simpkins JW. Effects of estradiol and progesterone on the sensitivity to pain and on morphine-induced antinociception in female rats. Horm Behav. 1991;25(2):217–228. doi: 10.1016/0018-506x(91)90052-j. doi: doi.org/10.1016/0018-506X(91)90052-J . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Brill C, Post JS, Ferguson T, Shabsin H, Tenberg J, Wooding K, Lauffer M, Thomas R, Buchmeier MA. Columbia Medical Plan Pain Management Group Manual. Weiner Pain Management – A Practical Guide for Clinicians. In: Richard eds., editor. 6th ed. 2002. pp. 69–70. [Google Scholar]
- 57.Fields HL. Understanding how opioids contribute to reward and analgesia. Reg Anesth Pain Med. 2007;32(3):242–246. doi: 10.1016/j.rapm.2007.01.001. doi: doi.org/10.1016/j.rapm.2007.01.001 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Stoeter P, Bauermann T, Nickel R, Corluka L, Gawehn J, Vucurevic G, Vossel G, Egle UT. Cerebral activation in patients with somatoform pain disorder exposed to pain and stress: an fMRI study. Neuroimage. 2007;36(2):418–430. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.01.052. doi: doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.01.052 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Logan H, Lutgendorf S, Rainville P, Sheffield D, Iverson K, Lubaroff D. Effects of stress and relaxation on capsaicin-induced pain. J Pain. 2001;2(3):160170. doi: 10.1054/jpai.2001.21597. doi: doi.org/10.1054/jpai.2001.21597 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Willer JC, Albe-Fessard D. Electrophysiological evidence for a release of endogenous opiates in stress-induced'analgesia' in man. Brain Res. 1980;198(2):419–426. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(80)90755-6. doi: doi.org/10.1016/0006-8993(80)90755-6 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Willer JC, Dehen H, Cambier J. Stress-induced analgesia in humans: endogenous opioids and naloxone-reversible depression of pain reflexes. Science. 1981;212(4495):689–691. doi: 10.1126/science.6261330. doi: doi.org/10.1126/science.6261330 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Bandura A, Cioffi D, Taylor CB, Brouillard ME. Perceived self-efficacy in coping with cognitive stressors and opioid activation. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1988;55(3):479–488. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.55.3.479. doi: doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.55.3.479 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Janssen SA, Arntz A. Anxiety and pain: attentional and endorphinergic influences. Pain. 1996;66(2-3):145–150. doi: 10.1016/0304-3959(96)03031-x. doi: 10.1016/0304-3959(96)03031-X . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Bragdon EE, Light KC, Girdler SS, Maixner W. Blood pressure, gender, and parental hypertension are factors in baseline and poststress pain sensitivity in normotensive adults . Int J Behav Med. 1997;4(1):17–38. doi: 10.1207/s15327558ijbm0401_2. doi: doi.org/10.1207/s15327558ijbm0401_2 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Caceres C, Burns JW. Cardiovascular reactivity to psychological stress may enhance subsequent pain sensitivity. Pain. 1997;69(3):237–244. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3959(96)03289-7. doi: doi.org/10.1016/S0304-3959(96)03289-7 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Washington LL, Gibson SJ, Helme RD. Age-related differences in the endogenous analgesic response to repeated cold water immersion in human volunteers. Pain. 2000;89(1):89–96. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3959(00)00352-3. doi: doi.org/10.1016/S0304-3959(00)00352-3 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Flor H, Birbaumer N, Schulz R, Grüsser SM, Mucha RF. Pavlovian conditioning of opioid and nonopioid pain inhibitory mechanisms in humans. Eur J Pain. 2002;6(5):395–402. doi: 10.1016/s1090-3801(02)00043-5. doi: doi.org/10.1016/j.rapm.2007.01.001 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.al'Absi M, Petersen KL. Blood pressure but not cortisol mediates stress effects on subsequent pain perception in healthy men and women. Pain. 2003;106(3):285–295. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3959(03)00300-2. doi: doi.org/10.1016/S0304-3959(03)00300-2 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Logan HL, Gedney JJ, Sheffield D, Xiang Y, Starrenburg E. Stress influences the level of negative affectivity after forehead cold pressor pain. J Pain. 2003;4(9):520–529. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2003.09.001. doi: doi.org/10.1016/j.jpain.2003.09.001 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Wiech K, Kalisch R, Weiskopf N, Pleger B, Stephan KE, Dolan RJ. Anterolateral prefrontal cortex mediates the analgesic effect of expected and perceived control over pain. J Neurosci. 2006;26:11501–11509. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2568-06.2006. doi: doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2568-06.2006 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Dufton LM, Konik B, Colletti R, Stanger C, Boyer M, Morrow S, Compas BE. Effects of stress on pain threshold and tolerance in children with recurrent abdominal pain. Pain. 2008;136(1-2):38–43. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2007.06.012. doi: doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2007.06.012 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Strigo IA, Simmons AN, Matthews SC, Craig AD, Paulus MP. Association of major depressive disorder with altered functional brain response during anticipation and processing of heat pain. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2008;65(11):1275–1284. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.65.11.1275. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.65.11.1275 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Kuehl LK, Michaux GP, Richter S, Schächinger H, Anton F. Increased basal mechanical pain sensitivity but decreased perceptual wind-up in a human model of relative hypocortisolism. Pain. 2010;149(3):539–546. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2010.03.026. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2010.03.026 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Gibbons CH, Adler GK, Bonyhay I, Freeman R. Experimental hypoglycemia is a human model of stress-induced hyperalgesia. Pain. 2012;153(11):2204–2209. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2012.06.030. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2012.06.030 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Crettaz B, Marziniak M, Willeke P, Young P, Hellhammer D, Stumpf A, Burgmer M. Stress-induced allodynia--evidence of increased pain sensitivity in healthy humans and patients with chronic pain after experimentally induced psychosocial stress. PLoS One. 2013;8(8):e69460. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0069460. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0069460 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Zou CJ, Liu JD, Zhou YC. Roles of central interleukin-1 on stress-induced-hypertension and footshock-induced-analgesia in rats. Neurosci Lett. 2001;311(1):41–44. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3940(01)02140-1. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3940(01)02140-1 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Hayati AA, Zalina I, Myo T, Badariah AA, Azhar A, Idris L. Modulation of formalin-induced Fos-like Immunoreactivity in the spinal cord by swim stress-induced analgesia, morphine and ketamine. German Medical Science (e-journal) . 2008;6 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Abbott FV, Franklin KBJ, Connell B. The stress of a novel environment reduces formalin pain: possible role of serotonin. Eur. J. Pharmacol. 1986;126:141–144. doi: 10.1016/0014-2999(86)90750-8. doi: doi.org/10.1016/0014-2999(86)90750-8 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Goyal R, Anil K. Protective effect of alprazolam in acute immobilization stress-induced certain behavioral and biochemical alterations in mice. Pharmacol Rep. 2007;59(3):284–290. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Amit Z, Galina ZH. Stess induced analgesia plays an adaptive role in the organization of behavioral responding. Brain Res Bull. 1988;21(6):955–958. doi: 10.1016/0361-9230(88)90033-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Al-Rahbi B, Zakaria R, Othman Z, Hassan A, Ahmad AH. Protective effects of Tualang honey against oxidative stress and anxiety-like behaviour in stressed ovariectomized rats. International Scholarly Research Notices. 2014:10. doi: 10.1155/2014/521065. doi: doi.org/10.1155/2014/521065 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Al-Rahbi B, Zakaria R, Othman Z, Hassan A, Ahmad AH. Enhancement of BDNF concentration and restoration of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis accompany reduced depressive-like behaviour in stressed ovariectomised rats treated with either Tualang honey or estrogen. Scientific World J. 2014;310821 doi: 10.1155/2014/310821. doi: 10.1155/2014/310821 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Azman KF, Zakaria R, Abd Aziz CB, Othman Z, Al-Rahbi B. Tualang honey improves memory performance and decreases depressive-like behavior in rats exposed to loud noise stress. Noise Health. 2015;17(75):83–89. doi: 10.4103/1463-1741.153388. doi: 10.4103/1463-1741.153388 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Al-Rahbi B, Zakaria R, Othman Z, Hassan A, Mohd Ismail ZI, Muthuraju S. Tualang honey supplement improves memory performance and hippocampal morphology in stressed ovariectomized rats. Acta Histochem. 2014;116(1):79–88. doi: 10.1016/j.acthis.2013.05.004. doi: 10.1016/j.acthis.2013.05.004 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Vierck Jr CJ. Animal models of pain. Wall and Melzack’s Textbook of Pain. In: McMahon SB, Koltzenburg M, editors. 5th ed. New York (NY): Elsevier; 2006. [Google Scholar]
- 86.Furst DE, Manning DC. Future directions in pain management. Clin Exp Rheumatol. 2001;19(6 Suppl 25):S71–S76. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Benfenati F. Synaptic plasticity and the neurobiology of learning and memory. Acta Biomed. 2007;78(Suppl 1):58–66. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Willer JC, De Broucker T, Le Bars D. Encoding of nociceptive thermal stimuli by diffuse noxious inhibitory controls in humans. J Neurophysiol. 1989;62(5):1028–1038. doi: 10.1152/jn.1989.62.5.1028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Janal MN, Colt EW, Clark WC, Glusman M. Pain sensitivity, mood and plasma endocrine levels in man following long-distance running: effects of naloxone. Pain. 1984;19(1):13–25. doi: 10.1016/0304-3959(84)90061-7. doi: doi.org/10.1016/0304-3959(84)90061-7 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Flor H, Grüsser SM. Conditioned stress-induced analgesia in humans. Eur J Pain. 1999;3(4):317–324. doi: 10.1053/eujp.1999.0137. doi: doi.org/10.1016/S1090-3801(99)90013-7 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Butler RK, Finn DP. Stress-induced analgesia. Prog Neurobiol. 2009;88(3):184–202. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2009.04.003. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2009.04.003 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Vidal C, Jacob JJ. Stress hyperalgesia in rats: an experimental animal model of anxiogenic hyperalgesia in human. Life Sci. 1982;31:1241–1244. doi: 10.1016/0024-3205(82)90352-6. doi: doi.org/10.1016/0024-3205(82)90352-6 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Quintero L, Moreno M, Avila C, Arcaya J, Maixner W, Suarez-Roca H. Long-lasting delayed hyperalgesia after subchronic swim stress. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2000;67(3):449–458. doi: 10.1016/s0091-3057(00)00374-9. doi: doi.org/10.1016/S0091-3057(00)00374-9 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Suarez-Roca H, Quintero L, Arcaya JL, Maixner W, Rao SG. Stress-induced muscle and cutaneous hyperalgesia: differential effect of milnacipran. Physiol Behav. 2006;88(1-2):82–87. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2006.03.010. doi: doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2006.03.010 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.da Silva Torres IL, Cucco SN, Bassani M, Duarte MS, Silveira PP, Vasconcellos AP, Tabajara AS, Dantas G, Fontella FU, Dalmaz C, Ferreira MB. Long-lasting delayed hyperalgesia after chronic restraint stress in rats-effect of morphine administration. Neurosci Res. 2003;45(3):277–283. doi: 10.1016/s0168-0102(02)00232-8. doi: doi.org/10.1016/S0168-0102(02)00232-8 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Gambarana C, Masi F, Tagliamonte A, Scheggi S, Ghiglieri O, De Montis MG. A chronic stress that impairs reactivity in rats also decreases dopaminergic transmission in the nucleus accumbens: a microdialysis study. J Neurochem. 1999;72(5):2039–2046. doi: 10.1046/j.1471-4159.1999.0722039.x. doi: doi.org/10.1016/S1090-3801(99)90013-7 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Jennings EM, Okine BN, Roche M, Finn DP. Stress-induced hyperalgesia. Prog Neurobiol. 2014;121:1–18. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2014.06.003. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2014.06.003 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Fass R, Tougas G. Functional heartburn: the stimulus, the pain, and the brain. Gut. 2002;51(6):885–892. doi: 10.1136/gut.51.6.885. doi: doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2004.07.012 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Nilsen KB, Westgaard RH, Stovner LJ, Helde G, Rø M, Sand TH. Pain induced by low-grade stress in patients with fibromyalgia and chronic shoulder/neck pain, relation to surface electromyography. Eur J Pain. 2006;10(7):615–627. doi: 10.1016/j.ejpain.2005.10.001. doi: doi.org/10.1016/j.ejpain.2005.10.001 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100.August KJ, Rook KS, Newsom JT. The joint effects of life stress and negative social exchanges on emotional distress. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2007;62(5):S304–S314. doi: 10.1093/geronb/62.5.s304. doi: doi.org/10.1093/geronb/62.5.S304 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.Swaab DF, Bao AM, Lucassen PJ. The stress system in the human brain in depression and neurodegeneration. Ageing Res Rev. 2005;4(2):141–194. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2005.03.003. doi: doi.org/10.1016/j.arr.2005.03.003 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102.Kyrou I, Chrousos GP, Tsigos C. Stress, visceral obesity, and metabolic complications. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2006;1083:77–110. doi: 10.1196/annals.1367.008. doi: doi.org/10.1196/annals.1367.008 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103.Faymonville ME, Mambourg PH, Joris J, Vrijens B, Fissette J, Albert A, Lamy M. Psychological approaches during conscious sedation. Hypnosis versus stress reducing strategies: a prospective randomized study. Pain. 1997;73(3):361–367. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3959(97)00122-X. doi: doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2004.07.012 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]