Abstract
Animals ubiquitously use chemical signals to communicate many aspects of their social life. These chemical signals often consist of environmental cues mixed with species-specific signals—pheromones—emitted by conspecifics. During their life, insects can use pheromones to aggregate, disperse, choose a mate, or find the most suitable food source on which to lay eggs. Before pupariation, larvae of several Drosophila species migrate to food sources depending on their composition and the presence of pheromones. Some pheromones derive from microbiota gut activity and these food-associated cues can enhance larval attraction or repulsion. To explore the mechanisms underlying the preference (attraction/repulsion) to these cues and clarify their effect, we manipulated factors potentially involved in larval response. In particular, we found that the (i) early exposure to conspecifics, (ii) genotype, and (iii) antibiotic treatment changed D. melanogaster larval behavior. Generally, larvae—tested either individually or in groups—strongly avoided food processed by other larvae. Compared to previous reports on larval attractive pheromones, our data suggest that such attractive effects are largely masked by food-associated compounds eliciting larval aversion. The antagonistic effect of attractive vs. aversive compounds could modulate larval choice of a pupariation site and impact the dispersion of individuals in nature.
Introduction
To develop and reproduce, animals need to perceive multimodal sensory signals informing them on the quality of their environment and on the presence and reproductive status of conspecifics [1]. Both environmental and species-specific cues often combine their effect to elicit the behavioral response in the receiver animal [2, 3, 4].
Among the multiple sensory cues used to communicate, chemical signals, or pheromones, are ubiquitously used by animals [5, 6]. In insects, pheromones are involved in many social behaviors occurring from larval to adult life. In adults, they can modulate aggregation, mating and aggression [7, 8, 9]. Pheromones are also important during pre-adult development. For example, larval aggregation in gregarious species often relies on chemical cues mixed in feces [10, 11], whereas in non-social insects, behavior can be influenced by adult chemical cues [12, 13]. Reciprocally, larvae developing in the food can produce chemical cues affecting adult female attraction, oviposition and maternal care [14, 15].
In Drosophila, the effect of adult pheromones has been deeply investigated on adult behavior, especially in D. melanogaster that is amenable to genetic manipulation. In this species, adult pheromones can modulate adult aggregation [16], courtship, mating [17, 18] and aggression [19, 20, 21]. Drosophila pheromones can also affect larval behavior and pupariation. A field study revealed that some chemical cues produced by D. simulans and D. buzzatii species developing in the same fruit can change their pupariation site [22]. These cues, produced both by adults and larvae, induce a repulsive behavior in larvae, this potentially affecting their subsequent choice of a pupariation site [13]. Early developmental exposure to these cues also altered larval and pupal behaviors in the two species [22]. Among the varied compounds inducing larval aggregation on food sources already processed by other larvae, two pheromones were identified together with the neurons involved in their detection [23, 24, 25].
The yeast present on the food can enhance Drosophila response [14] whereas the microbiota hosted in their digestive system [26] can dramatically modulate the quantity and quality of the chemicals produced by both larvae and adults and therefore strongly impact their behavior. This is probably the reason why antibiotic-treated Drosophila flies showed a substantially altered mating behavior [27].
Here, we explored some of the mechanisms underlying the behavioral effect induced by larval pheromones in order to determine whether these cues have an attractive or a repulsive effect. We manipulated several biological factors potentially involved in this response, such as group-effect and the influence of early developmental exposure to larval chemicals (Fig 1). To generalize our findings, and assess the accuracy of larval response, we compared the response of two species and of several wild-type and transgenic strains, raised either separately or mixed together and we also measured the effect of antibiotics on larval and pupariation behaviors.
Results
A- Response in D. melanogaster Wild-Type Strains
1- Canton-S strain: group tests
We first tested the response in groups of 10 larvae in the Canton-S (Cs) wild-type strain. Cs larvae, produced by a mass-rearing procedure, were given a two-food patches choice and their preference to either patch was measured during 30 min (Fig 1A). Their pupariation site was also determined, few days later. In the control test involving two plain food (PF) patches, a total of 70% Cs larvae—and later 20% pupae—aggregated on the patches without showing any preference (Fig 2A).
The experimental test consisted to pair one PF patch with a second patch impregnated with the food processed by Cs larvae (Cs larval food: CsLF). After 5 min of test, Cs larvae showed a preference for PF (and avoided CsLF), and this marked difference increased with time (Fig 2B). At 30 min, 67% larvae aggregated on both food patches with a clear preference to PF compared to CsLF (45.7 and 21.3%, respectively), whereas pupae were equally distributed on the two food patches. This indicates that Cs larvae, but not pupae, avoid the food labeled with their own food-derived compounds.
2- Dijon2000 strain: group and individual tests
To generalize this finding, we assayed larvae of a second wild-type strain, Dijon2000 (Di2). Mass-reared Di2 larvae tested in groups (of 10) showed no preference in the PF/PF control assay (data not shown) whereas in the experimental dual choice-test associating PF with DiLF (LF processed by Di2 larvae), they clearly preferred PF over DiLF (at 30 min: 64 and 24%, respectively; p<0.01; Fig 3A).
To detect for a potential group effect, we performed tests with individual Di2 larvae (also produced by a mass-rearing procedure). In the “PF/PF” control test, 84% larvae migrated on either PF patch without showing any preference (data not shown) whereas in the experimental “PF/DiLF” test, individual Di2 larvae preferred PF (p<0.001; Fig 3B). Larvae first oriented more frequently to the PF patch (N = 68) than the DiLF patch (N = 12; Two-tailed Poisson binomial test: p = 0.0107) but they took the same time to reach either patch (378±49 and 266±43 sec, respectively; Mann-Whitney test: Z = 1.1209; p = 0.263). Also, individual larvae spent more time on the PF patch than on the DiLF patch (1133±91 and 721±108 sec, respectively; Mann-Whitney test: Z = 2.737; p = 0.0061). Also, more individuals pupariated on the PF (47%) than on the DiLF (22%) patches, differently to Di2 tested in groups (Fig 3A). These data show that Di2 larvae, similarly to Cs larvae, are strongly repulsed by their own food-derived compounds. For the next tests, we preferentially used the Di2 strain whose larval global response was higher than in Cs strain. For the sake of clarity, we only show the larval distribution at 30 min represented as box and whisker plots. The detailed data indicating the dynamic response together with the pupariation site choice can be found in the Supporting Information section.
B- Effect of Early Experience
Based on earlier results obtained with other Drosophila species [22], we checked the effect of early developmental exposure to conspecifics. We raised Di2 larvae individually on fresh food (since their early embryonic development) and subsequently tested them by groups (of 10). The total number of larvae aggregating on food patches at 30 min was similar in the two raising conditions (87±5%; Fig 4). However, differently from mass-reared larvae (Fig 4A), larvae raised in isolation showed no food preference (Figs 1B and 4B; S1 Fig). In either case, no preference was detected in pupae. This experiment indicates a clear effect of the early exposure to larval-processed food.
C- Intra- and Interspecific Variations
1- Intra-specific variation
Our next aim consisted to search potential effects related to population-specific variation with regard to production of food-derived chemical cues. To test this, we compared the response of four strains: two wild-type strains (Cs and Di2), and two desat1 mutant lines altered for fatty acid metabolism (Fig 5). The original desat1-1573 strain is homozygous for an insertion in the desat1 gene (Desat). This is also the case for the desat1-GFP mutant line which additionally contains the UAS-GFP reporter transgene (GFP; useful for larva sorting; see next section). We assayed all 16 possible larval x food combinations to evaluate the behavioral effects related to (i) each larval experience, and (ii) the behavioral effect induced by food-derived compounds. Therefore, the four larvae (Di2, Cs, Desat, GFP = four rows of Fig 5) were presented to a dual choice-test always combining PF with each of the four LFs respectively processed by these larvae (DiLF, CsLF, DsLF, GfpLF = four columns of Figs 1D and 5). All larvae showed a preference for PF with some slight dynamic variation (S2 Fig). This indicates the general repulsive effect of larval-processed food either tested to their own strain (diagonal items on Fig 5) or to other strains.
To detect more subtle larval discrimination ability between compounds processed by different strains, Cs, Di2 and Desat larvae were given a simultaneous choice of two LFs (Figs 1D and 6). No preference was observed, except for Di2 larvae which slightly preferred DsLF over DiLF, at 30 min (p<0.05). Note the overall low number (<35%) of Cs and Desat larvae on CsLF/DiLF patches after 30 min (S3 Fig). This indicates that Di2 larvae show an acute discrimination of LFs and that when two LFs are simultaneously used, they induced an increased repulsive effect on larvae.
2- Mixed population cultures
Based on the ability of Di2 larvae to discriminate between DsLF and DiLF (Fig 7), we further explored the mechanistic origin of this difference. Based on the clear early food-exposure effect (Fig 4) [22], we attempted to quantitatively change the amount of population-specific compounds. Practically, we raised Di2 and GFP larvae together in different proportions. Then, we measured the preference of each co-cultured larvae in a dual choice associating PF with either DiLF or GfpLF (the GFP fluorescent larvae were separated from the Di2 larvae under UV light; Fig 1C). The two « pure » populations were used as controls (for GFP larvae, see left row data below 0% Di2 / 100% GFP; for Di2 larvae, see right row data below 100% Di2 / 0% GFP; Fig 7; S4 Fig). Most Di2 and GFP larvae raised in mixed cultures (with the 10/90, 50/50 and 90/10 ratios) showed a clear PF preference after 30 min. Only Di2 larvae raised in the 10/90 ratio (10% Di2 / 90% 1573-GFP) showed no PF preference in both tests. Therefore, when larvae of two genotypes were raised together, their subsequent food preference could be altered depending upon their respective frequency.
3- Intra- and interspecific responses
Based both on the Cs vs. Di2 intraspecific variation (Fig 6; S3 Fig) and on the relative conservation of larval pheromones between D. melanogaster and D. simulans [25], we performed cross-experiments between the two D. melanogaster strains and one D. simulans strain. More precisely, we measured the responses (i) of Cs and Di2 larvae to D. simulans LF (SimLF) and reciprocally (ii) of D. simulans larvae to CsLF or to DiLF (Fig 8; S5 Fig). Interestingly, Cs larvae, but not Di2 larvae, preferred PF over SimLF (Fig 8A) whereas, in the reciprocal experiment, D. simulans larvae preferred PF over both CsLF and DiLF (Fig 8B). This behavioral difference indicates that the production and/or perception of food-derived compounds vary between both D. melanogaster strains.
D- Effect of Antibiotics
Given the effect induced by antibiotic treatment on adult behavior [27], we investigated the behavioral effect of Ampicillin, Tetracycline, Rifampicin and Streptomycin during pre-adult development. These substances are know to affect different types of bacteria (Gram-positive and -negative bacteria; www.sigmaaldrich.com). We separately measured whether each of these four antibiotics added in the food could affect the behavior of such treated Di2 larvae (four rows in Fig 9; S6 Fig) in response to the four LFs respectively processed by these treated larvae (four columns in Fig 9; S6 Fig: AmLF, TeLF, RiLF and StLF). In most experimental tests, larvae clearly preferred PF over LFs. However, in Rifampicin-treated larvae, this preference decreased (in tests involving PF against AmLF or StLF) or disappeared (PF vs. TeLF or RiLF). Differently, larval PF preference was not altered in all control tests either involving (i) Di2 larvae presented to the four treated LFs, or (ii) the four antibiotic-treated larvae presented to DiLF (Fig 10; S7 Fig; see also Fig 1D). Therefore, antibiotics can induce very different effects, and more specially Rifampicin, with regard to the production and/or perception of larval food-derived compounds.
Discussion
General Effect of Food-Derived Pheromones on Larval Behavior
Our principal goal consisted to measure larval food preference to food-derived molecules, some of which acting as pheromones [13], and to determine whether larvae were attracted or repulsed by such compounds. Moreover, we explored some of the mechanisms underlying both the production and behavioral response to larval compounds. Overall, we found that most larvae preferred plain food (PF) whereas they avoided larval processed food (LF). The fact that the total number of larvae present on the two food patches was similar in the control (PF/PF) and in the experimental tests (PF/LF) indicates that a similar number of larvae responded to either food patch whereas the difference of distribution between the two patches was largely due to the compounds present in most LFs inducing a repulsive effect. The total number of larvae dramatically decreased when two LFs were combined indicating a enhanced aversive effect induced by two patches (Fig 6). Larval aversion against LF started after 5 to 10 min in wild-type larvae tested to wild-type processed LFs, or several minutes later with some manipulated larvae and/or LFs. The number of larvae on LF patches was generally stable after 5 min whereas the number on PF patches increased until 15 to 30 min (S1–S7 Figs). The repulsive effect of LF was not due to a potential group-effect bias, at least in the Di2 strain (Fig 3) [28].
Divergence with Previous Reports
Strikingly, the repulsive effect on larvae of LF-associated compounds found here diverges from the attractive response described in previous reports [22, 24, 25]. Such discrepancy can be partly explained by the divergence of methods and parameters. It appears that D. simulans and D. buzzatii larvae individually tested [22]) differently responded than when tested in groups [13] (this report; Fig 8). Although no group-effect affected the response of the D. melanogaster Di2 strain (Fig 3), we cannot exclude the possibility that a similar effect exists in other Drosophila species or populations. A second study measuring the initial attraction in single larva and the food consumed by groups during several hours reported a higher performance on LF [24]). Differently, “our” larvae which showed more initial attraction on PF than to LF, did not show faster migration to either patch. This seems to support the olfactory effect of LF-associated cues [29] revealing, in our case, that these cues did not induce an attractive effect but rather a repulsive one. A third study performed with groups of larvae reported an attractive effect for two larval pheromones [(Z)-5- and (Z)-7-tetradecenoic acids: 5TD and 7TD, respectively] when separately tested on agarose [25]. However, given that when the two compounds were combined, they induced no attraction, it is thus possible that their interaction (eventually in addition to other food-derived compounds not present in agarose but in plain food) induced a different effect. Indeed, the complex food-derived mixture induced a clear aversive effect ([13]; this report). This indicates that in our experiments, the larval attractive effect of 5TD or of 7TD (separately tested) is partially masked by the global repulsive effect induced by other food-derived compounds additionally the PF context.
Effect of Experience
Similarly to D. simulans and D. buzzatii species [22], the preference of D. melanogaster larvae was altered depending on their early exposure to their own transformed food. Another report showed a “learning” effect enhancing larval attraction to their own labeled food [24]. Differently, in our hands, early exposure did not induce attraction, but rather suppressed repulsion and/or induced indifference ([30]; Fig 4). Given that the relative behavioral valence between the two experiments varied in the same direction (increased attractiveness = decreased aversiveness), the absolute difference between both experiments may be only due to the food context (see above). Moreover, some of our effects could be also caused by social interaction. Since we did not dissociate the « chemical » from the « social » informations, we cannot exclude this possibility. However, the fact that Di2 larvae (raised in the 10/90 ratio with GFP larvae; Fig 7; S4 Fig) were not repulsed by LFs suggests that larval aversive behavior normally relies more on chemical than on social information. Based on this, we believe that the difference observed between mass- and individually-reared larvae was simply due to the quantitative variation of food-derived compounds released in the food by larvae. This means that exposure to a low amount of chemicals (produced by a single larvae) was not sufficient to induce an effect differently to that induced by mass-reared larvae which produced—and were exposed to—higher amounts of these compounds. Whatever factor(s) is (are) involved, the clear avoidance of mass-reared larvae to their own compounds demonstrates the strong effect of prior exposure to these compounds. Together, our results indicate that larval migration depends on a mixture of compounds that can either induce attractive or repulsive effects, the combination of which induces food preference. Early developmental exposure to the repulsive cues may be more potent than exposure to the attractive cues, this explaining why the early exposure of Di2 larvae to some treatments (such as Rifampicin; Fig 9; see below) induced no subsequent, or less, larval repulsion to LFs.
Effect of Genotype and Microbiota
Beside this clear “early-exposure effect”, our other experiments show that the use of different populations or antibiotic treatments can differently alter larval behaviors. This may result of a variable developmental impregnation induced by different food-derived compounds.
We also report intraspecific difference between wild-type strains (Cs and Di2; Figs 6 and 8) or wild-type and transgenic strains (Fig 7). In particular, the asymetric “loss” of discrimination of 10% Di2 larvae—but not of GFP larvae—raised with 90% larvae of the other genotype indicates a variation for food-derived compounds between the two strains.
Populations (or genotypes) of a same species can also vary for their microbiota profile [26], this largely impacting the quality and quantity of food-derived compounds. The selective elimination of some bacteria clearly affects larval attraction [31], and this may explain the contrasted effects induced by our different antibiotic treatments. In particular, the altered response of Rifampicin-treated larvae and pupae may result of an abnormal production of aversive compounds that are normally perceived and learned by larvae during their early development. This may explain why larvae and pupae, not imprinted by these compounds, have lost their preference to PF (or their aversion against LF). Given that larvae treated with the three other antibiotics, as well as control larvae, showed a normal response to RiLF, this suggests that these compounds should be associated with other(s) chemical(s) to allow a correct associative learning process [29].
Ecological Relevance
We do not know yet whether the neurological mechanisms underlying this effect fit with those described for other types of larval learning [32]. Such learning process may involve the association between food and food-derived compounds, or pheromones, as in mammals [33]. In Drosophila, the food-derived pheromones produced by developing larvae could increase the salience of some food components subsequently associated with these pheromones. Early developmental chemosensory exposure could also change the adult preference to similar food compounds. This was observed with pure fatty acids which, after early larval exposure, changed female food preference for egg-laying behavior [34]. This is relevant to our study since larvae produce several fatty acids in LF [13]. Moreover, the larva-to-adult persistence of the « food-pheromone » preference could be transmitted to the next generation if females resulting of exposed larvae continuously lay eggs on the same preferred substrate. This effect, iteratively repeated generation after generation, could promote the selection of some of the genes underlying « food-pheromone » chemosensory preference, this explaining some tight plant-insect relationships.
Material and Methods
Strains
Drosophila strains were raised in 150 ml glass vials containing 50ml of yeast/cornmeal/agar medium and kept in a breeding room at 24.5±0.5°C with 65±5% humidity on a 12:12h light/dark cycle (subjective day from 8:00am to 8:00pm). Flies were transferred every two/three days to avoid larval competition and to regularly provide abundant progeny for testing. All behavioral experiments were performed under similar conditions.
We used two D. melanogaster wild-type strains: Canton-S (Cs), a old-established strain widely used in fly laboratories, and Dijon 2000 (Di2), a strain maintained in our lab for 15 years which showed very stable behavioral performances.
We also used two transgenic strains: desat1-1573-Gal4 (abbreviated as Desat) and desat1-1573-Gal4; UAS-CD8-GFP (abbreviated “1573-GFP” or “GFP” in the text). The two strains are homozygous for a P-Gal4 element inserted in the desat1 gene regulatory region [35] potentially affecting larval metabolism [36] with potential consequence on the production of food-derived compounds. Additionally to the P-Gal4 elements, the 1573-GFP strain also contains two copies of the UAS-CD8-GFP reporter transgene which shows fluorescent expression in the tissues where desat1-1573-Gal4 is expressed [37].
The D. simulans strain (gift of Prof. Raùl Godoy-Herrera, Santiago, University of Chile) was derived from multi-female lines originated from fruits collected in Chile [22] and maintained in our laboratory for more than four years before testing.
Behavior
Preparation of experiments
Two hours before the experiment, the food patches to be tested (Whatman paper grade 42, 1.5 cm diameter, pre-washed in distilled water, in ethanol, and dried overnight at 70°C) were impregnated either with plain food (PF), or with food processed by larvae of mixed ages (1st instar (L1) to 3rd instar (L3)) resulting of mass mating (larval food = LF). Adults were rapidly removed after egg-laying. Therefore, apart larval cues, LFs could contain chemical traces left by adults [13]. However, we can exclude the effect of adult cues on larval repulsion since the mixed-culture tests involving the transfer of eggs to fresh PF showed the same global aversive effect (Fig 7).
Behavioral parameters
Tests were always performed with early L3 individuals, between 12:30 and 17:00, under white light at 24.5±0.5°C. Just before the test, L3 were separated from the food and maintained in distilled water. Tests were carried out in freshly prepared Petri dishes (9.5 cm diameter, 1 cm high) containing a 2% water agar layer (thickness 5 mm). Each Petri dish contained two food patches (separated by approximately 40 mm and placed in diametrically opposed zones) impregnated with various food types (see above). Food-patches—from which excess food was removed with a spatula—were separately pinned down on the agar dish with a thin needle. Depending on the test, a group of ten larvae, or a solitary larva, was transferred, using a fine brush, at a mid-distance between the patches. Then, the dish was covered with a lid to reduce evaporation of the tested media and the time of observation started. For the “individual larva” experiment, the time to reach the first chosen patch and the duration spent on the patch was noted. For group experiments, the number of larvae present on each patch was noted each min for the first 5 min of the experiment, and then every 5 min until 30 min. The 30 min duration was determined based on a previous report [13]. Petri dishes containing assayed larvae were further kept at 24.5±0.5°C for the next two days and the number of pupae on each patch was noted.
In control experiments, we tested the response to a pair of patches impregnated with P-food. In other experiments, we either paired one P-food patch with one “impregnated-food” patch, or two impregnated-food patches. Control and “impregnated-food” experiments were simultaneously performed.
Statistics
For each group experiment, we assessed the statistical difference for larval (and pupal) distribution between both food patches using a Wilcoxon test (XLStats) with the R® sofware. For individual tests, we used the binomial Poisson distribution test. In the principal section, and for the sake of clarity, we only show the larval distribution on the two food patches at 30 min, as box and wisker plots. In the Supporting Information section, we show the distribution at all time-points (+ pupae) to better visualize the dynamic response of larvae, but for the experiments involving many conditions, we only show four time-points (3, 5, 15 and 30 min) as representative data points. These histograms show the mean (± sem) of 10–31 experiments except for individual larva tests (N = 100) and always correspond to pooled experiments obtained on several days.
Manipulation of Biological Factors
Social deprivation
In most experiments, we tested individuals issued from mass-reared culture except in one experiment (Fig 4B) where we tested the response of larva raised in isolation. In this case, freshly laid eggs (4±1 hours) were individually deposited in a fresh food vial and kept until early L3 stage for behavioral testing.
Mixed cultures
For mixed cultures associating Di2 and desat1-1573-GFP larvae, a controlled number of eggs of each genotype was deposited in a fresh food vials following the 10/90, 50/50 and 90/10 ratio to obtain a overall number of 100 egg/vial. Experiments involving “pure” Di2 and desat1-1573-GFP strains were also performed following our standard procedure (Fig 1C). Few hours before the test, larvae were sorted by genotypes according to the presence/absence of fluorescence in their body. This separation was carried out (without anaesthesia) with a fine brush under a Leica stereomicroscope MZ12 equipped with a fluorescent light emitter.
Antibiotic treatments
Antibiotic food was prepared with 100 ml PF initially put to a boil and then cooled down before addition of each antibiotic (present as a powder). For this amount of plain food, we used either 50mg Ampicillin, 20mg Rifampicin, 10mg Streptomycin ou 5mg Tetracycline (Sigma). Di2 females were allowed to lay eggs on each antibiotic-rich food and two days later, they were removed out of the vials. These four antibiotics were chosen based on (i) their effect on Drosophila adult behavior [27] and (ii) their complementary spectrum of bactericide effect (in particular for their antimicrobial activity against gram-positive and -negative bacteria; www.sigmaaldrich.com).
Supporting Information
Acknowledgments
Serge Loquin and José Solonot are thanked for their technical help, Claude Everaerts for help with the statistics, and John Ewer for reading a part of the ms. Two anonymous reviewers are also thanked for their constructive comments to improve the ms.
Data Availability
All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files.
Funding Statement
Funded by Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (Insb) http://www.cnrs.fr/insb/ (JPF, JFF and JC), Burgundy Regional Council (PARI 2012) http://www.region-bourgogne.fr/ (JFF), UNiversité de Bourgogne http://www.u-bourgogne.fr/ (JT), Agence nationale de la Recherche (GUSTAILE2012) http://www.agence-nationale-recherche.fr/ (JFF), and CONICYT (MEC80140013) http://www.conicyt.cl/ (JFF). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
References
- 1.North G, Greenspan RJ (2007) Invertebrate Neurobiology. Cold Spring Harbor Monograph Series 2007.
- 2.Czaczkes TJ, Grüter C, Jones SM, Ratnieks FL (2011) Synergy between social and private information increases foraging efficiency in ants. Biol Letters 7: 521–524. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Grosjean Y, Rytz R, Farine JP, Abuin L, Cortot J, Jefferis GSXE et al. (2011) An olfactory receptor for food-derived odours promotes male courtship in Drosophila. Nature 478: 236–240. 10.1038/nature10428 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Secondi J, Okassa M, Sourice S, Thery M (2014) Habitat-dependent species recognition in hybridizing newts. Evol Biology 41: 71–80. [Google Scholar]
- 5.Dulac C, Wagner S (2006) Genetic analysis of brain circuits underlying pheromone signaling. Annu Rev Genet 40: 449–467. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Wyatt TD (2014) Pheromones and Animal Behaviour, Chemical Signals and Signatures, (Cambridge University Press, Cambridge: ) 2nd Ed. [Google Scholar]
- 7.Hassanali A, Njagi PGN, Bashir MO (2005) Chemical ecology of locusts and related acridids. Annu Rev Entomol 50: 223–245. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Gomez-Diaz C, Benton R (2013) The joy of sex pheromones. Embo Rep 14: 874–883. 10.1038/embor.2013.140 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Bubak AN, Grace JL, Watt MJ, Renner KJ, Swallow JG (2014) Neurochemistry as a bridge between morphology and behavior: Perspectives on aggression in insects. Curr Zool 60: 778–790. [Google Scholar]
- 10.Flowers RW, Costa JT (2003) Larval communication and group foraging dynamics in the red-headed pine sawfly, Neodiprion lecontei (Fitch) (Hymenoptera: Symphyta: Diprionidae). Annals Entomol Soc Amer 96: 336–343. [Google Scholar]
- 11.Pires HHR, Lorenzo MG, Diotaiuti L, Lazzari CR., Lorenzo-Figueiras A. N (2002) Aggregation behaviour in Panstrongylus megistus and Triatoma infestans: inter and intraspecific responses. Acta Tropica 81: 47–52. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Poivet E, Rharrabe K, Monsempes C, Glaser N, Rochat D, Renou M et al. (2012) The use of the sex pheromone as an evolutionary solution to food source selection in caterpillars. Nat. Commun. 3:1047 10.1038/ncomms2050 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Farine JP, Cortot J, Ferveur JF (2014) Larval and adult chemical cues affect larval food preference and pupariation behavior in two Drosophila species. Proc Biol Sci / The Royal Society 281: 20140043. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Witzgall P, Proffit M, Rozpedowska E, Becher PG, Andreadis S, Coracini M et al. (2012) « This is not an apple »–yeast mutualism in codling moth. J Chem Ecol 38: 949–957. 10.1007/s10886-012-0158-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Mas F, Koelliker M (2008) Maternal care and offspring begging in social insects: chemical signaling, hormonal regulation and evolution. Anim Behav 76: 1121–1131. [Google Scholar]
- 16.Bartelt RJ, Schaner AM, Jackson LL (1985) Cis-vaccenyl acetate as an aggregation pheromone in Drosophila melanogaster. J Chem Ecol 11: 1747–1756. 10.1007/BF01012124 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Ferveur JF (2005) Cuticular hydrocarbons: Evolution and role in the pheromonal communication of Drosophila. Behav Genet 35: 279–295. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Thistle R, Cameron P, Ghorayshi A, Dennison L, Scott K. (2012) Contact chemoreceptors mediate male-male repulsion and male-female attraction during Drosophila courtship. Cell 149:1140–1151. 10.1016/j.cell.2012.03.045 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Dahanukar A, Ray A (2011) Courtship, aggression and avoidance: pheromones, receptors and neurons for social behaviors in Drosophila. Fly 5: 58–63. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Liu W, Liang X, Gong J, Yang Z, Zhang YH, Zhang JX et al. (2011) Social regulation of aggression by pheromonal activation of Or65a olfactory neurons in Drosophila. Nature Neuroscience 14: 896–902. 10.1038/nn.2836 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Fernandez MP, Kravitz EA (2013) Aggression and courtship in Drosophila: pheromonal communication and sex recognition. J Comp Physiol A 199: 1065–1076. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Beltrami M, Medina-Munoz MC, Del Pino F, Ferveur JF, Godoy-Herrera R (2012) Chemical cues influence pupation behavior of Drosophila simulans and Drosophila buzzatii in nature and in the laboratory. PLoS One 7: e39393 10.1371/journal.pone.0039393 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Beltrami M, Medina-Munoz MC, Arce D, Godoy-Herrera R (2010) Drosophila pupation behavior in the wild. Evolutionary Ecology 24: 347–358. [Google Scholar]
- 24.Durisko Z, Dukas R (2013) Attraction to and learning from social cues in fruit fly larvae. Proc Biol Sci / The Royal Society 280:20131398. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Mast JD, De Moraes CM, Alborn HT, Lavis LD, Stern DL (2014) Evolved differences in larval social behavior mediated by novel pheromones. Elife 1014 Dec 12, 3: e04205 10.7554/eLife.04205 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Newell PD, Douglas AE (2014) Interspecies interactions determine the impact of the gut microbiota on nutrient allocation in Drosophila melanogaster. Applied Environ Microbiol 80: 788–796. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Sharon G, Segal D, Ringo JM, Hefetz A, Zilber-Rosenberg I, Rosenberg E (2010) Commensal bacteria play a role in mating preference of Drosophila melanogaster. Proc Nat Acad Sci USA 107: 20051–20056. 10.1073/pnas.1009906107 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Allee WC, Bowen ES (1932) Studies in animal aggregations: mass protection against colloidal silver among goldfishes. J Exp Zool 61:185–207. [Google Scholar]
- 29.Del Pino F, Jara C, Pino L, Godoy-Herrera R (2014) The Neuro-ecology of Drosophila pupation behavior. PLoS One 9(7): e102159 10.1371/journal.pone.0102159 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Cobb M, Bruneau S, Jallon JM (1992) Genetic and developmental factors in the olfactory response of Drosophila melanogaster larvae to alcohols. Proc R Soc Lond B 248:103–109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Venu I, Durisko Z, Xu J, Dukas R (2014) Social attraction mediated by fruit flies' microbiome. J Exp Biol 217: 1346–1352. 10.1242/jeb.099648 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Gerber B, Stocker RF (2007) The Drosophila larva as a model for studying chemosensation and chemosensory learning: A review. Chem Senses 32: 65–89. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Coureaud G, Moncomble AS, Montigny D, Dewas M, Perrier G, Schaal B (2006) A pheromone that rapidly promotes learning in the newborn. Curr Biol 16: 1956–1961. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Flaven-Pouchon J, Garcia T, Abed-Vieillard D, Farine JP, Ferveur JF, Everaerts C (2014) Transient and permanent experience with fatty acids changes Drosophila melanogaster behaviour and fitness. PLoS One 9: e92352 10.1371/journal.pone.0092352 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Marcillac F, Bousquet F, Alabouvette J, Savarit F, Ferveur JF (2005) A mutation with major effects on Drosophila melanogaster sex pheromones. Genetics 171: 1617–1628. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Baker KD, Thummel CS (2007) Diabetic Larvae and Obese Flies—Emerging Studies of Metabolism in Drosophila. Cell Metabolism 6: 257–266. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Marcillac F, Grosjean Y, Ferveur JF (2005) A single mutation alters production and discrimination of Drosophila sex pheromones. Proc R Soc London B 272: 303–309. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files.