Abstract
The associations between snack food consumption, parent feeding practices and general parenting in overweight in obese children are largely unknown. Therefore, we examined these relationships in 117 treatment-seeking overweight and obese children (10.40 ± 1.35 years; 53% female; 52% Caucasian; BMI-z: 2.06 ± 0.39). Children consumed a dinner meal, completed an Eating in the Absence of Hunger (EAH) free access paradigm (total EAH intake=EAH%-total; sweet food intake=EAH%-sweet), and completed the Child Report of Parent Behavior Inventory. Parents completed the Child Feeding Questionnaire. Child EAH%-total and EAH%-sweet were positively associated with dinner consumption (p’s<.01). Girls had significantly higher EAH%-total compared to boys (p<.05). In separate models, higher EAH%-total was associated with greater use of maternal psychological control (p<.05) and EAH%-sweet was positively associated with parent monitoring (p<.05). In analyses examining factors associated with the consumption of specific foods, EAH snack food, parent restriction, pressure to eat, monitoring, and maternal psychological control were positively correlated with intake of Hershey’s® chocolate bars (p’s<.05). In summary, parental monitoring is associated with child sweet snack food intake and maternal psychological control is associated with child total snack food consumption. Future research should evaluate the complex relationship between child eating and parenting, especially with regard to subgroups of foods.
Keywords: childhood obesity, overeating, parent feeding practices
Introduction
Childhood overweight and obesity affects approximately one-third of all children in the United States, impacting an estimated nine million children (Ogden, Carroll, Kit, & Flegal, 2014). Unfortunately, child eating behaviors and weight status have been shown to remain consistent well into adulthood (Craigie, Lake, Kelly, Adamson, & Mathers, 2011; Singh, Mulder, Twisk, van Mechelen, & Chinapaw, 2008). Although children who are overweight are often assumed to be a homogeneous group, there is a growing interest in defining behavioral phenotypes to ultimately explore etiological mechanisms and to develop targeted treatments (Boutelle, et al., 2014; Field, Camargo, & Ogino, 2013). Excessive intake of highly palatable calorie dense foods is one of the most proximal causes of rising obesity rates during the past three decades (Swinburn, et al., 2009). The current obesogenic environment, which provides continuous access to highly palatable foods in combination with limited physical activity options, may promote overeating especially among children that experience higher levels of food cue responsivity or have a tendency to engage in disinhibited eating behaviors. Thus, it is crucial to identify the factors that contribute to overeating in children in order to better determine mechanisms to target in prevention and treatment interventions.
Eating in the absence of hunger (EAH), a measure of disinhibited eating, has been implicated in the behavioral etiological pathway of obesity in children (Birch, Fisher, & Davison, 2003a; Faith, et al., 2006). EAH is typically measured using a laboratory paradigm that evaluates the amount of food consumed during a free access snack session after a meal (Birch, Fisher, & Davison, 2003b). Studies using the EAH paradigm have shown that EAH was positively related to girls’ weight-for-height (Cutting, Fisher, Grimm-Thomas, & Birch, 1999) and overweight status among five and seven year old girls, even after adjusting for sex and age-based intake requirements (Fisher & Birch, 2002). In a Hispanic-only sample of 5–18 year olds, overweight children consumed 6.5% more calories during the EAH paradigm than non-overweight children (Fisher, Cai, et al., 2007). When comparing intake across discordant weight siblings, older overweight and obese siblings consumed more calories in an EAH paradigm compared to both older and younger normal weight siblings (Kral, et al., 2012). Finally, longitudinal studies of overweight girls have found that EAH increased over eight years regardless of weight-status (Francis, Ventura, Marini, & Birch, 2007); in addition, greater increases in EAH were observed two (Fisher & Birch, 2002) and four years later (Shunk & Birch, 2004) in longitudinal studies of overweight five-year-old girls. Conversely, some studies have found no significant relationships between EAH and weight gain over a one-year time period after controlling for baseline weight, age, sex, and pubertal status (Butte, et al., 2007). For example, several studies with 7–12 year old children (Moens & Braet, 2007) found that increased weight was associated with decreased EAH food consumption. It was speculated that high social desirability, particularly among girls, may explain this inverse relationship (Hill, et al., 2008). Given the lack of agreement in studies regarding the relationship between weight and EAH intake (Butte, et al., 2007; Hill, et al., 2008; Moens & Braet, 2007), and that not all overweight children universally display this behavior (Hill, et al., 2008; Sonneville, et al., 2013), EAH may represent a unique disinhibited eating behavior within the heterogeneous obese population.
Some limitations in the current cross-sectional and longitudinal studies may explain the mixed findings regarding the association between EAH and weight. For instance, not all studies estimated energy intake equations to calculate energy consumed based on age and sex during the EAH snack paradigm (Fisher & Birch, 2002; Fisher, Liu, Birch, & Rolls, 2007; Kral, et al., 2012; Shunk & Birch, 2004). Furthermore, the majority of studies have evaluated EAH across weight status, including both normal weight and overweight children (Birch, et al., 2003b; Butte, et al., 2007; Faith, et al., 2006; Fisher & Birch, 2002; Fisher, Cai, et al., 2007; Spruijt-Metz, Lindquist, Birch, Fisher, & Goran, 2002). Cross-sectional studies that compare overweight and normal weight groups are valuable for illustrating general characteristic differences between these populations, but these studies do not specifically describe the EAH phenotype that may be more pronounced in overweight children. Better understanding this phenotype could help produce more targeted interventions for overweight children.
Parents play a critical role in the development of eating behaviors in children and could be associated with aberrant eating behaviors, such as EAH. Parent feeding practices, such as controlling feeding practices, restriction, and pressure to eat, are common approaches used by parents in an attempt to encourage children to consume a healthy diet (Faith, Scanlon, Birch, Francis, & Sherry, 2004). However, these feeding practices may not be effective in promoting healthy eating behaviors in children (Savage, Fisher, & Birch, 2007). Moreover, controlling food intake, instead of allowing children to respond to their own internal cues of hunger and satiety, may disrupt the child’s ability to self-regulate and could lead to disinhibited eating (Rollins, Savage, Fisher, & Birch, 2015). For example, pressuring a child to eat and encouraging a child to eat beyond satiety are feeding practices that are positively associated with overeating in young children (Birch, et al., 2003b; Faith, et al., 2004; Fisher & Birch, 2002; Kral & Faith, 2008; Remy, Issanchou, Chabanet, Boggio, & Nicklaus, 2015). In a longitudinal study of five year old girls, the use of restrictive feeding practices predicted EAH two years later, even after controlling for Body Mass Index (BMI) and baseline EAH intake (Fisher & Birch, 2002). Another study found that restrictive feeding practices among overweight mothers of five-year-old girls predicted EAH four years later (Francis & Birch, 2005). Other feeding practices such as using food to regulate emotions are also related to increased child consumption of sweet foods in the absence of hunger (Blissett, Haycraft, & Farrow, 2010). These studies suggest the unintentional impact of parent feeding practices on the development of a potentially maladaptive eating behavior.
However, specific parent feeding practices do not act alone and exist within the broader context of general parenting style. It is possible that general parenting style affects child eating behavior via parent feeding practices. General parenting style includes higher-order constructs that contribute to the socio-emotional context of the parent-child interaction (K. Rhee, 2008), and provide a framework for which children interpret the specific parenting practices that parents implement (K. Rhee, 2008). General parenting style, often defined by varying levels of warmth, support, and behavioral and psychological control (Schludermann, 1988), has been associated with child food intake in cross-sectional studies and may promote or maintain maladaptive eating behaviors (Rodenburg, Oenema, Kremers, & van de Mheen, 2012; van der Horst, et al., 2007). Cross-sectional studies suggest that an authoritative parenting style, compared to authoritarian parenting style, was associated with lower adolescent caloric intake when combined with limitations on sugar sweetened beverages (van der Horst, et al., 2007). In another study, parenting characterized by high levels of warmth and support with clear communication and appropriate boundary-setting has been associated with greater fruit and vegetable intake (Kremers, Brug, de Vries, & Engels, 2003; Schmitz, et al., 2002). Firm maternal parenting has also been associated with decreased snacking in overweight children (K. E. Rhee, et al., 2015). Conversely, high parental psychological control (control of child’s behavior through psychological means such as love withdrawal and guilt induction), combined with low support and low behavioral control, has been correlated with lower fruit consumption in children (Rodenburg, et al., 2012). Although these studies suggest that general parenting style may influence child overeating, there is no available research exploring the relationship between general parenting style and aberrant eating behaviors, such as EAH. Given the crucial role that parents play in facilitating healthy eating behaviors and weight change, it is important to examine the unique contribution of both parent feeding practices and general parenting with child EAH.
EAH is measured using a laboratory paradigm, in which the child is fed a meal until full, and then is given free access to a variety of foods. These foods range from sweet and salty snack foods to buffet foods, and very little attention has been paid to overconsumption of specific types of foods in the EAH paradigm. Combining all foods consumed in the EAH is based on the assumption that eating behavior is the same across foods. However, sweet foods may contribute more to overeating as sweet taste preferences are influenced by innate biology and learned experiences (Conner, Haddon, Pickering, & Booth, 1988; Ventura & Mennella, 2011; Drewnowski, Mennella, Johnson, & Bellisle, 2012). Although there is much controversy as to the role of sweet foods in the development of obesity (Benton, 2010), sugar in particular has been studied as a contributor to food addiction, referencing a specific uncontrollable drive to eat sugar (Avena, Rada, & Hoebel, 2008). Foods high in sugar may contribute to overeating through hormonal and metabolic changes in eating (Ludwig, et al., 1999). Furthermore, parents may also behave differently with sweet foods, compared to other foods, especially in overweight and obese children, and may restrict and/or monitor their child’s consumption of these foods more than others (Seburg, et al., 2014). Currently, no study has uniquely examined the consumption of sweet foods specifically in the EAH paradigm.
Thus, the present study seeks to evaluate the associations between child EAH (EAH-total and EAH-sweet), parent feeding practices and general parenting among a sample of 7–12 year old overweight and obese children and their parents. This study proposes to examine these relationships in an entirely overweight and obese sample (as opposed to a healthy weight or heterogeneous weight sample). Since EAH is a behavioral phenotype more characteristic of overweight and obese individuals, examining a sample within this specific weight range allows for a more focused evaluation of this disinhibited eating behavior to ultimately develop targeted interventions. This study also evaluates these relationships in grade-school aged children instead of younger children, as the majority of weight-loss programs for children are conducted in this age range (A. Ho, Kennedy, & Dimitropoulos, 2012; M. Ho, et al., 2013). In line with previous research, we hypothesize that controlling parent feeding practices (characterized as restriction, monitoring, and pressure to eat) will be positively associated with child total EAH and sweet food consumption in our sample. In terms of general parenting, we hypothesize that higher rejection, psychological control, and lax control will be associated with greater child EAH and sweet food intake.
METHODS
Participants
Data were obtained at the baseline assessment (prior to treatment) for 117 treatment seeking overweight and obese (BMI > 85th %ile) 7–12 year old children (mean age = 10.40 ± 1.40 years; 53% female; 54% Caucasian; BMI-z: 2.06 ± 0.39) and their parents (42.40 ± 6.20 years; 91% female; 70% Caucasian; BMI: 31.70 ± 7.00 kg/m2; see Table 1). Study recruitment took place in the greater Minneapolis/St. Paul area of Minnesota. Physician referrals, direct mailings, and advertisements were all used to recruit participants interested in a research study evaluating a treatment for overeating (Boutelle, et al., 2011). Participants were excluded if either parent or child was currently participating in a weight-loss treatment, taking a medication that would affect weight loss or appetite, had any food allergies, did not like to eat cheese pizza, or did not speak English. All participating parents signed an informed consent form and all participating children provided an informed assent. The University of Minnesota Institutional Review Board approved this study. All data analyzed for this manuscript were obtained during the baseline assessment visit.
Table 1.
N=117 pairs | |
---|---|
Child | |
Sex (% female) | 53% |
Mean Age (SD) | 10.42 (1.35) |
Race | |
Caucasian | 54% |
African American | 14% |
Multi-Race | 20% |
Other | 12% |
BMI | 27.22 (4.56) |
BMI-Z | 2.06 (.39) |
EAH (percent of daily caloric needs) | 15.22 (11.60) |
Range=.24–96.25 | |
Parent | |
Gender (% female) | 91% |
Marital Status (% currently married) | 69% |
Education (% college graduates) | 58% |
Race | |
Caucasian | 70% |
African American | 15% |
Multi-Race | 8% |
Other | 7% |
BMI | 31.82 (6.97) |
Measures
Eating in the Absence of Hunger (EAH)
EAH was assessed using a free access paradigm. Children were instructed to arrive hungry to the laboratory. All visits took place in the late afternoon or early evening. Upon arriving at the laboratory, each child ate a standard ad libitum dinner, which included cheese pizza, carrots, applesauce, and beverage choices of milk, juice, or water with their parent. Child satiety levels were measured using a self-report cartoon representation of three levels of fullness (Faith, et al., 2006), in addition to two questions that asked each child about his or her level of hunger and fullness using a 1–5 scale (1: “not at all hungry/full” to 5: “extremely hungry/full”) (Boutelle, et al., 2011). If a child was not full (rated 4 or 5 on the scale), they were encouraged to eat until they were full. Calories consumed during the dinner were recorded. Ten minutes after the child and parent finished dinner, they were separated and the child was brought to a private room and asked to complete a taste test with small pre-weighed bowls of 11 sweet and savory snack foods (popcorn (277.34g), Cheez-its® (344.67g), potato chips (224.33g), pretzels (302.58g), Cheetos® (277.34g), Fig Newtons® (199.90g), Hershey’s® chocolate bars (477.72g), Skittles® (44.44g), M & M’s® (416.59g), chocolate chip cookies (223.01g), and Jelly Belly® jelly beans (459.30g)) while the parent completed a survey. Following the taste test, the child was left alone for a 10-minute free access session, with toys, games, and the snack foods. After 10 minutes, the research assistant returned to the room, and the remaining food was weighed. Percent of daily caloric needs consumed during both dinner (Dinner%) and the free access paradigm (EAH%-total) was calculated by taking the number of calories consumed by the child and dividing by the child’s estimated daily caloric needs based on formulas taking into account weight, age, height, sex and physical activity level. As a conservative measure, a physical activity level of “low active” was used for all children (Trumbo et al., 2002). We also evaluated the percent of daily calorie needs consumed in sweet foods in the EAH paradigm (EAH%-sweet; chocolate chip cookies, M & M’s®, Skittles®, Jelly Belly® jelly beans, Hershey’s® chocolate bars, and Fig Newtons®).
Parent feeding behavior
Parents completed the Birch Child Feeding Questionnaire (CFQ) (Birch, et al., 2001), which is a 31-item survey assessing parent attitudes and practices regarding child weight and eating behaviors. Of the seven subscales, Restriction (e.g., I have to be sure that my child does not eat too many high-fat foods), Pressure to Eat (e.g., My child should always eat all of the food on her plate), and Monitoring (e.g., How much do you keep track of the sweets (candy, ice cream cake, pies, pastries) that your child eats?) were included in the analyses. This measure has demonstrated adequate validity and reliability (Birch, et al., 2001; Kaur, et al., 2006). In the current study, internal consistency was acceptable (Cronbach’s α’s = .70–.91), except for the pressure to eat subscale (Cronbach’s α = .56).
General parenting
Children completed the Child’s Report of Parental Behavior Inventory (CRPBI-30) (Schludermann, 1988), a 30-item questionnaire that asks the child to rate how much each statement is reflective of their mother’s behaviors on a 3-point scale (1=not like parent, 2=somewhat like parent, 3=a lot like parent). Statements load onto three parenting dimensions: acceptance vs. rejection, psychological control vs. autonomy, and firm vs. lax control. These scales have demonstrated good test-retest reliability, as well as significant associations with various aspects of family functioning and child outcomes (B. Collins & Collins, 1990; Schaefer, 1965; Schluder. E & Schluder. S, 1970; Steinberg, Dornbusch, & Brown, 1992; Steinberg, Elmen, & Mounts, 1989). In the current study, adequate internal consistency was observed for each subscale (Cronbach’s α’s = .74 – .88). Because the majority (91%) of the parents who participated in the study and attended the sessions were mothers, we utilized child report of their mother’s parenting style on the CRPBI-30.
Anthropometrics
Children were weighed using a calibrated scale, and height was measured using a standard stadiometer, in duplicate. The average weight and height for each child was converted to body mass index (BMI, kg/m2). BMI and BMI percentiles-for-age were calculated using the Center for Disease Control and Prevention 2000 growth charts (Kuczmarski, et al., 2002).
Demographics
Demographics, including child age and gender, were obtained through self-report surveys.
Analyses
Pearson correlations were used to determine associations between EAH%, EAH%-sweet, Dinner %, child demographics, and child and parent BMI. Correlations with EAH% salty were initially included in order to determine whether or not any relationships with EAH% total were driven by the influence of salty snacks or sweet snacks. However, because the model with EAH%-salty was not significant, further analyses with EAH% salty were not conducted. Separate linear regression models were used to evaluate the relationship between parent feeding practices and child EAH%-total and EAH%-sweet. Similarly, separate linear regression models were used to evaluate the relationship between general parenting styles and child EAH%-total and EAH%-sweet. All regression models controlled for demographic and BMI variables that were significantly correlated with EAH%-total and EAH%-sweet (see Table 1 and results section for specific variables).
RESULTS
EAH%-total, EAH%-sweet, dinner consumption, parenting and demographics
EAH%-total in this sample of overweight children showed a wide variability as expected, and ranged from 0.24% to 96% (M = 15%, SD = 12%) of estimated total daily caloric intake requirements. EAH%-sweet ranged from 0% to 93% (M = 12%; SD = 11%). Children consumed an average of 28% (SD = 10%) of daily caloric needs at dinner. The proportion of EAH%-sweet to EAH%-total was .76, thus there was a higher proportion of calories consumed from sweet foods compared to total EAH calories consumed.
Correlations between demographic variables (child age, child BMI, parent BMI), Dinner%, and EAH%-total and EAH%-sweet showed that dinner% was positively associated with both EAH%-total (r = .27, p < .01) and EAH%-sweet (r = .28, p < .01). Independent samples t-tests showed that girls had significantly higher total EAH%-total than boys, t(115)=−2.009, p=.041 (girls M = 17% (SD = 14%); boys M = 13% (SD = 8%). However, this effect was not significant once the foods were limited to sweet foods only, t(115)=−1.727, p>.05 (EAH%-sweet; girls M = 13% (SD = 13%); boys M = 10% (SD = 6%); p > .05). Thus, subsequent regression analyses with EAH% controlled for child sex and Dinner%. Analyses with EAH%-sweet controlled for Dinner%. No other demographic variables were significantly associated with EAH% or EAH%-sweet in this sample.
We conducted t-tests to examine gender differences in parenting styles and feeding practices received. We found that boys (M=21.71, SD=3.489) reported higher maternal firm control than girls (M=20.28, SD=3.195), t(106)=2.216, p=.029. There were no other significant gender differences in parenting styles and feeding practices (all p’s>.05). Child age was significantly negatively correlated with maternal acceptance (r=−.246, p=.01) and maternal psychological control (r=−.310, p=.001) and significantly positively correlated with maternal firm control (r=.195, p=.043).
We examined correlations between parent feeding practices and general parenting style and found that only psychological control was significantly positively correlated with pressure to eat (r=.226, p<.05).
Parent feeding practices and child EAH
Separate multiple linear regression analyses were conducted between parent feeding practices and child EAH%-total and EAH%-sweet (Table 3). The overall model was significant for EAH%-total (F (5,111) = 3.878, p = .003); however, none of the parent feeding practices independently significantly predicted child EAH%-total (all p’s >.05). In terms of EAH%-sweet, the overall model was also significant (F (4,112) = 4.778, p = .001) and parent monitoring was significantly and positively associated with EAH%-sweet (β = .181, p = .048).
Table 3.
EAH(%)-Totala | EAH(%)-Sweetb | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
| ||||||||
B | SE | β | CIc | B | SE | β | CIc | |
Restriction | .023 | .018 | .121 | −.011 to .058 | .020 | .016 | .113 | −.012 to .051 |
Pressure to Eat | .009 | .015 | .057 | −.021 to .039 | .012 | .013 | .083 | −.015 to .038 |
Monitoring | .022 | .012 | .163 | −.002 to .047 | .022 | .011 | .181* | .000 to .044 |
Child sex and dinner calories included as covariates in model.
Dinner calories included as covariate in model.
95% Confidence interval for B coefficient.
p < .05
General parenting and child EAH
Separate multiple linear regression analyses, controlling for child sex and Dinner%, were conducted for general parenting with child EAH%-total and EAH%-sweet (Table 4). The overall model was significant for EAH%-total (F (5,93) = 3.732, p = .004), as maternal psychological control was significantly associated with EAH%-total (β = .213, p = .039). In terms of EAH%-sweet, the overall model was also significant (F (4,94) = 3.620, p = .009); however, none of the general parenting styles were individually significantly related to EAH%-sweet (all p’s > .05).
Table 4.
EAH(%)-Totala | EAH(%)-Sweetb | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
| ||||||||
B | SE | β | CIc | B | SE | β | CIc | |
Acceptance | −.002 | .003 | −.062 | −.009 to .004 | −.001 | .003 | −.041 | −.007 to .005 |
Psychological Control | .006 | .003 | .213* | .000 to .011 | .005 | .003 | .182 | −.001 to .010 |
Firm control | .002 | .004 | −.056 | −.009 to .005 | −.003 | .003 | −.090 | −.010 to .004 |
Child sex and dinner calories included as covariates in model.
Dinner calories included as covariate in model.
95% Confidence interval for B coefficient.
p < .05
Exploratory analyses
Because our results were mixed in terms of the relationship between parent feeding and general parenting and EAH%-total and EAH%-sweet, we decided to post-hoc explore the relationships among the individual foods used in the EAH paradigm to investigate which foods in particular were primarily influencing the associations between parenting and EAH. Notably, there was variation between the individual foods on the amount of calories that were consumed (Table 5). This emphasizes the uniqueness of each food and suggests that each food may differentially relate to parenting. Thus, Pearson correlations were utilized to evaluate the relationships between parent feeding variables, general parenting style variables, and percent of daily calories consumed of the sweet foods presented in the EAH paradigm: Fig Newtons®, Hershey’s® chocolate bars, Skittles®, M & M’s®, chocolate chip cookies, and Jelly Belly® jelly beans. Results showed that Hershey’s® chocolate bar intake was significantly positively correlated with restriction (r = .222, p < .05), pressure to eat (r = .295, p < .01), monitoring (r = .197, p < .05), and maternal psychological control (r = .258, p < .01). There were no other correlations between parenting variables and specific sweet foods (all p’s > .05).
Table 5.
EAH Food | Mean | Standard Deviation |
---|---|---|
Popcorn | 7.28 | 15.32 |
Cheez-its® | 22.28 | 34.11 |
Cheetos® | 24.83 | 38.06 |
Potato chips | 23.07 | 42.47 |
Pretzels | 5.14 | 13.56 |
Skittles® | 47.63 | 67.00 |
Hershey’s® chocolates | 98.93 | 129.88 |
Chocolate chip cookies | 63.25 | 78.24 |
Jelly beans | 23.23 | 43.64 |
M & M’s® | 33.29 | 53.58 |
Fig Newtons® | 17.27 | 36.30 |
DISCUSSION
This study is the first to evaluate the association between parenting and EAH%-total and EAH%-sweet in a sample of treatment-seeking overweight and obese children. Results suggest that our hypotheses were partially supported. Both parent feeding practices (i.e., monitoring) and general parenting (i.e., maternal psychological control) were associated with higher levels of child EAH%-sweet and EAH%-total respectively. Girls in this sample ate significantly more of their daily caloric needs in the EAH paradigm compared to boys. Additionally, in contrast to previous weight heterogeneous samples, this sample consisted entirely of overweight and obese sample of children. Notably, EAH ranged from 0.24% to 96%, suggesting that not all overweight and obese children display this behavior and that individual differences in this variable are particularly important to examine. Exploratory analyses of relationships between parenting and specific foods in the EAH paradigm revealed that child intake of Hershey’s® chocolate bars was associated with parental restriction, pressure to eat, monitoring, and maternal psychological control.
We conceptualized that parent feeding practices exist within the broader context of general parenting style. In fact, we found that psychological control was significantly positively correlated with pressure to eat. Thus, it is possible that pressure to eat affects EAH via general parenting. However, not all feeding and general parenting variables were related, suggesting that specific feeding and general parenting might also independently affect child overeating. Results also indicated that monitoring and perceived maternal psychological control were related to increased child EAH%-sweet and EAH%-total respectively. Parent monitoring and maternal psychological control may represent more indirect methods of parental control that are being used in response to the child’s eating behaviors. From a developmental standpoint, it is possible that younger children (i.e. younger than 7 years old) may be more responsive to more overt parent control over eating whereas older children (7–12 years old), such as that in our sample, may be more impacted by psychological forms of general maternal control strategies to manage eating behaviors. Psychological control is also seen as a coercive and maladaptive parenting style that may lead to problematic eating behaviors such as EAH. This study’s data suggest that children who perceive their mothers as using more coercive practices to reduce their overeating or that those children who perceive more psychological control from their mothers tend to eat more beyond satiety. Thus, controlling feeding practices and maternal parenting styles may have a paradoxical effect on child overeating. In the EAH laboratory paradigm, parents are not present in the room, thus when children perceive parental control as being temporarily lifted during the free access portion of the task, they may overcompensate by eating foods that are typically viewed as “forbidden.” In interpreting the results of this study, it is important to consider the bidirectional nature of the relationship between child overeating and parenting. It is also possible to conceive that EAH can likewise elicit greater parental control.
In terms of parental restriction, the results from this study both support and refute previous research. In our sample of overweight and obese children in later childhood/preteen years, restrictive feeding style was not associated with EAH%-total or EAH%-sweet. With the exception of parental monitoring, our findings mirror those of another study of healthy and overweight 8–13 year old children which did not find that parental pressure to eat, monitoring and restriction were associated with child EAH (Moens & Braet, 2007). Both studies included samples of older participants in grade school and middle childhood, which may explain the discrepancy in findings compared to other studies that focused primarily on preschool age children that suggested that restrictive feeding practices are associated with greater EAH or food intake (Birch, et al., 2003a; Boots, Tiggemann, Corsini, & Mattiske, 2015; Jansen, Mulkens, & Jansen, 2007; Johnson & Birch, 1994; Sonneville, et al., 2013). These studies also examined children across the weight-spectrum and some did not control for BMI at baseline. Therefore, additional research is needed to clarify how reports of restrictive feeding style may differ among parents of overweight and obese children compared to samples of families with children of varying weight status. It is also possible that restrictive feeding style by parents of overweight and obese children does not vary as much as it does in samples of families with children of varied weights. However, likely due to methodological differences, previous research has revealed no consistent pattern in the degree of variation of this scale; studies in children and youth have found variation in this measure that are as low as 0.1 (Birch & Fisher, 2000) to as high as 1.13 (Joyce & Zimmer-Gembeck, 2009).
It is interesting that girls had higher EAH than boys in our sample. These results are particularly meaningful given that EAH is age- and gender-adjusted, thus the metabolic needs of girls versus boys were accounted for in the analyses. This finding is inconsistent with one other study that reported on sex differences in EAH, which found that boys consumed more than girls in the EAH paradigm (Faith, et al., 2006). However, this sample differed from that of our study in that the children were younger (5 years old) and either at-risk or not at-risk for obesity based on maternal pre-pregnancy body weight, which may explain the inconsistency. Regardless of sex, children who consumed more calories at dinner also ate more overall and consumed more sweet foods during the EAH paradigm. The significant positive correlation between dinner calories and EAH% suggests that disinhibited overeating behaviors may cascade into future eating sessions. This is consistent with other data that suggests that overweight and obese children do not compensate for higher calorie consumption earlier in the day (Kral, et al., 2012). Considering that caloric intake is one of the targets for weight management programs, it may be important to note that consumption during dinner does not inhibit future eating.
This study is novel as it evaluated overall EAH%-total as well as EAH%-sweet. These analyses produced some interesting considerations. Our data suggest that parental monitoring is associated with child overconsumption of sweet foods in particular, instead of snack foods in general. In a study with healthy and overweight 4–7 year old children, the threat of parental monitoring and actual monitoring was related to children choosing foods lower in sugar, whereas when children were allowed to freely choose their food without parental monitoring (threat or actual), they selected more non-nutritious foods, particularly those high in sugar (Klesges, Stein, Eck, Isbell, & Klesges, 1991). One interpretation is that child consumption of sweet foods may be especially sensitive to parent monitoring and that children may overeat by consuming sweet foods when parent supervision is not present. As these data are cross sectional, the reverse may also be possible: child intake of sweet foods may have been particularly concerning for parents and parents may monitor their children more closely if they displayed this behavior.
In the exploratory analyses, we examined the relationship among the individual sweet foods used in the EAH paradigm and parenting practices to better understand the unique contribution of the individual foods within the EAH%-total and EAH%-sweet composite scores. Interestingly, we found that Hershey’s® bars were positively correlated with all three parent feeding practices as well as maternal psychological control. Hershey’s® bars are characteristically different from the other snack foods served in a number of ways. Hershey’s chocolates had the highest calories consumed and the largest variance, so Hershey’s may have had more significant correlations as a result of greater variance. Parents might perceive Hershey’s® bars to be more calorically-dense than other snack foods which may then elicit more controlling parenting practices to reduce consumption. Hershey’s® bars are typically individually wrapped which may make it more conducive for parents to monitor the intake because the wrappers provide evidence of consumption. Thus, one interpretation is that wrapping and perceived caloric-density may be underlying characteristics of foods that contribute to more controlling parental responses. The above findings on the unique contributions of individual sweet snack foods suggest that the variation in the specific foods provided during the free access session may be contributing to the variability in findings across studies utilizing the EAH paradigm as a measure of overeating (Lansigan, Emond, & Gilbert-Diamond, 2015). Future research should consider the contribution that individual foods have on overall EAH to better understand which foods may elicit overeating. Factors such as cultural and regional food preferences, and a food’s nutrient composition, satiety level, shape, texture, or size, may impact parent perceptions of foods and thus their responses and behaviors towards them.
Strengths of this study include the moderate sample size of treatment-seeking overweight and obese children and their parents, and the wide range of EAH%-total (0.24% to 96%). This population of treatment-seeking children and parents mirrored families that would present for treatment to community clinics and intervention programs. We also used validated measures of parent feeding practices and general parenting style, as well as measuring EAH using the laboratory paradigm. As in all studies, this study also had several limitations. It is possible that there were no significant models with EAH%-salty in this study because only 5 of the 11 EAH foods presented were salty, compared to 6 sweet foods. This may have resulted in there being a higher proportion (76%) of calories from sweet foods consumed. However, this further emphasizes the importance of examining sweet foods in particular in the EAH paradigm as there may be a greater tendency or preference to overeat with sweet foods. The observed internal consistency for the pressure to eat subscale was notably low in our sample. Perhaps because this is a treatment seeking overweight and obese sample, parents may be more conflicted about whether they should pressure their child to eat. Parents may have feeding practices that they grew up with, e.g., must clean your plate, but because their child is overweight, they may hold back on that some times. This might explain some of the inconsistency of their responses within this subscale. However, our study is not unique in finding a low internal consistency for this subscale. A recent 2013 paper examining the relationship between parenting practices and child eating behaviors (overeating, loss of control eating, and disordered eating attitudes) in a sample of 8–12 year old Flemish children also reported poor internal consistency for the “pressure to eat” subscale (Cronbach’s alpha = .56 and .60; Matton, Goossens, Braet, & Van Durme, 2013). Not only does this present a limitation to our current study findings, it also suggests that perhaps additional parenting measures with better psychometric properties should be developed. In addition, this was a cross-sectional sample from which causation cannot be determined. It is also possible that there are no substantive differences between EAH relationships for different parent feeding practices or different parenting styles (please see regression coefficients and confidence intervals in Tables 3 and 4). Future studies should examine other unmeasured variables that may potentially contribute to variance in this study, such as child temperament (Anzman & Birch, 2009; C. Collins, Duncanson, & Burrows, 2014), adrenocortical regulation (Francis, Granger, & Susman, 2013), neurobiological activation of reward pathways (Born, et al., 2010), and genetics (Fisher, Cai, et al., 2007; Provencher, et al., 2005).
However, considering the strengths and limitations, this study suggests that there is a relationship between parent feeding strategies and children’s eating behavior, with marked variability among an overweight and obese sample. Our findings highlight the importance of controlling for dinner consumption, and to further explore these sex differences and the role sex might play in overeating behavior over time. Furthermore, this study has implications for developing interventions for overweight children, as controlling feeding and parenting practices were associated with higher EAH%-total, particularly for girls. It may be that specific sex-adapted interventions are also needed, considering the noted differences between girls and boys. The environmental contributors (parenting variables) as well as child feeding behaviors are important to evaluate in future studies elucidating the influences on child overconsumption of sweet foods. Research studies in the future could evaluate all of these important variables, in larger samples, over time, to ultimately develop targeted interventions to prevent the development of obesity and eating disorders in children and youth.
Table 2.
1 | 2 | 3 | 5 | 6 | 7 | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. EAH%-Total | -- | .963*** | −.099 | .031 | .152 | .269** |
2. EAH%-Sweet | -- | −.165 | −.049 | .147 | .283** | |
3. Child age | -- | .311** | −.012 | −.337*** | ||
5. Child BMI | -- | .338*** | −.150 | |||
6. Parent BMI | -- | .153 | ||||
7. Dinner % a | -- |
p < .05,
p < .01,
p < .001
Dinner calories = calories consumed at dinner as a percentage of daily caloric requirements
Acknowledgments
This study was funded by University of Minnesota Faculty Development Grant to Kerri Boutelle. Support to Kerri Boutelle (K02HL112042; DK075861; DK094475).
Footnotes
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References
- Anzman SL, Birch LL. Low inhibitory control and restrictive feeding practices predict weight outcomes. J Pediatr. 2009;155:651–656. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2009.04.052. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Avena NM, Rada P, Hoebel BG. Evidence for sugar addiction: behavioral and neurochemical effects of intermittent, excessive sugar intake. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2008;32:20–39. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.04.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Benton D. The plausibility of sugar addiction and its role in obesity and eating disorders. Clin Nutr. 2010;29:288–303. doi: 10.1016/j.clnu.2009.12.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Birch LL, Fisher JO. Mothers’ child-feeding practices influence daughters’ eating and weight. The American journal of Clinical Nutrition. 2000;71(5):1054–1061. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/71.5.1054. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Birch LL, Fisher JO, Davison KK. Learning to overeat: maternal use of restrictive feeding practices promotes girls’ eating in the absence of hunger. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition. 2003a;78:215–220. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/78.2.215. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Birch LL, Fisher JO, Davison KK. Learning to overeat: maternal use of restrictive feeding practices promotes girls’ eating in the absence of hunger. Am J Clin Nutr. 2003b;78:215–220. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/78.2.215. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Birch LL, Fisher JO, Grimm-Thomas K, Markey CN, Sawyer R, Johnson SL. Confirmatory factor analysis of the Child Feeding Questionnaire: a measure of parental attitudes, beliefs and practices about child feeding and obesity proneness. Appetite. 2001;36:201–210. doi: 10.1006/appe.2001.0398. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blissett J, Haycraft E, Farrow C. Inducing preschool children’s emotional eating: relations with parental feeding practices. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition. 2010;92:359–365. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.2010.29375. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boots SB, Tiggemann M, Corsini N, Mattiske J. Managing young children’s snack food intake. The role of parenting style and feeding strategies. Appetite. 2015;92:94–101. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2015.05.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Born JM, Lemmens SG, Rutters F, Nieuwenhuizen AG, Formisano E, Goebel R, Westerterp-Plantenga MS. Acute stress and food-related reward activation in the brain during food choice during eating in the absence of hunger. Int J Obes (Lond) 2010;34:172–181. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2009.221. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boutelle KN, Peterson CB, Crosby RD, Rydell SA, Zucker N, Harnack L. Overeating phenotypes in overweight and obese children. Appetite. 2014;76:95–100. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2014.01.076. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boutelle KN, Zucker NL, Peterson CB, Rydell SA, Cafri G, Harnack L. Two novel treatments to reduce overeating in overweight children: a randomized controlled trial. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2011;79:759–771. doi: 10.1037/a0025713. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Butte NF, Cai G, Cole SA, Wilson TA, Fisher JO, Zakeri IF, Ellis KJ, Comuzzie AG. Metabolic and behavioral predictors of weight gain in Hispanic children: the Viva la Familia Study. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition. 2007;85:1478–1485. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/85.6.1478. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Collins B, Collins T. Parent-professional relationships in the treatment of seriously emotionally disturbed children and adolescents. Soc Work. 1990;35:522–527. doi: 10.1093/sw/35.6.522. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Collins C, Duncanson K, Burrows T. A systematic review investigating associations between parenting style and child feeding behaviours. J Hum Nutr Diet. 2014;27:557–568. doi: 10.1111/jhn.12192. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Conner MT, Haddon AV, Pickering ES, Booth DA. Sweet tooth demonstrated: individual differences in preference for both sweet foods and foods highly sweetened. J Appl Psychol. 1988;73:275–280. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.73.2.275. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Craigie AM, Lake AA, Kelly SA, Adamson AJ, Mathers JC. Tracking of obesity-related behaviours from childhood to adulthood: A systematic review. Maturitas. 2011;70:266–284. doi: 10.1016/j.maturitas.2011.08.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cutting TM, Fisher JO, Grimm-Thomas K, Birch LL. Like mother, like daughter: familial patterns of overweight are mediated by mothers’ dietary disinhibition. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition. 1999;69:608–613. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/69.4.608. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Drewnowski A, Mennella JA, Johnson SL, Bellisle F. Sweetness and food preference. J Nutr. 2012;142:1142S–1148S. doi: 10.3945/jn.111.149575. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Faith MS, Berkowitz RI, Stallings VA, Kerns J, Storey M, Stunkard AJ. Eating in the absence of hunger: a genetic marker for childhood obesity in prepubertal boys? Obesity (Silver Spring) 2006;14:131–138. doi: 10.1038/oby.2006.16. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Faith MS, Scanlon KS, Birch LL, Francis LA, Sherry B. Parent-child feeding strategies and their relationships to child eating and weight status. Obes Res. 2004;12:1711–1722. doi: 10.1038/oby.2004.212. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Field AE, Camargo CA, Jr, Ogino S. The merits of subtyping obesity: one size does not fit all. JAMA. 2013;310:2147–2148. doi: 10.1001/jama.2013.281501. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fisher JO, Birch LL. Eating in the absence of hunger and overweight in girls from 5 to 7 y of age. Am J Clin Nutr. 2002;76:226–231. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/76.1.226. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fisher JO, Cai G, Jaramillo SJ, Cole SA, Comuzzie AG, Butte NF. Heritability of hyperphagic eating behavior and appetite-related hormones among Hispanic children. Obesity (Silver Spring) 2007;15:1484–1495. doi: 10.1038/oby.2007.177. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fisher JO, Liu Y, Birch LL, Rolls BJ. Effects of portion size and energy density on young children’s intake at a meal. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition. 2007;86:174–179. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/86.1.174. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Francis LA, Birch LL. Maternal weight status modulates the effects of restriction on daughters’ eating and weight. Int J Obes (Lond) 2005;29:942–949. doi: 10.1038/sj.ijo.0802935. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Francis LA, Granger DA, Susman EJ. Adrenocortical regulation, eating in the absence of hunger and BMI in young children. Appetite. 2013;64:32–38. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2012.11.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Francis LA, Ventura AK, Marini M, Birch LL. Parent overweight predicts daughters’ increase in BMI and disinhibited overeating from 5 to 13 years. Obesity (Silver Spring) 2007;15:1544–1553. doi: 10.1038/oby.2007.183. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hill C, Llewellyn CH, Saxton J, Webber L, Semmler C, Carnell S, van Jaarsveld CH, Boniface D, Wardle J. Adiposity and ‘eating in the absence of hunger’ in children. Int J Obes (Lond) 2008;32:1499–1505. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2008.113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ho A, Kennedy J, Dimitropoulos A. Neural correlates to food-related behavior in normal-weight and overweight/obese participants. PLoS One. 2012;7:e45403. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0045403. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ho M, Garnett SP, Baur LA, Burrows T, Stewart L, Neve M, Collins C. Impact of dietary and exercise interventions on weight change and metabolic outcomes in obese children and adolescents: a systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized trials. JAMA Pediatr. 2013;167:759–768. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2013.1453. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jansen E, Mulkens S, Jansen A. Do not eat the red food!: prohibition of snacks leads to their relatively higher consumption in children. Appetite. 2007;49:572–577. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2007.03.229. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson SL, Birch LL. Parents’ and children’s adiposity and eating style. Pediatrics. 1994;94:653–661. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Joyce JL, Zimmer-Gembeck MJ. Parent feeding restriction and child weight. The mediating role of child disinhibited eating and the moderating role of the parenting context. Appetite. 2009;52(3):726–734. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2009.03.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaur H, Li C, Nazir N, Choi WS, Resnicow K, Birch LL, Ahluwalia JS. Confirmatory factor analysis of the child-feeding questionnaire among parents of adolescents. Appetite. 2006;47:36–45. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2006.01.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klesges RC, Stein RJ, Eck LH, Isbell TR, Klesges LM. Parental Influence on Food Selection in Young-Children and Its Relationships to Childhood Obesity. American Journal of Clinical Nutrition. 1991;53:859–864. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/53.4.859. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kral TV, Allison DB, Birch LL, Stallings VA, Moore RH, Faith MS. Caloric compensation and eating in the absence of hunger in 5- to 12-y-old weight-discordant siblings. Am J Clin Nutr. 2012;96:574–583. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.112.037952. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kral TV, Faith MS. Influences on Child Eating and Weight Development from a Behavioral Genetics Perspective. J Pediatr Psychol. 2008;34:596–605. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsn037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kremers SP, Brug J, de Vries H, Engels RC. Parenting style and adolescent fruit consumption. Appetite. 2003;41:43–50. doi: 10.1016/s0195-6663(03)00038-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuczmarski RJ, Ogden CL, Guo SS, Grummer-Strawn LM, Flegal KM, Mei Z, Wei R, Curtin LR, Roche AF, Johnson CL. 2000 CDC Growth Charts for the United States: methods and development. Vital Health Stat. 2002;11:1–190. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lansigan RK, Emond JA, Gilbert-Diamond D. Understanding eating in the absence of hunger among young children: A systematic review of existing studies. Appetite. 2015;85:36–47. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2014.10.032. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ludwig DS, Majzoub JA, Al-Zahrani A, Dallal GE, Blanco I, Roberts SB. High glycemic index foods, overeating, and obesity. Pediatrics. 1999;103:E26. doi: 10.1542/peds.103.3.e26. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matton A, Goossens L, Braet C, Van Durme K. Continuity in primary school children’s eating problems and the influence of parental feeding strategies. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 2013;42(1):52–66. doi: 10.1007/s10964-012-9794-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moens E, Braet C. Predictors of disinhibited eating in children with and without overweight. Behav Res Ther. 2007;45:1357–1368. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2006.10.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ogden CL, Carroll MD, Kit BK, Flegal KM. Prevalence of childhood and adult obesity in the United States, 2011–2012. JAMA. 2014;311:806–814. doi: 10.1001/jama.2014.732. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Provencher V, Perusse L, Bouchard L, Drapeau V, Bouchard C, Rice T, Rao DC, Tremblay A, Despres JP, Lemieux S. Familial resemblance in eating behaviors in men and women from the Quebec Family Study. Obes Res. 2005;13:1624–1629. doi: 10.1038/oby.2005.199. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Remy E, Issanchou S, Chabanet C, Boggio V, Nicklaus S. Impact of adiposity, age, sex and maternal feeding practices on eating in the absence of hunger and caloric compensation in preschool children. Int J Obes (Lond) 2015;39:925–930. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2015.30. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rhee K. Childhood overweight and the relationship between parent behaviors, parenting style, and family functioning. Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science. 2008;615:12–37. [Google Scholar]
- Rhee KE, Boutelle KN, Jelalian E, Barnes R, Dickstein S, Wing RR. Firm maternal parenting associated with decreased risk of excessive snacking in overweight children. Eating and Weight Disorders-Studies on Anorexia Bulimia and Obesity. 2015;20:195–203. doi: 10.1007/s40519-014-0164-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rodenburg G, Oenema A, Kremers SP, van de Mheen D. Parental and child fruit consumption in the context of general parenting, parental education and ethnic background. Appetite. 2012;58:364–372. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2011.11.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rollins BY, Savage JS, Fisher JO, Birch LL. Alternatives to restrictive feeding practices to promote self-regulation in childhood: a developmental perspective. Pediatr Obes. 2015 doi: 10.1111/ijpo.12071. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Savage JS, Fisher JO, Birch LL. Parental influence on eating behavior: conception to adolescence. J Law Med Ethics. 2007;35:22–34. doi: 10.1111/j.1748-720X.2007.00111.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schaefer ES. Children’s Reports of Parental Behavior: An Inventory. Child Dev. 1965;36:413–424. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schluder E, Schluder S. Replicability of Factors in Childrens Report of Parent Behavior (CRPBI) Journal of Psychology. 1970;76:239–249. [Google Scholar]
- Schludermann EH, Schludermann SM. Children’s Report on Parent Behavior (CRPBI-108, CRPBI-30) for older children and adolescents. Winnipeg, MB, Canada: University of Manitoba; 1988. [Google Scholar]
- Schmitz KH, Lytle LA, Phillips GA, Murray DM, Birnbaum AS, Kubik MY. Psychosocial correlates of physical activity and sedentary leisure habits in young adolescents: the Teens Eating for Energy and Nutrition at School study. Prev Med. 2002;34:266–278. doi: 10.1006/pmed.2001.0982. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seburg EM, Kunin-Batson A, Senso MM, Crain AL, Langer SL, Levy RL, Sherwood NE. Concern about Child Weight among Parents of Children At-Risk for Obesity. Health Behav Policy Rev. 2014;1:197–208. doi: 10.14485/HBPR.1.3.4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shunk JA, Birch LL. Girls at risk for overweight at age 5 are at risk for dietary restraint, disinhibited overeating, weight concerns, and greater weight gain from 5 to 9 years. Journal of the American Dietetic Association. 2004;104:1120–1126. doi: 10.1016/j.jada.2004.04.031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Singh AS, Mulder C, Twisk JWR, van Mechelen W, Chinapaw MJM. Tracking of childhood overweight into adulthood: a systematic review of the literature. Obesity Reviews. 2008;9:474–488. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-789X.2008.00475.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sonneville KR, Rifas-Shiman SL, Haines J, Gortmaker S, Mitchell KF, Gillman MW, Taveras EM. Associations of parental control of feeding with eating in the absence of hunger and food sneaking, hiding, and hoarding. Child Obes. 2013;9:346–349. doi: 10.1089/chi.2012.0149. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spruijt-Metz D, Lindquist CH, Birch LL, Fisher JO, Goran MI. Relation between mothers’ child-feeding practices and children’s adiposity. Am J Clin Nutr. 2002;75:581–586. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/75.3.581. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steinberg L, Dornbusch SM, Brown BB. Ethnic differences in adolescent achievement. An ecological perspective. Am Psychol. 1992;47:723–729. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.47.6.723. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steinberg L, Elmen JD, Mounts NS. Authoritative parenting, psychosocial maturity, and academic success among adolescents. Child Dev. 1989;60:1424–1436. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1989.tb04014.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Swinburn BA, Sacks G, Lo SK, Westerterp KR, Rush EC, Rosenbaum M, Luke A, Schoeller DA, DeLany JP, Butte NF, Ravussin E. Estimating the changes in energy flux that characterize the rise in obesity prevalence. Am J Clin Nutr. 2009;89:1723–1728. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.2008.27061. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Trumbo P, Schlicker S, Yates AA, Poos M Food and Nutrition Board of the Institute of Medicine, The National Academies. Dietary reference intakes for energy, carbohydrate, fiber, fat, fatty acids, cholesterol, protein and amino acids. J Am Diet Nutr. 2002;102:1621–30. doi: 10.1016/s0002-8223(02)90346-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van der Horst K, Kremers S, Ferreira I, Singh A, Oenema A, Brug J. Perceived parenting style and practices and the consumption of sugar-sweetened beverages by adolescents. Health Educ Res. 2007;22:295–304. doi: 10.1093/her/cyl080. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ventura AK, Mennella JA. Innate and learned preferences for sweet taste during childhood. Curr Opin Clin Nutr Metab Care. 2011;14:379–384. doi: 10.1097/MCO.0b013e328346df65. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]