Skip to main content
Journal of Clinical and Diagnostic Research : JCDR logoLink to Journal of Clinical and Diagnostic Research : JCDR
. 2016 Feb 1;10(2):CC27–CC31. doi: 10.7860/JCDR/2016/17607.7299

Vestibular Stimulation for Stress Management in Students

Sai Sailesh Kumar 1, Archana Rajagopalan 2, Joseph Kurien Mukkadan 3,
PMCID: PMC4800522  PMID: 27042457

Abstract

Introduction

Although several methods are developed to alleviate stress among college students, logistic limitations in adopting them have limited their utility.

Aim

Hence, we aimed to test a very practical approach to alleviate stress among college students by achieving vestibular stimulation using swings.

Materials and Methods

In this study 60 male and female participants were randomly assigned into vestibular stimulation or control groups. Depression, anxiety, stress scores, sleep quality, heart rate, blood pressure, Autonomic functions, respiratory, haematological, cognitive function, Quality of life were recorded before and after 1st, 7th, 14th, 21st, 28th days of vestibular stimulation.

Results

STAI S and STAI T scores were significantly improved on day 28th following vestibular stimulation. Diastolic and mean arterial blood pressure were significantly decreased and remained within normal limits in vestibular group on day 28th following vestibular stimulation. Postural fall in blood pressure was significantly improved on day 14 onwards, following vestibular stimulation. Respiratory rate was significantly improved on day 7 onwards, following vestibular stimulation. PSQI sleep disturbance, PSQI sleep latency, PSQI total score and bleeding time was significantly improved following vestibular stimulation.

Conclusion

Our study supports the adoption of vestibular stimulation for stress management. Hence, placement of swings in college campuses must be considered, which may be a simple approach to alleviate stress among college students.

Keywords: Anxiety, Depression, Natural vestibular stimulation, Swings

Introduction

Swinging is a natural way to stimulate vestibular system, which is the first sensory-system that starts functioning in the course of early development [1]. Vestibular system has complex network with diverse pathways, including the cortical and subcortical structures of central nervous system [2]. Vestibular system consists of afferent and efferent connections with most of the brain structures involved in regulation of stress, sleep and cognition [2]. Hippocampus is the area of continued neurogenesis, where new neurons are formed throughout the life [3]. Several studies have reported evidence for hippocampal networks and vestibular lesions leading to impairment in spatial memory in humans [46]. Vestibular dysfunction can lead to depression, anxiety, postural incoordination, occulo-motor changes and behavioural changes [7]. It was reported that vestibular stimulation improves cognitive functions [8].

Most of the students in higher education experience stress due to demands of very competitive college life [9]. High levels of stress can negatively affect cognitive functions (attention, comprehension, thinking, memory, problem solving, etc.,) and decrease academic performance in college students [10,11]. Though several methods are developed to alleviate stress among college students, minimum attention is given to arrange swings in the campus and providing adequate time to the students to use the swing. Most of the students reported that swings are a nice way to enjoy fresh breeze. Swinging in a green space will provide students an opportunity to alleviate stress [8]. Swinging on a swing is a natural method of stimulating vestibular system. Vestibular stimulation may alleviate stress by inhibiting hypothalamo- pituitary adrenal-axis and sympathetic- adreno medullary axis [912]. Vestibular stimulation decreases heart rate and blood pressure within normal limits through vagal stimulation [13]. The present study was aimed to assess the evidence for stress alleviating effects of vestibular stimulation among college students.

Materials and Methods

Study Design: Experimental (pre and post with control) design.

Participants, inclusion and exclusion criteria: Sixty male and female college students were a part for this research. The study was approved by Institutional Ethics committee (10th Jan 2014) of Little Flower Hospital and Research Center. After obtaining written informed consent from all the participants, they were randomly assigned to control (females (n=17), males (n=13) and intervention groups (females (n=24), males (n=6) by random numbers generated by computer. A detailed medical history was obtained from all participants and they underwent a standard physical examination, which enabled us to exclude individuals involved in drug/alcohol abuse, and those taking any kind of medication or suffering from any somatic or mental disorders. The selected female students were having regular menstrual cycles for at least 6 months prior to this study. Students with a history of use of corticosteroids in the past year, students with a history of antidepressant medication and students on hormone supplements including oral contraceptives and ear infections or any vestibular disturbances were excluded from the study.

Laboratory setting: The present study was conducted at Department of Physiology, Little Flower Institute of Medical Sciences and Research Centre (LIMSAR), Angamaly, Kerala, India. After recording baseline (pre-intervention) scores, intervention was given to the participants for 28 days and post- intervention scores were recorded and compared to observe effect of our intervention.

Vestibular stimulation: Vestibular stimulation was achieved by swinging on a swing, according to their comfort. (Back to front direction) [14,15].

Assessment of depression, anxiety and stress: Assessment of depression, anxiety and stress was done by using standard questionnaires after obtaining permission to reproduce the questionnaires and in consultation with psychiatrist [16,17].

State-Trait Anxiety Inventory for AdultsTM: The STAI inventory comprises of state and trait forms, to assess anxiety levels. Each form consists of 20 questions with total scores that range from a minimum of 20 to a maximum of 80. Anxiety scores ≤ 40 were considered as low anxiety while a score of > 40 were considered as high anxiety. License obtained from Mind Garden, Inc., to reproduce State-Trait Anxiety Inventory for AdultsTM in our research.

Depression Anxiety Stress Scale (DASS): DASS is a 42 item questionnaire designed to assess depression, anxiety and stress levels.

The students’ Stress Questionnaire: The students stress questionnaire is a standard questionnaire, designed by Ministry of Social Security, National Solidarity and Reform Institutions, to assess stress levels in students (www.gov.mu/portal/sites/suicideprevention/file/student).

Assessment of Cognition

Verbal and spatial memory test: Verbal and spatial memory test is a standardized test to assess cognition levels by assessing spatial and verbal memory scores [18,19].

Assessment of cardio-respiratory parameters: Blood pressure was recorded by using Diamond digital sphygmomanometers (BPDG024) and P02, pulse rate was recorded by using pulse oximeter (EDAN H100B) respiratory rate was measured manually [14,20].

The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI): It is a standardized instrument to assess quality of sleep through seven components of sleep. Permission obtained to use the questionnaire for our research [21].

The Epworth Sleepiness Scale (ESS): It is a standard questionnaire to assess observe abnormal excessive day time sleepiness [22].

Quality of Life: The WHO-QOL BREF is a standardized questionnaire developed as a cross-culturally comparable tool to assess an individual’s perceptions in the context of their culture and value systems, their personal goals, standards and concerns [23]. Twenty six questions are used to measure the quality of life in four domains––physical, psychological, social and environmental. Each item is scored on a five-point Likert scale [24].

Haematological parameters: Bleeding time was estimated by duke method and clotting time was estimated by capillary tube method. Increase in cortisol causes atrophy of all lymphoid tissues and decreases output of lymphocytes. Cortisol decreases eosinophils, lymphocytes and monocytes and increases red blood cells and platelets [2527].

Autonomic Function Test

Blood pressure –response to standing: It was reported that stress can modulates autonomic functions in students [28]. The test was performed by measuring the blood pressure in lying position and again in standing position. The postural fall in blood pressure was taken as difference between the systolic blood pressure lying and systolic blood pressure standing [29].

Statistical Analysis

Data is represented as Mean±SD and were analysed using SPSS 20.0. Statistical tests applied are Two-Way ANOVA and Bonferroni posttests. The p-value <0.05 was taken as significant.

Results

Results were presented in [Table/Fig-1,2,3,4,5,6,7,8,9 and 10]. [Table/Fig-1] presents demographic information and [Table/Fig-2] presents mean frequency, intensity and duration of vestibular stimulation. [Table/Fig-3,4,5,6,7,8,9 and 10] presents variations in different parameters before and after vestibular stimulation.

[Table/Fig-1]:

[Table/Fig-1]:

Age, Body weight, height and BMI of participants (VF- vestibular group females, VM-Vestibular group males, CM- Control group males, CF- Control group females).

[Table/Fig-2]:

[Table/Fig-2]:

Mean frequency, intensity, duration of vestibular stimulation. (Frequency, intensity, duration was measured manually as previously described) [15].

[Table/Fig-3]:

[Table/Fig-3]:

Student’s Stress, STAI S, STAI T scores, Heart rate in participants before and after vestibular stimulation. (VF- vestibular stimulation group females, VM- Vestibular stimulation group males, CM- control group males, CF- control group females).

[Table/Fig-4]:

[Table/Fig-4]:

Blood pressure changes in participants before and after vestibular stimulation.

[Table/Fig-5]:

[Table/Fig-5]:

Autonomic function test values, spatial and verbal memory test scores and Po2 levels before and after vestibular stimulation.

[Table/Fig-6]:

[Table/Fig-6]:

Respiratory rate, bleeding time, clotting time and DASS depression scores before and after vestibular stimulation.

[Table/Fig-7]:

[Table/Fig-7]:

Anxiety, stress scores, PSQI duration of sleep, PSQI sleep disturbance before and after vestibular stimulation. (DASS A- DASS anxiety scores, DASS S- DASS stress scores, PSQI DISTB- PSQI sleep disturbance).

[Table/Fig-8]:

[Table/Fig-8]:

PSQI sleep latency, PSQI day dysfunction due to sleepiness, PSQI efficiency, PSQI overall sleep quality before and after vestibular stimulation.

[Table/Fig-9]:

[Table/Fig-9]:

PSQI need medicines to sleep, PSQI total score, Epworth sleepiness scale total score before and after vestibular stimulation.

[Table/Fig-10]:

[Table/Fig-10]:

WHO QOL BREF transformed scores before and after vestibular stimulation (T1- physical health, T2-psychological, T3-social relationships, T4-Environment).

Student’s stress scores and heart rate did not change in vestibular group after vestibular stimulation. STAI S and STAI T scores were significantly different in vestibular and control groups on 28th day of vestibular stimulation. STAI S and STAI T scores of day 0 Vs day 28 in vestibular female (VF) and vestibular male (VM) were statistically significant [Table/Fig-3]. Systolic blood pressure, pulse pressure was within normal limits in vestibular group after vestibular stimulation. Diastolic blood pressure and mean arterial blood pressure were significantly decreased and remained within normal limits in vestibular group on 28th day of vestibular stimulation. Diastolic blood pressure significantly decreased in vestibular female (VF) and vestibular male (VM) when compared with baseline values. Postural fall in blood pressure was significantly different between control and vestibular groups on 14, 21, 28 days of vestibular stimulation. Postural fall in blood pressure of day 0 Vs day 14, 21 and 28 in VF and VM was significantly different [Table/Fig-4,5].

Spatial and verbal memory scores were not significantly different between control and vestibular groups before and after vestibular stimulation. Spatial memory scores of day 0 Vs day 28 in VF was significantly increased. Verbal memory scores of Day 0 Vs day28 in VF and VM were significantly increased. Po2 was not significantly different between control and vestibular groups before and after vestibular stimulation [Table/Fig-5].

Respiratory rate was significantly decreased in vestibular groups on days 7, 14, 21 and 28 when compared with control group. Respiratory rate of day 0 Vs day 21 and 28 in VF and VM was significantly decreased. Bleeding but not clotting time was significantly different between control and vestibular groups before and after vestibular stimulation [Table/Fig-6].

DASS depression scores were not significantly different between control and vestibular groups before and after vestibular stimulation. DASS depression scores of day 0 Vs day 21 and 28 in VF and VM was significantly different. DASS anxiety scores were significantly decreased in vestibular group on 14, 21, 28 days. DASS stress scores were not significantly different between control and vestibular groups before and after vestibular stimulation [Table/Fig-6,7].

PSQI sleep disturbance, PSQI sleep latency, PSQI total score was significantly decreased in vestibular after vestibular stimulation, which indicates improvement in sleep quality. PSQI sleep disturbance was significantly decreased before Vs after in VF. PSQI sleep latency was significantly decreased before Vs after in VF and VM groups. Epworth sleepiness scale total score was significantly decreased in vestibular group after vestibular stimulation, which indicates decrease in excessive daytime sleepiness [Table/Fig-7,8 and 9].

WHO QOL BREF transformed scores of four domains were not significantly different between control and vestibular groups before and after vestibular stimulation. WHO QOL BREF T2 (psychological domain) of Day 0 Vs day 28 in VF is significantly increased [Table/Fig-10].

Discussion

In our study significant decrease in salivary cortisol, autonomic and haematological parameters were observed following controlled vestibular stimulation [14]. We have attributed these beneficial effects of vestibular stimulation to inhibition of stress axes. However in the present study we have did not observed any change in student’s stress scores, DASS depression and stress scores, which may be due to short term vestibular stimulation.

It was reported that balance control and anxiety are closely linked [8]. Vestibular training may reduce some forms of anxiety, while cognitive and behavioural therapy of anxiety improves balancing performance [30]. Our study concurs with previous studies, as we have observed significant decrease in STAI S and STAI T scores and DASS anxiety scores following vestibular stimulation. Earlier studies reported that heart rate, blood pressure decreased following vestibular stimulation [31]. In the present study we have observed significant decrease in diastolic pressure and mean arterial blood pressure in vestibular group when compared to control group.

Previous researchers testified that anatomical connections exist between vestibular and autonomic nuclei [32]. Vestibular lesions modulated autonomic functions in humans and animals [33]. In the present study, systolic blood pressure difference in sitting and standing postures was significantly different in vestibular group when compared to control group. Vestibular system is connected with the brain areas related to cognition and bilateral vestibular lesion and can influence defects in spatial memory [34]. Our previous study categorically confirms that caloric vestibular stimulation with hot water, enhances cognition [35]. However, in the present study we observed only gradual improvement in spatial and verbal memory test scores. Vestibular stimulation modulates abdominal (expiratory) and phrenic (inspiratory) nerves activity [36]. Electrical or natural stimulation of the vestibular system results in changes in blood pressure and respiratory motor output and helps to maintain homeostasis during movement [37]. In the present study we have observed significant decrease in the respiratory rate in vestibular group when compared with control group. Although no significant change in p02 was observed. Also, bleeding but not clotting time was significantly different in control and vestibular groups before and after vestibular stimulation, hence short-term Vestibular stimulation may not have significant influence on bleeding and clotting time.

It is our routine day experience that motion promotes sleep as we become sleepy in boats, planes and cars [38]. It was reported that vestibular stimulation has positive impact on sleep [3941]. Our study provides further evidence that vestibular stimulation improves sleep quality as PSQI total score and Epworth sleepiness score was decreased significantly after vestibular stimulation. Indeed vestibular stimulation improves quality of life in patients with benign paroxysmal positional vertigo, irrespective of age and gender [42]. Our study concurs with earlier findings that we have observed marginal improvement in all four domains of WHO QOL BREF.

Limitations and Future Perspectives

The study sample size was small and duration of intervention was short. In our future studies, we plan to increase sample size and long-term intervention.

Conclusion

Our study provides preliminary evidence that vestibular stimulation can be used for stress management in students. We recommend further detailed study with higher sample size and long-term vestibular stimulation to improve impact of this practically applicable intervention to reduce stress.

Acknowledgments

We acknowledge with thanks all students of Little Flower Institute of Medical Sciences and Research centre, Angamaly for their active participation in the study. We also thank Dr B Uday Kumar Reddy, President, International Stress Management Association (ISMA IND), for his valuable suggestions.

Financial or Other Competing Interests

None.

References

  • [1].Sailesh KS, Archana R, Antony NJ, Mukkadan JK. You are never too old to swing. Research Journal of Pharmaceutical, Biological and Chemical Sciences. 2014;5(5):612–15. [Google Scholar]
  • [2].Gurvich C, Maller JJ, Lithgow B, Haghgooie S, Kulkarni J. Vestibular insights into cognition and psychiatry. Brian research. 2013;1537:244–59. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2013.08.058. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [3].Kempermann G. Why New Neurons? Possible Functions for Adult Hippocampal Neurogenesis. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2002;22(3):635–38. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-03-00635.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [4].Besnard S, et al. Influence of vestibular input on spatial and non-spatial memory and on hippocampal NMDA receptors. Hippocampus. 2012;22:814–26. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20942. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [5].Brandt T, et al. Vestibular loss causes hippocampal atrophy and impaired spatial memory in humans. Brain. 2005;128:2732–41. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh617. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [6].Hufner K, et al. Spatial memory and hippocampal volume in humans with unilateral vestibular deafferentation. Hippocampus. 2007;17:471–85. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20283. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [7].Balaban CD. Neural substrates linking balance control and anxiety. Physiol Behav. 2002;77:469–75. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(02)00935-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [8].Deroualle D, Lopez C. Toward a vestibular contribution to social cognition. Front Integr Neurosci. 2014;8:16. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2014.00016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [9].White-Traut RC, Schwertz D, McFarlin B, Kogan J. Salivary cortisol and behavioural state responses of healthy newborn infants to tactile-only and multisensory interventions. Res Nurs Health. 1988;11:31–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1552-6909.2008.00307.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [10].Yoo KH. The effects of auditory and vestibular stimulation on stress hormones in preterm infants. J Korean Acad Fundam Nurs. 2004;11:203–12. [Google Scholar]
  • [11].Markia B, Kovacs ZI, Palkovits M. Projections from the vestibular nuclei to the hypothalamic paraventricular nucleus: morphological evidence for the existence of a vestibular stress pathway in the rat brain. Brain Struct Funct. 2008;213:239–45. doi: 10.1007/s00429-008-0172-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [12].Winter L, Kruger THC, Laurens J, Engler H, Schedlowsk M, Straumann D, Wollmer MA. Vestibular stimulation on a motion- simulator impacts on mood states. Frontiers Psychology. 2012;3:499. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00499. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [13].Doneshka P, Levi E. Structural model of vestibular effects on the blood pressure. Acta Physiol Pharmacol Bulg. 1976;2(1):88–93. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [14].Sailesh KS, Archana R, Mukkadan JK. Controlled Vestibular Stimulation: A Physiological Method of Stress Relief. J Clin Diagn Res. 2014;8(12):BM01–02. doi: 10.7860/JCDR/2014/10312.5298. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [15].Sailesh KS, Mukkadan JK. Controlled vestibular stimulation, standardization of a physiological method to release stress in college students. Indian J Physiol Pharmacol. 2015;59(4):436–41. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [16].Spielberger CD. State-Trait Anxiety Inventory: Bibliography. 2nd ed. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press; 1989. [Google Scholar]
  • [17].Lovibond SH, Lovibond PF. Manual for the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales. 2nd. Ed. Sydney: Psychology Foundation; 1995. [Google Scholar]
  • [18].Baddeley AD. Your memory – a user’s guide. New York: Avery; 1993. [Google Scholar]
  • [19].Naveen KV, Nagarathna R, Nagendra HR, Telles S. Yoga breathing through a particular nostril increases spatial memory scores without lateralized effects. Psychol Rep. 1997;81:555–61. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1997.81.2.555. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [20].Kumar SS, George J, Mohamed JVK, Divya, Jose A, Mukkadan JK. Pulse rate and Sp02 in different phases of menstrual cycle in healthy women of kerala. Janaki medical college Journal of Medical Sciences. 2013;1(2):65–69. [Google Scholar]
  • [21].Buysse DJ, Reynolds III CF, Monk TH, Berman SR, Kupfer DJ. The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index: A new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Journal of Psychiatric Research. 1989;28(2):193–213. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(89)90047-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [22].Johns MW. A new method for measuring daytime sleepiness: The Epworth sleepiness scale. Sleep. 1991;14:540–45. doi: 10.1093/sleep/14.6.540. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [23].The World Health Organization Quality of Life assessment (WHOQOL): Position paper from the World Health Organization. Soc Sci Med. 1995;41:1403–09. doi: 10.1016/0277-9536(95)00112-k. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [24].Development of the World Health Organization WHOQOL-BREF quality of life assessment. The WHOQOL Group. Psychol Med. 1998;28:551–58. doi: 10.1017/s0033291798006667. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [25].Sai SK, Jasira VKM, George J, Mukkadan JK. Bleeding time and clotting time in healthy male and female college students of karukutty village, Kerala. Health prospect. Journal of public health. 2013;12(1):1–4. [Google Scholar]
  • [26].Hall JE. Guyton and Hall Text book of Medical Physiology. 12th edition. Haryana, India: Reed Elsevier India Private Limited; 2012. p. 931. [Google Scholar]
  • [27].Sharma N, Gupta V. Effect of examination stress on haematological and hemodynamic parameters in students. Int J Pharmacol Physiol. 2015;1(1):24–29. [Google Scholar]
  • [28].Srinivasan K, Vaz M, Sucharita S. A study of stress and autonomic nervous function in first year undergraduate medical students. Indian J Physiol Pharmacol. 2006;50(3):257–64. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [29].Ewing DJ, Clarke BF. Diagnosis and management of diabetic autonomic neuropathy. British Medical Journal. 1982;285:916–18. doi: 10.1136/bmj.285.6346.916. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [30].Kalueff AV, Ishikawa K, Griffith AJ. Anxiety and otovestibular disorders: Linking behavioural phenotypes in men and mice. Behavioural Brain Research. 2008;186:1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2007.07.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [31].Clench M, Williams B. Psycho/physiological effects of vestibular and audio stimulation: The Trinity Table. Subtle Energies & Energy Medicine. 2005;16(2):29–32. [Google Scholar]
  • [32].Biaggioni I, Costa F, Kaufmann H. Vestibular influences on autonomic cardiovascular control in humans. J Vestib Res. 1998;8(1):35–41. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [33].Yatesa BJ, Bronsteinc AM. The effects of vestibular system lesions on autonomic regulation: Observations, mechanisms, and clinical implications. Journal of Vestibular Research. 2005;15:119–29. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [34].Gurvich C, Maller JJ, Lithgow B, Haghgooie S, Kulkarni J. Vestibular insights into cognition and psychiatry. Brain Research. 2013;1537:244–59. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2013.08.058. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [35].Gopinath A, Archana R, Sailesh KS, Mukkadan JK. Effect of caloric vestibular stimulation on memory. Int J Pharm Bio Sci. 2015;6(3):(B)453–59. [Google Scholar]
  • [36].Rossiter CD, Hayden NL, Stocker SD, Yates BJ. Changes in outflow to respiratory pump muscles produced by natural vestibular stimulation. J Neurophysiol. 1996;76(5):3274–84. doi: 10.1152/jn.1996.76.5.3274. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [37].Yates BJ, Millert AD. Physiological evidence that the vestibular system participates in autonomic and respiratory control. Journal of Vestibular Research. 1998;8(1):17–25. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [38].Howard IP. Human visual orientation. New York: Wiley and sons; 1982. pp. 360–66. [Google Scholar]
  • [39].Krystal AD, Zammit GK, Wyatt JK, Quan SF, Edinger JD, White DP, et al. The Effect of Vestibular Stimulation in a Four-Hour Sleep Phase Advance Model of Transient Insomnia. J Clin Sleep Med. 2010;6(4):315–21. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [40].Woodward S, Tauber ES, Spielmann AJ, Thorpy MJ. Effects of otolithic vestibular stimulation on sleep. Sleep. 1990;13(6):533–37. doi: 10.1093/sleep/13.6.533. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [41].Ter Vrugt D, Peterson DR. The effects of vertical rocking frequencies on the arousal levels in two month old infants. Child develop. 1973;44:205–09. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • [42].Socher DD, Socher JA, Azzi VJB. Evaluation of quality of life pre- and post-vestibular rehabilitation in patients with benign paroxysmal positional vertigo associated with Meniere’s disease. Int Arch Otorhinolaryngol. 2012;16(4):430–36. doi: 10.7162/S1809-97772012000400002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Journal of Clinical and Diagnostic Research : JCDR are provided here courtesy of JCDR Research & Publications Private Limited

RESOURCES