Abstract
Background
Alternating hemiplegia of childhood is a very rare and serious neurodevelopmental syndrome; its genetic basis has recently been established. Its characteristic features include typically-unprovoked episodes of hemiplegia and other transient or more persistent neurological abnormalities.
Methods
We used transcranial magnetic stimulation to assess the effect of the condition on motor cortex neurophysiology both during and between attacks of hemiplegia. Nine people with alternating hemiplegia of childhood were recruited; eight were successfully tested using transcranial magnetic stimulation to study motor cortex excitability, using single and paired pulse paradigms. For comparison, data from ten people with epilepsy but not alternating hemiplegia, and ten healthy controls, were used.
Results
One person with alternating hemiplegia tested during the onset of a hemiplegic attack showed progressively diminishing motor cortex excitability until no response could be evoked; a second person tested during a prolonged bilateral hemiplegic attack showed unusually low excitability. Three people tested between attacks showed asymptomatic variation in cortical excitability, not seen in controls. Paired pulse paradigms, which probe intracortical inhibitory and excitatory circuits, gave results similar to controls.
Conclusions
We report symptomatic and asymptomatic fluctuations in motor cortex excitability in people with alternating hemiplegia of childhood, not seen in controls. We propose that such fluctuations underlie hemiplegic attacks, and speculate that the asymptomatic fluctuation we detected may be useful as a biomarker for disease activity.
Introduction
Alternating hemiplegia of childhood (AHC) is a rare, severe neurodevelopmental disease with characteristic transient and unpredictable episodes of hemiplegia or tetraplegia. The designation is misleading; although it presents during childhood, affected children continue to manifest symptoms and signs of the condition into adulthood. The condition is also associated with other paroxysmal disturbances such as epileptic seizures, tonic or dystonic spells, abnormal eye movements, dyspnoea and transient autonomic dysfunction [1,2]. Symptoms typically improve following sleep. We have recently demonstrated dynamic alterations in the ECG, unrelated to hemiplegic episodes or epileptic seizures, suggesting paroxysmal electrophysiological dysfunction affecting heart as well as brain in AHC [3]. There is typically also permanent neurological dysfunction, including developmental delay, intellectual difficulties, and in some cases movement disorder (for instance choreoathetosis, dystonia or ataxia). Epileptic seizures occur in 18%-50% of those with AHC [1,2,4,5]. Disease prevalence is estimated at 1:1,000,000, which may be an underestimate caused by variability in clinical presentation, lack of awareness of the condition, and frequent misdiagnosis as epilepsy or cerebral palsy [6]. A recent Danish study suggested a prevalence of 1:100,000 [7].
Standard neuroimaging is typically normal in AHC [2,1,8,9], although abnormalities such as non-specific cerebral atrophy, vermian atrophy, hippocampal sclerosis and cortical dysplasia have been reported [9]. Single-photon emission computed tomography (SPECT) scanning shows ictal cerebral hypoperfusion [8,10].
Mutations in the ATP1A3 gene, encoding Na+,K+-ATPase α3, account for around 80% of cases [5,11–14]; most are de novo. The encoded protein, the α3 subunit of Na+K+ATPase, is expressed widely in neuronal and cardiac tissue, contributing to trans-membrane sodium and potassium ion gradients, which are vital for cellular excitability [15].
The mutations causing AHC do not seem to affect gene expression, but instead modify protein function [5,16]. AHC-causing ATP1A3 mutations cluster in particular gene regions, leading to speculation that a specific change in protein function causes the condition. Conversely, mutations causing rapid-onset dystonia parkinsonism, an allelic condition caused by ATP1A3 mutations, do not cluster in particular gene regions. The specific mutation present may influence disease severity [12,14,16,17].
Various approaches have been employed to understand the effects of AHC-causing ATP1A3 mutations on the function of the protein and on cells and model organisms. Cell-based assays show a reduction in ADP production [5], and a reduction in transmembrane current [18], both implying reduced activity of the Na+,K+ ATPase transport mechanism. Structural modelling of mutant proteins has also suggested that mutations causing AHC may lead to impaired Na+ and K+ transport [19]. In vitro and in silico modelling suggest that the most common AHC-causing mutations cause loss of forward cycling and dominant negativity, and that loss of proton transport may be correlated with disease severity [20].
Mutations in ATP1A3 have been studied in animal models including mice, drosophila and zebrafish, and lead to varying degrees of neurological impairment including seizures and motor abnormalities. Some animal models exhibit a pathological response to stress, including seizures and balance problems (mice) and paralysis (drosophila) [16]. One mouse model with heterozygous mutation (Atp1a3Myk/+) demonstrates frontal hypometabolism, as well as reduced thalamocortical connectivity [21]. A novel knock-in mouse model carrying the most common heterozygous mutation causing AHC (p.D801N) was reported recently; this mouse displays many of the clinical features of AHC, and hippocampal slices show increased excitability in response to electrical stimulation [22].
Despite this progress, it is still not known how ATP1A3 mutations cause the clinical phenotype of AHC and in particular the hemiplegic attacks. The clinical pattern of weakness, the resolution of symptoms during sleep, the other neurological manifestations of the condition, the expression of the causative gene and SPECT studies together point to a central mechanism for the hemiplegic attacks, with motor cortex a possible substrate.
Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation (TMS) can be used non-invasively to stimulate motor cortex, triggering a motor evoked potential (MEP) in a contralateral target muscle. The ease of activation of motor cortex is a measure of cortical excitability, while paired pulse paradigms test inhibitory and excitatory intracortical circuitry [23].
We used TMS to probe the mechanisms of hemiplegic attacks in people with AHC, and hypothesised that TMS would detect fluctuations in motor cortical excitability not seen in controls.
Methods
The study was approved by National Research Ethics Service Committee London–Camden and Islington. TMS parameters were within the current safety guidelines [24]. Written informed consent was obtained. In people with mild learning difficulties, an approved adapted information sheet and consent form were used. In those lacking capacity, a relative was asked to provide assent. A safety questionnaire was completed for each participant.
Over a two year period, nine people with AHC were recruited from outpatient clinics, referrals and the Alternating Hemiplegia of Childhood UK support group. Eight were successfully tested, one was unable to cooperate. Ten healthy control subjects, on no medication, were recruited from a local database of volunteers. Ten consecutive people with epilepsy, on anti-epileptic medication, were recruited from clinic as further controls; none had a convulsive seizure in the 48 hours preceding or following testing. Intellectual difficulties were categorized as severe, moderate, mild, borderline or none according to ICD-10 guidelines [25]. Where formal neuropsychometry was unavailable, the degree of intellectual difficulties was assessed by a clinician blinded to the study outcome.
TMS was performed using two Magstim 2002 stimulators connected using a Bistim module, with a D70 alpha coil (Magstim, Whitland, UK), controlled by Micro 1401 hardware and Signal software (Cambridge Electronic Design, Cambridge, UK). EMG data were recorded from the contralateral adductor pollicis brevis (APB) muscle using surface electrodes, amplified using a Cambridge Electronic Design 1902 EMG amplifier, filtered using a 20Hz to 1kHz bandpass filter and 50Hz notch filter, and digitised at 2kHz for offline analysis.
Motor cortex was identified initially using skull landmarks. The motor hot-spot for the APB muscle, defined as the point on the scalp where stimulation produced the largest and most consistent EMG response, was identified. The hot-spot was marked on a tight-fitting cap to ensure a consistent site of stimulation. Where possible, the site was also marked using an ANT Visor neuronavigation system (ANT Neuro, Enschede, Netherlands); this included all controls and three out of five people with AHC who tolerated full testing. Data for each subject were collected during one sitting, in which the marked cap was not moved; only one subject was tested on two separate occasions, as outlined in the results section.
MEP was defined as the discrete EMG response seen in the target muscle 20-30ms after a TMS stimulus applied to the motor hot-spot. MEP amplitude was measured peak-to-peak and MEP latency was defined as the time interval between the TMS pulse and the first deviation of greater than three times the amplitude of the greatest fluctuation in the baseline resting EMG during 50ms prior to the stimulus.
These paradigms were used:
-
Resting Motor Threshold (rMT)
With the subject relaxed, stimulation intensity was increased from 40% total machine output in 2% increments until a reliable EMG response was obtained. The intensity was then decreased in 1% increments until rMT was found, defined as the minimum TMS stimulus intensity required to give a peak-to-peak EMG amplitude of 0.1mV or greater in at least 5 out of 10 trials from the relaxed target muscle. rMT is a correlate of motor cortex excitability, with a low rMT corresponding to high excitability and vice versa [23].
-
Stimulus response curve
EMG responses were measured with a range of TMS intensities (80%, 90%, 100%, 110%, 120% of rMT). Ten trials were performed at each intensity, in a randomised order. Average EMG amplitudes were plotted against stimulus size. This provides a more robust measure of neuronal excitability, using a wider range of stimulus intensities [23].
-
Short interval intracortical inhibition (SICI) and Intracortical Faciliation (ICF)
A subthreshold conditioning stimulus (80% rMT) was followed by a suprathreshold test stimulus (110% rMT) with an inter-stimulus interval of 2ms (SICI) and 10ms (ICF). Single, unconditioned pulses were used as a control paradigm. Ten trials of each paradigm were performed and averaged (a total of 20 paired pulses and 10 single pulses). The conditioned EMG amplitude is normalised to the unconditioned EMG amplitude, and is typically reduced in SICI and increased in ICF. SICI is mediated by GABAA-ergic intracortical circuits [26], while ICF is mediated by glutamatergic intracortical circuits, along with a possible reduction in GABA-ergic inhibition [23].
-
Long interval intracortical inhibition (LICI)
A supra-threshold conditioning pulse (110% rMT) was followed by a supra-threshold test pulse (110% rMT) with an inter-stimulus interval of 100ms; ten trials were performed. The EMG response to the test pulse was normalised to that generated by the conditioning stimulus; the conditioned response is typically reduced. This inhibition is mediated by GABAB-ergic intracortical circuits [23].
Stimulus-response curves were generated at two time points (Time 1 and Time 2). The stimulus-response curve for all was measured immediately after the motor threshold had been determined, and this was taken as Time 1. If a change in excitability was later detected, the stimulus-response curve was repeated and this was taken as Time 2. If no change was detected, the stimulus-response curve was repeated at the end of the testing session (an interval of 30 minutes), and this was taken as Time 2.
The mean total number of pulses administered to collect a full data-set in a single hemisphere was 318 (range 302–328). Subjects were required to sit still and remain alert with their eyes open, with the target muscle relaxed. Cooperation was confirmed by investigator observation and by EMG monitoring.
Statistical analysis
Results for cases and controls were compared using unpaired t-tests (rMT, SICI, ICF, LICI).
Where one individual was tested at two time-points, these results were compared using paired t-tests (MEP size, MEP latency). For stimulus response curves, a multivariate ANOVA was used to adjust for multiple comparisons. P values of 0.05 or lower were interpreted as statistically significant. All analysis was performed using IBM SPSS Statistics (Version 22.0, Armonk, NY).
Results
Subjects
Nine adults with a clinical diagnosis of AHC (A1-A9) underwent TMS. Five tolerated TMS well enough for a complete dataset to be collected (two both hemispheres, three single hemisphere); one (A2) was initially tested using a clinical protocol, before also later undergoing TMS using our research protocol on a separate occasion. Three further people with AHC only tolerated testing of rMT, as they found testing uncomfortable and did not wish to continue. One person with more severe intellectual difficulties did not tolerate any interpretable testing. Eight of the nine had known ATP1A3 mutations; one had a clinical diagnosis of AHC but no mutation was identified with whole genome sequencing (see Table 1). Ten consecutive people with treated epilepsy but not AHC (C1-C10) and ten healthy subjects on no medication (C11-20) were recruited as controls.
Table 1. Age, sex, mutation (subjects with AHC only), epilepsy diagnosis (epilepsy control subjects only), TMS paradigms tested, medication at time of testing and degree of intellectual difficulties for all subjects.
Subjects with AHC | |||||
Subject number | Sex, Age | ATP1A3 mutation | Testing Performed | Medication at time of testing (mg/day) | Intellectual Difficulties |
A1 | M, 18 | c.2401G>A | rMT | None | Mild* |
A2 | F, 27 | c.2431T>C | rMT, SRC, SICI, ICF, LICI | TPM 150, PHT 125, BAC 30, FLN 15 | Mild* |
A3 | F, 31 | None detected | rMT, SRC, SICI, ICF, LICI | ZNS 150, FLN 40, PZT 1, BAC 100, | None |
A4 | F, 36 | c.2839G>A | rMT, SRC, SICI, ICF, LICI | BAC 10 | Mild |
A5 | M, 19 | c.2314A>C | rMT | TPM 75, FLN 10, PZT 1, OMP 20 | Mild |
A6 | F, 35 | c.2318A>G | rMT, SRC, SICI, ICF, LICI | FLN 7.5 | Mild |
A7 | F, 18 | c.2839G>A | rMT | None | Mild* |
A8 | M, 18 | c.1072G>A | Testing impossible | PHT 300, VPA 1400 | Severe* |
A9 | F, 33 | c.1010T>G | rMT, SRC, SICI, ICF, LICI | LTG 400, PGB 200, AZM 200 | None |
Control subjects with epilepsy | |||||
Subject number | Sex, Age | Epilepsy Diagnosis | Testing Performed | Medication at time of testing (mg/day) | Intellectual Difficulties |
C1 | M, 18 | Focal Symptomatic | rMT, SRC, SICI, ICF, LICI | VPA 1000, PGB 400 | None |
C2 | F, 28 | ADNFLE | rMT, SRC, SICI, ICF, LICI | OXC 900, PER 10, PGB 225 | None |
C3 | M, 50 | Focal Cryptogenic | rMT, SRC, SICI, ICF, LICI | PHT 200, LCM 200 | None |
C4 | F, 61 | Focal Cryptogenic | rMT, SRC, SICI, ICF, LICI | PHT 400, LCM 600 | None |
C5 | F, 47 | Genetic Generalised | rMT, SRC, SICI, ICF, LICI | LTG 300, CLB 10 ZNS 200 | None |
C6 | M, 44 | Unclassified | rMT, SRC, SICI, ICF, LICI | LCM 400, OXC 1200 PER 2 | None |
C7 | F, 27 | Genetic Generalised | rMT, SRC, SICI, ICF, LICI | PHT 475, LMT 300 | None |
C8 | M, 32 | Genetic Generalised | rMT, SRC, SICI, ICF, LICI | VPA 2600, TPM 100, CLB 40 | None |
C9 | M, 29 | Focal Cryptogenic | rMT, SRC, SICI, ICF, LICI | LTG 100, CBZ 1200, PER 12, LEV 3000 | None |
C10 | F, 21 | Focal Cryptogenic | rMT, SRC, SICI, ICF, LICI | LEV 1500 | None |
Healthy control subjects | |||||
Subject number | Sex, Age | Epilepsy Diagnosis | Testing Performed | Medication at time of testing (mg/day) | Intellectual Difficulties |
C11 | F,34 | N/A | rMT, SRC, SICI, ICF, LICI | None | None |
C12 | F, 36 | N/A | rMT, SRC, SICI, ICF, LICI | None | None |
C13 | M, 35 | N/A | rMT, SRC, SICI, ICF, LICI | None | None |
C14 | F, 30 | N/A | rMT, SRC, SICI, ICF, LICI | None | None |
C15 | M, 32 | N/A | rMT, SRC, SICI, ICF, LICI | None | None |
C16 | M, 42 | N/A | rMT, SRC, SICI, ICF, LICI | None | None |
C17 | M, 27 | N/A | rMT, SRC, SICI, ICF, LICI | None | None |
C18 | F, 30 | N/A | rMT, SRC, SICI, ICF, LICI | None | None |
C19 | M, 29 | N/A | rMT, SRC, SICI, ICF, LICI | None | None |
C20 | M, 23 | N/A | rMT, SRC, SICI, ICF, LICI | None | None |
* indicates neuropsychometry assessment unavailable, intellectual difficulties assessed by clinician blinded to study outcome.
Abbreviations
rMT = motor threshold
SRC = stimulus-response curve
SICI = short interval intracortical inhibition
ICF = intracortical facilitation
LICI = long interval intracortical inhibition
TPM = topiramate
PHT = phenytoin
BAC = baclofen
FLN = flunarizine
PZT = pizotifen
ZNS = zonisamide
VPA = valproate
LTG = lamotrigine
PGB = pregabalin
AZM = acetazolamide
OMP = omeprazole
LTD = loratidine
OXC = oxcarbazepine
PER = perampanel
LCM = lacosamide
CLB = clobazam
LEV = levetiracetam
ADNFLE = autosomal dominant nocturnal frontal lobe epilepsy.
Table 1 shows their demographic information, genetic diagnosis (for those with AHC), epilepsy diagnosis (for epilepsy controls), medication at time of testing and degree of intellectual difficulties. There were no significant differences in age or proportions of males to females between the AHC group and either control group (age: unpaired, two-tailed t-test, P = 0.09 (AHC vs epilepsy controls), P = 0.07 (AHC vs healthy controls); sex: Fisher’s exact test P = 0.65 (AHC vs epilepsy controls), P = 0.37 (AHC vs healthy controls). Intellectual difficulties are common in AHC, and this is reflected in our population. Those with AHC were taking a mean of 2.1 medications each, while epilepsy controls were taking a mean of 2.5 medications each (see Table 1); there was no significant difference (unpaired, two-tailed t-test, P = 0.53).
TMS during hemiplegic attacks
A1 was tested during a prolonged hemiplegic attack. Onset of left sided weakness occurred five days prior to testing, with partial resolution by the day of testing; mild ongoing weakness was evident on examination. Right-sided symptoms had occurred for two days prior to testing and had not resolved, with moderate weakness evident at the time of testing. His legs were mildly affected and he was able to walk. No motor response could be obtained at intensities of up to 80% maximum stimulator output on either side. Higher strengths were not tolerated and he declined further testing with TMS when well. rMT above 80% is rare in the general population; in a study of rMT in 141 healthy people, using the same model TMS machine (but a different coil), two people (1.4%) had rMT over 80% [27]. A1 was not taking any medication at all at the time of testing.
A2 experienced an acute bilateral hemiplegic attack during her first TMS testing session. At the time, she was experiencing frequent (daily) brief bilateral attacks; her mother, who was present during testing, confirmed this attack was typical. During stimulation with an intensity of 130% rMT, motor responses gradually decreased over a one-minute period, until no response could be obtained. Testing was repeated five minutes later, following recovery, using the same stimulus intensity; no similar decrement in MEPs was found. These data are shown in Fig 1. MEP latency showed greater variability during the onset of hemiplegic symptoms compared to after recovery, although average latency was similar (see Fig 1).
TMS between hemiplegic attacks
rMT was measured in seven people with AHC without hemiplegic symptoms, or any other paroxysmal episodes, at the time of testing. Average rMT was 53% of maximum machine output, standard deviation (SD) 8.2%, range 38–64% (see Fig 2). This rMT is lower than the average rMT in our healthy control group (61%, range 37–82%), although the difference was not statistically significant (unpaired, two-tailed t-test P = 0.15). Controls with epilepsy had a significantly higher average rMT of 69% (SD 10.0%), (unpaired, two-tailed t-test P = 0.002), implying higher cortical excitability in the AHC group between hemiplegic attacks compared to people with epilepsy on treatment.
There was no significant difference between cases and controls in SICI, ICF or LICI, see Fig 2 (unpaired two-tailed t-tests: AHC vs epilepsy controls—SICI, P = 0.46; ICF, P = 0.70; LICI, P = 0.13; AHC vs healthy controls—SICI, P = 0.09; ICF, P = 0.75; LICI, P = 0.06).
Three out of five people with AHC who tolerated full testing (subjects A2, A3, A4) showed a discrete increase or decrease in MEP amplitudes at some point during a single testing session, without any hemiplegic symptoms, and without any change in stimulus size or other experimental conditions. Fig 3 compares ten traces recorded at the start of testing (labelled “Time 1”) with ten traces recorded after an interval (labelled “Time 2”), with a change in cortical excitability detected.
The change in amplitude was statistically significant in all three as shown in Table 2. Between Time 1 and Time 2, subjects were sitting relaxed in the testing room, undergoing TMS testing with other paradigms.
Table 2. Intra-individual variation in motor evoked potential amplitudes in people with AHC.
Time 1 | Time 2 | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Subject | Mean Amplitude (mV) | SD | Mean Amplitude (mV) | SD | P value (2-tailed unpaired t-test) |
A2 | 0.238 | 0.086 | 0.405 | 0.139 | 0.002 |
A3 | 0.137 | 0.065 | 0.053 | 0.024 | 0.008 |
A4 | 0.401 | 0.084 | 0.709 | 0.424 | 0.024 |
Mean amplitudes and standard deviation (SD) of MEPs at Time 1 and Time 2. Responses are to stimulation at 110% rMT. P values are for a two-tailed unpaired t-test comparing MEP sizes at Time 1 and Time 2. Note that in this small sample there is no consistent pattern such that rMT is always bigger at either Time 1 or Time 2.
Individual A2 also showed a statistically significant change in MEP latency from Time 1 to Time 2, shown in Fig 3 (Time 1, mean 22.97ms, SD 0.47ms; Time 2, mean 21.67ms, SD 1.02ms; two-tailed unpaired t-test, p<0.001). Subjects A3 and A4 did not show any change in latency. No change in excitability or latency was seen in two other participants with AHC who tolerated full testing, or in any controls.
Time 1 and Time 2 were less than 30 minutes apart. There was no accompanying clinical correlate, and none of these people experienced paroxysmal hemiplegic symptoms during testing; the bilateral hemiplegic attack in A2 occurred during an earlier testing session. A2 exhibited some dystonic posturing of the left arm between Time 1 and Time 2; recordings were taken from the left adductor pollicis brevis muscle. There was no significant change in pre-stimulus muscle activation between Time 1 and Time 2 (50ms pre-stimulus EMG quantified using a root mean squared model, compared using unpaired, 2-tailed t-tests). The site of stimulation was marked on a tight fitting latex cap that was not moved between testing sessions. In A3 and A4, the position of the coil was also confirmed using neuronavigation. There was no change in TMS stimulus intensity. All subjects were awake and responsive during testing, with their eyes open.
Stimulus-response curves showed significant changes from Time 1 to Time 2 in A2 (only right hemisphere tested, P<0.001), A3 (right hemisphere P = 0.02, left hemisphere P<0.001) and A4 (right hemisphere P = 0.004, left hemisphere no significant change). The stimulus-response curves are illustrated in Fig 4. A3 and A4 had both hemispheres tested during the same testing session; in A3 a significant change in cortical excitability was evident in both hemispheres while in A4, a change was only seen in one hemisphere. Both increases and decreases in excitability were seen, in different participants.
Testing was performed in ten epilepsy controls and ten healthy controls using an identical experimental paradigm. None of them showed a significant change in cortical excitability during the testing period. Stimulus-response curves and P values for epilepsy controls are shown in Fig 5, and for healthy controls in Fig 6.
Discussion
Our key finding is the fluctuation in cortical excitability seen in people with AHC, but not in controls. The fluctuation appears to affect the size and latency of MEP for a given stimulus strength and was seen in three of five people with AHC who tolerated full testing, but none of our 20 controls. This instability was most marked during the onset of a hemiplegic attack, but was also present without any obvious clinical correlate in asymptomatic individuals. No such fluctuation has been reported in TMS studies across several neurological conditions; for example, Parkinson’s disease [28], multiple sclerosis [29] and epilepsy [30]. The findings seem unique to AHC and may constitute an electrophysiological correlate of paretic or plegic attacks.
Two subjects were tested during a bilateral hemiplegic attack: no MEPs could be recorded in either. In A2, a brief bilateral hemiplegic attack occurred during testing, preceded and followed by a period of normal cortical excitability. A decline in cortical excitability during the onset of the attack was recorded, with consecutive MEPs falling in amplitude and fluctuating in latency until no MEP could be elicited, in parallel with the clinical development of a bilateral hemiplegic attack. Subject A2 was experiencing frequent brief bilateral hemiplegic attacks around the time of testing, and her mother (who was present) confirmed that the attack was typical. Her hemiplegic attacks do not affect alertness or level of consciousness.
A1 was tested only during a prolonged bilateral hemiplegic attack; he declined further testing when his symptoms had improved. During his attack, despite stimulation of both hemispheres up to the maximum tolerated (80% of total) stimulator output, no MEPs could be recorded. None of the other participants with AHC had an rMT above 60%. Taken together, these findings demonstrate low motor cortex excitability during hemiplegic episodes.
Three individuals with AHC showed variation in cortical excitability, without any simultaneous hemiplegic symptoms, as demonstrated by a change in MEP size and latency, and a shift in the stimulus-response curve. The stimulus-response curve is a commonly tested TMS parameter. Its stability in individuals is well documented, even when people are tested on different days [31,32]. Fluctuations of this nature during a single testing session have not been previously reported and we have not encountered similar results despite significant experience in the field.
Between attacks, people with AHC had rMT lower than our healthy volunteers, despite several of our participants with AHC taking medication known to increase rMT. This difference did not reach statistical significance, but is in the same direction as animal data suggesting increased cortical excitability in AHC [22]. Our disease controls, on a similar number of medications, had a significantly higher average rMT than the AHC group. We note, however, that medication in the two groups does not match exactly; more disease controls were taking sodium channel blockers, which are known to increase rMT, and this may account for the difference observed. Further investigation would require a larger dataset, and ideally would include TMS testing of people with AHC on no medication.
Results of paired pulse testing and cortical silent period were similar to controls, suggesting that intracortical excitatory and inhibitory function is relatively unaffected by AHC. Subtle abnormalities in these circuits might not be detected by our paradigms.
Some dynamic factors affect cortical excitability. Pre-activation of the target muscle increases excitability but this possibility was excluded by evaluation of the EMG traces. Mental imagery relating to movement of the target muscle can also reduce motor threshold [33], but no mental imagery of movement was encouraged. Participants might conceivably become increasingly relaxed as they acclimatised to testing. These factors should have applied equally to controls.
Accidentally changing position of the TMS coil during an experiment would affect the stimulus-response curve. An unintended change in position would move away from the motor hotspot, which should reduce MEP size. An increase (as seen in A2 and A4) could only be generated if the motor hotspot had originally been identified incorrectly, and the coil was then accidentally moved to a better stimulation site. The position of the coil was marked on a tight-fitting cap that was kept in place throughout testing. By comparison, many have reported TMS experiments in the same people performed on different days, which requires the motor hot-spot to be newly identified for each test. Few of these other reported experiments used neuronavigation and the same spot may not have been selected when the repeat experiments were performed in those studies. Nonetheless, those experiments achieved reliable data, and were sensitive enough to detect small changes in cortical excitability, such as those caused by a single dose of an anti-epileptic drug [34]. In two participants in whom we recorded a change in excitability, neuronavigation confirmed that there had been no change in position of the coil. Controls were tested under exactly the same conditions, and no similar change was seen. People with AHC and controls were all tested by experienced TMS operators (WMS, GS, DH, MD).
Of three participants in whom fluctuating excitability was recorded, two had proven mutations in ATP1A3 (A2, A4). No mutation was found in A3 (tested previously with whole genome sequencing); around 20% of people with a clinical diagnosis of AHC have no detected mutation in ATP1A3 [5]. An unknown second gene may lead to the same clinical and neurophysiological phenotype. Alternatively, some mutations affecting ATP1A3 may be missed using current techniques.
Two people with AHC and known ATP1A3 mutations, in whom full testing was possible, did not show fluctuating cortical excitability. Both these participants live independently and are at the mild end of the AHC spectrum. However, more data would be required to establish any possible link between neurophysiology and phenotype or mutation type in humans. An effect of ATP1A3 mutation type on physiology has recently been demonstrated in Xenopus oocytes [20].
Our work has limitations. Inevitably in such a rare condition numbers are small. Only two subjects could be tested during hemiplegic episodes. Among those with AHC, not all were able to tolerate full testing. Severe intellectual difficulties may limit participation in TMS research and the subject we recruited with severe intellectual difficulties (A8) could not be tested; excluding such people may introduce selection bias. Several further individuals with AHC had intellectual and behavioural difficulties; some found TMS uncomfortable and did not complete testing. Those with mild intellectual difficulties who tolerated testing cooperated well. Fluctuating cortical excitability was seen in those with and without intellectual difficulties.
Some participants were taking neurologically-active medication (see Table 1). Our epilepsy controls were also taking medication; their MEPs and stimulus-response curves do not show similar variability (see Fig 5). Inevitably, the drugs taken by the AHC group and controls do not match exactly; for instance, flunarizine is used in AHC but not used in epilepsy. One participant in whom we measured a fluctuation was only taking baclofen, a drug known not to affect MT or MEP size [34]. No similar fluctuations during a single TMS testing session have been reported in extensive TMS data from healthy subjects given anti-epileptic and other drugs [34]. Participant A1 was on no medication when tested during a hemiplegic attack; medication effects cannot explain his low cortical excitability, and neither can they explain the transiently reduced cortical excitability seen in A2 during a hemiplegic episode.
rMT and the stimulus-response curve are complex and compound measures that rely on the entire motor pathway to be functioning. There is evidence that hemiplegic attacks in AHC have a central mechanism, but changes at any point from the motor cortex down to the muscles themselves could affect the recordings. ATP1A3 is expressed in the cortex but also in other brain regions, the spinal cord, motor nerves and muscle [35,36]. A recent study of somatosensory evoked potentials in people with AHC suggested possible disinhibition in the somatosensory system [4]. This phenomenon also appeared to show dynamic fluctuations, particularly during hemiplegic attacks. A recent knock-in mouse model with clinical features closely mimicking AHC showed both increased neuronal excitability (possibly predisposing to seizures) and increased predisposition to cortical spreading depression, which was suggested as a possible mechanism for hemiplegic attacks [22]. We recently showed abnormalities in the ECG in AHC, with characteristics suggesting defective repolarisation reserve: the abnormalities were dynamic, increasing in prevalence with age, but also changing over time, and even beat-to-beat, in the same individual [3]. We propose that AHC is characterised by sub-clinical dynamic instability in excitable tissues, which can become clinically manifest as hemiplegic episodes and occasionally dysfunction in other affected tissues. This hypothesis is testable in some existing models, and further work will be needed to explore how these dynamic fluctuations are caused by the mutant ATP1A3 protein.
Conclusions
This study used TMS to study motor cortex excitability in people with AHC. Low excitability was found during hemiplegic attacks. Between attacks, cortical excitability showed asymptomatic fluctuations, not seen in controls. We hypothesize that the asymptomatic excitability changes we observed were due to altered function of the ATP1A3 mutant protein, and that larger fluctuations, either spontaneous or triggered by external factors, cause hemiplegic attacks. It is possible that the observed fluctuation might act as a biomarker for disease activity, providing information as a surrogate for hemiplegic attacks that occur on a much longer time scale. Such a biomarker might be of value, for example, for trials of novel treatments for AHC.
Supporting Information
Acknowledgments
We are grateful to Jill Bailey and the Alternating Hemiplegia of Childhood UK Support Group for their help with recruiting people for this study and to Gail Bell for advice on statistical analysis.
Abbreviations
- AHC
alternating hemiplegia of childhood
- SPECT
single-photon emission computed tomography
- TMS
transcranial magnetic stimulation
- MEP
motor evoked potential
- APB
adductor pollicis brevis
- rMT
resting motor threshold
- SICI
short interval intracortical inhibition
- ICF
intracortical faciliation
- LICI
long interval intracortical inhibition
- SD
standard deviation
Data Availability
All relevant data are within the paper and supporting information files.
Funding Statement
The study was funded by Epilepsy Society (UK) and undertaken at University College London Hospitals/University College London, which received a proportion of funding from the Department of Health’s National Institute for Health Research Biomedical Research Centres funding scheme. JWS is supported by the Marvin Weil Epilepsy Research Fund.
References
- 1.Bourgeois M, Aicardi J, Goutières F. Alternating hemiplegia of childhood. J Pediatr. 1993. May;122(5):673–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Mikati MA, Kramer U, Zupanc ML, Shanahan RJ. Alternating hemiplegia of childhood: clinical manifestations and long-term outcome. Pediatr Neurol. 2000. August;23(2):134–41. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Jaffer F, Avbersek A, Vavassori R, Fons C, Campistol J, Stagnaro M, et al. Faulty cardiac repolarization reserve in alternating hemiplegia of childhood broadens the phenotype. Brain. 2015. August 20;awv243. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Vollono C, Rinalduzzi S, Miliucci R, Vigevano F, Valeriani M. Somatosensory system hyperexcitability in alternating hemiplegia of childhood. Eur J Neurol Off J Eur Fed Neurol Soc. 2014. July 17; [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Heinzen EL, Swoboda KJ, Hitomi Y, Gurrieri F, Nicole S, de Vries B, et al. De novo mutations in ATP1A3 cause alternating hemiplegia of childhood. Nat Genet. 2012. September;44(9):1030–4. 10.1038/ng.2358 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Neville B, Ninan M. The treatment and management of alternating hemiplegia of childhood. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2007. October 1;49(10):777–80. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Hoei-Hansen CE, Dali CÍ, Lyngbye TJB, Duno M, Uldall P. Alternating hemiplegia of childhood in Denmark: clinical manifestations and ATP1A3 mutation status. Eur J Paediatr Neurol EJPN Off J Eur Paediatr Neurol Soc. 2014. January;18(1):50–4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Zupanc ML, Dobkin JA, Perlman SB. 123I-iodoamphetamine SPECT brain imaging in alternating hemiplegia. Pediatr Neurol. 1991. February;7(1):35–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Panagiotakaki E, Gobbi G, Neville B, Ebinger F, Campistol J, Nevšímalová S, et al. Evidence of a non-progressive course of alternating hemiplegia of childhood: study of a large cohort of children and adults. Brain. 2010. December 1;133(12):3598–610. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Aminian A, Strashun A, Rose A. Alternating hemiplegia of childhood: studies of regional cerebral blood flow using 99mTc-hexamethylpropylene amine oxime single-photon emission computed tomography. Ann Neurol. 1993. January;33(1):43–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Ishii A, Saito Y, Mitsui J, Ishiura H, Yoshimura J, Arai H, et al. Identification of ATP1A3 mutations by exome sequencing as the cause of alternating hemiplegia of childhood in Japanese patients. PloS One. 2013;8(2):e56120 10.1371/journal.pone.0056120 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Panagiotakaki E, De Grandis E, Stagnaro M, Heinzen EL, Fons C, Sisodiya S, et al. Clinical profile of patients with ATP1A3 mutations in Alternating Hemiplegia of Childhood—a study of 155 patients. Orphanet J Rare Dis [Internet]. 2015. September 26 [cited 2015 Oct 12];10 Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4583741/ [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Rosewich H, Thiele H, Ohlenbusch A, Maschke U, Altmüller J, Frommolt P, et al. Heterozygous de-novo mutations in ATP1A3 in patients with alternating hemiplegia of childhood: a whole-exome sequencing gene-identification study. Lancet Neurol. 2012. September;11(9):764–73. 10.1016/S1474-4422(12)70182-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Viollet L, Glusman G, Murphy KJ, Newcomb TM, Reyna SP, Sweney M, et al. Alternating Hemiplegia of Childhood: Retrospective Genetic Study and Genotype-Phenotype Correlations in 187 Subjects from the US AHCF Registry. PloS One. 2015;10(5):e0127045 10.1371/journal.pone.0127045 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Dobretsov M, Stimers JR. Neuronal function and alpha3 isoform of the Na/K-ATPase. Front Biosci J Virtual Libr. 2005;10:2373–96. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Heinzen EL, Arzimanoglou A, Brashear A, Clapcote SJ, Gurrieri F, Goldstein DB, et al. Distinct neurological disorders with ATP1A3 mutations. Lancet Neurol. 2014. May;13(5):503–14. 10.1016/S1474-4422(14)70011-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Sasaki M, Ishii A, Saito Y, Morisada N, Iijima K, Takada S, et al. Genotype-phenotype correlations in alternating hemiplegia of childhood. Neurology. 2014. February 11;82(6):482–90. 10.1212/WNL.0000000000000102 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Koenderink JB, Geibel S, Grabsch E, De Pont JJHHM, Bamberg E, Friedrich T. Electrophysiological analysis of the mutated Na,K-ATPase cation binding pocket. J Biol Chem. 2003. December 19;278(51):51213–22. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Ogawa H, Shinoda T, Cornelius F, Toyoshima C. Crystal structure of the sodium-potassium pump (Na+,K+-ATPase) with bound potassium and ouabain. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2009. August 18;106(33):13742–7. 10.1073/pnas.0907054106 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Li M, Jazayeri D, Corry B, Melodi McSweeney K, Heinzen EL, Goldstein DB, et al. A functional correlate of severity in alternating hemiplegia of childhood. Neurobiol Dis. 2015. February 12; [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Kirshenbaum GS, Dawson N, Mullins JGL, Johnston TH, Drinkhill MJ, Edwards IJ, et al. Alternating hemiplegia of childhood-related neural and behavioural phenotypes in Na+,K+-ATPase α3 missense mutant mice. PloS One. 2013;8(3):e60141 10.1371/journal.pone.0060141 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Hunanyan AS, Fainberg NA, Linabarger M, Arehart E, Leonard AS, Adil SM, et al. Knock-in mouse model of alternating hemiplegia of childhood: Behavioral and electrophysiologic characterization. Epilepsia. 2015. January;56(1):82–93. 10.1111/epi.12878 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Terao Y, Ugawa Y. Basic mechanisms of TMS. J Clin Neurophysiol Off Publ Am Electroencephalogr Soc. 2002. August;19(4):322–43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Rossi S, Hallett M, Rossini PM, Pascual-Leone A. Safety, ethical considerations, and application guidelines for the use of transcranial magnetic stimulation in clinical practice and research. Clin Neurophysiol Off J Int Fed Clin Neurophysiol. 2009. December;120(12):2008–39. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.World Health Organization. The ICD-10 classification of mental and behavioural disorders: clinical descriptions and diagnostic guidelines Geneva: Harvard; 1992. [Google Scholar]
- 26.Rothwell JC, Day BL, Thompson PD, Kujirai T. Short latency intracortical inhibition: one of the most popular tools in human motor neurophysiology. J Physiol. 2009. January 15;587(Pt 1):11–2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Wassermann EM. Variation in the response to transcranial magnetic brain stimulation in the general population. Clin Neurophysiol. 2002. July;113(7):1165–71. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Cantello R, Tarletti R, Civardi C. Transcranial magnetic stimulation and Parkinson’s disease. Brain Res Brain Res Rev. 2002. February;38(3):309–27. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Kale N, Ozdemir G, Coban E, Agaoglu J. Evaluating TMS Motor Evoked Potentials in Early Stages of Multiple Sclerosis Aid in Predicting Disability (P4.191). Neurology. 2014. August 4;82(10 Supplement):P4.191–P4.191. [Google Scholar]
- 30.Bauer PR, Kalitzin S, Zijlmans M, Sander JW, Visser GH. Cortical excitability as a potential clinical marker of epilepsy: a review of the clinical application of transcranial magnetic stimulation. Int J Neural Syst. 2014. March;24(2):1430001 10.1142/S0129065714300010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Boroojerdi B, Battaglia F, Muellbacher W, Cohen L. Mechanisms influencing stimulus-response properties of the human corticospinal system. Clin Neurophysiol. 2001. May;112(5):931–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Malcolm MP, Triggs WJ, Light KE, Shechtman O, Khandekar G, Gonzalez Rothi LJ. Reliability of motor cortex transcranial magnetic stimulation in four muscle representations. Clin Neurophysiol Off J Int Fed Clin Neurophysiol. 2006. May;117(5):1037–46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Abbruzzese G, Trompetto C, Schieppati M. The excitability of the human motor cortex increases during execution and mental imagination of sequential but not repetitive finger movements. Exp Brain Res. 1996. October;111(3):465–72. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Ziemann U. TMS and drugs. Clin Neurophysiol. 2004. August;115(8):1717–29. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Edwards IJ, Bruce G, Lawrenson C, Howe L, Clapcote SJ, Deuchars SA, et al. Na+/K+ ATPase α1 and α3 isoforms are differentially expressed in α- and γ-motoneurons. J Neurosci Off J Soc Neurosci. 2013. June 12;33(24):9913–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Bøttger P, Tracz Z, Heuck A, Nissen P, Romero-Ramos M, Lykke-Hartmann K. Distribution of Na/K-ATPase alpha 3 isoform, a sodium-potassium P-type pump associated with rapid-onset of dystonia parkinsonism (RDP) in the adult mouse brain. J Comp Neurol. 2011. February 1;519(2):376–404. 10.1002/cne.22524 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
All relevant data are within the paper and supporting information files.