Abstract
Childhood abuse is a risk factor for the development of externalizing characteristics and disorders, including antisocial personality disorder and psychopathy. However, the precise relationships between particular types of childhood maltreatment and subsequent antisocial and psychopathic traits remain unclear. Using a large sample of incarcerated adult male criminal offenders (n = 183), the current study confirmed that severity of overall childhood maltreatment was linked to severity of both psychopathy and antisocial personality disorder in adulthood. Moreover, this relationship was particularly strong for physical abuse and the antisocial facet of psychopathy. Sexual abuse history was uniquely related to juvenile conduct disorder severity, rather than adult psychopathy or antisocial behaviors. Additionally, there was a significantly stronger relationship between childhood maltreatment and juvenile conduct disorder than between childhood maltreatment and ASPD or psychopathy. These findings bolster and clarify the link between childhood maltreatment and antisocial behavior later in life.
Keywords: psychopathy, childhood maltreatment, antisocial personality, conduct disorder
INTRODUCTION
Childhood abuse and neglect have been associated with the subsequent development of diverse forms of psychopathology, including both internalizing and externalizing symptoms. Although the relationship between abuse early in life and the development of internalizing disorders such as depression and anxiety has been widely recognized (De Bellis, 2001), the relationship between childhood abuse and subsequent externalizing symptomology is less clear (but see Beauchaine & McNulty, 2013), particularly in regard to how early maltreatment may exacerbate the development of severe externalizing disorders such as antisocial personality disorder (ASPD) and psychopathy in adulthood.
ASPD is characterized by impulsive, reckless, and aggressive behaviors that commonly result in persistent criminal offending beginning early in development (Association, 2013). The diagnosis of ASPD in adulthood requires that the individual meet criteria for conduct disorder (CD) before the age of 15. Nearly 80% of the adult prison population meets diagnostic criteria for ASPD (Hare, 1983). By contrast, psychopathy refers to a more specific cluster of traits, with a prevalence of 15–25% among the adult prison population (Hart & Hare, 1996). In addition to the reckless and antisocial behavioral components of ASPD, the diagnosis of psychopathy also entails interpersonal and affective characteristics such as grandiosity, pathological lying, and callousness (Hare, 1996a). The most frequently used measure of psychopathy in forensic samples, the Psychopathy Checklist-Revised ((Hare & Neumann, 2006) captures these characteristics in both a two-Factor model (Harpur, Hakstian, & Hare, 1988) and a four-Facet model (Hare & Neumann, 2005). The interpersonal and affective features of psychopathy load onto Factor 1, whereas the lifestyle and antisocial features load onto Factor 2. Factor 1 can be further disaggregated into two Facets; Facet 1 encapsulates the interpersonal components of psychopathy (e.g., glibness, grandiosity) and Facet 2 encompasses the affective traits (e.g., callousness, shallow affect). Similarly, Factor 2 can be broken into Facets 3 and 4; Facet 3 includes the lifestyle features of psychopathy (e.g., impulsivity, irresponsibility) whereas Facet 4 corresponds to the antisocial and criminal features of psychopathy, including characteristics that closely approximate childhood CD (e.g., juvenile delinquency, early behavioral problems). Within this framework, ASPD and psychopathy share Factor 2 traits, whereas Factor 1 is unique to psychopathy. Accordingly, while virtually all highly psychopathic criminal offenders will meet criteria for ASPD, not all who meet for ASPD also meet for psychopathy (Hare, 1996c). Recent empirical studies demonstrate pronounced differences in specific cognitive, affective, and neurobiological characteristics between individuals with ASPD-only and individuals with ASPD+psychopathy (Gregory et al., 2012; Kolla et al., 2013). However, it is unclear whether ASPD and psychopathy differ in terms of their associations with childhood abuse history.
Both ASPD and psychopathy have been associated with adverse early life experiences, including childhood abuse. For instance, diagnosis of ASPD has been associated with severe trauma history, particularly with high rates of physical and sexual abuse (Bierer et al., 2003; Egeland, Yates, Appleyard, & Van Dulmen, 2002). Similarly, juvenile delinquency has been specifically associated with parental hostility (Raskin White, Bates, & Buyske, 2001), and some have found that youth who experienced physical abuse early in childhood were at greater risk for being arrested as juveniles, for both violent and non-violent offenses (Lansford et al., 2007). Using a sample of incarcerated men, Poythress, Skeem, and Lilienfeld (2006) found that high rates of abuse predicted elevated levels of psychopathy, but this relationship was specific to the impulsive-lifestyle features that are associated with both psychopathy and ASPD (Facet 4 items, the juvenile/antisocial features of psychopathy, were not specifically considered in this study). Graham, Kimonis, Wasserman, and Kline (2012) reported similar findings, including a relationship between childhood abuse and Facet 4, though their sample consisted of civilly committed sex offenders, so it is unclear how well these findings would generalize to non-sex offender populations.
One significant gap in the existing literature in this area is how specific types of maltreatment may influence long-term developmental trajectories. While physical abuse has frequently been linked to externalizing behaviors (Lansford, et al., 2007), sexual and emotional abuses have greater associations with depression, suicidality, and dissociation (Kisiel & Lyons, 2014; Plunkett et al., 2001; Shapero et al., 2014). Less is known about the long-term consequences of childhood neglect, but there is evidence that emotional neglect, specifically, relates to blunted affective expression, such as alexithymia and the unemotional features of psychopathy (Aust, Härtwig, Heuser, & Bajbouj, 2013; Kimonis, Fanti, Isoma, & Donoghue, 2013). As such, it is possible that emotional neglect is a unique risk factor for the development of psychopathic traits, whereas sexual and emotional abuse are more related to ASPD, which has high rates of comorbidity with internalizing symptoms and suicidality (Verona, Patrick, & Joiner, 2001).
In sum, the extant evidence has identified a relationship between childhood abuse and subsequent ASPD and psychopathy symptomology in child and adolescent samples, as well as in adult criminal offenders. However, there remain several unanswered questions regarding the specificity of these relationships. A better understanding of how childhood maltreatment differentially relates to severe externalizing outcomes may provide further evidence of overlapping, but distinct etiologies and therefore, inform and encourage more specified treatment efforts. Along these lines, it is also informative to investigate which construct is most strongly associated with abuse history. Such information may guide treatment and intervention efforts among afflicted youth and adults.
In addition to replicating the general association between history of childhood maltreatment and adolescent/adult antisocial behavior (as measured by severity of conduct disorder symptoms, ASPD adult symptoms, and psychopathy scores in a large group of incarcerated criminal offenders), the present study will determine whether and how specific subtypes of abuse and neglect (e.g., physical, sexual, emotional) relate differentially to conduct disorder, ASPD, and psychopathy, respectively. Given the broad association between physical abuse and externalizing, we predict that childhood physical abuse will relate to all three measures (conduct disorder, ASPD, and psychopathy), and most strongly to Factor 2 (including both Facets 3 and 4) of psychopathy. Furthermore, given the association between emotional abuse, sexual abuse, and internalizing features, we predict that emotional and sexual abuse will have a relationship with CD, ASPD, and Factor 2 of psychopathy, but not Factor 1. Finally, given the link between emotional neglect and blunted emotional expression, we predict that emotional neglect will have a specific relationship with Factor 1 of psychopathy.
METHODS
Participants
Participants included n = 183 adult males incarcerated at medium security prisons in Wisconsin. Individuals were eligible for participation if they were between the ages of 18 and 55, had no documented diagnosis of a psychotic disorder, and were not currently taking psychotropic medications. Additionally, participants were eligible if they had a 4th grade reading level or above and scored a 70 or above on a standardized measure of intelligence (Wechsler, 1981). Individuals meeting inclusion criteria were asked to participate in an ongoing study on the causes of incarceration and informed that participation was completely voluntary and would have no impact on their incarceration status.
Five participants were excluded due to missing data. One participant was excluded due to outlier data with undue influence on the regression models (Cook, 1977). Descriptive information is included in Table 1.
Table 1.
Variables | M | SD | % |
---|---|---|---|
Age | 32.62 | 7.74 | |
IQ | 100.46 | 12.75 | |
WAS | 12.76 | 9.12 | |
PCL-R score | 22.98 | 6.86 | |
Factor 1 | 8.99 | 2.96 | |
Facet 1 | 3.06 | 1.97 | |
Facet 2 | 5.93 | 1.53 | |
Factor 2 | 12.03 | 4.35 | |
Facet 3 | 6.85 | 2.12 | |
Facet 4 | 5.15 | 2.91 | |
ASPD total | 6.73 | 3.84 | |
ASPD Adult | 3.47 | 1.75 | |
CD | 3.26 | 2.83 | |
CTQ total score | 47.03 | 18.69 | |
Physical Abuse | 9.83 | 4.85 | |
Physical Neglect | 8.15 | 3.56 | |
Emotional Abuse | 10.30 | 5.16 | |
Emotional Neglect | 10.96 | 5.09 | |
Sexual Abuse | 7.69 | 4.95 | |
Race | |||
Caucasian | 61% | ||
African American | 37% | ||
Other | 3% |
Assessments
Psychopathy
The Psychopathy Checklist-Revised (PCL-R) was used to assess psychopathy (Hare, 2003). The PCL-R is a scale of 20 items rated 0–2 based on the degree to which the trait is present. Within offender populations the PCL-R is the most widely utilized measure of psychopathy (Hare & Neumann, 2008). We computed Factor and Facet scores based on published guidelines (Hare, 2003). Trained undergraduates and professional staff performed all clinical assessments based on information obtained during interviews and reviews of institutional files. PCL-R scores were examined continuously for all regression analyses. In our sample, PCL-R scores ranged from 7–37. Inter-rater reliability ratings were available for 13 participants and yielded a high intra-class correlation (r = .98) for PCL-R total scores, as well as Factor (Factor 1, r = .94),(Factor 2, r = .98) and Facet (Facet 1, r = .91), (Facet 2, r = .88), (Facet 3, r =.89), (Facet 4, r = .98) scores.
ASPD and CD
ASPD symptoms in adulthood and CD symptoms in childhood were assessed based on criteria set forth by the DSM-5 (Association, 2000). Following the PCL-R interview, trained staff assessed ASPD and CD criteria using information gathered from the PCL-R interview and review of institutional files. To receive a diagnosis of ASPD, individuals were required to satisfy at least 3 of a possible 15 CD symptoms (e.g., cruelty to animals before age 15, truancy before age 13), as well as 3 of a possible 7 adult ASPD symptoms (e.g., impulsivity/failure to plan ahead, reckless disregard for safety of others). Total number of criteria met for both CD and adult ASPD was recorded for each participant to assess severity of behavior. In this inmate sample, number of CD symptoms ranged from 0–13, and number of adult ASPD symptoms ranged from 0–7.
Childhood Abuse
The Childhood Trauma Questionnaire (CTQ) was used to assess experienced childhood abuse (Bernstein & Fink, 1998). The CTQ is a 28-item scale comprised of five subscales which assess different types of trauma, including physical abuse (e.g., “I was punished with a belt, a board, a cord, or some other hard object”), physical neglect (e.g., “I didn’t have enough to eat”), emotional abuse (e.g., “People in my family said hurtful or insulting things to me”), emotional neglect (e.g., “I felt loved” [reverse scored]), and sexual abuse (e.g., “Someone tried to make me do sexual things or watch sexual things”). All subscales consist of five items scored on a five-point rating scale from “never true” (1) to “very often true” (5). Two items on the physical neglect scale and all items on the emotional neglect scale are reverse scored.
IQ
Intelligence was assessed using the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised (Wechsler, 1981) or the Shipley Institute of Living Scale (Zachary & Shipley, 1986). IQ scores were included as covariates in all regression analyses.
Socioeconomic Status
Parent’s education level was used as a proxy for socioeconomic status (SES). Following Hollingshead (1975) guidelines, parent’s education was coded as follows: 1: did not complete junior high school, 2: completed junior high school but did not enter high school, 3: completed some high school, 4: graduated high school, 5: some college, 6: completed four-year college degree, 7: graduate school. Mother’s and father’s education level were included as separate covariates in regression analyses.
Data Analyses
Using multiple linear regression, we first examined the relationship between PCL-R total scores and CTQ total and subscale scores. Second, we examined the relationship between PCL-R Factor and Facet scores and CTQ scores. Third, we examined the relationship between ASPD symptoms (total number of criteria met for adult ASPD symptoms) and CTQ scores. Fourth, we examined the relationship between CD symptoms (number of criteria met) and CTQ scores.
Finally, we examined whether CTQ total scores were more strongly related to CD scores than adult ASPD or PCL-R scores by computing Pearson product-moment correlation coefficients for each relationship. We then compared the relative strengths between these relationships using Steiger’s Z test for two correlated correlation coefficients (Meng, Rosenthal, & Rubin, 1992; Steiger, 1980).
Antisocial personality features vary as a function of age, race, IQ, and socioeconomic status (SES) (Harpur & Hare, 1994; Kandel et al., 1988; Piquero, Moffitt, & Lawton, 2005) and prevalence of childhood maltreatment varies across racial and socioeconomic backgrounds (Scher, Forde, McQuaid, & Stein, 2004). As such, age, race and IQ were included as covariates in all models. Because only a subset of offenders had SES data available (n = 141), we performed separate follow-up analyses including this proxy measure of SES as a covariate in the models to confirm the main results. Zero-order correlations among all predictor variables are included in Table 2.
Table 2.
Variables | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | 9 | 10 | 11 | 12 | 13 | 14 | 15 | 16 | 17 | 18 | 19 | 20 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. Age | −0.05 | 0.04 | −0.15 | −0.05 | −0.07 | −0.02 | −0.16 | −0.06 | 0.04 | −0.12 | −0.09 | −0.06 | −0.06 | 0.19 | 0.11 | −0.04 | 0.17 | 0.14 | 0.29 | |
2. IQ | −0.21 | 0.08 | 0.14 | −0.17 | −0.07 | −0.18 | 0.01 | −0.15 | −0.10 | −0.20 | −0.14 | −0.04 | 0.05 | −0.03 | −0.05 | 0.05 | 0.11 | 0.06 | ||
3. Race | −0.09 | −0.11 | 0.23 | 0.14 | 0.21 | 0.19 | 0.02 | 0.17 | 0.19 | 0.06 | 0.17 | 0.03 | 0.12 | 0.05 | 0.17 | 0.14 | 0.29 | |||
4. Mom Edu | 0.45 | 0.01 | 0.09 | −0.02 | 0.07 | 0.08 | −0.02 | −0.02 | 0.03 | −0.17 | −0.17 | −0.05 | −0.13 | −0.14 | −0.16 | −0.16 | ||||
5. Dad Edu | −0.18 | −0.11 | −0.19 | 0.04 | −0.16 | −0.14 | −0.18 | −0.13 | −0.25 | −0.12 | −0.17 | −0.07 | −0.06 | −0.09 | −0.04 | |||||
6. PCL-R | 0.81 | 0.91 | 0.73 | 0.64 | 0.73 | 0.85 | 0.79 | 0.64 | 0.22 | 0.32 | 0.2 | 0.11 | 0.15 | 0.06 | ||||||
7. Factor 1 | 0.52 | 0.89 | 0.81 | 0.34 | 0.52 | 0.66 | 0.46 | 0.13 | 0.23 | 0.12 | 0.06 | 0.12 | −0.02 | |||||||
8. Factor 2 | 0.46 | 0.42 | 0.83 | 0.9 | 0.73 | 0.62 | 0.22 | 0.31 | 0.19 | 0.13 | 0.14 | 0.07 | ||||||||
9. Facet 1 | 0.45 | 0.33 | 0.47 | 0.57 | 0.4 | 0.16 | 0.22 | 0.15 | 0.08 | 0.12 | 0.04 | |||||||||
10. Facet 2 | 0.3 | 0.42 | 0.56 | 0.38 | 0.06 | 0.17 | 0.05 | 0.01 | 0.09 | −0.09 | ||||||||||
11. Facet 3 | 0.51 | 0.61 | 0.44 | 0.17 | 0.21 | 0.17 | 0.12 | 0.1 | 0.06 | |||||||||||
12. Facet 4 | 0.66 | 0.63 | 0.2 | 0.32 | 0.17 | 0.1 | 0.12 | 0.06 | ||||||||||||
13. ASPD | 0.42 | 0.15 | 0.22 | 0.14 | 0.05 | 0.14 | 0.03 | |||||||||||||
14. CD | 0.35 | 0.4 | 0.28 | 0.24 | 0.23 | 0.21 | ||||||||||||||
15. CTQ Tot | 0.81 | 0.7 | 0.88 | 0.81 | 0.68 | |||||||||||||||
16. Phys Ab | 0.54 | 0.67 | 0.52 | 0.4 | ||||||||||||||||
17. Phys Neg | 0.53 | 0.6 | 0.23 | |||||||||||||||||
18. Emo Ab | 0.69 | 0.52 | ||||||||||||||||||
19. Emo Neg | 0.38 | |||||||||||||||||||
20. Sex Ab |
Note: Bolded p < .05
RESULTS
PCL-R Total Score
PCL-R total scores were significantly associated with CTQ total scores (t(183) = 3.67, p < .001, partial η2 = .07). Analyses of CTQ subscales revealed that this relationship held for physical abuse history (t(183) = 4.70, p < .0001, partial η2 = .11), physical neglect history (t(183) = 2.93, p < .01, partial η2 = .04), emotional abuse (t(183) = 2.15, p < .01, partial η2 = .02), and emotional neglect history (t(183) = 2.95, p < .01, partial η2 = .05). There was no relationship with sexual abuse (p =.16) (see Table 3).
Table 3.
CTQ Total Score | Physical Abuse | |||||||||
B | SE B | β | dR2 | R2 | B | SE B | β | dR2 | R2 | |
PCL-R Score | 0.70 | 0.19 | 0.26 | 0.06 | 0.13 | 0.23 | 0.05 | 0.33 | 0.06 | 0.14 |
Factor1 | 0.14 | 0.50 | 0.02 | 0.00 | 0.13 | 0.02 | 0.13 | 0.01 | 0.00 | 0.15 |
Factor 2 | 1.13 | 0.35 | 0.27 | 0.05 | 0.39 | 0.09 | 0.35 | 0.09 | ||
Facet 1 | 0.54 | 0.80 | 0.06 | 0.00 | 0.09 | 0.21 | 0.04 | 0.00 | ||
Facet 2 | −0.48 | 1.01 | −0.04 | 0.00 | 0.12 | −0.16 | 0.26 | −0.05 | 0.00 | 0.15 |
Facet 3 | 0.92 | 0.74 | 0.11 | 0.01 | 0.16 | 0.19 | 0.07 | 0.00 | ||
Facet 4 | 1.26 | 0.57 | 0.20 | 0.03 | 0.55 | 0.15 | 0.33 | 0.07 | ||
ASPD Adult | 1.37 | 0.77 | 0.13 | 0.02 | 0.08 | 0.6 | 0.2 | 0.22 | 0.05 | 0.08 |
CD | 2.52 | 0.45 | 0.39 | 0.14 | 0.21 | 0.7 | 0.16 | 0.41 | 0.16 | 0.19 |
Physical Neglect | Emotional Abuse | |||||||||
B | SE B | β | dR2 | R2 | B | SE B | β | dR2 | R2 | |
PCL-R Score | 0.11 | 0.04 | 0.21 | 0.04 | 0.05 | 0.12 | 0.05 | 0.16 | 0.06 | 0.02 |
Factor1 | 0.10 | 0.10 | 0.09 | 0.01 | 0.05 | −0.05 | 0.14 | −0.03 | 0.00 | 0.09 |
Factor 2 | 0.13 | 0.07 | 0.17 | 0.02 | 0.27 | 0.10 | 0.23 | 0.04 | ||
Facet 1 | 0.21 | 0.16 | 0.12 | 0.01 | 0.03 | 0.22 | 0.01 | 0.00 | ||
Facet 2 | −0.07 | 0.20 | −0.03 | 0.00 | 0.05 | −0.15 | 0.28 | −0.05 | 0.00 | 0.08 |
Facet 3 | 0.11 | 0.15 | 0.07 | 0.00 | 0.26 | 0.21 | 0.11 | 0.01 | ||
Facet 4 | 0.13 | 0.11 | 0.11 | 0.01 | 0.27 | 0.16 | 0.16 | 0.02 | ||
ASPD Adult | 0.19 | 0.15 | 0.09 | 0.009 | 0.01 | 0.12 | 0.22 | 0.04 | 0.002 | 0.05 |
CD | 0.37 | 0.09 | 0.3 | 0.09 | 0.09 | 0.53 | 0.13 | 0.29 | 0.08 | 0.13 |
Emotional Neglect | Sexual Abuse | |||||||||
B | SE B | β | dR2 | R2 | B | SE B | β | dR2 | R2 | |
PCL-R Score | 0.16 | 0.05 | 0.21 | 0.04 | 0.09 | 0.07 | 0.05 | 0.10 | 0.01 | 0.10 |
Factor1 | 0.14 | 0.14 | 0.08 | 0.01 | 0.09 | −0.06 | 0.14 | −0.04 | 0.00 | 0.09 |
Factor 2 | 0.20 | 0.10 | 0.18 | 0.02 | 0.12 | 0.10 | 0.11 | 0.01 | ||
Facet 1 | 0.10 | 0.22 | 0.04 | 0.00 | 0.09 | 0.22 | 0.04 | 0.00 | ||
Facet 2 | 0.18 | 0.28 | 0.06 | 0.00 | 0.07 | −0.27 | 0.28 | −0.08 | 0.01 | 0.09 |
Facet 3 | 0.14 | 0.21 | 0.06 | 0.00 | 0.24 | 0.21 | 0.10 | 0.01 | ||
Facet 4 | 0.23 | 0.16 | 0.13 | 0.01 | 0.05 | 0.16 | 0.03 | 0.00 | ||
ASPD Adult | 0.26 | 0.21 | 0.09 | 0.008 | 0.06 | 0.13 | 0.2 | 0.05 | 0.002 | 0.1 |
CD | 0.54 | 0.12 | 0.3 | 0.09 | 0.15 | 0.34 | 0.12 | 0.21 | 0.04 | 0.14 |
Note: Age, Race, and IQ included as covariates in all models;
Bolded p < .05
PCL-R Factors and Facets
When scores for both PCL-R Factors were included in the model, Factor 2 scores were significantly associated with CTQ total scores (t(172) = 3.27, p <.01, partial η2 = .06), physical abuse subscores (t(172) = 4.49, p <.001, partial η2 = .10), emotional abuse subscores (t(172) = 2.74, p <.01, partial η2 = .04), and emotional neglect subscores (t(172) = 2.10, p <.001, partial η2 = .03). There was a marginal relationship between Factor 2 scores and physical neglect subscores, (p = .051). There was no significant relationship between Factor 1 scores and CTQ total or subscale scores (p’s > .1)
When scores for all four PCL-R Facets were included in the model, Facet 4 scores were significantly associated with CTQ total scores (t(167) = 2.17, p = .04,, partial η2 = .03) and physical abuse subscores (t(167) = 3.67, p < .001, partial η2 = .07). No other associations between Facet scores and CTQ scores were significant (p’s > .05).
ASPD Symptoms
Number of adult ASPD symptoms were significantly associated with physical abuse subscores (t(181) = 3.01, p < .01, partial η2 = .05), but no other associations between adult ASPD symptoms and CTQ scores were significant (p’s > .1)
CD Symptoms
Number of CD symptoms was significantly associated with CTQ total scores (t(181) = 5.74, p < .0001, partial η2 = .15), and all CTQ subscales: physical abuse (t(181) = 6.08, p <.0001, partial η2 = .17), physical neglect (t(181) = 4.12, p < .001, partial η2 = .09), emotional abuse (t(181) = 4.19, p < .001, partial η2 = .09), emotional neglect (t(181) = 4.37, p <. 0001, partial η2 = .10), and sexual abuse (t(181) = 3.02, p < .01, partial η2 = .05).
Comparison of Correlation Strength
The relationship between CD symptom severity and CTQ total scores (r = .33) was significantly stronger than the relationship between PCL-R score and CTQ total scores (r = .21; z = 2.04, p < .05) and significantly stronger than the relationship between ASPD symptom severity and CTQ total scores (r = .37; z = 2.90, p < .01).
Follow-up SES Analyses
The main analyses between PCL-R scores and CTQ scores were reexamined in a subset of offenders, including SES as a covariate. Largely, all significant findings remained the same, with a few exceptions. With SES included in the model, PCL-R scores were no longer associated with emotional abuse, (b = .09, t(133) = 1.49, p = .14, ΔR2 = .02, R2 = .09), Factor 2 scores were not associated with emotional neglect (b = .17, t(126) = 1.53, p = .13, ΔR2 = .02, R2 = .11), and only marginally associated with emotional abuse (b = .21, t(126) = 1.90, p = .06, ΔR2 = .03, R2 = .09), and Facet 4 was not associated with CTQ total scores (b = .97, t(121) = 1.50, p = .14, ΔR2 = .02, R2 = .13),
DISCUSSION
Through a systematic study of adult criminal offenders, we have replicated a strong relationship between childhood maltreatment and subsequent externalizing psychopathology. We report four main results. First, overall psychopathy severity (PCL-R total score) was associated with overall childhood maltreatment severity (CTQ total score). Second, this maltreatment-psychopathy relationship was strongest between physical abuse and the antisocial features of psychopathy (Factor 2/Facet 4 subscores). Third, conduct disorder was associated with a history of sexual abuse, whereas ASPD and psychopathy were not. Finally, the relationship between childhood maltreatment and conduct disorder symptom severity was significantly stronger than the relationship between childhood maltreatment and psychopathy or adult ASPD severity. Together, these findings pinpoint particular psychopathological sequelae of specific types of adverse early-life experiences. Here we discuss each of these main findings in turn.
Consistent with prior research, psychopathy total scores were significantly associated with greater abuse histories overall. Individuals with high scores on measures of psychopathy reported experiencing substantial maltreatment, including both abuse (i.e., physical, emotional, sexual) and neglect (i.e., physical, emotional). Weiler and Widom (1996) suggest this broad relationship between psychopathy and childhood abuse may mediate the relationship between childhood abuse and violent behavior. Specifically, they suggest that childhood abuse and/or neglect increases the risk of developing psychopathic personality features, which consequently results in an increased risk for engaging in violent and aggressive behavior.
Despite significant zero-order correlations between abuse subtypes and Factor 1(see Table 2), after partialling out the effects of Factor 2, Factor 1 did not uniquely account for the variance in CTQ total scores or subscores. This finding suggests that childhood maltreatment has a stronger relationship with Factor 2 traits, such as poor behavioral controls and impulsivity, than with the interpersonal and affective components of Factor 1. More specifically, only Facet 4 was linked to greater abuse overall, as well as to physical abuse, when controlling for the other three Facets.
The impact of childhood maltreatment on emotion regulation could provide important insight into the mechanisms contributing to the relationship between childhood abuse history, antisocial personality features and criminality across development, as well as the non-significant relationship between childhood abuse and Factor 1 traits. For instance, Kim and Cicchetti (2010) reported that compared to non-maltreated children, maltreated children exhibited deficient emotion regulation. Among these individuals, lower emotion regulation was associated with greater externalizing symptoms, contributing to increased peer rejection and, consequently, even greater externalizing symptoms. Within this framework, it is possible that disturbed emotion regulation, particularly emotional reactivity (e.g., Kolla, et al., 2013) is a particular consequence of childhood maltreatment that puts affected individuals at risk for developing externalizing disorders like CD and ASPD. Such difficulty regulating emotions then contributes to increased social dysfunction, which in turn, leads to greater disinhibition and criminality.
The emotional profile associated with psychopathy, however, does not fit this model. Factor 1, specifically, is associated with blunted affect and social dominance (Harpur, Hare, & Hakstian, 1989). As such, it is possible that while childhood maltreatment puts individuals at risk for developing CD and ASPD via emotional dysregulation, a different mechanism is responsible for the relationship between the lifestyle and criminal features of psychopathy and childhood maltreatment. It could be that innate callousness and the ability to navigate and manipulate social situations protect these individuals from developing emotionally and socially dysregulated behavior subsequent to experiencing childhood maltreatment. Further research is required in order to investigate these possibilities.
Though we predicted that increased ASPD and CD symptoms, but not psychopathy, would relate to history of sexual abuse, only CD symptomology was associated with greater sexual abuse history. While the differential developmental pathways associated with sexual abuse (Plunkett, et al., 2001) may shed light onto the non-significant relationship with psychopathy, it is less clear why sexual abuse history would relate only to CD and not ASPD. Recently, Ogloff et al. (2012) reported that after a 45 year follow up, men who had been sexually abused as children were significantly more likely to have been convicted of a sexual offense than men who did not experience childhood sexual abuse. Similarly, other studies have suggested that adult men who have experienced childhood sexual abuse often report difficulty with sexuality, such as sexual confusion and functioning (O’Leary, Easton, & Gould, 2015). With this in mind, it is possible that childhood sexual abuse contributes to more globally impaired functioning and behavioral problems in childhood, but the long-term consequences of sexual abuse, among men, relates more specifically to sexual dysfunction rather than personality pathology.
Finally, we found that the relationship between childhood abuse history and conduct disorder severity was significantly stronger than the relationship between child abuse history and adult psychopathy scores. These findings suggest that the significant relationship between childhood maltreatment and distal adult antisociality may be impacted by early onset antisociality, more proximal to the experience of maltreatment. While it is fairly intuitive that childhood conduct problems, compared to adult conduct problems, would evidence a stronger relationship with childhood maltreatment, such findings may encourage further study on early, trauma-focused intervention among juveniles with severe conduct problems. Importantly, recent work examining environmental contributions to the presentation of psychopathic traits in adolescents has identified a subtype of juvenile psychopath characterized by greater abuse history and PTSD symptomology (Kimonis, Frick, Cauffman, Goldweber, & Skeem, 2012; Kimonis, Skeem, Cauffman, & Dmitrieva, 2011; Tatar, Cauffman, Kimonis, & Skeem, 2012). This suggests that further examination of psychopathic variants within the adult offender population may help clarify the abuse-psychopathy connection, and that trauma-focused therapy may be a worthwhile effort when treating traumatized juveniles with psychopathic traits.
The reported findings remained largely unchanged when controlling for socioeconomic status, with a few exceptions: PCL-R scores were no longer associated with emotional abuse, Factor 2 scores were not associated with emotional neglect and only marginally associated with emotional abuse, and Facet 4 was not associated with CTQ total scores. Of note, in our sample, mother’s education level education level was significantly and negatively correlated with CTQ total, emotional abuse and emotional neglect scores, whereas father’s education level was significantly and negatively correlated with PCL-R total scores and Factor 2 scores. Though speculative, this finding may suggest that, between parents, there are differential contributions to the experience of childhood maltreatment and development of externalizing symptoms.
Because the measure of CD used in this study followed DSM-IV criteria, the qualifier of “limited pro-social emotions” in the diagnosis now included in the DSM-5 was not utilized. As such, future research should investigate the relationship between CD with and without these “callous-unemotional” (CU) traits as it relates to adult ASPD and psychopathy and childhood abuse. If the relationship between abuse and psychopathy were specific to more general externalizing features of psychopathy, we would anticipate that the relationship between abuse and adult psychopathy might have differential relationships for individuals who exhibited high vs. low CU traits in adolescence. Though it remains unclear how maltreatment may impact antisocial behavior and psychopathic traits across development, there is some evidence that environmental factors, such as quality of parenting and socioeconomic status, relate to stability of psychopathic traits throughout early to late adolescence (Frick, Kimonis, Dandreaux, & Farell, 2003).
This study has several notable strengths. First, we were able to examine relationships between childhood maltreatment history in both ASPD offenders and psychopathic offenders, contributing further knowledge regarding differential abuse histories among antisocial personalities. Additionally, while previous studies have frequently used composite measures of abuse, the current study examined specific types of abuse and neglect. We also assessed specific subsets of psychopathic characteristics (i.e., Factors and Facets). Together, these analyses revealed more precise relationships between subtypes of childhood abuse and subset of psychopathic traits than had previously been known. Finally, this study compared the differential strengths of the relationships between childhood abuse, conduct disorder severity, and severity of psychopathy, thus highlighting the pivotal role that juvenile behavior problems play in linking childhood maltreatment to adult psychopathy. Our findings thus substantially augment the body of empirical evidence investigating the relationship between childhood abuse and psychopathy within an adult sample.
Despite these strengths, this study had several limitations. The first limitation involves the use of self-report measures of abuse in a population characterized by dishonesty. It is possible that psychopathic offenders reported greater abuse histories inaccurately. However, all participants were informed that their participation would have no impact on their incarceration status, thus reducing motivation to falsify abuse histories. Additionally, there is evidence that psychopathic offenders provide accurate self-reports, even for ostensibly negative traits and experiences. A widespread self-report measure of psychopathic characteristics, the Psychopathic Personality Inventory (Lilienfeld & Andrews, 1996), has been validated in incarcerated samples (Poythress, Edens, & Lilienfeld, 1998). Edens, Poythress, and Lilienfeld (1999) have shown that the PPI, like the PCL-R, predicts increased risk of aggressive institutional behavior. Thus, it appears that psychopathic offenders provide valid self-report data on structured questionnaires (Lilienfeld, Fowler, & Patrick, 2006). Relatedly, the current study utilized a cross-sectional design with retrospective, self-report data. Future studies should employ longitudinal designs in order to further examine the long-term effects of childhood abuse and neglect and, more specifically, how they relate the development of psychopathic traits and general externalizing behavior.
In addition, because the data in this study are correlational in nature, it is not possible to conclusively infer causality or directionality between variables. For example, it is possible that childhood maltreatment impacts the development of antisocial behavior in adolescence and adulthood, but it is also possible that these children have inherently difficult temperaments, therefore instigating abusive relationships with their caretakers (Plomin, 1995). Given the many risk factors for antisociality, it is also important to consider genetic contributions to the development of CD, ASPD, and psychopathy, as well as gene–by-environment relationships (see Farrington, 2005; Herndon & Iacono, 2005).
If childhood abuse does indeed influence the development of antisocial behavior and personalities, it is unclear what mechanism may be responsible for this developmental trajectory. Though the proposed model emphasizes the role of emotion dysregulation, childhood abuse is also associated with cognitive abnormalities (Beers & De Bellis, 2002). As such, it is also possible that abuse early in life results in cognitive deficits that contribute to criminal offending behavior. Accordingly, it is important for future research to specify potential mechanisms underlying the relationship between abuse and externalizing behavior and psychopathy.
In sum, the current study confirms that individuals with ASPD and individuals with psychopathy evince extensive abuse histories. We found that this relationship was strongest between physical abuse and the antisocial features of psychopathy. Furthermore, it appears that sexual abuse has a unique relationship with CD symptomology. The study data also indicate that the relationship between childhood maltreatment and the juvenile antisocial features of psychopathy is stronger than the relationship between childhood maltreatment and the adult manifestation of psychopathy. Given that psychopathic individuals commit a disproportionate amount of crime (Hare, McPherson, & Forth, 1988), if abuse does influence criminal behavior, particularly during adolescence, it may be possible to diminish some of this violent, criminal behavior if therapeutic intervention is offered early in life.
Acknowledgements
We thank the many individuals at the Wisconsin Department of Corrections who made this research possible, and are especially indebted to Deputy Warden Tom Nickel, Warden Randy Hepp, and Dr. Kevin Kallas.
Footnotes
The authors declare no competing financial interests.
REFERENCES
- Association AP. DSM-IV-TR: Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, text revision. American Psychiatric Association; 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Association AP. DSM 5. American Psychiatric Association; 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Aust S, Härtwig EA, Heuser I, Bajbouj M. The role of early emotional neglect in alexithymia. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy. 2013;5(3):225. [Google Scholar]
- Beauchaine TP, McNulty T. Comorbidities and continuities as ontogenic processes: Toward a developmental spectrum model of externalizing psychopathology. Development and psychopathology. 2013;25(4 0 2):1505–1528. doi: 10.1017/S0954579413000746. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beers SR, De Bellis MD. Neuropsychological function in children with maltreatment-related posttraumatic stress disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2002;159(3):483–486. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.3.483. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bernstein DP, Fink L. Childhood trauma questionnaire: A retrospective self-report: Manual. Harcourt Brace & Company; 1998. [Google Scholar]
- Bierer LM, Yehuda R, Schmeidler J, Mitropoulou V, New AS, Silverman JM, Siever LJ. Abuse and neglect in childhood: relationship to personality disorder diagnoses. CNS spectrums. 2003 doi: 10.1017/s1092852900019118. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cook RD. Detection of influential observation in linear regression. Technometrics. 1977:15–18. [Google Scholar]
- De Bellis MD. Developmental traumatology: The psychobiological development of maltreated children and its implications for research, treatment, and policy. Development and psychopathology. 2001;13(03):539–564. doi: 10.1017/s0954579401003078. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Edens JF, Poythress NG, Lilienfeld SO. Identifying inmates at risk for disciplinary infractions: a comparison of two measures of psychopathy. Behavioral Sciences & the Law. 1999;17(4):435–443. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1099-0798(199910/12)17:4<435::aid-bsl356>3.0.co;2-z. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1099-0798(199910/12)17:4<435::AID-BSL356>3.0.CO;2-Z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Egeland B, Yates T, Appleyard K, Van Dulmen M. The long-term consequences of maltreatment in the early years: A developmental pathway model to antisocial behavior. Children's services: Social policy, research, and practice. 2002;5(4):249–260. [Google Scholar]
- Farrington DP. Childhood origins of antisocial behavior. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy. 2005;12(3):177–190. [Google Scholar]
- Frick PJ, Kimonis ER, Dandreaux DM, Farell JM. The 4 year stability of psychopathic traits in non - referred youth. Behavioral Sciences & the Law. 2003;21(6):713–736. doi: 10.1002/bsl.568. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Graham N, Kimonis ER, Wasserman AL, Kline SM. Associations among childhood abuse and psychopathy facets in male sexual offenders. Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment. 2012;3(1):66. doi: 10.1037/a0025605. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gregory S, Simmons A, Kumari V, Howard M, Hodgins S, Blackwood N. The antisocial brain: psychopathy matters: a structural MRI investigation of antisocial male violent offenders. Archives of general psychiatry. 2012;69(9):962–972. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2012.222. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hare RD. Diagnosis of antisocial personality disorder in two prison populations. 1983 doi: 10.1176/ajp.140.7.887. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hare RD. Psychopathy a clinical construct whose time has come. Criminal justice and behavior. 1996a;23(1):25–54. [Google Scholar]
- Hare RD. Psychopathy and antisocial personality disorder: A case of diagnostic confusion. Psychiatric Times. 1996c;13(2):39–40. [Google Scholar]
- Hare RD. Manual for the Revised Psychopathy Checklist 2nd ed. Toronto, ON, Canada: Multi-Health Systems; 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Hare RD, McPherson LM, Forth AE. Male psychopaths and their criminal careers. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1988;56(5):710. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.56.5.710. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hare RD, Neumann CS. Structural models of psychopathy. Current psychiatry reports. 2005;7(1):57–64. doi: 10.1007/s11920-005-0026-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hare RD, Neumann CS. The PCL-R assessment of psychopathy. Handbook of psychopathy. 2006:58–88. [Google Scholar]
- Harpur T, Hakstian R, Hare RD. Factor structure of the Psychopathy Checklist. Journal of consulting and clinical psychology. 1988;56(5):741. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.56.5.741. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harpur T, Hare RD, Hakstian R. Two-factor conceptualization of psychopathy: Construct validity and assessment implications. 1989;1:6–17. Retrieved from. [Google Scholar]
- Harpur TJ, Hare RD. Assessment of psychopathy as a function of age. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1994;103(4):604–609. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.103.4.604. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hart SD, Hare RD. Psychopathy and antisocial personality disorder. Current Opinion in Psychiatry. 1996;9(2):129–132. [Google Scholar]
- Herndon RW, Iacono WG. Psychiatric disorder in the children of antisocial parents. Psychological Medicine. 2005;35(12):1815–1824. doi: 10.1017/S0033291705005635. doi: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hollingshead AB. Four factor index of social status. 1975 [Google Scholar]
- Kandel E, Mednick SA, Kirkegaard-Sorensen L, Hutchings B, Knop J, Rosenberg R, Schulsinger F. IQ as a protective factor for subjects at high risk for antisocial behavior. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1988;56(2):224–226. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.56.2.224. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim J, Cicchetti D. Longitudinal pathways linking child maltreatment, emotion regulation, peer relations, and psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines. 2010;51(6):706–716. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02202.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kimonis E, Fanti KA, Isoma Z, Donoghue K. Maltreatment profiles among incarcerated boys with callous-unemotional traits. Child maltreatment. 2013;18(2):108–121. doi: 10.1177/1077559513483002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kimonis E, Frick PJ, Cauffman E, Goldweber A, Skeem J. Primary and secondary variants of juvenile psychopathy differ in emotional processing. Development and psychopathology. 2012;24(3):1091. doi: 10.1017/S0954579412000557. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kimonis E, Skeem JL, Cauffman E, Dmitrieva J. Are secondary variants of juvenile psychopathy more reactively violent and less psychosocially mature than primary variants? Law and Human Behavior. 2011;35(5):381–391. doi: 10.1007/s10979-010-9243-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kisiel CL, Lyons JS. Dissociation as a mediator of psychopathology among sexually abused children and adolescents. 2014 doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.7.1034. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kolla NJ, Malcolm C, Attard S, Arenovich T, Blackwood N, Hodgins S. Childhood maltreatment and aggressive behaviour in violent offenders with psychopathy. Canadian journal of psychiatry. Revue canadienne de psychiatrie. 2013;58(8):487–494. doi: 10.1177/070674371305800808. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lansford JE, Miller-Johnson S, Berlin LJ, Dodge KA, Bates JE, Pettit GS. Early physical abuse and later violent delinquency: A prospective longitudinal study. Child Maltreatment. 2007;12(3):233–245. doi: 10.1177/1077559507301841. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lilienfeld SO, Andrews BP. Development and preliminary validation of a self-report measure of psychopathic personality traits in noncriminal population. Journal of personality assessment. 1996;66(3):488–524. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa6603_3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lilienfeld SO, Fowler KA, Patrick C. The self-report assessment of psychopathy. Handbook of psychopathy. 2006:107–132. [Google Scholar]
- Meng X-L, Rosenthal R, Rubin DB. Comparing correlated correlation coefficients. Psychological bulletin. 1992;111(1):172. [Google Scholar]
- O’Leary P, Easton SD, Gould N. The Effect of Child Sexual Abuse on Men: Toward a Male Sensitive Measure. Journal of Interpersonal Violence. 2015 doi: 10.1177/0886260515586362. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ogloff JR, Cutajar MC, Mann E, Mullen P, Wei FTY, Hassan HAB, Yih TH. Child sexual abuse and subsequent offending and victimisation: A 45 year follow-up study. Trends and Issues in Crime and Criminal Justice. 2012;440:1–6. [Google Scholar]
- Piquero AR, Moffitt TE, Lawton B. Race and Crime: The Contribution of Individual, Familial, and Neighborhood-Level Risk Factors to Life-Course-Persistent Offending. In: Kempf-Leonard DFHK, editor. Our children, their children: Confronting racial and ethnic differences in American juvenile justice. Chicago, IL: US: University of Chicago Press; 2005. pp. 202–244. [Google Scholar]
- Plomin R. Genetics and children's experiences in the family. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 1995;36(1):33–68. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1995.tb01655.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Plunkett A, O'Toole B, Swanston H, Oates RK, Shrimpton S, Parkinson P. Suicide risk following child sexual abuse. Ambulatory Pediatrics. 2001;1(5):262–266. doi: 10.1367/1539-4409(2001)001<0262:srfcsa>2.0.co;2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Poythress NG, Edens JF, Lilienfeld SO. Criterion-related validity of the Psychopathic Personality Inventory in a prison sample. Psychological Assessment. 1998;10(4):426–430. [Google Scholar]
- Poythress NG, Skeem JL, Lilienfeld SO. Associations among early abuse, dissociation, and psychopathy in an offender sample. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2006;115(2):288–297. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.115.2.288. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raskin White H, Bates ME, Buyske S. Adolescence-limited versus persistent delinquency: extending Moffitt's hypothesis into adulthood. Journal of abnormal psychology. 2001;110(4):600. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.110.4.600. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2003.09.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scher CD, Forde DR, McQuaid JR, Stein MB. Prevalence and demographic correlates of childhood maltreatment in an adult community sample. Child Abuse & Neglect. 2004;28(2):167–180. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2003.09.012. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2003.09.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shapero BG, Black SK, Liu RT, Klugman J, Bender RE, Abramson LY, Alloy LB. Stressful Life Events and Depression Symptoms: The Effect of Childhood Emotional Abuse on Stress Reactivity. Journal of Clinical Psychology. 2014;70(3):209–223. doi: 10.1002/jclp.22011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Steiger JH. Tests for comparing elements of a correlation matrix. Psychological Bulletin. 1980;87(2):245. [Google Scholar]
- Tatar JR, Cauffman E, Kimonis ER, Skeem JL. Victimization history and posttraumatic stress: An analysis of psychopathy variants in male juvenile offenders. Journal of Child & Adolescent Trauma. 2012;5(2):102–113. [Google Scholar]
- Verona E, Patrick CJ, Joiner TE. Psychopathy, antisocial personality, and suicide risk. Journal of abnormal psychology. 2001;110(3):462. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.110.3.462. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wechsler D. Wechsler adult intelligence scale-revised. Psychological Corporation; 1981. [Google Scholar]
- Weiler BL, Widom CS. Psychopathy and violent behaviour in abused and neglected young adults. Criminal Behaviour and Mental Health. 1996;6(3):253–271. [Google Scholar]
- Zachary RA, Shipley WC. Shipley institute of living scale: Revised manual. WPS, Western Psychological Services; 1986. [Google Scholar]