Abstract
Background: Food fussiness (FF) is common in early childhood and is often associated with the rejection of nutrient-dense foods such as vegetables and fruit. FF and liking for vegetables and fruit are likely all heritable phenotypes; the genetic influence underlying FF may explain the observed genetic influence on liking for vegetables and fruit. Twin analyses make it possible to get a broad-based estimate of the extent of the shared genetic influence that underlies these traits.
Objective: We quantified the extent of the shared genetic influence that underlies FF and liking for vegetables and fruit in early childhood with the use of a twin design.
Design: Data were from the Gemini cohort, which is a population-based sample of twins born in England and Wales in 2007. Parents of 3-y-old twins (n = 1330 pairs) completed questionnaire measures of their children’s food preferences (liking for vegetables and fruit) and the FF scale from the Children’s Eating Behavior Questionnaire. Multivariate quantitative genetic modeling was used to estimate common genetic influences that underlie FF and liking for vegetables and fruit.
Results: Genetic correlations were significant and moderate to large in size between FF and liking for both vegetables (−0.65) and fruit (−0.43), which indicated that a substantial proportion of the genes that influence FF also influence liking. Common genes that underlie FF and liking for vegetables and fruit largely explained the observed phenotypic correlations between them (68–70%).
Conclusions: FF and liking for fruit and vegetables in young children share a large proportion of common genetic factors. The genetic influence on FF may determine why fussy children typically reject fruit and vegetables.
Keywords: child, eating, food, fussiness, genetic, heritability, infant, liking, preferences
See corresponding editorial on page 961.
INTRODUCTION
It is common for young children to reject certain types of foods, especially those with certain textures or flavors [so-called food fussiness (FF)7]. Fussy eating behavior typically emerges in toddlerhood (1, 2) and can result in lower dietary variety and quality (3–5). The foods rejected most frequently by fussier children tend to be the nutrient-dense, healthier foods such as vegetables and fruit (6). Food neophobia, a distinct but related characteristic that refers specifically to the refusal of unfamiliar foods, is also associated with low vegetable and fruit acceptance (7–10). Because of the importance of vegetable and fruit consumption for health (11–13) and the low rates of vegetable consumption reported in children in developed countries (13–15), it would be valuable to gain a better understanding of the relation between fussy and neophobic behaviors and the rejection of vegetables and fruit specifically.
Studies that have explored the origins of food rejection have often focused on negative reactions to new foods (neophobia), but children may also start to refuse previously liked or accepted foods, thereby becoming increasingly selective in their eating (5), and these different types of food refusal are closely linked (7, 16). The 6-item FF scale from the Children’s Eating Behavior Questionnaire (CEBQ) is an established measure of children’s food rejection and measures both aspects of neophobia (e.g., “My child refuses new foods at first”) and more general fussy eating (e.g., “My child is difficult to please with meals”).
From twin studies, it has been well established that neophobia, when measured independently of other fussy behaviors, has a strong genetic basis (72–78%) in the early childhood period (17, 18). A substantial genetic influence has also been shown for young children’s preferences for vegetables and fruit with heritability estimates in the order of 37–54% (19, 20). One hypothesis is that part of the heritability of vegetable and fruit liking reflects the genetic influence on fussiness and neophobia and that a large proportion of the observed phenotypic association between vegetable and fruit liking and fussiness and neophobia is mediated by the genetic influence on fussiness and neophobia.
In the current study, we used pediatric twin data from the Gemini cohort to test this hypothesis. We used multivariate quantitative genetic analyses to quantify the extent to which genetic influences that underlie FF are the same as those that underlie vegetable and fruit liking and the extent to which common genetic influences explain the observed phenotypic associations between FF and vegetable and fruit liking.
METHODS
Sample
Data were from children participating in the Gemini twin study. Gemini is a birth cohort of twins born between March and December 2007 in England and Wales (21). All families with twins born in this period (n = 6754) were contacted by the Office for National Statistics and were invited to participate in the study. Of these, 2402 families returned a baseline questionnaire (36% response rate) when the twins were, on average (±SD), 8.2 ± 2.2 mo old (Supplemental Figure 1). This study used data from families who completed the measures of food liking and the CEBQ (22) when the children were aged 3.5 ± 0.3 y (n = 2686). Ethical approval was granted by the Joint University College London/University College London Hospitals Committee on the Ethics of Human Research.
Measures
Zygosity was determined in same-sex pairs with the use of a validated questionnaire (23). In addition, DNA-based zygosity testing was conducted in a random sample of 81 Gemini pairs, which resulted in 100% correspondence between questionnaire-allocated and DNA-tested zygosity (24). In the current sample, zygosity was uncertain in 13 pairs because of inconclusive questionnaire results or missing data, and therefore, these pairs were excluded from the analyses. The sex, date of birth, weight at birth, and gestational age of the children were reported in the baseline questionnaire, and the exact age at the assessment of food liking and FF was calculated from the date of data collection.
The measure of food liking used in this study has been described previously (19). Briefly, parents reported their children’s liking for multiple individual foods on a 6-point scale with the following response options: likes a lot, likes, neither likes nor dislikes, dislikes, dislikes a lot, and never tried (the last category was recoded to missing). Responses were scored 2, 1, 0, −1, and −2; with zero indicating a neutral opinion, positive values indicating liking, and negative values indicating dislike. A total of 75 foods were grouped into 6 categories that were primarily derived from a principal components analysis. Foods were required to have been tried by ≥75% of children to be included in the principal components analysis. The food categories included in the current study were vegetables (19 foods; e.g., broccoli) and fruit (16 foods; e.g., bananas). Scale scores were calculated as the mean liking for the component food items. In order for a scale score to be calculated, participants were required to have completed greater than one-half of the food items within each scale.
Parents reported on their children’s fussiness with the use of the CEBQ FF scale, which was designed to assess neophobic, fussy, and picky eating behaviors in children (22). The 6 items (example item: “My child refuses new foods at first”) were scored on a 5-point scale labeled never, rarely, sometimes, often, or always. Mean scores were calculated for each child (range: 1–5) with higher scores indicating greater fussiness. Complete data were required on ≥4 items.
Statistical analyses
Heritability was estimated with the use of intraclass correlations (ICCs) and maximum-likelihood structural equation modeling (MLSEM). Analyses were conducted on food liking and FF scores that had been residualized for age and sex effects with the use of a regression procedure. This method took into account the exact correlation for age (and sex within same-sex twin pairs) that could inflate the estimate of shared environmental effects (C) (25).
Twin studies make it possible to estimate the extent of genetic influence on a characteristic by comparing the degree of resemblance between monozygotic twin pairs (who share 100% of their genes) and dizygotic twin pairs (who share, on average, 50% of their segregating genes). ICCs provide an indication of the size of the genetic effect on a single characteristic, whereby the greater the resemblance between monozygotic and dizygotic twins, the larger the genetic influence on that trait. Cross-twin, cross-trait (CT/CT) correlations form the basis of multivariate heritability. They show how, within a twin pair, the score of twin 1 for trait A (e.g., FF) varies in relation to the score of twin 2 for trait B (e.g., liking for vegetables). Similar to simple ICCs, higher CT/CT correlations between monozygotic and dizygotic pairs indicate that shared genetic influences are driving the phenotypic association between the traits.
MLSEM was used to derive more-reliable estimates of genetic and environmental influences on the traits and the common influence between them and to provide 95% CI and goodness-of-fit statistics. MLSEM provides estimates for additive genetic effects (A), C, or unique environmental effects (E) by producing a large number of possible variable values and comparing them to the variance-covariance structures observed in the actual data in an iterative process. The estimates selected are those that produce variance-covariance structures that most closely resemble the actual data (26).
CT/CT ICCs were calculated for monozygotic and dizygotic pairs for the residualized vegetable- and fruit-liking scores paired with residualized FF scores. For each combination of food liking and FF, there were 2 CT/CT correlations as follows: 1) FF in twin 1 was correlated with vegetable liking in twin 2, and 2) vegetable liking in twin 1 was correlated with FF in twin 2. These correlations were compared with the phenotypic correlations calculated with the use of Pearson’s product-moment correlation coefficients to identify evidence of the underlying common genetic influence for both traits. Twin correlations were conducted with the use of SPSS version 21 for Windows software (SPSS Inc).
The MLSEM used a correlated factors model (26, 27). This model provides the following 2 pieces of information about shared genetic effects between measured phenotypes: 1) pairwise etiologic correlations and 2) bivariate heritability estimates. Etiologic correlations quantify the extent to which common genetic factors or common environments influence 2 phenotypes (e.g., vegetable liking and FF). Etiologic correlations can be interpreted in the same way as Pearson correlation coefficients such that a positive etiologic correlation indicates that the influences that cause an individual to score higher on one particular phenotype also tend to make them score higher on the other phenotype; in the same way, a negative correlation would indicate that the same influences that cause an individual to score higher on one phenotype tend to make them score lower on the other phenotype. Bivariate estimates quantify the extent to which common factors (A, C, or E) explain the observed phenotypic association (e.g., between vegetable liking and FF). We also tested submodels by systematically dropping components of variance (A, C, or E) and covariance, but all submodels led to a worsening of fit according to the likelihood ratio test and Aikaike’s information criterion. MLSEM was conducted with the use of Mx Maximum-Likelihood Structural Equation Modeling Software (version 32; Virginia Commonwealth University).
RESULTS
Sample characteristics are provided in Table 1. The phenotypic correlations between FF and liking for vegetables and fruit are shown in Table 2. FF was significantly negatively correlated with liking for both vegetables (−0.61, P < 0.01) and fruit (−0.42, P < 0.01), such that fussier children tended to dislike vegetables and fruit. Sizes of the associations were moderate to large.
TABLE 1.
Characteristic | Study sample (n = 2660 children) |
Sex, n (%) | |
M | 1316 (49.5) |
F | 1344 (50.5) |
Gestational age, wk | 36.18 ± 2.511 |
Weight at birth, kg | 2.45 ± 0.54 |
Zygosity, n (%) | |
Monozygotic | 916 (34.4) |
Dizygotic | 1744 (65.6) |
Food fussiness2 | 2.65 ± 0.85 |
Vegetable liking3 | 0.44 ± 0.61 |
Fruit liking3 | 1.00 ± 0.64 |
Mean ± SD (all such values).
Higher scores indicate greater fussiness (range: 1–5).
Higher scores indicate a higher liking (range: −2 to 2).
TABLE 2.
CT/CT ICCs (95% CIs)4 |
||||
Scales | Phenotypic correlations2 | Twin and scale3 | Monozygotic | Dizygotic |
Vegetable liking × FF | −0.61 | Twin 1 V × twin 2 FF | −0.58 (−0.64, −0.52) | −0.29 (−0.35, −0.23) |
Twin 2 V × twin 1 FF | −0.58 (−0.64, −0.52) | −0.32 (−0.38, −0.26) | ||
Fruit liking × FF | −0.42 | Twin 1 F × twin 2 FF | −0.45 (−0.53, −0.38) | −0.20 (−0.26, −0.13) |
Twin 2 F × twin 1 FF | −0.42 (−0.50, −0.34) | −0.19 (−0.26, −0.13) |
CT/CT, cross-twin, cross-trait; F, fruit liking; FF, food fussiness; ICC, intraclass correlation; V, vegetable liking.
Pearson’s product-moment correlation coefficients; n = 2523–2660. All correlations were significant at the 0.01 level (2 tailed).
Randomly allocated twin (1 or 2) and the scale used in the CT/CT correlation. CT/CT ICCs were calculated for monozygotic and dizygotic pairs for the residualized vegetable and fruit liking scores paired with residualized FF scores.
Monozygotics: n = 438–458 pairs; dizygotics: n = 855–872 pairs.
Twin correlations
The pairwise CT/CT correlations between each of the food-liking scales and FF are also shown in Table 2. The CT/CT correlations between FF and both vegetable and fruit liking were significant (the 95% CIs did not cross 0) and moderate to large for monozygotic twins, whereas the dizygotic correlations, although still significantly different from zero, were considerably smaller. This pattern of high similarity indicated that shared genes were contributing to the observed phenotypic correlations between FF and vegetable liking and FF and fruit liking. In keeping with the phenotypic correlations, the CT/CT ICCs were negative, which indicated that, if one twin within a pair scored highly on FF, his or her co-twin tended to score lower on liking for fruit or vegetables.
Multivariate MLSEM
Figure 1 shows the univariate estimates for A, C, and E (single-headed straight arrows) derived for the 3 traits from the multivariate model as well as the etiologic correlations between them (double-headed curved arrows). The univariate estimates for FF established that it was highly heritable (A: 0.78; 95% CI: 0.73, 0.82) with the majority of the remaining variance being explained by unique environment effects (E: 0.17; 95% CI: 0.15, 0.20). The shared environment had a very small effect on FF (C: 0.05; 95% CI: 0.02, 0.09). Univariate estimates were virtually the same for vegetable liking and fruit liking. Heritability was moderate for both vegetable liking (A: 0.53; 95% CI: 0.46, 0.61) and fruit liking (A: 0.52; 95% CI: 0.44, 0.61) in keeping with estimates reported previously (19). Estimates of shared environmental influences were also moderate for both vegetable liking (C: 0.36; 95% CI: 0.28, 0.43) and fruit liking (C: 0.35; 95% CI: 0.27, 0.43).
The phenotypic correlations derived from the structural equation modeling, the pairwise etiologic correlations, and the bivariate estimates (the proportion of the phenotypic correlations explained by common genetic or environmental influences) are shown in Table 3. The genetic correlations between liking for vegetables and FF (−0.65) and liking for fruit and FF (−0.42) were significant and moderate to large, which indicated that many of the genetic factors that underlie FF also influence liking for both vegetables and fruit. The negative genetic correlation for vegetable liking and FF was significantly stronger than the association for fruit liking and FF, which showed that a particularly high proportion of the genetic influences that drive increased FF were also behind decreased liking for vegetables. This result supports the hypothesis that some of the genetic influence on liking for vegetables and fruit reflects a genetic influence on FF.
TABLE 3.
Variance components for bivariate A, C, and E, phenotypic correlation, % (bivariate estimates)3 |
Etiologic correlation (95% CI)4 |
||||||
Food preference and FF scales | Phenotypic correlation (95% CI)2 | A | C | E | rg | rc | re |
Vegetable liking × FF | −0.60 (−0.66, −0.55) | 70 (0.42) | 21 (0.13) | 9 (0.05) | −0.65 (−0.71, −0.59) | −0.97 (−1.00, −0.78) | −0.38 (−0.46, −0.29) |
Fruit liking × FF | −0.40 (−0.46, −0.36) | 66 (0.26) | 24 (0.10) | 10 (0.04) | −0.42 (−0.50, −0.34) | −0.75 (−0.96, −0.39) | −0.27 (−0.36, −0.18) |
n = 2660 children. A, additive genetic effects, C, shared environment effects; E, unique environment effects; FF, food fussiness; rc, shared environmental correlation; re, unique environmental correlation; rg, genetic correlation.
Phenotypic correlations were derived from structural equation modeling.
Proportions of variance in the phenotypic correlation that are explained by common A, C, and E derived from structural equation modeling and converted to percentages for ease of interpretation. The sum of the bivariate components (shown in parentheses) equals the phenotypic correlation. All bivariate estimates were significant.
A genetic, shared environmental or unique environmental correlation was significant if the 95% CI did not include zero; all correlations in the model were significant.
The bivariate heritability estimates (A) indicated significant genetic contributions to the phenotypic associations between liking for vegetables and FF and between liking for fruit and FF. The bivariate heritability estimates were similar for vegetable liking and FF (70%) and for fruit liking and FF (66%), which suggested that the majority of the phenotypic correlation between each of these pairs of traits could be ascribed to common genetic factors, thereby indicating that the majority of the observed phenotypic associations between FF and liking for both vegetables and fruit were genetically mediated.
DISCUSSION
The results from this study support the hypothesis that a significant proportion of the genetic influence on liking for vegetables and fruit reflects a genetic influence on FF and that the majority of the observed phenotypic associations between FF and liking for vegetables and fruit are genetically mediated. These findings show that FF and liking for vegetables and fruit are heritable traits in young children and, also, that common genes are driving the association between fussy eating and decreased preferences for these nutritious foods. FF was shown to be highly heritable (78%) with only a moderate influence of the environment on this characteristic in young children. These results are comparable to findings from twin studies that investigated genetic and environmental influences on food neophobia, which have previously been estimated as 72% heritable in 4–7-y-olds and 78% heritable in 8–11-y-olds (17, 18).
The strong phenotypic associations observed between higher FF and lower liking for vegetables and fruit also support previous findings (3–5, 9). These associations were largely driven by genetic influences that were shared with FF (70% for vegetables and 66% for fruit). The phenotypic relation between liking for vegetables and FF was particularly strong, and shared genes appeared to be the largest contributor to this association. These results suggest fussy children display lower liking for vegetables primarily because both of these traits are driven by the same underlying genetic factors.
The findings of this study raise the following question: What are the common genetic factors driving the associations between these traits? Note that the food groups most commonly rejected by fussy eaters (i.e., vegetables, fruit, and, to a lesser extent, protein) (10) are also those for which liking is most heritable (19, 20). Research on the topic of genetically determined variation in human taste sensitivity has focused on sensitivity to the compounds phenylthiocarbamide and 6-n-propylthiouracil (PROP) and the associated taste 2 receptor member 38 (TAS2R38) gene. PROP sensitivity has been used as a marker for general taste acuity, and the most frequently applied test of human taste function has involved asking people to rate the intensity of this compound (28). The use of PROP taster status as a marker of general taste sensitivity or ability has received criticism (28, 29), and growing evidence has pointed to a complex etiology of taste perception or preference (30–32).
Traits such as food preferences and FF are likely to be highly polygenic with many genes each contributing a small amount to the genetic variation in these phenotypes. This contribution makes it difficult to identify the specific genes responsible although there has been progress in the detection of genetic influences on other complex polygenic traits such as obesity (33) and aspects of appetite, including satiety (34). To date, 97 genetic variants have been shown to contribute to the variation in body mass (35). The large genetic correlations observed between food liking and FF in this study suggest that, if the genes that contribute to the variation in fussiness were to be identified, they would also likely influence liking for vegetables and fruit. However, the etiologic correlations were not complete, which indicated that there was also some genetic heterogeneity in the 3 traits. Therefore, the wider search for genes that underlie food preferences would benefit from the measuring of the many dimensions that characterize taste sensitivity, oral sensitivity, and food rejection to obtain a complete picture.
Common shared environmental factors were also shown to contribute to the association between FF and vegetable liking (21%) and FF and fruit liking (24%). Although the shared environment only explained a small proportion of the variance in FF (5%), almost all of the shared environmental influences that contributed to FF also contributed to vegetable preferences (shared environmental correlation: 0.97), whereas almost three-quarters of these influences were shared between FF and fruit preferences (shared environmental correlation: 0.72). Common shared environmental influences likely include the early family feeding environment, with siblings raised in the same household sharing similar diets, early feeding experiences, and food exposures. Other shared environmental factors such as food availability, common illnesses, and parental modeling may also contribute to the commonalities in children’s food preferences and fussiness.
A considerable proportion of the variance in liking for fruit and vegetables and in FF was also independent insofar as the phenotypic associations were not complete. This unique variance may have reflected distinct mechanisms that are exclusive to each trait, which may also include behavioral or psychological traits such as other appetitive or personality phenotypes. Previous research has shown that individuals who are more sensation seeking tend to be much-less food neophobic (3, 36), possibly because they have lower levels of neophobia in all domains (37). Other personality factors, such as anxiety (3), neuroticism (38) and openness (39), have also been shown to be related to food rejection, and selective eating behaviors have been linked with psychopathologic symptoms including depression and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (40).
To our knowledge, this is the first study to explore the extent to which FF and liking for specific food groups share common genetic influences. The large sample size and multivariate design provided robust estimates for the heritability of FF and also for the shared pathways that influence FF and liking for vegetables and fruit. However, there were several limitations that should be acknowledged. The large sample size prohibited behavioral observations, and parent-reported measures of food liking and FF were necessary because of the young age of the children (41). The children in this study were 3 y old; because FF emerges in early childhood and may peak in the preschool years (42), the findings from this study may reflect a very specific period in development, thereby limiting the wider implications of these results. There is a need for replication of this research at older ages.
In conclusion, this novel investigation into the shared influences that underlie FF and liking for vegetables and fruit in early childhood provides strong evidence that common genetic influences are driving the observed phenotypic associations between these traits. These findings may help to explain why, of all the food groups, vegetables and fruit are the foods that are rejected most often by fussier children.
Acknowledgments
The authors’ responsibilities were as follows—AF, CHMvJ, and CHL: conducted the research and analyzed the data; AF and CHL: wrote the manuscript; AF: had primary responsibility for the final content of the manuscript; and all authors: designed the research and read and approved the final manuscript. None of the authors reported a conflict of interest related to the study.
Footnotes
Abbreviations used: CEBQ, Children’s Eating Behavior Questionnaire; CT/CT, cross-twin, cross-trait; FF, food fussiness; ICC, intraclass correlation; MLSEM, maximum-likelihood structural equation modeling; PROP, 6-n-propylthiouracil; TAS2R38, taste 2 receptor member 38.
REFERENCES
- 1.Carruth BR, Ziegler PJ, Gordon A, Barr SI. Prevalence of picky eaters among infants and toddlers and their caregivers’ decisions about offering a new food. J Am Diet Assoc 2004;104(1 Suppl 1):s57–64. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Cashdan E. A sensitive period for learning about food. Hum Nat. 1994;5:279–91. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Galloway AT, Lee Y, Birch LL. Predictors and consequences of food neophobia and pickiness in young girls. J Am Diet Assoc 2003;103:692–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Jacobi C, Agras WS, Bryson S, Hammer LD. Behavioral validation, precursors, and concomitants of picky eating in childhood. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2003;42:76–84. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Dovey TM, Staples PA, Gibson EL, Halford JC. Food neophobia and ‘picky/fussy’ eating in children: a review. Appetite 2008;50:181–93. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Galloway AT, Fiorito L, Lee Y, Birch LL. Parental pressure, dietary patterns, and weight status among girls who are “picky eaters”. J Am Diet Assoc 2005;105:541–8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Cooke L, Carnell S, Wardle J. Food neophobia and mealtime food consumption in 4-5 year old children. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 2006;3:14. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Cooke L, Wardle J, Gibson EL. Relationship between parental report of food neophobia and everyday food consumption in 2-6-year-old children. Appetite 2003;41:205–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Howard AJ, Mallan KM, Byrne R, Magarey A, Daniels LA. Toddlers’ food preferences. The impact of novel food exposure, maternal preferences and food neophobia. Appetite 2012;59:818–25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Russell CG, Worsley A. A population-based study of preschoolers’ food neophobila and its associations with food preferences. J Nutr Educ Behav 2008;40:11–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.WHO. Diet, nutrition and the prevention of chronic diseases. Report of a joint FAO/ WHO Expert Consultation. Technical Report Series, No. 916 [Internet]. Geneva (Switzerland):World Health Organization; 2003 [cited 2015 Jun 1]. Available from: http://www.who.int/dietphysicalactivity/publications/trs916/en/.
- 12.WHO. Global status report on noncommunicable diseases 2010 [Internet]. Geneva (Switzerland):World Health Organization; 2011 [cited 2015 Jun 1]. Available from: http://www.who.int/nmh/publications/ncd_report2010/en/.
- 13.Lock K, Pomerleau J, Causer L, Altmann DR, McKee M. The global burden of disease attributable to low consumption of fruit and vegetables: implications for the global strategy on diet. Bull World Health Organ 2005;83:100–8. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Siega-Riz AM, Deming DM, Reidy KC, Fox MK, Condon E, Briefel RR. Food consumption patterns of infants and toddlers: where are we now? J Am Diet Assoc 2010;110:S38–51. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Sproston K, Primatesta P, editors. Health survey for England 2002. London: Stationery Office; 2003.
- 16.Potts HW, Wardle J. The list heuristic for studying personality correlates of food choice behaviour: a review and results from two samples. Appetite 1998;30:79–92. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Cooke L, Haworth CM, Wardle J. Genetic and environmental influences on children’s food neophobia. Am J Clin Nutr 2007;86:428–33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Faith MS, Heo M, Keller KL, Pietrobelli A. Child food neophobia is heritable, associated with less compliant eating, and moderates familial resemblance for BMI. Obesity (Silver Spring) 2013;21:1650–5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Fildes A, van Jaarsveld CH, Llewellyn CH, Fisher A, Cooke L, Wardle J. Nature and nurture in children’s food preferences. Am J Clin Nutr 2014;99:911–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Breen FM, Plomin R, Wardle J. Heritability of food preferences in young children. Physiol Behav 2006;88:443–7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.van Jaarsveld CH, Johnson L, Llewellyn C, Wardle J. Gemini: a UK twin birth cohort with a focus on early childhood weight trajectories, appetite and the family environment. Twin Res Hum Genet 2010;13:72–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Wardle J, Guthrie CA, Sanderson S, Rapoport L. Development of the Children’s Eating Behaviour Questionnaire. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2001;42:963–70. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Price TS, Freeman B, Craig I, Petrill SA, Ebersole L, Plomin R. Infant zygosity can be assigned by parental report questionnaire data. Twin Res 2000;3:129–33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.van Jaarsveld CH, Llewellyn CH, Fildes A, Fisher A, Wardle J. Are my twins identical: parents may be misinformed by prenatal scan observations. BJOG 2012;119:517–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.McGue M, Bouchard TJ. Adjustment of twin data for the effects of age and sex. Behav Genet 1984;14:325–43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Plomin R, DeFries JC, McClearn GE, McGuffin P. Behavioral genetics. 5th ed. New York: Worth; 2008. [Google Scholar]
- 27.Neale MC, Maes HM. Methodology for genetic studies of twins and families. Dordrecht (Netherlands): Kluwer Academic Publishers; 2001. [Google Scholar]
- 28.Lim J, Urban L, Green BG. Measures of individual differences in taste and creaminess perception. Chem Senses 2008;33:493–501. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Reed DR. Birth of a new breed of supertaster. Chem Senses 2008;33:489–91. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Tepper BJ, White EA, Koelliker Y, Lanzara C, d’Adamo P, Gasparini P. Genetic variation in taste sensitivity to 6-n-propylthiouracil and its relationship to taste perception and food selection. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2009;1170:126–39. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Pronin AN, Xu H, Tang H, Zhang L, Li Q, Li X. Specific alleles of bitter receptor genes influence human sensitivity to the bitterness of aloin and saccharin. Curr Biol 2007;17:1403–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Wang JC, Hinrichs AL, Bertelsen S, Stock H, Budde JP, Dick DM, Bucholz KK, Rice J, Saccone N, Edenberg HJ, et al. Functional variants in TAS2R38 and TAS2R16 influence alcohol consumption in high-risk families of African-American origin. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 2007;31:209–15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Hebebrand J, Volckmar AL, Knoll N, Hinney A. Chipping away the ‘missing heritability’: GIANT steps forward in the molecular elucidation of obesity - but still lots to go. Obes Facts 2010;3:294–303. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Llewellyn CH, Trzaskowski M, van Jaarsveld CH, Plomin R, Wardle J. Satiety mechanisms in genetic risk of obesity. JAMA Pediatr 2014;168:338–44. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Locke AE, Kahali B, Berndt SI, Justice AE, Pers TH, Day FR, Powell C, Vedantam S, Buchkovich ML, Yang J, et al. Genetic studies of body mass index yield new insights for obesity biology. Nature 2015;518:197–206. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Pliner P, Melo N. Food neophobia in humans: effects of manipulated arousal and individual differences in sensation seeking. Physiol Behav 1997;61:331–5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Pliner P, Hobden K. Development of a scale to measure the trait of food neophobia in humans. Appetite 1992;19:105–20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Steptoe A, Pollard TM, Wardle J. Development of a measure of the motives underlying the selection of food: the food choice questionnaire. Appetite 1995;25:267–84. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.McCrae RR, Costa PT Jr, Terracciano A, Parker WD, Mills CJ, De Fruyt F, Mervielde I. Personality trait development from age 12 to age 18: longitudinal, cross-sectional, and cross-cultural analyses. J Pers Soc Psychol 2002;83:1456–68. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Rangan AM, Kwan J, Flood VM, Louie JC, Gill TP. Changes in ‘extra’ food intake among Australian children between 1995 and 2007. Obes Res Clin Pract 2011;5:e1–78. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Hammond J, Nelson M, Chinn S, Rona RJ. Validation of a food frequency questionnaire for assessing dietary-intake in a study of coronary heart-disease risk-factors in children. Eur J Clin Nutr 1993;47:242–50. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Cardona Cano S, Tiemeier H, Van Hoeken D, Tharner A, Jaddoe VWV, Hofman A, Verhulst FC, Hoek HW. Trajectories of picky eating during childhood: a general population study. Int J Eat Disord 201548:570–9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]