Skip to main content
Journal of Neurophysiology logoLink to Journal of Neurophysiology
. 2016 Jan 20;115(3):1314–1323. doi: 10.1152/jn.00311.2015

Dynamic taste responses of parabrachial pontine neurons in awake rats

Madelyn A Baez-Santiago 1,3,, Emily E Reid 2, Anan Moran 2,3,4,5, Joost X Maier 6, Yasmin Marrero-Garcia 2, Donald B Katz 1,2,3
PMCID: PMC4808110  PMID: 26792879

Abstract

The parabrachial nuclei of the pons (PbN) receive almost direct input from taste buds on the tongue and control basic taste-driven behaviors. Thus it is reasonable to hypothesize that PbN neurons might respond to tastes in a manner similar to that of peripheral receptors, i.e., that these responses might be narrow and relatively “dynamics free.” On the other hand, the majority of the input to PbN descends from forebrain regions such as gustatory cortex (GC), which processes tastes with “temporal codes” in which firing reflects first the presence, then the identity, and finally the desirability of the stimulus. Therefore a reasonable alternative hypothesis is that PbN responses might be dominated by dynamics similar to those observed in GC. Here we examined simultaneously recorded single-neuron PbN (and GC) responses in awake rats receiving exposure to basic taste stimuli. We found that pontine taste responses were almost entirely confined to canonically identified taste-PbN (t-PbN). Taste-specificity was found, furthermore, to be time varying in a larger percentage of these t-PbN responses than in responses recorded from the tissue around PbN (including non-taste-PbN). Finally, these time-varying properties were a good match for those observed in simultaneously recorded GC neurons—taste-specificity appeared after an initial nonspecific burst of action potentials, and palatability emerged several hundred milliseconds later. These results suggest that the pontine taste relay is closely allied with the dynamic taste processing performed in forebrain.

Keywords: parabrachial nucleus, taste processing, temporal coding, gustatory cortex


the taste system exists to facilitate identification of nutrients and toxins in an animal's potential diet (Boudreau et al. 1985; Scott et al. 1999). Much of this process is performed within the central nervous system (CNS), a fact evident in the ease and speed with which taste preferences are changed by experience (see Galef 1986; Monk et al. 2014; Spector et al. 1988). However, much remains to be learned about how such identifications arise in the CNS—specifically, what neural mechanisms assign meaningful labels (e.g., chemical identity, hedonic attributes) to tastes and where these mechanisms reside.

It is clear that the nature of taste coding changes between the periphery, where processing units appear to be simply labeled for specific stimulus types (Scott 2004; Zhao et al. 2003), and cortex, where neurons respond to tastes with complex, perceptually relevant dynamics—responding first to the presence of a taste on the tongue, then to its physical properties, and finally to its palatability, all across the first second of firing (Bahar et al. 2004; Grossman et al. 2008; Katz et al. 2001; Maier and Katz 2013; Piette et al. 2012; Sadacca et al. 2012; Samuelsen et al. 2012). It is less clear whether the regions between the tongue and forebrain are more “periphery-like” or “cortex-like.”

Of particular interest in this regard are responses of pontine parabrachial nucleus (PbN) neurons in the brain stem. In the rodent, PbN neurons receive taste information almost directly from the tongue, via a single intervening way station in the rostral nucleus of the solitary tract (rNTS; Granata and Kitai 1989; Halsell et al. 1996; Suwabe and Bradley 2009). PbN plays a central role in gustation (Sewards 2004) and is necessary for palatability processing (Spector 1995), taste-reward processing (Mungarndee et al. 2008), and taste learning (Grigson et al. 1997; Reilly et al. 1993; Sakai and Yamamoto 1998; Yamamoto et al. 1995). This involvement of PbN in the most basic of taste processes likely reflects its placement as a bottleneck in the ascending signal, and is thus certainly consistent with the suggestion that taste responses in PbN might best reflect simple representations of taste input ascending from the periphery.

In line with this prediction, most examinations of PbN taste responses have reported little in the way of cortexlike dynamics. These taste responses, which have been observed exclusively in the brachium conjuctivum, medial PbN, and external lateral and ventral lateral subregions of PbN, are typically summarized in terms of spikes per second averaged across 5–10 s, with the primary realm of controversy being whether they are best described as narrowly (Geran and Travers 2009; Nishijo and Norgren 1990, 1991, 1997) or broadly (Di Lorenzo 1988) tuned.

On the other hand, the majority of input to PbN is not ascending signals from rNTS but rather feedback from forebrain structures such as gustatory cortex (GC) (Zhang et al. 2011). Such feedback has been shown to have a significant impact on PbN taste responses (Li et al. 2005; Lundy and Norgren 2004; Smith et al. 1998) and on taste-induced behavior (Grill and Norgren 1978; Tokita et al. 2004, 2007; Touzani et al. 1997). It is therefore reasonable to hypothesize that PbN taste responses may be dominated by temporal structures similar to those observed in cortex, produced via this pathway. Recent work has in fact suggested that responses in rNTS (Di Lorenzo and Victor 2003; Roussin et al. 2012), and PbN itself (Geran and Travers 2013; Weiss et al. 2014), do vary through time in an information-rich manner (dynamics that are masked in analyses of multisecond averages of firing).

Here we further probe the possible temporal structure of taste responsiveness of PbN neurons in light of the above considerations, testing the specific hypothesis that PbN taste response dynamics will be similar to those of simultaneously recorded GC neurons. GC neurons were simultaneously assayed with those in taste-PbN (t-PbN) to ensure that comparisons of the two structures were made using data obtained under identical conditions. Our data support this hypothesis: as in GC, PbN taste responses progress from fluid responsive to taste specific, and from there to palatability related, all on a timescale similar to simultaneously recorded cortical responses.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Subjects

Female Long-Evans rats (n = 19, 275–340 g at time of surgery) from Charles River Laboratories served as subjects. Females were chosen for docility and because our previous work has suggested little to no difference between males and females with regard to response dynamics, at least in cortex. While shifting estradiol levels undoubtedly impact some aspects of taste activity (Curtis et al. 2004; Curtis and Contreras 2006; Di Lorenzo and Monroe 1989; Stratford et al. 2006), these effects would, if anything, reduce the reliability of our results; thus any observed results are those that are robust enough to be insensitive to estrus state. Rats were maintained on a 12:12-h light-dark schedule and given ad libitum access to chow and water, unless otherwise specified. All sessions took place within the light period, and all surgical and experimental protocols were reviewed and approved by the Brandeis University Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (IACUC).

Surgery

Rats were anesthetized with intraperitoneal injections of a ketamine-xylazine cocktail (100 and 10 mg/kg rat weight, respectively). Stable levels of anesthesia were maintained with 0.1 ml/h injections of the same cocktail. Rats were placed in a Kopf stereotaxic apparatus, and four sterile stainless steel ground screws were inserted into the skull. Craniotomies were then made, through which bundles of 16 formvar-coated 25-μm nichrome wires attached to a movable mini-microdrive (Katz et al. 2001) were implanted unilaterally 1 mm above PbN (−9.1 6 mm AP and ±2 mm ML from bregma; 6–7 mm DV from dura) and GC (1.4 A and ± 5.0 ML from bregma; −5.5 DV). To avoid puncturing the transverse sinus, the PbN microelectrode bundle was oriented 20° off the vertical with the tip pointing caudal. To avoid any risk of damaging the target region before recording (and to avoid neuron preselection), we did not use a search electrode before electrode implantation. Although this method increased the placement variability, that variability proved useful, as recordings obtained from areas outside the “waist” and lateral taste regions of PbN were used as control data.

After insertion of electrode bundles, bilateral intraoral cannulas (IOCs) were implanted and secured near the headstage (Phillips and Norgren 1970) to allow for tastant delivery, and the entire headstage was cemented to the skull with dental acrylic. After surgery, rats were given postoperative analgesics (Rimadyl, 4.4 mg/kg) and antibiotics (Pro-Pen-G, 150,000 U/kg) once a day for at least 2 days. Rats recovered for 7 days after surgery before undergoing habituation and experimental sessions (see below). During the recovery period, rats were provided with ad libitum access to chow and water.

Experimental Design

After recovery from surgery, rats were placed on water restriction (15 ml/day, given during the late morning) and habituated to the recording chamber and to fluid delivery through the IOC. During subsequent recording sessions, the responses of small ensembles of PbN and GC single neurons to passive administration of reagent-grade chemicals dissolved in double-distilled water [room temperature 40-μl aliquots of (in mM) 100 sucrose, 100 NaCl, 10 citric acid, and 1 quinine HCl] were recorded (see below); concentrations were selected to approximate those used in previous studies of this sort (Chen et al. 2011b; Frank et al. 2003; Weiss et al. 2014).

Taste solutions were maintained under slight nitrogen pressure and delivered directly into the mouth by means of a manifold of five miniature polyimide tubes slid to the bottom of the IOC (to ensure there was no taste mixing). The combination of manifold and pressure ensured that relatively broad tongue coverage was achieved within only a few milliseconds of the computer-controlled solenoid opening—a similar delay for each taste (variability on the order of a few milliseconds) that was easily accounted for in our analyses. Tastes were selected randomly without replacement and interleaved with water rinse. There was a 15-s gap between each delivery to ensure resumption of baseline firing. Recording sessions lasted 25–35 min, such that rats received between 12 and 15 trials of each tastant overall (total fluid volume ∼2.5 ml).

Each rat underwent two or three recording sessions (1/day). Electrode tips were lowered by 75 μm between each recording day, enabling recordings within t-PbN, outside of t-PbN (non-t-PbN), and outside of PbN (non-PbN) in the same animal. The electrodes were moved up to a total of 0.5 mm in depth by the end of the recording.

Electrophysiology

During recording sessions, neural signals were collected from electrode bundles in PbN (16 electrodes) and GC (16 electrodes). Differential recordings were fed into a parallel processor, which digitized, amplified, filtered, and saved the signals (Plexon). Action potentials of signal-to-noise ratio > 3:1 were isolated online and set to cross a designated amplitude threshold. Single-unit action potentials were discriminated and isolated with a template algorithm and cluster cutting techniques (Offline Sorter, Plexon). All other analyses of electrophysiological data were performed with MATLAB (http://www.mathworks.com/).

Data Analysis

All analyses were performed on the evoked nonnormalized firing rates in the 2,000 ms after stimulation—a period encapsulating the time that it takes an animal to identify the taste, decide on its current hedonic value, and act upon that decision (Halpern and Tapper 1971; Perez et al. 2013). Peristimulus time histograms (PSTHs) were produced from the firing of isolated single neurons, and these PSTHs served as the basic input to statistical analysis. In a small number of cases, electrical noise caused by solenoid opening contaminated the first 30 ms of the PSTHs; this contamination was removed by an algorithm that ignored above-baseline numbers of “action potentials” within the −10 to +30 ms period of the response, a period that did not overlap any portion of the taste responses themselves.

Fluid responsiveness.

Classification of fluid-responsive (and taste-specific, see below) units followed definitions used in previous publications (Maier et al. 2012; Piette et al. 2012; Sadacca et al. 2012): A neuron was deemed “fluid responsive” if any of the tastants significantly altered that neuron's baseline firing rate. For this analysis, evoked nonnormalized responses to tastant administration were averaged for each tastant (across 2,000 ms) and compared to baseline firing rate (1,000 ms before tastant delivery) with paired t-tests. If stimulus-induced firing rates were significantly higher or lower than the baseline firing rate (with P < 0.001), they were considered fluid responsive. This analysis was then repeated for small time windows (200 ms, slid through the 2-s response window in 50-ms increments).

Note that this measure does not differentiate or specify differences in magnitude to the evoked response of each tastant or temporal differences between the evoked responses—that is, it identifies neurons that were modulated by fluid delivery, without determining whether those neurons were modulated distinctly by different tastants.

Taste-specificity.

To identify which fluid-responsive neurons were “taste specific” (i.e., for which the normalized response to at least one tastant differed significantly from that to at least one other, in terms of either magnitude or time course), we applied a two-way ANOVA with taste and time factors to 10 consecutive, nonoverlapping time windows of that neuron's evoked responses (window size: 250 ms; time span: 0–2,000 ms relative to taste delivery). A significant main effect of taste in such an ANOVA implies taste-specificity in the overall 2-s average of firing (i.e., the overall firing rate response to at least one taste was significantly different from the overall average firing rate response to at least one other taste). A significant taste × time interaction, meanwhile, can be interpreted as indicating “temporal coding” (i.e., different time courses of responses for different tastes; Li et al. 2013).

Note again that it is possible to have fluid-responsive neurons for which responses are not taste specific; furthermore, by the above definitions, neurons can also be taste specific without being fluid responsive. This is due to the fact that fluid responsiveness is evaluated with a paired t-test that compares a large bin of poststimulus time to baseline and that this long time average may possibly obscure brief modulations (even a strong brief response to a single taste may be nonsignificant when all tastant trials are averaged). A biphasic response, for example, initially excitatory then inhibitory, can be canceled when averaged across time. Alternatively, a neuron might produce a mild inhibitory response to one stimulus and a mild excitatory response to another, such that neither manages to deviate significantly from baseline even though they differ significantly from each other. In each of these cases, the neuron's responses would be classified as “taste specific” despite not being deemed “fluid responsive” overall.

Ancillary analyses shed light on the magnitude of taste-specificity uncovered in the two-way ANOVAs. We used the sums of squares differences (η, a step in the quantification of variability leading to the ANOVA F-test) as estimates of effect size. Effect size estimates for all neurons from these analyses, and from similar sets of one-way ANOVAs examining the impact of taste delivery on firing rate of individual neurons in a sliding window over time (window size: 500 ms; step size: 100 ms), were themselves evaluated by ANOVA and/or paired t-test.

Palatability relatedness.

To determine whether neurons produced “palatability-related” responses, we calculated the absolute correlation coefficient (r) between each neuron's firing rate responses for each tastant and a simple established linear palatability function (which were estimated, based on the above references, to be sucrose > NaCl > citric acid > quinine). The magnitude of the correlation provided a measure of how well between-stimulus differences in response were accounted for by palatability. A neuron's response was considered “palatability related” if this correlation achieved statistical significance (Fontanini et al. 2009; Li et al. 2013; Piette et al. 2012).

It is worth noting that the temporal resolution of the above analysis is dependent on bin and sample sizes. We therefore performed one ancillary analysis of the above-described palatability relatedness plot—a normalized CUSUM chart. This procedure, a part of “change point” analysis (Kass-Hout et al. 2012; Page 1954; Weinberger et al. 2006), is commonly used to assess shifts in the mean of any kind of time series. In this case, we examined the time series of correlations between firing and palatability, using CUSUM to better visualize the time point at which correlations between the neuronal firing and palatability along the neuronal responses between neurons from different regions increase.

To that end, we first calculated the firing correlation to palatability in a short time window (200 ms), slid the window forward by 50 ms, and recalculated the correlation. Once we had a time series of correlations (starting 1,000 ms before stimulus delivery and ending 2,000 ms after stimulus delivery), we transformed the time series by subtracting the mean correlation from each of these instantaneous correlations. The sum of these differences of successive comparisons was then accumulated (hence “CUSUM”), and the PbN and GC charts were normalized to one another (with their minimum at zero) to facilitate interregional comparison.

If the mean correlations in such plots remain stable across the entire period, this time series will therefore wander randomly around a single value. If, however, the correlation increases within a particular time window (as we propose), the mean value will be intermediate between the two extremes, and therefore the differences between the individual points and the mean will be negative before the time of the increase and positive afterward; in this case, the CUSUM will become more and more negative until the time point of the correlation change, and will then head positive.

Each of these analyses offers information regarding the time course of palatability-related firing. Of course, no across-trial measure of this firing will provide precise evaluation; any useable bin size will necessarily smooth the data and smear the information.

Histology

Analysis of the electrophysiological data was done blind to histological confirmation of recording sites. After the last experimental session, rats were deeply anesthetized with an overdose of the same ketamine-xylazine cocktail used in surgeries, and small electrolytic lesions were made to mark the final location of electrode tips from which neurons of interest were recorded (via 7-μA DC current for 7 s). Rats were then perfused with 10% formalin, and brains were harvested and sectioned at a thickness of 60 μm and stained with cresyl violet (Sigma-Aldrich) to visualize Nissl substance in the neurons. Electrode trajectories were reconstructed based on information regarding their implantation, movement, and visible tracks, in reference to sections containing the electrode tracks and lesions, in relation to the standard rat brain atlas (Paxinos and Watson 2007); we extrapolated from our records of the electrode movements across recording days to identify the anatomical locations of each recording.

RESULTS

The responses of histologically verified (Fig. 1) brain stem neurons (n = 46) to solutions of basic tastes (sucrose, NaCl, citric acid, and quinine) were recorded from 19 awake rats (2.0 ± 0.2 brain stem neurons/ensemble). In a subset of these rats (n = 5), cortical neurons were simultaneously recorded with brain stem neurons. A total of 8 PbN-GC ensembles were isolated, with an average of 2.0 ± 0.4 brain stem neurons/ensemble and 3.0 ± 0.8 GC neurons/ensemble. A minimum of 13 responses to each taste were assayed in each recording session, along with spontaneous activity between each stimulus delivery.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1.

Histological analysis of recording sites in the pontine parabrachial nucleus (PbN). A: photomicrographs of PbN from a representative rat. Histology panels show a PbN recording lesion in the external lateral subnucleus (A1) and in the “waist” area of PbN (A2) and in gustatory cortex (GC; A3). B: schematic of coronal sections through the PbN [reprinted from Paxinos and Watson (2007) with permission] containing reconstructions of all electrode tracts and approximate recording sites. Numbers at top indicate distance from bregma (in mm). Red asterisks show locations that provided recordings of taste-responsive cells in taste PbN (t-PbN), green asterisks show locations that provided outside of t-PbN (non-t-PbN) taste-responsive cells, and blue asterisks outside of PbN (non-PbN) taste-responsive cells. Black dots in the images represent mesencephalic trigeminal neurons. BC, brachium conjuctivum; Me5, mesencephalic trigeminal neurons; DLL, dorsal lateral lemniscus; DTg, dorsal tegmental nucleus; KF, Kölliker-Fuse nucleus; LDT, laterodorsal tegmental nucleus; mcp, middle cerebellar peduncle; MPB, medial parabrachial nucleus; MPBe, external medial parabrachial nucleus; PTg, pedunculopontine tegmental nucleus.

The mean spontaneous firing rate (average activity in the 1.0 s before tastant delivery) of the PbN sample was 13.9 ± 3.7 (SE) spikes/s, a number that is in good agreement with those reported in other studies using chronically implanted PbN electrodes in rats (Di Lorenzo 1988; Nishijo and Norgren 1990, 1991, 1997; Weiss et al. 2014). The mean spontaneous firing rate for the simultaneously recorded GC neurons was lower—4.3 ± 1.0 (SE) spikes/s, a number also consistent with previous reports (Katz et al. 2001; Piette et al. 2012; Yamamoto et al. 1984).

Example raster plots and PSTHs generated from t-PbN (Fig. 2A), non-t-PbN (Fig. 2B), and non-PbN (Fig. 2C) neurons reveal the expected “fluid responsiveness”—that is, their poststimulus firing rates differed significantly from baseline on average—emerging almost immediately after taste delivery and peaking at ∼200–250 ms after stimulus onset. This fluid responsiveness proved representative of the whole data set (as we demonstrate in greater detail below), in which ∼70% (32/46) of the recorded brain stem sample were fluid responsive—46% (6/13) of t-PbN neurons, 67% (12/18) of non-t-PbN neurons, and 93% (14/15) of non-PbN neurons were modulated by oral delivery of fluid stimuli, according to paired t-tests (P value = 0.001).

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2.

Representative data. Averaged peristimulus time histograms (PSTHs) from representative t-PbN (A), non-t-PbN (B), and non-PbN (C) neurons after the administration of 12–15 trials (each) of 4 tastants [sucrose, NaCl, citric acid (CA), quinine (Q)]. x-Axis is time (0 represents the time of taste administration), and y-axis is firing rate. According to our analyses, all 3 neurons are taste specific (i.e., significant effect of taste, or significant taste × time interaction, in a 2-way ANOVA; see text for details). Above each set of PSTHs are raster plots showing firing (each colored mark = an action potential) in each trial to each taste. Note that in A electrical noise caused by the solenoid opening was removed by an algorithm that ignored above-baseline numbers of “action potentials” within the −10 to +30 ms period of the response, a period that did not overlap with any portion of the taste responses themselves.

The high rate of nonspecific responsiveness, the striking of the tongue by fluid, may reflect the prevalence of somatosensory innervation in the regions in and surrounding t-PbN (Baird et al. 2001; Halsell and Travers 1997; Karimnamazi and Travers 1998), as it did in previously recorded GC neurons (Katz et al. 2001). While we did not deliver water as a “taste stimulus,” examination of responses to water rinse trials (immediately following delivery of another tastant) suggested that indeed the same is true in the pons: 11 of 13 neurons showing either excitatory or inhibitory early responses (numbers of these were about equal) to tastes also showed the same response to the water rinse; these responses in general did not appear to be taste related (but see discussion).

However, while there were high rates of simple responsivity in all areas examined, “taste-specificity” was focused more in t-PbN responses than in non-t-PbN and non-PbN responses. Ninety-two percent (12/13) of t-PbN neurons responded distinctly to different tastes (that is, they were not only fluid responsive but also taste specific), whereas fewer non-t-PbN and non-PbN units—61% (11/18) and 73% (11/15), respectively—fired in a taste-specific manner.

Furthermore, even those non-t-PbN and non-PbN neurons that did produce taste-specific responses failed to do so robustly. This can be seen in the example neurons in Fig. 2: Although all three example neurons produced significant taste-specific responses (in that 2-way ANOVAs showed significant main effects of taste), visual inspection suggests that the variance of the responses as a function of taste was broader (that is, there were larger differences in responses to different tastes, i.e., more taste-specificity) for the neuron in t-PbN (Fig. 2A) than for those of the other brain stem neurons (Fig. 2, B and C). Statistical analysis of the total data set confirmed these appearances, showing that the average effect size for taste (η, essentially the raw SStaste value from the ANOVA) across the taste-specific subset of neurons (a data selection criterion that removed any confound that might emerge because of the factoring in of a higher percentage of nonsignificant responses in nontaste regions) was significantly higher for t-PbN neurons than for other neurons [t(33) = 2.56, P = 0.015].

Thus the above data suggest not only that pontine taste activity is centered in the classically defined PbN taste subregions (Nishijo and Norgren 1990, 1997; Williams et al. 1996; Yamamoto et al. 1980, 1994, 2009) but also that even the neurons outside of this region that do demonstrate taste-specificity do so only weakly—perhaps because of stray spread of connectivity. Thus our results are consistent with those of previous studies.

Taste-specificity observed in t-PbN responses was not only of greater magnitude than that observed in other regions, however—it was more temporally complex as well. The pattern of responses across tastes for the t-PbN neuron shown in Fig. 2A, for instance, appears to shift precipitously across poststimulus time, whereas those of the non-t-PbN neuron shown in Fig. 2B and the non-PbN neuron shown in Fig. 2C do not; the “best stimulus” for the t-PbN neuron shifted at ∼750 ms after taste delivery (at the time that the evoked response to citric acid decreased), whereas the stimulus causing the strongest response was stable across a full 2 s for both of the other examples.

Again, the example results proved representative of the whole data set, according to which the differential taste-specificity of t-PbN responses (described above) to a large extent reflects the greater prevalence of temporal coding. Figure 3A presents the proportion of neurons that showed taste-specificity (i.e., a significant main effect of taste) and temporal coding (i.e., a significant taste × time interaction). This presentation reveals that the most noticeable difference between the entire sample of t-PbN neurons and that of other brain stem neurons had to do with the proportion of neurons for which taste-specificity changed through time. Whereas approximately half of the neurons in t-PbN produced time-varying patterns of taste-specificity, only a small subset of the neurons outside of t-PbN showed the same effect; specifically, 54% of taste-specific t-PbN neurons showed temporal complexity, while only 6% of non-t-PbN and 27% of non-PbN neurons did so, that is, 10 times as many t-PbN neurons as non-t-PbN neurons produced time-varying responses.

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3.

t-PbN responses are time varying. A: proportion of brain stem neurons showing significant (P < 0.001) “taste” (filled bars) main effects and “taste × time” interactions (hatched bars) according to 2-way ANOVAs (see materials and methods). B: proportion of neurons from each region showing taste-specific responses in specific postdelivery bins (by moving-window analysis).

Furthermore, analysis performed on all of the taste-specific neurons in the data set (i.e., restricted to only those neurons that produced significant taste and taste × time effects, thus removing the confound of nonresponsive neurons) revealed that it was the time-varying component of the two-way ANOVAs (i.e., the η values for the taste × time interaction) that differentiated neurons in the three regions [F(2,34) = 9.02, P = 0.0008]; the main effect for taste did not differentiate the groups [F(2,34) = 1.15, P = 0.3287]. Roughly half of the taste-specific responses recorded from t-PbN (a number that far exceeds chance and that is significantly higher than that observed outside of t-PbN) were time varying.

The above two results—that taste-specificity in brain stem responses outside of t-PbN is of relatively low magnitude and lacking in temporal features—imply that what little taste-specificity is apparent outside t-PbN should only be visible in multiple-second-long time averages of activity. We tested this additional prediction by performing a moving-window analysis in which we explicitly examined taste responses in 500-ms time bins (with a 100-ms step size). Figure 3B presents the results of this reanalysis, showing the percentage of brain stem neurons with significant taste-specific responses as a function of response time. This presentation confirms the suggestions that 1) t-PbN taste responses are more robust than those observed elsewhere, and in fact are for the most part the only pontine taste responses that are large enough in magnitude to be significant in small-bin analyses, and 2) these responses appear only after a 200- to 250-ms period of poststimulus time in which responses are nonspecific.

This latter point (which can also be observed in Fig. 2, in which the PSTHs essentially overlie one another for the first few hundred milliseconds) reveals a specific similarity between PbN taste response dynamics and those previously observed in forebrain structures such as basolateral amygdala (Fontanini et al. 2009), central amygdala (Sadacca et al. 2012), lateral hypothalamus (Li et al. 2013), and most noticeably primary gustatory cortex (Fontanini and Katz 2006; Katz et al. 2001; Maier and Katz 2013; Piette et al. 2012; Sadacca et al. 2012). Despite being relatively close to the peripheral taste cells, which react to taste input quickly (Chen et al. 2011b; Zhang et al. 2003), t-PbN neurons appear, like those in forebrain structures, to demonstrate an initial period of nonspecific response followed by the receipt of specific taste information only 200–250 ms after taste delivery.

Statistical confirmation of this relatively slow onset of taste-specificity is shown in Fig. 4, which presents the average effect sizes (ηtaste from the set of neurons showing taste-specificity) for data compiled into moving windows. It can be seen that the aforementioned significant taste × time interaction to some degree reflects the fact that t-PbN taste-specificity does not begin to exceed that of nontaste neurons until 200–250 ms after taste delivery, just as is consistently reported for forebrain neurons. Thus a basic feature of cortical (and amygdalar and hypothalamic) taste responses also appears in PbN responses.

Fig. 4.

Fig. 4.

Taste-specificity of taste-PbN responses emerges after a few hundred milliseconds. Average effect size (η) obtained with 1-way ANOVA with taste as a factor (250-ms bins shifted by 50 ms) over the population of brain stem neurons.

Of course, the most distinctive feature characterizing cortical single-neuron responses to taste is not their relatively “slow” onset but rather the emergence of palatability-related firing between 0.5 and 1.0 s after stimulus delivery, that is, the relatively late rise in correlation between firing rates and taste palatability shown in experiments using basic tastes (Fontanini and Katz 2006; Katz et al. 2001; Piette et al. 2012), a range of NaCl concentrations (Sadacca et al. 2012), and binary taste mixtures (Maier and Katz 2013). To determine whether brain stem taste responses contain this feature, we performed a moving-window analysis of the correlation between taste-induced firing and taste palatability on the entire t-PbN sample and directly compared the results of this analysis to those of an identical analysis of simultaneously recorded GC neurons—simultaneous recording being an important control against the possibility of some minor differences in how past GC data were gathered.

Figure 5A shows the rasters and PSTHs for a pair of simultaneously recorded t-PbN and GC neurons, in both of which palatability-related firing appeared by ∼1.0 s after stimulus delivery; insets show the quality of single-neuron isolation. The PbN neuron (Fig. 5A, left) responded most to citric acid and quinine, the more aversive tastes, and the GC neuron (Fig. 5A, right) responded most to the more palatable tastes, sucrose and NaCl, but both patterns were found in both samples. Analysis of the full data sets, including even nonresponsive t-PbN and GC neurons (Fig. 5B), reveals that the correlations of PbN and GC taste responses with taste palatability emerge in similar manners (see also Katz et al. 2001; Piette et al. 2012; Sadacca et al. 2012), at least to the limits of our present analytic resolution. The brief “chop” in the GC correlation function (not observed in any of the above-mentioned studies) between 750 and 950 ms likely reflects random noise courtesy of the small size of the GC sample and did not obscure the obvious similarity in timing—a moving-window analysis of the correlation between firing rates and taste palatability failed to reveal differences between the time in which palatability-related responses emerged for t-PbN and simultaneously recorded GC neurons [2-way ANOVA, location × time: F(1,11) < 1, P = 0.83].

Fig. 5.

Fig. 5.

Palatability in t-PbN and GC responses emerges with similar time courses. A: trial-by-trial raster plots and PSTHs for single neurons simultaneously recorded from t-PbN (left) and GC (right). Gray dashed lines denote the approximate boundaries between early, middle, and late modulations (i.e., nonspecific activity, chemosensory responses, and palatability-related epochs as first described in Katz et al. 2001). Insets: waveforms for the respective single units (thick black lines, plus standard-deviation confidence intervals), with the background noise (thin gray waveforms) shown for comparison. B: moving-window analysis of the correlation between firing rates and taste palatability (y-axis) across poststimulus time (x-axis) performed separately for t-PbN and GC, showing that palatability-related firing emerges at approximately the same time in the 2 regions. Correlation magnitude within the 2 regions was normalized to facilitate comparison of time course. C: normalized CUSUM (y-axis) chart showing t-PbN and GC across time (x-axis). The correlation at each moment in time was subtracted from the average value for the −1,000 to 2,000 ms period, and the differences of successive comparisons were summed.

Figure 5C presents the CUSUM charts for the data presented in Fig. 5B, again normalized to one another. As predicted, these charts are not flat—rather, the cumulative sum of the differences between the instantaneous correlations and the average correlations decreases until ∼500 ms after stimulus delivery and then increases, reflecting the fact that the correlations with palatability increased sharply at that time point for neurons in both regions.

In sum, the taste responses of PbN neurons, the locations of which correspond well with previous reports, strongly resemble cortical responses in their dynamics. Specifically, they progress through a sequence of three epochs—an initial nonspecific response is followed by taste-specificity that emerges around 200–250 ms and finally gives way to palatability relatedness > 500 ms after taste delivery. This suggests that PbN, while relatively close to the periphery, produces taste responses that are part and parcel of the larger CNS taste processing system.

DISCUSSION

The goal of these experiments was to determine whether PbN responses recorded from awake rats are dominated by temporal structure akin to that observed in GC or whether, alternatively, PbN taste responsiveness is best described as being a simpler matter of response magnitude and breadth. Our results support the former conclusion. We demonstrate that while fluid-responsive neurons are present to some degree outside of t-PbN (and non-PbN as well), taste-specificity (i.e., distinct neural responses to different tastes) was to a large extent concentrated in t-PbN and stronger in t-PbN neurons than in even the few other brain stem neurons in which it was significant. Furthermore, the taste-specific responses of t-PbN neurons were time varying approximately four times as often as those recorded from outside this region—complexity that resembled that observed in GC neurons: taste-specificity and palatability-related responses emerge at what are at least similar time points after stimulation in t-PbN and GC.

This conclusion makes sense in light of the fact that PbN is not only an early taste relay but also part of a highly distributed system. PbN neurons target several forebrain structures, including insular GC (Kosar et al. 1986a, 1986b; Saper 1982), central amygdala (Ottersen 1981), and hypothalamus (Norgren 1974). Each of these regions in turn feeds information back upon PbN—feedback that has been shown to have a significant impact on basic responses to ascending taste signals (Li et al. 2005; Lundy and Norgren 2004; Smith et al. 1998; Zhang et al. 2011). It is therefore reasonable to expect that perception-related taste dynamics observed in forebrain should be associated with similarly time-varying properties in PbN taste responses. And indeed, taste responses in PbN ensembles appear to progress, as they do in GC (Katz et al. 2001; Piette et al. 2012; Sadacca et al. 2012), through coherent firing rate “epochs”—nonspecific firing appears first, followed by chemosensory and then evaluative information.

It is possible that responses in the very first central taste relay, the NST in the medulla, might demonstrate “less” of a dynamic quality than those in the PbN, although NST is also a target of substantial amounts of forebrain feedback (Cho et al. 2002, 2003, 2008; Di Lorenzo and Monroe 1995; Li et al. 2008, 2015). Work from the Di Lorenzo lab has investigated the transformation of responses between the two structures (Di Lorenzo and Monroe 1997; Weiss et al. 2014) and extensively examined temporal coding in each (Chen et al. 2011a; Chen and Di Lorenzo 2008; Di Lorenzo 1988, 2000; Di Lorenzo and Lemon 2000; Di Lorenzo and Monroe 1995); their results, from awake behaving animals, suggest that there is an increased complexity of response in pontine taste neurons compared with medullary.

Our work is distinct from classic work on brain stem temporal coding, which suggested that the onset of certain taste responses (notably NaCl) was earlier and faster than that of other taste responses (Di Lorenzo and Schwartzbaum 1982a, 1982b; Perrotto and Scott 1976); we, meanwhile, have observed no between-taste differences in the onset or magnitude of response dynamics. The difference between the present and previous results probably lies in the timescale of analysis: the earlier-described between-taste differences were on the scale of seconds, whereas here the observed response dynamics fall mostly within the first second of the responses—occurring on a timescale of tens and hundreds of milliseconds.

On the other hand, this work supports recent analyses by Weiss et al. (2014), who compared NTS and PbN taste response from freely licking rats and reported that PbN taste responses are more temporally complex than those in NTS. Specifically, these researchers showed that more bits of information regarding taste quality are present within the first 2 s of PbN taste responses than in NTS and that PbN taste cells convey more taste quality information by spike timing than NTS cells (spike timing-related information was stronger than spike count-related information). These findings demonstrate that PbN taste neurons deliver taste quality information in a temporally dynamic manner (more so than the medullar relay closer to the periphery). Here we show that these dynamics are limited to responses within t-PbN proper, that they resemble previously described cortical dynamics, and that they support a switch from quality to evaluative (hedonic) coding.

Like Weiss et al. (2014), our conclusions (and the difference between them and those of classic studies described above) come by virtue of the statistical power afforded by the acquisition of multiple trials, which allowed analysis of the time courses of responses to the basic tastants and permitted confident conclusions to be reached on the basis of a necessarily small sample of brain stem neurons. The fact that we were able to discern in a set of 19 GC neurons the same basic pattern previously demonstrated in multiple larger data sets (Katz et al. 2001; Piette et al. 2012; Sadacca et al. 2012) validates our interpretation of the PbN responses. While our sample size was small, our results are still within the range of similar experimental setups for PbN recording experiments (Lei et al. 2007; Monk et al. 2014; Weiss et al. 2014). Experiments conducted by Weiss reported that of 49 PbN neurons only 16 cells exhibited temporal coding properties at 200 ms (with the number increasing to 38 when the first 2 s of the response was analyzed). In an anesthetized preparation, Lei et al. (2007) reported taste responses for 18 inhibitory PbN neurons. Despite our small sample size, our results are all supported by inferential statistics, a testament to the robustness of the phenomena.

We were actually somewhat surprised at the similarity of the PbN and GC dynamics. We naively expected information regarding taste identity/quality to appear earlier in PbN than in GC, for the simple reason that PbN is earlier in the nominal taste processing hierarchy. It is possible that such a difference exists below the resolution of the present analyses (see materials and methods), or even that the opposite pattern exists (as a function of feedback pathways), and that a larger brain stem neuron sample (which is prohibitively difficult to obtain from awake, freely moving rats) would allow us to make finer-grained distinctions. Despite the resolution limits of the present analyses, PbN responses appear to become reliably taste specific only after an initial 200- to 250-ms period of either no response or nondistinctive response. Analysis of water rinse responses suggests that these early t-PbN responses are, as they are in GC, somatosensory rather than taste related, although the fact that traces of the previous taste might remain on the tongue at the time of water rinse delivery (and the fact that 2/13 neurons showing early taste responses showed no water rinse response, a proportion that could, in larger samples, prove to be higher than chance) means that we cannot conclusively rule out the possibility that these represent very broad taste responses. Regardless, these responses become reorganized to reflect taste palatability in the second half of the first response second, again like those in GC.

Behavioral studies have shown that rats can make taste-specific decisions in ∼200 ms after taste delivery (Halpern and Tapper 1971; Stapleton et al. 2006). The discrepancy between our electrophysiological data and these results could reflect differences in experimental setups. Our rats received tastants through passive intraoral infusion. Animals that are trained to receive a stimulus at a specific time (or lick) can anticipate a taste stimulus, thus shortening the latency of the taste-specific activity (Fontanini et al. 2009; Stapleton et al. 2006). Behavioral context can also change the latencies of taste-specific activities. Rats are capable of quickly identifying and rejecting a taste stimulus to which they have learned to have an aversive response (Halpern and Tapper 1971). While the latencies to taste-specific activity in PbN may differ depending on the behavioral context, we suggest that the dynamics between PbN and GC will remain the same.

These findings emerge in what is admittedly a small sample of neurons. In this regard, our study is similar to several previous investigations of brain stem taste responses, which often make use of similarly small samples (such data being very difficult to collect; see Lei et al. 2007; Weiss et al. 2014). While it is possible that future larger studies will call our conclusions into question, the very fact that, despite the sample size, these conclusions are supported by statistical significance is a testament to the results' robustness.

Using a neurophysiological recording approach in awake rats and simultaneous recordings in both PbN and GC, we show that temporal codes in the PbN differ from the classic time-averaged codes for taste. These results offer a new glimpse into early stages of central gustatory coding, showing that even at the level of the brain stem taste processing is temporally dynamic, coding more than just taste quality.

GRANTS

This research was supported by National Institutes of Health Grants R01 DC-6666 and R01 DC-7703, the Charles A. King Trust Fellowship, and the Swartz Foundation.

DISCLOSURES

No conflicts of interest, financial or otherwise, are declared by the author(s).

AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS

Author contributions: M.A.B.-S. and D.B.K. conception and design of research; M.A.B.-S., E.E.R., and Y.M.-G. performed experiments; M.A.B.-S., A.M., and J.X.M. analyzed data; M.A.B.-S., A.M., J.X.M., and D.B.K. interpreted results of experiments; M.A.B.-S. and E.E.R. prepared figures; M.A.B.-S., E.E.R., and D.B.K. drafted manuscript; M.A.B.-S., E.E.R., and D.B.K. edited and revised manuscript; M.A.B.-S. and D.B.K. approved final version of manuscript.

REFERENCES

  1. Bahar A, Dudai Y, Ahissar E. Neural signature of taste familiarity in the gustatory cortex of the freely behaving rat. J Neurophysiol 92: 3298–3308, 2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Baird JP, Travers SP, Travers JB. Integration of gastric distension and gustatory responses in the parabrachial nucleus. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 281: R1581–R1593, 2001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Boudreau JC, Sivakumar L, Do LT, White TD, Oravec J, Hoang NK. Neurophysiology of geniculate ganglion (facial nerve) taste systems: species comparisons. Chem Senses 10: 89–127, 1985. [Google Scholar]
  4. Chen JY, Di Lorenzo PM. Responses to binary taste mixtures in the nucleus of the solitary tract: neural coding with firing rate. J Neurophysiol 99: 2144–2157, 2008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Chen JY, Victor JD, Di Lorenzo PM. Temporal coding of intensity of NaCl and HCl in the nucleus of the solitary tract of the rat. J Neurophysiol 105: 697–711, 2011a. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Chen X, Gabitto M, Peng Y, Ryba NJ, Zuker CS. A gustotopic map of taste qualities in the mammalian brain. Science 333: 1262–1266, 2011b. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Cho YK, Li CS, Smith DV. Taste responses of neurons of the hamster solitary nucleus are enhanced by lateral hypothalamic stimulation. J Neurophysiol 87: 1981–1992, 2002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Cho YK, Li CS, Smith DV. Descending influences from the lateral hypothalamus and amygdala converge onto medullary taste neurons. Chem Senses 28: 155–171, 2003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Cho YK, Mao L, Li CS. Modulation of solitary taste neurons by electrical stimulation of the ventroposteromedial nucleus of the thalamus in the hamster. Brain Res 1221: 67–79, 2008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Curtis KS, Contreras RJ. Sex differences in electrophysiological and behavioral responses to NaCl taste. Behav Neurosci 120: 917–924, 2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Curtis KS, Davis LM, Johnson AL, Therrien KL, Contreras RJ. Sex differences in behavioral taste responses to and ingestion of sucrose and NaCl solutions by rats. Physiol Behav 80: 657–664, 2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Di Lorenzo PM. Taste responses in the parabrachial pons of decerebrate rats. J Neurophysiol 59: 1871–1887, 1988. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Di Lorenzo PM. The neural code for taste in the brain stem: response profiles. Physiol Behav 69: 87–96, 2000. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Di Lorenzo PM, Lemon CH. The neural code for taste in the nucleus of the solitary tract of the rat: effects of adaptation. Brain Res 852: 383–397, 2000. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Di Lorenzo PM, Monroe S. Taste responses in the parabrachial pons of male, female and pregnant rats. Brain Res Bull 23: 219–227, 1989. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Di Lorenzo PM, Monroe S. Corticofugal influence on taste responses in the nucleus of the solitary tract in the rat. J Neurophysiol 74: 258–272, 1995. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Di Lorenzo PM, Monroe S. Transfer of information about taste from the nucleus of the solitary tract to the parabrachial nucleus of the pons. Brain Res 763: 167–181, 1997. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Di Lorenzo PM, Schwartzbaum JS. Coding of gustatory information in the pontine parabrachial nuclei of the rabbit: magnitude of neural response. Brain Res 251: 229–244, 1982a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Di Lorenzo PM, Schwartzbaum JS. Coding of gustatory information in the pontine parabrachial nuclei of the rabbit: temporal patterns of neural response. Brain Res 251: 245–257, 1982b. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Di Lorenzo PM, Victor JD. Taste response variability and temporal coding in the nucleus of the solitary tract of the rat. J Neurophysiol 90: 1418–1431, 2003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Fontanini A, Grossman SE, Figueroa JA, Katz DB. Distinct subtypes of basolateral amygdala taste neurons reflect palatability and reward. J Neurosci 29: 2486–2495, 2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Fontanini A, Katz DB. State-dependent modulation of time-varying gustatory responses. J Neurophysiol 96: 3183–3193, 2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Frank ME, Formaker BK, Hettinger TP. Taste responses to mixtures: analytic processing of quality. Behav Neurosci 117: 228–235, 2003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Galef BG., Jr Social identification of toxic diets by Norway rats (Rattus norvegicus). J Comp Psychol 100: 331–334, 1986. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Geran L, Travers S. Temporal characteristics of gustatory responses in rat parabrachial neurons vary by stimulus and chemosensitive neuron type. PLoS One 8: e76828, 2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Geran LC, Travers SP. Bitter-responsive gustatory neurons in the rat parabrachial nucleus. J Neurophysiol 101: 1598–1612, 2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Granata AR, Kitai ST. Intracellular study of nucleus parabrachialis and nucleus tractus solitarii interconnections. Brain Res 492: 281–292, 1989. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Grigson PS, Shimura T, Norgren R. Brainstem lesions and gustatory function. III. The role of the nucleus of the solitary tract and the parabrachial nucleus in retention of a conditioned taste aversion in rats. Behav Neurosci 111: 180–187, 1997. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Grill HJ, Norgren R. The taste reactivity test. II. Mimetic responses to gustatory stimuli in chronic thalamic and chronic decerebrate rats. Brain Res 143: 281–297, 1978. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Grossman SE, Fontanini A, Wieskopf JS, Katz DB. Learning-related plasticity of temporal coding in simultaneously recorded amygdala-cortical ensembles. J Neurosci 28: 2864–2873, 2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Halpern BP, Tapper DN. Taste stimuli: quality coding time. Science 171: 1256–1258, 1971. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Halsell CB, Travers SP. Anterior and posterior oral cavity responsive neurons are differentially distributed among parabrachial subnuclei in rat. J Neurophysiol 78: 920–938, 1997. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Halsell CB, Travers SP, Travers JB. Ascending and descending projections from the rostral nucleus of the solitary tract originate from separate neuronal populations. Neuroscience 72: 185–197, 1996. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Karimnamazi H, Travers JB. Differential projections from gustatory responsive regions of the parabrachial nucleus to the medulla and forebrain. Brain Res 813: 283–302, 1998. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Kass-Hout TA, Xu Z, McMurray P, Park S, Buckeridge DL, Brownstein JS, Finelli L, Groseclose SL. Application of change point analysis to daily influenza-like illness emergency department visits. J Am Med Inform Assoc 19: 1075–1081, 2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Katz DB, Simon SA, Nicolelis MA. Dynamic and multimodal responses of gustatory cortical neurons in awake rats. J Neurosci 21: 4478–4489, 2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Kosar E, Grill HJ, Norgren R. Gustatory cortex in the rat. I. Physiological properties and cytoarchitecture. Brain Res 379: 329–341, 1986a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Kosar E, Grill HJ, Norgren R. Gustatory cortex in the rat. II. Thalamocortical projections. Brain Res 379: 342–352, 1986b. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Lei Q, Yan JQ, Shi JH, Yang XJ, Chen K. Inhibitory responses of parabrachial neurons evoked by taste stimuli in rat. Sheng Li Xue Bao 59: 260–266, 2007. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Li CS, Cho YK, Smith DV. Modulation of parabrachial taste neurons by electrical and chemical stimulation of the lateral hypothalamus and amygdala. J Neurophysiol 93: 1183–1196, 2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Li CS, Lu DP, Cho YK. Descending projections from the nucleus accumbens shell excite activity of taste-responsive neurons in the nucleus of the solitary tract in the hamster. J Neurophysiol 113: 3778–3786, 2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Li CS, Mao L, Cho YK. Taste-responsive neurons in the nucleus of the solitary tract receive gustatory information from both sides of the tongue in the hamster. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 294: R372–R381, 2008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Li JX, Yoshida T, Monk KJ, Katz DB. Lateral hypothalamus contains two types of palatability-related taste responses with distinct dynamics. J Neurosci 33: 9462–9473, 2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Lundy RF Jr, Norgren R. Activity in the hypothalamus, amygdala, and cortex generates bilateral and convergent modulation of pontine gustatory neurons. J Neurophysiol 91: 1143–1157, 2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Maier JX, Katz DB. Neural dynamics in response to binary taste mixtures. J Neurophysiol 109: 2108–2117, 2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Maier JX, Wachowiak M, Katz DB. Chemosensory convergence on primary olfactory cortex. J Neurosci 32: 17037–17047, 2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Monk KJ, Rubin BD, Keene JC, Katz DB. Licking microstructure reveals rapid attenuation of neophobia. Chem Senses 39: 203–213, 2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Mungarndee SS, Lundy RF Jr, Norgren R. Expression of Fos during sham sucrose intake in rats with central gustatory lesions. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 295: R751–R763, 2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Nishijo H, Norgren R. Responses from parabrachial gustatory neurons in behaving rats. J Neurophysiol 63: 707–724, 1990. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Nishijo H, Norgren R. Parabrachial gustatory neural activity during licking by rats. J Neurophysiol 66: 974–985, 1991. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Nishijo H, Norgren R. Parabrachial neural coding of taste stimuli in awake rats. J Neurophysiol 78: 2254–2268, 1997. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Norgren R. Gustatory afferents to ventral forebrain. Brain Res 81: 285–295, 1974. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Ottersen OP. Afferent connections to the amygdaloid complex of the rat with some observations in the cat. III. Afferents from the lower brain stem. J Comp Neurol 202: 335–356, 1981. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Page E. Continuous inspection schemes. Biometrika 41: 100–115, 1954. [Google Scholar]
  55. Paxinos G, Watson C. The Rat Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates. New York: Elsevier/Academic, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  56. Perez IO, Villavicencio M, Simon SA, Gutierrez R. Speed and accuracy of taste identification and palatability: impact of learning, reward expectancy, and consummatory licking. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 305: R252–R270, 2013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Perrotto RS, Scott TR. Gustatory neural coding in the pons. Brain Res 110: 283–300, 1976. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Phillips M, Norgren R. A rapid method for permanent implantation of an intraoral fistula in rats. Behav Res Methods Instrum 2: 124, 1970. [Google Scholar]
  59. Piette CE, Baez-Santiago MA, Reid EE, Katz DB, Moran A. Inactivation of basolateral amygdala specifically eliminates palatability-related information in cortical sensory responses. J Neurosci 32: 9981–9991, 2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Reilly S, Grigson PS, Norgren R. Parabrachial nucleus lesions and conditioned taste aversion: evidence supporting an associative deficit. Behav Neurosci 107: 1005–1017, 1993. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Roussin AT, D'Agostino AE, Fooden AM, Victor JD, Di Lorenzo PM. Taste coding in the nucleus of the solitary tract of the awake, freely licking rat. J Neurosci 32: 10494–10506, 2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Sadacca BF, Rothwax JT, Katz DB. Sodium concentration coding gives way to evaluative coding in cortex and amygdala. J Neurosci 32: 9999–10011, 2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Sakai N, Yamamoto T. Role of the medial and lateral parabrachial nucleus in acquisition and retention of conditioned taste aversion in rats. Behav Brain Res 93: 63–70, 1998. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Samuelsen CL, Gardner MP, Fontanini A. Effects of cue-triggered expectation on cortical processing of taste. Neuron 74: 410–422, 2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Saper CB. Reciprocal parabrachial-cortical connections in the rat. Brain Res 242: 33–40, 1982. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Scott K. The sweet and the bitter of mammalian taste. Curr Opin Neurobiol 14: 423–427, 2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Scott TR, Giza BK, Yan J. Gustatory neural coding in the cortex of the alert cynomolgus macaque: the quality of bitterness. J Neurophysiol 81: 60–71, 1999. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Sewards TV. Dual separate pathways for sensory and hedonic aspects of taste. Brain Res Bull 62: 271–283, 2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Smith DV, Li CS, Davis BJ. Excitatory and inhibitory modulation of taste responses in the hamster brainstem. Ann NY Acad Sci 855: 450–456, 1998. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Spector AC. Gustatory function in the parabrachial nuclei: implications from lesion studies in rats. Rev Neurosci 6: 143–175, 1995. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Spector AC, Breslin P, Grill HJ. Taste reactivity as a dependent measure of the rapid formation of conditioned taste aversion: a tool for the neural analysis of taste-visceral associations. Behav Neurosci 102: 942–952, 1988. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Stapleton JR, Lavine ML, Wolpert RL, Nicolelis MA, Simon SA. Rapid taste responses in the gustatory cortex during licking. J Neurosci 26: 4126–4138, 2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Stratford JM, Curtis KS, Contreras RJ. Chorda tympani nerve transection alters linoleic acid taste discrimination by male and female rats. Physiol Behav 89: 311–319, 2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Suwabe T, Bradley RM. Characteristics of rostral solitary tract nucleus neurons with identified afferent connections that project to the parabrachial nucleus in rats. J Neurophysiol 102: 546–555, 2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Tokita K, Karadi Z, Shimura T, Yamamoto T. Centrifugal inputs modulate taste aversion learning associated parabrachial neuronal activities. J Neurophysiol 92: 265–279, 2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Tokita K, Shimura T, Nakamura S, Inoue T, Yamamoto T. Involvement of forebrain in parabrachial neuronal activation induced by aversively conditioned taste stimuli in the rat. Brain Res 1141: 188–196, 2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Touzani K, Taghzouti K, Velley L. Increase of the aversive value of taste stimuli following ibotenic acid lesion of the central amygdaloid nucleus in the rat. Behav Brain Res 88: 133–142, 1997. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Weinberger M, Mahant N, Hutchison WD, Lozano AM, Moro E, Hodaie M, Lang AE, Dostrovsky JO. Beta oscillatory activity in the subthalamic nucleus and its relation to dopaminergic response in Parkinson's disease. J Neurophysiol 96: 3248–3256, 2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Weiss MS, Victor JD, Di Lorenzo PM. Taste coding in the parabrachial nucleus of the pons in awake, freely licking rats and comparison with the nucleus of the solitary tract. J Neurophysiol 111: 1655–1670, 2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Williams JB, Murphy DM, Reynolds KE, Welch SJ, King MS. Demonstration of a bilateral projection from the rostral nucleus of the solitary tract to the medial parabrachial nucleus in rat. Brain Res 737: 231–237, 1996. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Yamamoto T, Fujimoto Y, Shimura T, Sakai N. Conditioned taste aversion in rats with excitotoxic brain lesions. Neurosci Res 22: 31–49, 1995. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Yamamoto T, Matsuo R, Kawamura Y. The pontine taste area in the rabbit. Neurosci Lett 16: 5–9, 1980. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Yamamoto T, Shimura T, Sakai N, Ozaki N. Representation of hedonics and quality of taste stimuli in the parabrachial nucleus of the rat. Physiol Behav 56: 1197–1202, 1994. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Yamamoto T, Takemura M, Inui T, Torii K, Maeda N, Ohmoto M, Matsumoto I, Abe K. Functional organization of the rodent parabrachial nucleus. Ann NY Acad Sci 1170: 378–382, 2009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Yamamoto T, Yuyama N, Kato T, Kawamura Y. Gustatory responses of cortical neurons in rats. I. Response characteristics. J Neurophysiol 51: 616–635, 1984. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Zhang C, Kang Y, Lundy RF. Terminal field specificity of forebrain efferent axons to the pontine parabrachial nucleus and medullary reticular formation. Brain Res 1368: 108–118, 2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Zhang Y, Hoon MA, Chandrashekar J, Mueller KL, Cook B, Wu D, Zuker CS, Ryba NJ. Coding of sweet, bitter, and umami tastes: different receptor cells sharing similar signaling pathways. Cell 112: 293–301, 2003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Zhao GQ, Zhang Y, Hoon MA, Chandrashekar J, Erlenbach I, Ryba NJ, Zuker CS. The receptors for mammalian sweet and umami taste. Cell 115: 255–266, 2003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Journal of Neurophysiology are provided here courtesy of American Physiological Society

RESOURCES