Abstract
With the Psychological Capital Questionnaire (PCQ) being the standard measure to assess psychological capital (PsyCap) in the context of organizations, this paper aims to broaden this domain-specific approach by introducing a measure with universal claim. Two studies were conducted to create and validate a German self-report scale (CPC-12) measuring PsyCap. We performed confirmatory factor analyses and correlations with other positive psychological constructs on the data of two German samples (N1 = 321; N2 = 202). The twelve-item CPC-12 exhibits the anticipated factorial structure with a very good model fit and associations to other constructs concur with previous findings with other measures of PsyCap.
Introduction
Psychological capital (PsyCap) draws from the significant body of research that Seligman and Csikszentmihalyi [1] have initiated in the wake of the positive psychology movement. In shifting the focus of psychological research from human deficits like mental illnesses to human assets, strengths were scientifically studied and have been found to allow individuals, groups or even organizations to thrive and prosper [1]. Extending these findings to the work place, Luthans [2] identified psychological constructs (i.e. self-efficacy, hope, optimism, happiness, and resilience), which met the criteria of being positive, based on theory and research, and state-like open to development, change and management for performance improvement. These five constructs were labeled positive organizational behavior (POB) [2]. Luthans and Youssef [3] bundled four of these states (hope, optimism, resilience, and self-efficacy) into the higher-order construct “positive psychological capital (PsyCap)”. This construct was developed to provide practitioners with a new framework to ensure a sustainable competitive advantage via human resources [3].
PsyCap as a whole is defined as “a core psychological factor of positivity in general, and [positive organizational behavior] criteria meeting states in particular, that go beyond human and social capital to gain a competitive advantage through investment/development of ‘who you are’”[4], its parts as follows: (a) Hope refers to an individual’s motivation to succeed at a specific task in a set context and the way or means by which that task may be accomplished [5]. (b) Optimism refers to an individual’s expectancy of positive outcomes [6]. (c) Resilience refers to the ability of an individual to bounce back from adversity, uncertainty, risk or failure, and adapt to changing and stressful life demands [7, 8]. (d) Self-efficacy refers to an individual’s confidence in their ability to mobilize their motivation, cognitive resources and courses of action to achieve high levels of performance [9].
PsyCap can be distinguished from other forms of people-related capital, specifically human (an individual’s stock of knowledge, skills and abilities that can be increased by experience and/or investment in education and training) [10] and social capital (the aggregate of the actual or potential resources that are connected to the possession of a durable network of relationships) [3, 11, 12]. It influences a variety of outcomes at the individual level of particular importance for organizations and even beyond the work place [13]. Previous research however focused heavily on a domain-specific measure settled in the context of work. PsyCap is shown to be associated with desirable employee attitudes, such as staying intentions [13], job satisfaction and commitment [14, 15]. Employees high in PsyCap are found to be more empowered, which subsequently leads to less turnover intentions [16], and the reduction of absenteeism [17]. Furthermore, individuals high in PsyCap perform better than those low in PsyCap since they can draw upon more resources to pursue goals [18, 19]. Most importantly, PsyCap is shown to be developable through training interventions [15], which makes it a useful and tangible construct actually able to influence individuals and even whole organizations in a positive way.
Besides these work place-specific benefits, studies found evidence linking PsyCap to an improved psychological and physical well-being by the reduction of stress [20]. Because of the reciprocal relationship between job satisfaction and life satisfaction [21], PsyCap enhances the latter. Due to the fact that PsyCap consists of more general constructs (hope, optimism, self-efficacy and resilience) the question at hand is—Is PsyCap merely a domain-specific construct with effects solely in work-related areas or is it a much broader construct influencing many possible areas of life?
Measuring Psychological Capital
The Psychological Capital Questionnaire (PCQ) [3] is widely recognized as the standard scale measuring PsyCap [22]. It was developed as a compound measure consisting of (modified) items from published scales for hope [23], optimism [24], resilience [25], and self-efficacy [26]. Predominantly, the PCQ was used in employee, manager and student samples [22], and its items are closely tied to the work place (i.e. “I feel confident contributing to discussions about the company's strategy.”). One can thus state that the PCQ is a domain-specific measure.
Since PsyCap is shown to be linked to outcomes of general importance for individuals [20], this study aims to design and validate a universal measure for the construct. Such a non-domain-specific measure could expedite research on PsyCap for constructs in other domains, i.e. sports and education. Therefore, we conducted two different studies. Study 1 drew from the item pool of published and proven measures for the four different PsyCap components to create a compound measure, which is deployable in a wide range of applications (including the work place). For testing convergent and discriminant validity, we additionally surveyed the existing PCQ and a measure for occupational self-efficacy. We hypothesized a strong positive association between the PCQ and our newly created measure (Compound Psychological Capital Scale– 12; CPC-12), a higher positive correlation between general self-efficacy and the CPC-12 compared to the PCQ and a lower positive one between occupational self-efficacy and the CPC-12 compared to the PCQ due to the domain specific traits of the PCQ. In study 2, we re-tested the factorial structure of the CPC-12. Furthermore, to test the external validity of the CPC-12, we also selected several important positive psychological constructs—affect, job satisfaction, satisfaction with life, subjective well-being, perceived social support, meaning of work, engagement, gratitude and personality. The specific hypotheses regarding the associations between the CPC-12 and these constructs are discussed below.
Positive affect (PA) and negative affect (NA)
The relationship between PA and PsyCap becomes obvious upon reviewing the literature relating PA to three major components of PsyCap: resiliency, self-efficacy and optimism. These studies found that people high in PA show more effective problem resolving skills, more mature coping efforts, experience less conflict at the office [27] and furthermore that positive emotions enhance resilience in the face of adversity [28]. People who show more PA are also more optimistic and more likely to maintain a positive outlook during times of adversity [27]. Additionally, they are found to be high in personal competence and self-esteem and report higher self-efficacy [27, 29, 30]. Little, Gooty [31] also reported significant positive correlations between all four components of PsyCap and PA (r = .28−.68).
There is evidently a remarkable overlap between PA and PsyCap. One study even found that the predictive power of PsyCap on work performance, motivation and job satisfaction becomes insignificant once one accounts for PA [31]. Nonetheless the same study also pointed out that none of the PA-items loaded with the PsyCap constructs, meaning that besides the overlap, they are still clearly distinct constructs. For these reasons we expect a strong correlation between CPC-12 and PA. Lyubomirsky, King [27] state that NA and PA “regularly show moderate inverse relations across individuals, justifying the use of such negative states as the inverse of PA” (p. 822), thus we expect a moderate or high negative correlation between NA and PsyCap. This makes sense if you bear in mind the negative effects that PsyCap has on states like stress and anxiety [20].
Job satisfaction
There is a clear relationship between job satisfaction and PsyCap. Studies indicate that people high in PsyCap also report higher job satisfaction [12, 13, 19]. Luthans, Avolio [19] report a positive correlation of r = .39, whereas the meta-analysis of Avey, Reichard [13] reports an even higher correlation of r = .50−.57. One explanation for this relationship is given by Avey, Reichard [13] who state: “Given the general expectancy of success derived from optimism and the belief in personal abilities derived from efficacy, those high in PsyCap report being more satisfied with their job.” (p. 132). In addition Luthans, Avolio [19] declare that employees who are hopeful and efficacious are more satisfied with their jobs due to better performance. They are confident to persist, accept challenges and put effort into achieving their goals (efficacy). Furthermore they identify subgoals and pathways to achieve them and are able to foresee and overcome obstacles by pursuing a variety of pathways (hope). We therefore expect the CPC-12 to positively correlate with job satisfaction in about the same range as stated above.
Satisfaction with life
Previous studies on PsyCap concentrated on work-related outcomes, i.e. job satisfaction [12]. Nonetheless work and non-work life influence each other [32] and there is a positive correlation between job satisfaction and life satisfaction [33]. Ford, Heinen [32] name time-based pressure as one major reason for this mutual interaction. If you have to work extra hours in the office you will have less time to enjoy time with family or friends, which can leave you unsatisfied and vice versa. Newman, Ucbasaran [12] also stated in their meta-analysis that besides predicting higher levels of work-family conflict, low PsyCap predicts less meaning of life, things that should result in a decrease in life satisfaction. Lastly it has been reported that life satisfaction is positively related to optimism and self-esteem [34], posing another connection to PsyCap. For these reasons, we expect a moderate to high positive correlation between the CPC-12 and life satisfaction.
Subjective well-being
Since well-being is measured in many studies as a compound construct consisting of positive and negative affect, life satisfaction and job satisfaction [35, 36], PsyCap, as expected, positively relates to well-being [22, 37–39]. Avey, Luthans [38] furthermore show that PsyCap can lead to psychological well-being over time. We therefore expect a high positive correlation between subjective well-being and the CPC-12.
Perceived Social Support
In their study “Very Happy People” Diener and Seligman stated that satisfying social relationships are central to human happiness, so much so that happiness cannot occur without them [40]. Findings from Karademas [41] support those claims as he reports a direct positive relationship between social support and life satisfaction as well as an indirect one through optimism. Optimistic people seem to positively appraise future events. To maintain such beliefs optimists rely on a “positive evaluation of the social context and its ‘ability’ to provide the necessary support” [41]. Social support has furthermore not only been found to increase optimism [41, 42] but also to be associated with self-esteem [42]. Positive correlations between social support and optimism have been found to be moderate (r = .29−.41), similar to the moderate positive ones with self-esteem (r = .44) [41, 42]. High levels of social support have also been found to reduce mortality and to result in positive health outcomes, due to social support functioning as an “exceptionally important stress resilience factor” [43]. We expect a small to moderate positive correlation between perceived social support and the CPC-12 situated at the lower edge of the range stated above.
Meaning of Work
When looking at meaning, its importance not only in the field of work, but for life in general [44], becomes clear. It is not just a positive influence on organizational performance or employee engagement [45]. Having meaning in life is also of great importance for living a “full life” [44], which is very desirable for many. Again there is a positive relationship with PsyCap as Coutu [46] reported a strong relationship between meaning-making and resilience. She stated that one of the things distinguishing resilient people from less resilient people is their ability to create significance and meaning in their hardships and their lives in general. In addition, she reported the effective use of constructing meaning in resilience trainings for business people. Resilient people are more likely to see themselves not as victims in adversity, but rather to recognize the lesson and learning opportunity in their hardship. They are therefore able to create meaning in difficult situations in life and at work [46]. The fact that a sense of self-worth and efficacy are found to be two important pillars in the search for meaning [47], and the finding that lower levels of PsyCap seem to result in lower levels of meaning of life [12] underline the positive relationship between meaning of work and PsyCap. We expect a moderate positive correlation between the CPC-12 and meaning of work.
Engagement
Engagement is characterized by vigor, dedication and absorption [48]. Vigor is a very similar construct to resilience as Bakker, Schaufeli [48] describe vigorous people as mentally resilient, willing to put great effort into their work and to show persistence in the face of adversity. Resilience is not the only personal resource that has been found to reciprocally influence work engagement [48, 49]. Self-efficacy as well as optimism have been found to be equally connected to engagement [50, 51]. These personal resources show moderate positive correlations with work engagement (r = .29−.54) [50, 51]. Xanthopoulou, Bakker [51] explain the relationship by suggesting that people high in self-efficacy, optimism and resilience are “confident about their capabilities and optimistic about their future, and thus may identify or even create more aspects of their environment that facilitate goal attainment. This capability leads to goal confrontation and consequently to work engagement” (p. 137). It also has been explicitly stated that people high in PsyCap are more committed to their jobs [13] as the organizations they work for fulfill their needs for accomplishment and efficacy, thus leading to them being more “likely to embed themselves and be enthusiastic about their work (engagement)” (p. 132). We therefore expect a solid moderate positive correlation between engagement and the CPC-12 in about a similar range as stated above.
Gratitude
The close relationship of gratitude and PsyCap was demonstrated in a study by Luthans, Youssef [52], in which they discussed a range of possible constructs to be included into PsyCap. They regard gratitude as a promising aspirant for inclusion describing it as “the extra mile willingly traveled by those with high PsyCap” [52]. They find that gratitude prevents people from having negative labels and thoughts about their fellow men, thus decreasing the positivity in those relationships, which would in turn lead to a lower level of PsyCap. They state that being grateful helps us to maintain a positive outlook on life and positively reinforce each other. Maintaining a positive view on life resembles parts of optimism and hope.This is also reflected in other studies, which found positive relationships between gratitude and optimism [53–55], hope [54] and life satisfaction and happiness [55]. McCullough, Emmons [54] report moderate positive correlations between gratitude and optimism (r = .28−.58) as well as for gratitude and the two factors of hope (r = .18−.67). Considering the connection of gratitude to some of the PsyCap components and its actual consideration as a component itself, we expect a moderate positive correlation of gratitude with the CPC-12.
Personality
A proactive personality is desirable at an individual level as it predicts life satisfaction [56], but also from an organization’s perspective as it is for example positively related to job performance [57]. It means for a person to have an enhanced ability and desire to control the surrounding environment in an active, self-determined way. These control tendencies facilitate effective coping with occupational stressors [56], thus showing similarities to resilience. Optimism also seems closely related to a proactive attitude. Schmitz and Schwarzer [58] identify optimistic expectancy as the quintessence of the construct and describe proactive people as considering life to be full of opportunities. They furthermore associate proactive attitude with Bandura’s self-efficacy, deeming the two to be very similar constructs [58]. We therefore expect a moderate to high positive correlation between proactive attitude and the CPC-12.
The “Big Five” personality traits extraversion (r = .36) and conscientiousness (r = .39) reportedly show the strongest relationship with PsyCap. The correlation of agreeableness with PsyCap (r = .06) is unremarkable, whereas openness (r = -.1) and neuroticism (r = -.12) show marginal negative correlations [19, 22]. The positive correlation with extraversion seems logical as it has also been found to be positively related to positive affect, life satisfaction and happiness [27].
Looking at some of the PsyCap components respectively, one study testing the relationship of resilience with personality traits found a strong positive correlation with extraversion (r = .61) and conscientiousness (r = .45) as well as a strong negative correlation with neuroticism (r = -.65). The correlation with agreeableness was unremarkable, but there was a small positive correlation with openness (r = .20) [59]. The strong negative relationship with neuroticism measures up to one’s expectations. Neurotic people are vulnerable to emotional distress and susceptible to negative emotions and poor coping [60]. Campbell-Sills, Cohan [59] explain the strong positive relationships to extraversion as likely reflecting “the benefits of positive affective style, capacity for interpersonal closeness, and high levels of social interaction and activity” (p. 594). They furthermore explicate that the positive relationship with resilience can be fully explained by the tendency of conscientious people to use task-oriented coping. Taking all these findings together we expect a similar pattern of correlations between the “Big Five” and the CPC-12, moderate positive correlations with extraversion and conscientiousness, a moderate negative correlation with neuroticism, a small correlation with openness and no correlation with agreeableness.
Study 1—Methods
Participants and Procedure
Study 1 consisted of a total of 334 participants. Thirteen people were excluded from the analysis (six due to implausible or missing job description e.g. “xxx”, seven due to implausible tenure). The remaining 321 participants averaged 34.89 years (SD = 12.78), 60% were women and 76.6% were employees, 8.4% were self-employed and 13.7% temporary workers. Participants worked on average 33.79 hours a week (SD = 13.39) and had been employed for half a month to 43 years (Memployment = 7.91 years, SD = 9.72). 48% of the participants were in possession of a university degree and another 25% graduated with the general qualification for university entrance. Participants were recruited by publishing the link to the survey in several online social media groups. The survey was conducted in German. All participants were volunteers, no compensation was supplied.
Materials
All scales were surveyed using a 6-point response format ranging from 1 = “strongly disagree” to 6 = “strongly agree” in order to minimize potential effects of different response formats on the new scale.
Hope
Hope was evaluated using the six item short version of the State Hope Scale (SHS) [23]. The German version of this scale was derived by translating the original version of the State Hope Scale [23] into German (including a retranslation for verification purposes) according to the guidelines for cross-cultural adaptation proposed by Guillemin, Bombardier [61].
Optimism
Optimism was evaluated using two different scales. (a) The affective valence of the orientation towards the future-questionnaire (Affektive Valenz der Zukunftsorientierung, AFF) [62] with five items including one inverted item. Cronbach’s α of the AFF was .82. (b) The revised German version of the Life Orientation Test (LOT-R) [63] with ten items (four filler items and three items respectively tapping the subscales optimism and pessimism; [64].
Resilience
Resilience was assessed using the German thirteen item short version of the Resilience Scale (RS-13) [65].
Self-efficacy
Self-efficacy was evaluated using two different measures. (a) The German General Self Efficacy Scale (GSE) [66] with ten items. (b) The German ten item short version of the Occupational Self-Efficacy Scale (OSE) [67].
Psychological capital
PsyCap was surveyed using the German version of the Psychological Capital Questionnaire [19] with 24 items. Six items each measured the four subscales hope, optimism, resilience and self-efficacy.
Construction of Compound-Psychological-Capital-Scale
The five selected scales (SHS, AFF, LOT-R, RS-13, and GSE) constitute the pool of items from which the Compound-Psychological-Capital questionnaire (CPC-12) was developed. All four constructs (i.e., hope, resilience, optimism, and self-efficacy) should have equal weight; hence, the best three items of each construct in terms of content and face validity were taken into account. Furthermore, following the concept of rational construction [68], only those items were included which met our claim of universality and are therefore not solely relevant to the workplace. All scales for the item pool were included in this study in their full length to be able to control their correlations comparing PCQ and CPC-12.
Data analysis
The fit of all tested structural equation models was examined using the criteria proposed by Hu and Bentler [69]. Beyond χ² significance testing these criteria comprise a standardized root-mean-square residual (SRMR) ≤ 0.08 in combination with at least one of the following fit indices: a root-mean-square error of approximation (RMSEA) ≤ 0.06, a lower bound of the 90% confidence interval of the RMSEA ≤ 0.06, a comparative fit index (CFI) ≥ 0.95, or a Tucker-Lewis-index (TLI) ≥ 0.95. The Satorra-Bentler adjusted χ² was calculated to adjust for non-normal distributions of the variables [70]. The confirmatory factor analyses were conducted using the “lavaan” package [71] of R statistical software [72]. Due to forced choice in the standardized questionnaires, there was no missing data.
Study 1—Results
Table 1 presents descriptive statistics, Cronbach’s α and the correlation matrix for the study variables. The strong positive relationship (r = .70, p < .001) between the PCQ and the newly created CPC-12 alludes to the measurement of a similar but not identical construct. As hypothesized the correlation between CPC-12 and general self-efficacy is higher compared to the PCQ, while the correlation between CPC-12 and occupational self-efficacy is lower compared to the PCQ.
Table 1. Descriptive statistics and inter-correlations for study 1.
M | SD | Hope | Optimism (AFF) | Optimism (LOT-R) | Resilience | General Self-Efficacy | Occupational Self-Efficacy | PsyCap (PCQ) | PsyCap (CPC-12) | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Hope | 4.25 | 1.04 | .84 | |||||||
Optimism (AFF) | 4.83 | 0.99 | .49*** | .82 | ||||||
Optimism (LOT-R) | 4.41 | 1.04 | .44*** | .58*** | .74 | |||||
Resilience | 4.64 | 0.91 | .56*** | .46*** | .41*** | .79 | ||||
General Self-Efficacy | 4.22 | 0.86 | .55*** | .42*** | .35*** | .70*** | .88 | |||
Occupational Self-Efficacy | 4.29 | 0.93 | .56*** | .47*** | .39*** | .56*** | .67*** | .85 | ||
PsyCap (PCQ) | 4.51 | 1.03 | .61*** | .56*** | .55*** | .59*** | .55*** | .77*** | .92 | |
PsyCap (CPC-12) | 4.44 | 0.99 | .79*** | .65*** | .53*** | .80*** | .82*** | .71*** | .70*** | .82 |
Notes: Cronbach’s α is displayed diagonal; AFF = Affektive Valenz der Zukunftsorientierung (Affective valence of the future orientation), LOT-R = Life Orientation Test—Revised, PsyCap = Psychological Capital, PCQ = Psychological Capital Questionnaire, CPC-12 = Compound Psychological Capital Scale.; p-scores:
* <.05,
** <.01,
*** <.001.
Table 2 exhibits measurement models for all selected scales, including the PCQ measure for PsyCap. To examine the expected factorial structure of the PCQ, we conducted a confirmatory factor analysis (CFA). We began by fitting this model with the six items for each facet (i.e., hope, resilience, optimism, and self-efficacy) and then fit each of the four dimensions to the higher-order PsyCap. The estimates of model fit (SRMR = .062, RMSEA = .061, CFI = .841) are not acceptable according to Hu and Bentler [69].
Table 2. Measurement models for study 1 using MLM estimator.
N factors | X² | df | p | SRMR | TLI | CFI | RMSEA | RMSEA 90%-CI | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
PsyCap (PCQ) | 4+g | 549.04 | 248 | <.001 | .062 | .823 | .841 | .061 | .055−.068 |
PsyCap (CPC-12) | 4+g | 77.727 | 50 | .007 | .046 | .950 | .962 | .042 | .024−.058 |
Notes: PsyCap = Psychological Capital, PCQ = Psychological Capital Questionnaire, CPC-12 = Compound Psychological Capital Scale.
To confirm the expected higher-order factor of PsyCap in the CPC-12, we conducted a CFA on the data analogous to the one above. We began by fitting this model with three items for each facet (i.e., hope, resilience, optimism, and self-efficacy) and then fit each of the four dimensions to the higher-order PsyCap. Results indicated the following estimates of model fit: SRMR = .046, RMSEA = .042, CFI = .962. The model seems to be a very good fit with all three indices meeting the cutoff criteria by Hu and Bentler [69].
Furthermore, each of the factor loadings was significant on their respective latent factor at p < .01 (Fig 1). The confirmatory factor analysis in Study 1 supports the proposed higher-order factor structure for the newly created PsyCap measure CPC-12.
Study 2—Methods
Participants and Procedure
Sample 2 consisted of a total of 202 participants (82.7% employees, 9.4% self-employed, and 7.9% temporary workers) between 18–72 years (Mage = 37.79, SD = 13.10). 72.3% were female. Participants worked on average 35.74 hours a week (SD = 11.18) and had been employed for one month to 45 years (Memployment = 9.37 years, SD = 9.74). 35.1% were in possession of a university degree and another 25.2% graduated with the general qualification for university entrance. Participants were recruited by publishing the link to the survey in several social media groups. The survey was conducted in German. All participants were volunteers, no compensation was supplied.
Materials
Psychological capital
PsyCap was measured with the CPC-12 (Study 1), using a 6-point response format ranging from 1 = “strongly disagree” to 6 = “strongly agree”.
Positive and negative affect
PA and NA were evaluated using the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule [73]. Participants responded with 20 items to the question asking how they felt “during the past two weeks” (1 = “very slightly or not at all” to 5 = “very much”). Ten items measured positive affect (e.g. excited, attentive) and ten items measured negative affect (e.g. guilty, afraid). The items were arranged randomly.
Job satisfaction
Job satisfaction was measured using three items [35, 74]. The first item measured general job satisfaction (“All things considered are you satisfied with your job?”), which participants were able to answer with “yes” or “no”. The second item (“How satisfied are you with your job in general?”) was rated using a 5-point scale from 1 = “very dissatisfied” to 5 = “very satisfied”. The third item asked participants to rate the percentage of time they feel satisfied, unsatisfied or neutral with their job in general (e.g. “The percent of time I feel satisfied with my present job.”). The analysis was conducted using the mean-score of the z-standardized items.
Satisfaction with life
Life satisfaction was measured using the Satisfaction with Life Scale [75], using a 5-point response format ranging from 1 = “strongly agree” to 5 = “strongly disagree”. Participants rated five given statements (e.g. “I am satisfied with my life.”).
Subjective well-being
The standardized z-scores of the four previously described scales were averaged to create subjective well-being. PA, NA and satisfaction with life were included because Arthaud-Day, Rode [76] found the three-factor model of subjective well-being consisting of PA, NA and life satisfaction to be superior to any other two- or one-factor model and the best fit to their data. Job satisfaction was then included to add a domain-specific focal point on work [35, 36] for the comparability to previous studies on well-being and PsyCap.
Perceived social support
A short version of the Perceived Support Questionnaire [77] was used to measure perceived social support. Using a 5-point response format ranging from 1 = “strongly disagree” to 5 = “strongly agree” participants had the possibility to rate to what extent the six given statements (e.g. “There is someone very close to me whose help I can always count on.”) fit their own lives.
Meaning of work
The Work and Meaning Inventory [78] was used to measure meaning of work. Using a 5-point response format ranging from 1 = “absolutely untrue” to 5 = “absolutely true” participants rated to what extent the ten given statements (e.g. “My work helps me make sense of the world around me.”) applied to them.
Engagement
To measure engagement the Utrecht Work Engagement Scale [79] was used. Given a 7-point Likert scale ranging from 1 = “never” to 7 = “always” participants were asked to rate nine presented statements (e.g. “I am immersed in my job.”).
Gratitude
Gratitude was evaluated using the Gratitude-Questionnaire [54]. Given a 7-point Likert scale ranging from 1 = “strongly disagree” to 7 = “strongly agree” participants were asked to rate to what extent the six presented statements (e.g. “I have so much in life to be thankful for.”) applied to them.
Proactive attitude
The Proactive Attitude Scale [58] was used to measure proactive attitude. Using a 4-point response format ranging from 1 = “not at all true” to 4 = “exactly true” participants rated to what extent the eight given statements (e.g. “I can choose my own actions.”) applied to them.
“Big Five”
The five personality traits extraversion, neuroticism, conscientiousness, agreeableness and openness were assessed using the Big Five Inventory (BFI-S) [80]. Participants rated 15 statements (e.g. “I see myself as someone who is outgoing, sociable.”) on a 7-point Likert scale from 1 = “does not apply to me at all” to 7 = “applies to me perfectly”.
Data Analysis
The fit of all tested structural equation models was examined using the same criteria as presented in study 1 [69]. According to these indices the model for subjective well-being, which consisted of four independent constructs, showed an acceptable fit when tested for the unidimensional character of the compound variable using CFA (Satorra-Bentler-χ ² (2, 202) = 4.172, p <.125, CFI = .957, SRMR = .035, RMSEA = .073, CIRMSEA = .00−.15).
The data analysis was run using the statistical software R [72]. The confirmatory factor analyses were conducted using the “lavaan” package [71], other used packages were “Hmisc” and “pastecs” [81].
We used multiple imputation methods [82] to impute the three missing item responses prior to the statistical analysis. This maximizes power [83] and produces accurate parameter estimates [84].
Study 2—Results
Results of the CFA for the CPC-12 indicated the following estimates of model fit: Satorra-Bentler-χ ² (50, 202) = 72.32, p <.021, CFI = .955, SRMR = .052, RMSEA = .047, CIRMSEA = .022−.068. All the indices can be deemed to be a good model fit according to Hu and Bentler [69].
Table 3 presents descriptive statistics, Cronbach’s α and bivariate correlations for the variables of study 2. All correlations are according to our hypotheses. Subjective well-being (r = .58), proactive attitude (r = .57) and positive affect (r = .54) showed the highest positive correlations, agreeableness showed no substantive correlation with the CPC-12, neuroticism and negative affect showed negative correlations.
Table 3. Descriptive statistics and inter-correlations for study 2.
M | SD | PsyCap | SWBa | PA | NAb | JSa | LS | PSS | MoW | Eng | Grat | ProA | Con | Extr | Neur | Open | Agree | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
PsyCap | 4.54 | 0.50 | .81 | |||||||||||||||
SWBa | 0a | 0.68a | .58*** | - | ||||||||||||||
PA | 3.40 | 0.60 | .54*** | .64*** | .86 | |||||||||||||
NA | 4.22 | 0.62 | -.25*** | -.68*** | -.16* | .64 | ||||||||||||
JSa | 0a | 0.81a | .40*** | .70*** | .30*** | -.33*** | .72 | |||||||||||
LS | 3.74 | 0.63 | .39*** | .71*** | .29*** | -.38*** | .27*** | .74 | ||||||||||
PSS | 4.42 | 1.3 | .22** | .41*** | .14 | -.35*** | .18* | .47*** | .87 | |||||||||
MoW | 3.64 | 0.76 | .28*** | .50*** | .38*** | -.25*** | .43*** | .30*** | .16* | .91 | ||||||||
Eng | 4.77 | 0.65 | .39*** | .60*** | .51*** | -.24*** | .52*** | .38*** | .21** | .70*** | .95 | |||||||
Grat | 5.88 | 0.8 | .27*** | .43*** | .26*** | -.23*** | .22** | .46*** | .45*** | .30*** | .33*** | .68 | ||||||
ProA | 3.07 | 0.39 | .57*** | .54*** | .39*** | -.35*** | .30*** | .46*** | .48*** | .41*** | .48*** | .48*** | .67 | |||||
Con | 5.48 | 0.89 | .29*** | .34*** | .29*** | -.27*** | .17* | .19** | .17* | .24*** | .37*** | .16* | .35*** | .55 | ||||
Extr | 5.12 | 1.11 | .24*** | .22** | .18* | -.20* | .05 | .17* | .30*** | .22** | .31*** | .25*** | .34*** | .18* | .70 | |||
Neur | 3.96 | 1.21 | -.49*** | -.35*** | -.20** | .34*** | -.17* | -.24*** | -.20** | -.17* | -.23*** | -.16* | -.37*** | -.12 | -.19** | .70 | ||
Open | 5.19 | 1.04 | .20** | .21** | .24*** | -.13 | .10 | .10 | .18** | .32*** | .33*** | .25*** | .30*** | .19** | .37*** | .-08 | .59 | |
Agree | 5.34 | 0.88 | .04 | .14* | .02 | -.13 | .07 | .18** | .23** | .17* | .17* | .24*** | .19** | .18* | .02 | .06 | .11 | .37 |
Notes: Cronbach’s αs are displayed diagonal; CPC-12 = Compound Psychological Capital Scale, SWBa = aubjective well-being, PA = positive affect, NA = negative affect, JSa = job satisfaction, LS = satisfaction with life, PSS = perceived social support, MoW = meaning of work, Eng = engagement, Grat = gratitude, ProA = proactive attitude, Con = conscientiousness, Extr = extraversion, Neur = meuroticism, Open = openness, Agree = agreeableness; p-scores:
* <.05,
** <.01,
*** <.001.
a standardized z-scores
Discussion
Since its emergence in 2004, the construct of PsyCap is assessed using the PCQ as the standard measure in more than 14 countries and languages, along with slight alterations of the scale to match the target group’s needs [85]. The domain-specific measure itself is tied to the working world, although studies indicate associations with psychological constructs important for students, the unemployed, and retirees alike [20]. Therefore, the aim of this study was to develop and validate a German compound measure for PsyCap, with the general claim of being applicable to all domains of life.
According to our results, the CPC-12 fits the proposed model of PsyCap very well. The four subscales hope, optimism, resilience, and self-efficacy are identifiable as subcomponents of the overall measure while the higher-order factor can incrementally explain additional variance in the data. The moderate to high correlations to other work-related (MOW, job satisfaction and engagement; r = .28−.40) and more general constructs of positive psychology (i.e. subjective well-being, proactive attitude, and gratitude; r = .22.—.58) are comparable to previous research on PsyCap and speak for the external validity of the CPC-12. The results indicate that PsyCap could in fact be a general construct and applicable to more areas than POB. By abstaining from items with work related connotations, the CPC-12 can be applied to many fields of interest, i.e. sports and education. The CPC-12 is also an alternative in work-related research for areas where the item wordings of the PCQ might not be suitable, i.e. volunteering or small organizations, since its correlations with work-related constructs are similar to the PCQ. It is important to notice that while the CPC-12 is a short and economic way to measure PsyCap, we recommend using the original scales we used for the item pool if a specific sub-facet is the key area of interest.
Future Directions
To achieve these desirable outcomes, future research should address the implementation of interventions that increase PsyCap. Although PsyCap is open to development and has already been proven to be alterable through interventions [5, 86], organizations to date still fail to increase their efforts to enhance this underemphasized positive core construct. We go one step further and propose to not only implement PsyCap interventions at the work place, but at schools, universities, or even in families. These interventions can be carried out on a group-level or as tailored training interventions based on individual scores. With the CPC-12 we provide a reliable tool to measure those individual PsyCap scores in many domains of life. In order to facilitate a reasonable interpretation of the scores, the quality criterion of standardization of the CPC-12 will have to be addressed to create up-to-date norms and to define the population to which they apply. The generalizability of the CPC-12 should be re-evaluated using different samples in other life-domains.
All in all, PsyCap seems to be multifarious and connected to a wide variety of other positive psychological constructs. High correlations with well-being, life satisfaction or job satisfaction make a case for PsyCap to become a focus of prospective research in positive psychology. We believe that people high in PsyCap are more likely to lead flourishing lives and probably even more likely to build a flourishing society.
Limitations
The following limitations should be kept in mind when interpreting the results. The high correlations between the CPC-12 and the scales used to create the CPC-12 in study 1 are in part due to common items in these scales and should not be interpreted without this context.
All participants were recruited online, which entails that findings may not generalize to people not using the internet or social networks. Although Gosling, Vazire, Srivastava and John [87] agree that data from the internet is not free from methodological constraints, they do emphasize that samples using online recruitment are as diverse, adjusted, at least as good in quality as most traditional methods and “not as flawed as is commonly believed” (p. 102). The use of a nonprobability sample in this study raises further concerns about generalizability.
In addition, the scales for gratitude (α = .68) and proactive attitude (α = .67) lack reliability (smaller than .70). Results should therefore be considered with caution. The same caution should apply to the five personality traits as they also lack reliability. We found low merits for conscientiousness (α = .55), openness (α = .59) and agreeableness (α = .37), the latter being extremely low.
Ethics Statement
This study is in accordance with the APA ethical principles regarding research with human participants. This study does not involve any conflict of ethics, since no clinical intervention was performed. Neither were blood or tissue samples taken for study purposes.
Participants were informed before participating that their responses would be treated confidentially and anonymously and that all data would be analyzed in a generalized manner so that no conclusions could be drawn about individual persons. The participants were informed that they would give their consent by proceeding past the welcome page of the online survey. This procedure is in accordance with the Freie Universität Berlin ethics committee’s guidelines. There was no contact between researchers and participants. Participation in this study was voluntary. This study was approved by the ethics committee of Freie Universität Berlin ID 102/2015.
Supporting Information
Data Availability
All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files.
Funding Statement
The authors have no support or funding to report.
References
- 1.Seligman ME, Csikszentmihalyi M. Positive psychology: An introduction. Am Psychol. 2000;55: 5–14. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Luthans F. Positive organizational behavior: Developing and managing psychological strengths. Acad Manage Exec. 2002;16: 57–72. [Google Scholar]
- 3.Luthans F, Youssef CM. Human, Social, and now Positive Psychological Capital Management: Investing in people for competitive advantage. Organ Dyn. 2004;33: 143–160. [Google Scholar]
- 4.Luthans F, Avolio BJ, Walumbwa FO, Li W. The psychological capital of Chinese workers: Exploring the relationship with performance. Manage Organ Rev. 2005;1: 249–271. [Google Scholar]
- 5.Luthans F, Norman SM, Avolio BJ, Avey JB. The mediating role of psychological capital in the supportive organizational climate—employee performance relationship. J Organ Behav. 2008;29: 219–238. [Google Scholar]
- 6.Scheier MF, Carver CS, Bridges MW. Optimism, pessimism, and psychological well-being In: Chang EC, editor. Optimism & pessimism: Implications for theory, research, and practice. Washington, DC, US: American Psychological Association; 2001. p. 189–216. [Google Scholar]
- 7.Masten A, Reed M. Resilience in development In: Snyder CR, López SJ, editors. Handbook of positive psychology. New York: Oxford University Press; 2002. p. 74–88. [Google Scholar]
- 8.Tugade MM, Fredrickson BL. Resilient individuals use positive emotions to bounce back from negative emotional experiences. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2004;86: 320–333. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Stajkovic AD, Luthans F. Self-efficacy and work-related performance: A meta-analysis. Psychol Bull. 1998;124: 240–261. [Google Scholar]
- 10.Becker G. Human Capital: A theoretical and empirical approach with special references to education. 3rd ed Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 1993. [Google Scholar]
- 11.Bourdieu P. The forms of capital (1986) In: Szeman I, Kaposy T, editors. Cultural theory: An anthology. Oxford: John Wiley & Sons; 2011. p. 81–93. [Google Scholar]
- 12.Newman A, Ucbasaran D, Zhu F, Hirst G. Psychological capital: A review and synthesis. J Organ Behav. 2014;35: 120–138. [Google Scholar]
- 13.Avey JB, Reichard RJ, Luthans F, Mhatre KH. Meta‐analysis of the impact of positive psychological capital on employee attitudes, behaviors, and performance. Human resource development quarterly. 2011;22: 127–152. [Google Scholar]
- 14.Larson M, Luthans F. Potential added value of psychological capital in predicting work attitudes. Journal of Leadership & Organizational Studies. 2006;13: 75–92. [Google Scholar]
- 15.Luthans F, Avey JB, Patera JL. Experimental analysis of a web-based training intervention to develop positive psychological capital. Acad Manag Learn & Edu. 2008;7: 209–221. [Google Scholar]
- 16.Avey JB, Hughes LW, Norman SM, Luthans KW. Using positivity, transformational leadership and empowerment to combat employee negativity. Leadership & Organization Development Journal. 2008;29: 110–126. [Google Scholar]
- 17.Avey JB, Patera JL, West BJ. The implications of positive psychological capital on employee absenteeism. Journal of Leadership & Organizational Studies. 2006;13: 42–60. [Google Scholar]
- 18.Hobfoll SE. Social and psychological resources and adaptation. Rev Gen Psychol. 2002;6: 307–324. [Google Scholar]
- 19.Luthans F, Avolio BJ, Avey JB, Norman SM. Positive psychological capital: Measurement and relationship with performance and satisfaction. Pers Psychol. 2007;60: 541–572. [Google Scholar]
- 20.Baron RA, Franklin RJ, Hmieleski KM. Why entrepreneurs often experience low, not high, levels of stress: The joint effects of selection and Psychological Capital. J Manage. 2016;42: 742–768. 10.1177/0149206313495411 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Judge TA, Watanabe S. Another look at the job satisfaction-life satisfaction relationship. J appl psychol. 1993;78: 939–948. [Google Scholar]
- 22.Dawkins S, Martin A, Scott J, Sanderson K. Building on the positives: A psychometric review and critical analysis of the construct of psychological capital. J Occup Organ Psych. 2013;86: 348–370. [Google Scholar]
- 23.Snyder CR, Sympson SC, Ybasco FC, Borders TF, Babyak MA, Higgins RL. Development and validation of the State Hope Scale. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1996;70: 321–335. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Scheier MF, Carver CS. Optimism, coping, and health: Assessment and implications of generalized outcome expectancies. Health Psychol. 1985;4: 219–247. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Wagnild GM, Young H. Development and psychometric evaluation of the resilience scale. J Nurs Meas. 1993;1: 165–178. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Parker SK. Enhancing role breadth self-efficacy: The roles of job enrichment and other organizational interventions. J Appl Psychol. 1998;83: 835–852. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Lyubomirsky S, King L, Diener E. The benefits of frequent positive affect: does happiness lead to success? Psychol bull. 2005;131: 803–855. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Tugade MM, Fredrickson BL, Feldman Barrett L. Psychological resilience and positive emotional granularity: Examining the benefits of positive emotions on coping and health. J Pers. 2004;72: 1161–1190. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Jundt D, Hinsz VB, editors. Are happier workers more productive workers? The impact of mood on self-set goals, self-efficacy, and task performance. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Midwestern Psychological Association, Chicago; 2001.
- 30.Lyubomirsky S, Tkach C, DiMatteo MR. What are the differences between happiness and self-esteem. Soc Indic Res. 2006;78: 363–404. [Google Scholar]
- 31.Little LM, Gooty J, Nelson DL. Positive psychological capital: Has positivity clouded measurement rigor In: Nelson DL, Cooper CL, editors. Positive organizational behavior: Accentuating the positives at work. London: SAGE Publicatios Ltd; 2007. p.191–209. [Google Scholar]
- 32.Ford MT, Heinen BA, Langkamer KL. Work and family satisfaction and conflict: a meta-analysis of cross-domain relations. J App Psychol. 2007;92: 57–80. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Rojas M. The complexity of well-being: A life-satisfaction conception and a domains-of-life approach. Researching well-being in developing countries: From theory to research. Paper presented at the international workshop on researching well-being in developing countries, Delmenhorst; 2004.
- 34.Lucas RE, Diener E, Suh E. Discriminant validity of well-being measures. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1996;71: 616–628. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Judge TA, Klinger R. Job satisfaction In: Eid M, Larsen RJ, editors. The Science of Subjective Well-Being. New York: The Guilford Press; 2008. p. 393–413. [Google Scholar]
- 36.Pavot W. The assessment of subjective well-being In: Eid M, Larsen RJ, editors. The Science of Subjective Well-Being. New York: The Guilford Press; 2008. p. 124–140. [Google Scholar]
- 37.Culbertson SS, Fullagar CJ, Mills MJ. Feeling good and doing great: The relationship between psychological capital and well-being. J occup health psych. 2010;1: 421–433. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Avey JB, Luthans F, Smith RM, Palmer NF. Impact of positive psychological capital on employee well-being over time. J occup health psych. 2010;15: 17–28. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Avey JB, Luthans F, Jensen SM. Psychological capital: A positive resource for combating employee stress and turnover. Hum Resour Manage. 2009;48: 677–693. [Google Scholar]
- 40.Diener E, Seligman ME. Very happy people. Psychol Sci. 2002;13: 81–84. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Karademas EC. Self-efficacy, social support and well-being: The mediating role of optimism. Pers Indiv Differ. 2006;40: 1281–1290. [Google Scholar]
- 42.Symister P, Friend R. The influence of social support and problematic support on optimism and depression in chronic illness: A prospective study evaluating self-esteem as a mediator. Health Psychol. 2003;22: 123–129. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Ozbay F, Fitterling H, Charney D, Southwick S. Social support and resilience to stress across the life span: A neurobiologic framework. Curr Psychiatry Rep. 2008;10: 304–310. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Seligman ME, Parks AC, Steen T. A balanced psychology and a full life. Philos Trans-R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2004: 1379–1382. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Cartwright S, Holmes N. The meaning of work: The challenge of regaining employee engagement and reducing cynicism. Hum Resour Manage R. 2006;16: 199–208. [Google Scholar]
- 46.Coutu DL. How resilience works. Harvard Bus Rev. 2002;80: 46–56. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Baumeister RF. Meanings of life. 1st ed New York: Guilford Press; 1991. [Google Scholar]
- 48.Bakker AB, Schaufeli WB, Leiter MP, Taris TW. Work engagement: An emerging concept in occupational health psychology. Work Stress. 2008;22: 187–200. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Bakker AB, Gierveld J, Van Rijswijk K. Succesfactoren bij vrouwelijke schoolleiders in het primair onderwijs: Een onderzoek naar burnout, bevlogenheid en prestaties [Success factors among female school principals in primary teaching: A study on burnout, work engagement, and performance]. Diemen, The Netherlands: Right Management Consultants; 2006. [Google Scholar]
- 50.Xanthopoulou D, Bakker AB, Demerouti E, Schaufeli WB. Reciprocal relationships between job resources, personal resources, and work engagement. J Vocat behav. 2009;74: 235–244. [Google Scholar]
- 51.Xanthopoulou D, Bakker AB, Demerouti E, Schaufeli WB. The role of personal resources in the job demands-resources model. Int J Stress Manage. 2007;14: 121–141. [Google Scholar]
- 52.Luthans F, Youssef CM, Avolio BJ. Psychological capital: Developing the human competitive edge. 1st ed New York: Oxford University Press; 2006. [Google Scholar]
- 53.Emmons RA, McCullough ME. Counting blessings versus burdens: An experimental investigation of gratitude and subjective well-being in daily life. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2003;84: 377–389. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.McCullough ME, Emmons RA, Tsang J-A. The grateful disposition: a conceptual and empirical topography. J Pers Soc psychol. 2002;82: 112–127. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Rash JA, Matsuba MK, Prkachin KM. Gratitude and well‐being: Who benefits the most from a gratitude intervention? Appl Psychol: Health Well‐Being. 2011;3: 350–369. [Google Scholar]
- 56.Cunningham CJ, De La Rosa GM. The interactive effects of proactive personality and work-family interference on well-being. J Occup Health Psych. 2008;13: 271–282. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Crant JM. The Proactive Personality Scale and objective job performance among real estate agents. J Appl Psychol. 1995;80: 532–537. [Google Scholar]
- 58.Schmitz GS, Schwarzer R. Proaktive Einstellung von Lehrern: Konstruktbeschreibung und psychometrische Analysen. Zeitschrift für Empirische Pädagogik. 1999;13: 3–27. [Google Scholar]
- 59.Campbell-Sills L, Cohan SL, Stein MB. Relationship of resilience to personality, coping, and psychiatric symptoms in young adults. Behav res ther. 2006;44: 585–599. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Costa P, McCrae RR. Revised NEO Personality Inventory and NEO Five-Factor Inventory: Professional manual. 1st ed Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. Inc; 1992. [Google Scholar]
- 61.Guillemin F, Bombardier C, Beaton D. Cross-cultural adaptation of health-related quality of life measures: Literature review and proposed guidelines. J Clinical Epidemiol. 1993;46: 1417–1432. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Brandtstädter J, Wentura D. Veränderungen der Zeit-und Zukunftsperspektive im Übergang zum höheren Erwachsenenalter: entwicklungspsychologische und differentielle Aspekte. Z Entwickl und Padagogis; 1994;26: 2–21. [Google Scholar]
- 63.Glaesmer H, Hoyer J, Klotsche J, Herzberg PY. Die deutsche version des Life-Orientation-Tests (LOT-R) zum dispositionellen Optimismus und Pessimismus. Z Gesundh. 2008;16: 26–31. [Google Scholar]
- 64.Herzberg PY, Glaesmer H, Hoyer J. Separating optimism and pessimism: A robust psychometric analysis of the revised Life Orientation Test (LOT-R). Psychol Assessment. 2006;18: 433–438. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Leppert K, Koch B, Brähler E, Strauß B. Die Resilienzskala (RS)–Überprüfung der Langform RS-25 und einer Kurzform RS-13. Klinische Diagnostik und Evaluation. 2008;1: 226–243. [Google Scholar]
- 66.Jerusalem M, Schwarzer R. Allgemeine Selbstwirksamkeit In: Schwarzer R, Jerusalem M, editors. Skalen zur Erfassung von Lehrer-und Schülermerkmalen. Berlin: Freie Universität Berlin; 1999. p. 13–14. [Google Scholar]
- 67.Schyns B, von Collani G. A new occupational self-efficacy scale and its relation to personality constructs and organizational variables. Eur J Work Organ Psy. 2002;11: 219–241. [Google Scholar]
- 68.Moosbrugger H, Kelava A. Testtheorie und Fragebogenkonstruktion. 2nd ed Heidelberg: Springer; 2007. [Google Scholar]
- 69.Hu Lt, Bentler PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct Equ Modeling. 1999;6: 1–55. [Google Scholar]
- 70.Finney SJ, DiStefano C. Non-normal and categorical data in structural equation modeling In: Hancock GR, Mueller RO, editors. Structural equation modeling: A second course. 2nd ed Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishing; 2013. p. 439–492. [Google Scholar]
- 71.Rosseel Y. lavaan: An R package for structural equation modeling. J Stat Softw. 2012;48: 1–36. [Google Scholar]
- 72.R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria; 2014. Available: http://www.R-project.org/. [Google Scholar]
- 73.Watson D, Clark LA, Tellegen A. Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1988;54: 1063–1070. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Judge TA, Boudreau JW, Bretz RD. Job and life attitudes of male executives. J Appl Psychol. 1994;79: 767–782. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Schumacher J. SWLS–Satisfaction with life scale In: Schumacher J, Klaiberg A, Braehler E, editors. Diagnostische Verfahren zu Lebensqualität und Wohlbefinden. Göttingen: Hogrefe; 2003. p. 305–309. [Google Scholar]
- 76.Arthaud-Day ML, Rode JC, Mooney CH, Near JP. The subjective well-being construct: A test of its convergent, discriminant, and factorial validity. Soc Indic Res. 2005;74: 445–476. [Google Scholar]
- 77.Kliem S, Mößle T, Rehbein F, Hellmann D, Zenger M, Brähler E. A brief form of the Perceived Social Support Questionnaire (F-SozU) was developed, validated and standardized. J Clin Epidemiol. 2014;68: 551–562. 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2014.11.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Steger MF, Dik BJ, Duffy RD. Measuring meaningful work: The work and meaning inventory (WAMI). J Career Assessment. 2012. February 19 10.1177/1069072711436160 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Balducci C, Fraccaroli F, Schaufeli WB. Psychometric properties of the Italian version of the Utrecht Work Engagement Scale (UWES-9). Eur J Psychol Assess. 2010;26: 143–149. [Google Scholar]
- 80.Gerlitz J-Y, Schupp J. Zur Erhebung der Big-Five-basierten persoenlichkeitsmerkmale im SOEP. DIW Research Notes 4; 2005.
- 81.Grosjean P, Ibanez F. Pastecs: Package for analysis of space-time ecological series. R package version 1.3–18; 2014.
- 82.Enders CK. Applied missing data analysis. New York: Guilford Publications; 2010. [Google Scholar]
- 83.Gottschall AC, West SG, Enders CK. A comparison of item-level and scale-level multiple imputation for questionnaire batteries. Multivar Behav Res. 2012;47: 1–25. [Google Scholar]
- 84.Enders CK. Dealing with missing data in developmental research. Child Dev Perspect. 2013;7: 27–31. [Google Scholar]
- 85.Luthans F, Youssef-Morgan CM, Avolio BJ. Psychological Capital and beyond. 1st ed New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2015. [Google Scholar]
- 86.Luthans F, Avey JB, Avolio BJ, Norman SM, Combs GM. Psychological capital development: Toward a micro‐intervention. J Organ Behav. 2006;27: 387–393. [Google Scholar]
- 87.Gosling SD, Vazire S, Srivastava S, John OP. Should we trust web-based studies? A comparative analysis of six preconceptions about internet questionnaires. Am Psychol. 2004;59: 93–104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files.