Summary
Human primary visual cortex (V1) has long been associated with learning simple low-level visual discriminations [1] and is classically considered outside of neural systems that support high-level cognitive behavior in contexts that differ from the original conditions of learning, such as recognition memory [2, 3]. Here, we used a novel fMRI-based dichoptic masking protocol—designed to induce activity in V1, without modulation from visual awareness—to test whether human V1 is implicated in human observers rapidly learning and then later (15–20 min) recognizing a non-conscious and complex (second-order) visuospatial sequence. Learning was associated with a change in V1 activity, as part of a temporo-occipital and basal ganglia network, which is at variance with the cortico-cerebellar network identified in prior studies of “implicit” sequence learning that involved motor responses and visible stimuli (e.g., [4]). Recognition memory was associated with V1 activity, as part of a temporo-occipital network involving the hippocampus, under conditions that were not imputable to mechanisms associated with conscious retrieval. Notably, the V1 responses during learning and recognition separately predicted non-conscious recognition memory, and functional coupling between V1 and the hippocampus was enhanced for old retrieval cues. The results provide a basis for novel hypotheses about the signals that can drive recognition memory, because these data (1) identify human V1 with a memory network that can code complex associative serial visuospatial information and support later non-conscious recognition memory-guided behavior (cf. [5]) and (2) align with mouse models of experience-dependent V1 plasticity in learning and memory [6].
Keywords: recognition memory, non-conscious memory, primary visual cortex, sequence learning, hippocampus, implicit, relational
Highlights
-
•
Sequence learning and recognition memory can operate without visual awareness
-
•
V1, hippocampus, and basal ganglia support learning of a non-conscious sequence
-
•
Old/new status of non-conscious recognition probes modulates V1-hippocampal coupling
-
•
V1 activity predicts non-conscious recognition memory performance
Rosenthal et al. demonstrate that responses in human primary visual cortex (V1) support perceptual learning and recognition of a complex and non-conscious visuospatial sequence, and predict high-level, non-conscious recognition memory performance. These results elaborate on V1’s established role in simple, low-level perceptual learning.
Results
Recent evidence has identified mouse primary visual cortex (V1) with coding simple serial associations and timing information, when learning a repeating sequence of visible gratings [7], and with a form of recognition memory that is selective for the orientation of visible gratings [6]. In humans, a frontal-occipital negative-going event-related potential (ERP) signal has been identified with a guess response that is associated with the recognition of single visible items [8]. It is thus conceivable that experience-dependent changes in human V1 may be relevant for later recognition memory-guided behavior, but critically, this needs to be established outside of the general modulation in V1 associated with visual awareness [9].
We combined fMRI with a novel dichoptic masking protocol, involving separate learning and recognition phases associated with the presentation of a complex and non-conscious visuospatial sequence of targets appearing across four monocular locations. Figure 1 illustrates the protocol (see Figure 1 legend for details; see also Movie S1 and Supplemental Experimental Procedures for further details).
This visual presentation protocol was expected to be associated with V1 activity because monocular inputs are retained in early visual cortex [10] and dichoptic masking has been identified with responses in V1 [11]. During the learning phase, we ensured that the observers sustained their attention to single targets that appeared on each trial by asking them to count the number of trials with a large-diameter target (LDT; targets could be of either standard or large size—see Supplemental Experimental Procedures). Performance on the LDT counting task was at ceiling (see Supplemental Results). The LDT counting task was also expected to drive V1 activity, because V1 codes for magnitude differences between stimuli [12].
An unexpected non-conscious recognition memory test followed 15–20 min after the learning phase (see Figure 1 and its accompanying legend for a description of the recognition procedure and the features of the retrieval cues). On each trial of the recognition test, observers were asked to discriminate whether the retrieval cue was drawn from the old (trained) sequence or from a new sequence and to rate the confidence in their response on a six-point scale (1–3, “old”; 4–6, “new”; see Figure 1). Crucially, old and new retrieval cues were perceived in the same serial order due to dichoptic presentation. As can be seen in Figure 1, the sequence of locations that distinguished old (e.g., 3-4-1-2-4-3) from new retrieval cues (e.g., 1-4-3-2-4-1) was confined to the monocular level. For the above examples, both old and new retrieval cues were perceived as L-R-L-R-R-L (see Table S3 and Figure 1). Therefore, serial order information that distinguished old and new retrieval cues was not available to visual awareness (as confirmed by psychophysical tests reported below).
Before presenting the neuroimaging data, we report the results from behavioral control experiments demonstrating that (1) the participants did not have conscious access to the four (monocular) locations (see Figure S1 and Supplemental Results) and (2) sequence learning operated independently of motor-based mechanisms, i.e., did not involve somatomotor responses and eye movements that would generate stimulus-motor response bindings coinciding with the structure of the visuospatial sequence (cf. [4, 13]). Observers’ inability to identify the monocular locations was consistent with a lack of knowledge, even about the overall basic sequence structure (as assessed on a post-learning awareness questionnaire; see Supplemental Results).
Neural Substrates of Sequence Learning
In line with prior studies of sequence learning, whole-brain analyses tested for linear effects of exposure to the non-conscious structured second-order conditional (SOC)-based sequence by comparing it against a pseudorandom baseline sequence. Estimates were derived separately for structured and pseudorandom blocks, and these were compared at a higher level of analysis (see Figure 2A legend and Supplemental Information). Activity was observed in a set of regions that included right putamen and pallidum (Montreal Neurological Institute [MNI] 20 4 0, Z = 3.62), right insula (40 −2 −14, Z = 3.59), inferior temporal gyrus (46 −54 −8, Z = 3.78), hippocampus (30 −20 −12, Z = 2.59), lateral occipital cortex (34 −82 −2, Z = 3.39), fusiform gyrus (30 −66 −6, Z = 3.61), lingual gyrus (24 −60 −8, Z = 3.56), and occipital pole (8 −96 0, Z = 3.33), which notably included the intracalcarine cortex of primary visual cortex (6 −88 −2, Z = 3.09). Figure 2A depicts the brain activity maps (in yellow). Learning was also examined by testing linear effects of exposure to the structured sequence alone (Figure 2A, indicated in green), which again showed activity in the intracalcarine sulcus (peak MNI −18 −64 4, Z = 3.3), extending into the lingual gyrus and precuneus. Activity in V1 is thus unlikely to reflect relative differences in novelty of the pseudorandom versus structured sequences within a run, and, as we discuss later, the change in V1 activity predicted behavior on the non-conscious recognition test, which could not be solved on the basis of relative novelty.
Unlike the non-conscious recognition test, where old and new retrieval cues were perceived in the same serial order, there was a visible difference in perceived L-R serial order between structured and pseudorandom blocks during the learning phase (see Supplemental Experimental Procedures), and not all low-level structural properties were equated across the two sequence types. In line with previous studies [15], the serial order of the visual targets (L-R) on unstructured baseline blocks was pseudorandom to facilitate learning of SOC structured sequence. This may have modulated how attention was allocated during learning and hence could account for the observed learning activity in V1. Therefore, a separate control fMRI study was conducted to examine the learning of a SOC sequence under conditions where the four locations were available to visual awareness. All other aspects of the control fMRI study were equated with the dichoptic learning protocol, including the LDT counting task, and learning without motor responses (see Supplemental Experimental Procedures section “fMRI control experiment: Experimental procedures”). Learning the visible sequence was associated with a right fronto-parietal network, which also included subcortical foci in thalamus and left putamen (Figure 2B). These regions were used as a mask to test for learning-related activity in the dichoptic learning protocol. Only the left putamen (−24 10 −2, Z = 3.76, corrected) was associated with learning-related activity under dichoptic masking. Conversely, when the regions that showed learning-related activity during dichoptic masking of the sequence (i.e., primary visual, occipito-temporal cortex, hippocampus, and basal ganglia) were used as a mask to test for learning-related activity associated with the visible sequence, activity was confined to foci in the right putamen (26 4 −10, Z > 2.3, corrected). Again, there was no evidence of additional overlap. Finally, an unpaired t test was performed to compare the parameter estimates for the learning effects associated with conscious visible sequences with those in the dichoptic masking experiment. The results showed that learning-related activity in superior frontal and prefrontal areas was greater with visible than with the masked sequences.
Together, these results indicate that learning-related activity in the dichoptic masking protocol is unlikely to have been driven by perceptual differences between structured and pseudorandom sequences, sensory adaptation, or other non-specific learning effects, because these factors were equated in both fMRI studies, yet the learning-related brain networks did not map across the learning of conscious visible and non-conscious sequences. We present additional information from the non-conscious recognition test below in support of this interpretation.
Behavioral Evidence for Non-conscious Recognition Memory
Non-conscious recognition memory was first examined using a contrast based on confidence ratings associated with all old and all new stimuli (retrieval cues). Although the proportion of correct “old”/“new” discriminations was at chance (mean = 0.51 [SEM = 0.01]; t(17) = 0.68, p > 0.05), the memory ratings associated with all “old” and all “new” retrieval cues were significantly different (mean “old” = 2.64 [SEM = 0.04], mean “new” = 3.16 [SEM = 0.07]; t(17) = −7.95, p < 0.001; Figure S2), indicating recognition of the non-conscious sequence. This result is in keeping with recent behavioral evidence of recognition memory without visual awareness [16, 17] and aligns with other behavioral evidence that has examined the learning of non-conscious first-order sequences [18].
Additional analyses were based on receiver operating characteristics (ROC) of type 1 performance (i.e., sensitivity to “old”/“new” retrieval cues) and type 2 performance (i.e., how memory confidence relates to “old”/“new” discriminations), and also type 1 d′ and type 2 meta-d′ [19] (i.e., the efficacy with which observers’ confidence ratings discriminated between their own correct and incorrect memory decisions). Type 1 sensitivity for “old”/“new” discrimination was at chance (i.e., for sensitivity based on the area under the type 1 ROC: t(17) = 1.45, p = 0.17, mean = 0.52, SEM = 0.01, chance = 0.5; for type 1 d′: t(17) = 0.59, p = 0.57, mean = 0.03, SEM = 0.05, chance = 0), whereas type 2 sensitivity was significantly above chance (for sensitivity based on the area under the type 2 ROC: t(17) = 5.79, p < 0.001, mean = 0.63, SEM = 0.02, chance = 0.5; for type 2 meta-d′: t(17) = 4.90, p < 0.001, mean = 0.9, SEM = 0.18, chance = 0) (see Figure S3, Supplemental Results, and Tables S1 and S2). These results are consistent with recent evidence indicating that metacognitive processes (e.g., related to perceptual decision making or memory) can be successfully deployed even when type 1 sensitivity is null [20, 21], i.e., under conditions that are independent of visual awareness [22]. The current study and other recent studies set precedents that require additional experimental work to examine the relationship between visual awareness and higher-order cognition.
There was no significant difference in the manual response times (RTs) to “old” and “new” retrieval cues (mean RTs, 2018 ms versus 2119 ms, respectively; t(17) = 0.86, p = 0.40). Therefore, any increase in perceptual fluency (i.e., the perceived speed of processing an item), induced by prior exposure, did not lead to faster identification times (response priming), which suggests that response fluency did not serve as a basis on which to recognize prior occurrence (cf. [23]). Notably, prior studies have reported RT differences between old and new retrieval cues based on visible sequences (e.g., [24]). The absence of such priming effects in the current study may be because motor-based responses were not performed during learning.
Relevance of Learning-Related Activity for Subsequent Non-conscious Recognition Memory
The relevance of learning-related activity for knowledge of the trained SOC sequence was examined using a robust multiple regression analysis based on the magnitude of the corrected blood-oxygen-level-dependent (BOLD) signal change during learning within the calcarine sulcus and six other functional regions-of-interest (ROIs) (hippocampus, inferior temporal sulcus, insula, putamen, fusiform cortex, and lingual gyrus). Each was entered as a variable in a multiple linear regression using Huber’s method of correction for outliers against the magnitude of non-conscious recognition memory (old minus new mean confidence ratings). Learning-related activity within the calcarine sulcus predicted behavior on the non-conscious recognition test (β = 0.35, p < 0.05), whereas the activity estimates from the six other ROIs were not significant predictors of non-conscious recognition memory (all p’s > 0.21). If learning-related activity in V1 had merely reflected low-level sensory adaptation effects, low-level structural or relative novelty differences, and/or perceived differences in serial order between the structured and pseudorandom sequences, then the V1 signal change during learning should not have predicted recognition memory, because structural and perceived differences were equated between old and new retrieval cues used on the non-conscious recognition test (Table S3).
Neural Substrates of Non-conscious Recognition Memory
First, we assessed BOLD activity for the old < new contrast using a whole-brain mass-univariate analysis. Non-conscious recognition-related activity was observed in a visual cluster with a peak in the lingual gyrus (MNI −20 −50 −4, Z = 3.7), which extended into the intra-calcarine sulcus in V1 (−4 −80 2, Z = 2.79). Left and right lateral occipital cortex (LOC) also exhibited activity, with peaks in left inferior LOC (−52 −76 −6, Z = 3.64) and right superior LOC (28 −88 26, Z = 4.39). Notably, the old < new effect was also found in the left hippocampus (−22 −38 0, Z = 3.49). Figure 3A depicts the brain activity maps. No significant brain changes were found in the old > new contrast.
Second, we performed the same analyses using functional ROIs derived from the intracalcarine cluster observed during the learning phase, with voxelwise correction for multiple comparisons (p < 0.05). Notably, learning and non-conscious recognition-related activity overlapped in the intracalcarine cortex (MNI −0 −80 2, Z = 3.17 and −14 −68 8, Z = 3.29). This overlap suggests that V1 activity can occur even when there are no low-level structural differences between the sequences that form the basis of the contrast, as compared to the structured versus pseudorandom block-based linear contrast in the learning phase.
Third, following previous studies that identified the conscious recall of visible grating pairs with hippocampal-V1 coupling [25] and coupling between the hippocampus and early visual cortex with implicit statistical learning of visible stimuli [26], we tested for functional connectivity between hippocampus and V1 during non-conscious recognition by means of psychophysiological interaction (PPI) analysis. A mask of the hippocampus was drawn from the responsive voxels in the old < new contrast described above and used to define the seed region’s time course. Given our a priori interest in V1, this analysis used a region-of-interest approach with an occipital mask. Figure 3B illustrates the PPI results. A significant cluster was found in left V1 (MNI 10 −70 12) that, critically, coincided with the intra-calcarine cortex (Z > 2.3, p < 0.05 corrected for the occipital mask). This V1 cluster exhibited increased functional coupling with the hippocampus during the presentation of old relative to new retrieval cues. The same result was observed in the adjacent intra-calcarine area of the right hemisphere (Z > 2, p < 0.05, corrected). Individual measures of functional connectivity did not correlate with non-conscious recognition memory behavior (p > 0.05).
Finally, the link between human V1 and calcarine sulcus is well described [27], and activity in the intracalcarine cortex and its vicinity coincided with probabilistic V1. Nonetheless, given our a priori interest in V1, a functional localizer was administered in a separate fMRI scan to map regions in early visual cortex that were associated with visual stimulation at the monocular locations. V1 was automatically defined on each individual structural scan by applying the method developed by Hinds and colleagues [28]. This method is implemented in Freesurfer [29], in order to predict the location of the stria of Gennari—an anatomical marker of primary visual cortex—with reference to cortical surface topology, which is demonstrated to be as accurate as a retinotopic mapping [30]. Individual masks with a probability of 0.99 of occurring in V1 were obtained on each observer to mitigate uncertainties about architectonic borders. Individual masks were then derived for the intersection of active voxels in visual cortex seen on the localizer and the V1 area derived from Freesurfer. These masks were used to extract individual parameter estimates for the old < new contrast. A one-sample t test showed that the old < new recognition effect was significantly different from chance (t(17) = 2.14, p < 0.05).
Behavioral Significance of Activity Associated with Non-conscious Recognition Memory
This issue was examined by using the same robust multiple-regression-based method described earlier, but with recognition fMRI-based ROIs in the calcarine sulcus, hippocampus, and right occipital cortex. The difference in activity within the calcarine sulcus predicted behavior in the non-conscious recognition test (β = 0.31, p < 0.05). Likewise, activity in the right occipital cortex also predicted behavior in the non-conscious recognition test, with a negative association (β = −0.47, p < 0.05), whereas the activity estimate from the hippocampus was not a significant predictor (p = 0.32). These results suggest that activity in V1 and the occipital cortex both predicted non-conscious recognition memory but that only the magnitude of the old < new response in V1 was linked with improved non-conscious recognition memory. One interpretation of these associations is that non-conscious recognition processes may operate not only by local signal differences in V1 activity but also by concurrent inhibition in connected occipital structures that respond to the monocular stimuli. Push-pull processes of this type may optimize discrimination processes in the service of non-conscious recognition memory, but definitive hypotheses about possible gating mechanisms involving regional activity within occipital cortex associated with non-conscious recognition memory await further investigation.
Discussion
Learning-related activity occurred in cortical and subcortical regions, including primary visual cortex, as part of a temporo-occipital and basal ganglia network. In contrast to the much-studied serial reaction time task used to investigate sequence learning [31], observers did not direct motor responses or motor attention to the visual stimuli. Therefore, activity reflected complex learning in the absence of mechanisms related to motor responding, and the overlap with several regions identified in visuomotor sequence learning [4, 15] is thereby consistent with common mechanisms of perceptual and motor learning [32]. By contrast, V1 activity has not been seen in prior investigations of explicit (conscious) and implicit (non-conscious) human visuomotor sequence learning of visible stimuli, nor was it observed in our control fMRI learning study based on a visible SOC sequence. V1 has however been implicated with coding simple serial associations and timing information in an experimental animal mouse model, when studied using stimuli (visible gratings) designed to induce activity in V1 [7].
Unlike later visual cortical areas such as the perirhinal and infero-temporal cortex, there is little experimental data to implicate V1 in recognition memory. Most notably, recent work in a mouse model has linked V1 with a form of recognition memory based on discriminations between visible old and novel oriented gratings [6, 33]. We extend these observations by showing that early visual cortices in humans, including V1, a region implicated in perception and perceptual learning of low-level features [34], is associated with learning and subsequent non-conscious recognition memory of a repeating complex visuospatial sequence. Importantly, by broadening the scope of investigation to high-level memory-guided behavior, we have demonstrated that experience-dependent changes in V1 can (1) occur even when the spatial location of targets is masked from visual awareness, (2) arise over an hour rather than several days (cf. [1]), (3) operate in the absence of reward or punishment [35], and (4) generate a signal that predicts behavior (i.e., recognition confidence) outside of the original conditions of (perceptual) learning (cf. [36]).
Isolating non-conscious processes in recognition memory has proved difficult to achieve. One potential reason is that previous behavioral paradigms have administered visible stimuli at encoding and as retrieval cues ([8, 37]; but see [16]). The key advantage of our protocol is that activity in visual cortex and the hippocampus were not mediated by modulation due to conscious perceptual expectations (related to the four monocular locations) [38], the reinstatement of episodic information [39], or other old/new differences in processes—such as evaluation and decision making—that operate “downstream” of conscious retrieval. Furthermore, the non-conscious recognition effect seen here is distinguished from other non-conscious forms of memory, such as repetition priming, because repetition-related occipital fluency effects (1) are often unrelated to behavior [40], (2) if present, only prime a particular response such as identification [41], rather than supporting behavior outside of the original study context (i.e., discrimination based on old/new recognition confidence ratings), and (3) involve behavioral phenomena that are considered to be short lived [42], rather than operating 15–20 min after initial learning, as in the current study.
The enhanced functional coupling observed between V1 and the hippocampus during non-conscious recognition is noteworthy (1) given evidence from experimental animal models that implicate the entire ventral visual-to-hippocampal stream in memory for visible items [43] and (2) in light of data in humans wherein hippocampal activity has been aligned with (conscious) memory strength rather than recollection and familiarity [44] and has been shown to be sensitive to the distinction between (visible) old versus novel stimuli [45]. Unlike in previous studies, the hippocampal activity seen here was not attributable to a general novelty signal, a difference in familiarity, or conscious memory strength (cf. [46, 47]), because the new retrieval cues were not based on novel stimuli with respect to the learning phase (cf. [16]), and the serial order of the (binocular) perceived two-location old and new retrieval cues was equally familiar. Old and new retrieval cues were distinguished only in terms of their unique non-conscious serial order, specified at the masked four monocular locations. More broadly, the observed hippocampal activity is notionally consistent with the proposal that relational complexity, rather than mechanisms associated with conscious perception, modulates hippocampal engagement [48].
In summary, our results identify experience-related plasticity in a visual area as early as human V1 with behavior on a recognition memory test that excluded mnemonic mechanisms related to visual awareness. The results are central to broadening the scope of theoretical work on the role of early visual cortex in learning and memory, and go beyond dominant dual-process (episodic) models of recognition memory that have centered on explaining the conscious retrieval of episodic traces or familiarity mediated by the medial temporal lobe.
Author Contributions
Conceptualization, C.R.R. and D.S.; Methodology, C.R.R.; Software, C.R.R. and D.S.; Formal Analysis, C.R.R. and D.S.; Investigation, C.R.R., S.K.A., and C.A.A.; Resources, C.R.R.; Writing – Original Draft, C.R.R.; Writing – Review & Editing, C.R.R., D.S., and C.K.; Visualization, C.R.R. and D.S.; Supervision, C.R.R. and D.S.; Project Administration, C.R.R.; Funding Acquisition, C.R.R., C.K., and D.S.
Acknowledgments
All participants provided informed written consent in accordance with the terms of approval granted by the local research ethics committee and the principles expressed in the Declaration of Helsinki, and were naive to the purpose of the study. This work was supported by the Wellcome Trust (WT073735MA; C.R.R. and C.K.; http://www.wellcome.ac.uk/), the Medical Research Council (UK, 89631; D.S.; http://www.mrc.ac.uk/), the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Oxford Biomedical Research Centre based at Oxford University Hospitals NHS Trust and University of Oxford (C.R.R., C.A.A., and C.K.; http://oxfordbrc.nihr.ac.uk/), and the Dementias and Neurodegenerative Diseases Research Network (C.A.A.; https://www.crn.nihr.ac.uk/dementia). We are grateful for comments made by five anonymous reviewers on earlier versions of the manuscript.
Published: March 3, 2016
Footnotes
This is an open access article under the CC BY license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Supplemental Information includes three figures, three tables, Supplemental Experimental Procedures, Supplemental Results, and one movie and can be found with this article online at http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2016.01.040.
Contributor Information
Clive R. Rosenthal, Email: clive.rosenthal@clneuro.ox.ac.uk.
David Soto, Email: d.soto@bcbl.eu.
Supplemental Information
References
- 1.Sasaki Y., Nanez J.E., Watanabe T. Advances in visual perceptual learning and plasticity. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2010;11:53–60. doi: 10.1038/nrn2737. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Eichenbaum H., Yonelinas A.P., Ranganath C. The medial temporal lobe and recognition memory. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2007;30:123–152. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.30.051606.094328. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Clark R.E. Recognition memory: an old idea given new life. Curr. Biol. 2013;23:R725–R727. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.07.037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Albouy G., Sterpenich V., Balteau E., Vandewalle G., Desseilles M., Dang-Vu T., Darsaud A., Ruby P., Luppi P.-H., Degueldre C. Both the hippocampus and striatum are involved in consolidation of motor sequence memory. Neuron. 2008;58:261–272. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.02.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Squire L.R., Wixted J.T., Clark R.E. Recognition memory and the medial temporal lobe: a new perspective. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2007;8:872–883. doi: 10.1038/nrn2154. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Cooke S.F., Komorowski R.W., Kaplan E.S., Gavornik J.P., Bear M.F. Visual recognition memory, manifested as long-term habituation, requires synaptic plasticity in V1. Nat. Neurosci. 2015;18:262–271. doi: 10.1038/nn.3920. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Gavornik J.P., Bear M.F. Learned spatiotemporal sequence recognition and prediction in primary visual cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 2014;17:732–737. doi: 10.1038/nn.3683. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Voss J.L., Paller K.A. An electrophysiological signature of unconscious recognition memory. Nat. Neurosci. 2009;12:349–355. doi: 10.1038/nn.2260. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Watkins S., Shams L., Josephs O., Rees G. Activity in human V1 follows multisensory perception. Neuroimage. 2007;37:572–578. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.05.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Hubel D.H. Exploration of the primary visual cortex, 1955-78. Nature. 1982;299:515–524. doi: 10.1038/299515a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Macknik S.L., Martinez-Conde S. Dichoptic visual masking reveals that early binocular neurons exhibit weak interocular suppression: implications for binocular vision and visual awareness. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2004;16:1049–1059. doi: 10.1162/0898929041502788. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Murray S.O., Boyaci H., Kersten D. The representation of perceived angular size in human primary visual cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 2006;9:429–434. doi: 10.1038/nn1641. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Smith S.M., Jenkinson M., Woolrich M.W., Beckmann C.F., Behrens T.E., Johansen-Berg H., Bannister P.R., De Luca M., Drobnjak I., Flitney D.E. Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. Neuroimage. 2004;23(Suppl 1):S208–S219. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Worsley K.J. Statistical analysis of activation images. In: Jezzard P., Matthews P.M., Smith S.M., editors. Functional MRI: An Introduction to Methods. Oxford University Press; 2001. pp. 251–270. [Google Scholar]
- 15.Schendan H.E., Searl M.M., Melrose R.J., Stern C.E. An FMRI study of the role of the medial temporal lobe in implicit and explicit sequence learning. Neuron. 2003;37:1013–1025. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00123-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Chong T.T., Husain M., Rosenthal C.R. Recognizing the unconscious. Curr. Biol. 2014;24:R1033–R1035. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2014.09.035. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Rosenthal C.R., Kennard C., Soto D. Visuospatial sequence learning without seeing. PLoS ONE. 2010;5:e11906. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0011906. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Atas A., Faivre N., Timmermans B., Cleeremans A., Kouider S. Nonconscious learning from crowded sequences. Psychol. Sci. 2014;25:113–119. doi: 10.1177/0956797613499591. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Maniscalco B., Lau H. A signal detection theoretic approach for estimating metacognitive sensitivity from confidence ratings. Conscious. Cogn. 2012;21:422–430. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2011.09.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Scott R.B., Dienes Z., Barrett A.B., Bor D., Seth A.K. Blind insight: metacognitive discrimination despite chance task performance. Psychol. Sci. 2014;25:2199–2208. doi: 10.1177/0956797614553944. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Charles L., Van Opstal F., Marti S., Dehaene S. Distinct brain mechanisms for conscious versus subliminal error detection. Neuroimage. 2013;73:80–94. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.01.054. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Jachs B., Blanco M.J., Grantham-Hill S., Soto D. On the independence of visual awareness and metacognition: a signal detection theoretic analysis. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 2015;41:269–276. doi: 10.1037/xhp0000026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Berry C.J., Shanks D.R., Speekenbrink M., Henson R.N. Models of recognition, repetition priming, and fluency: exploring a new framework. Psychol. Rev. 2012;119:40–79. doi: 10.1037/a0025464. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Shanks D.R., Channon S., Wilkinson L., Curran H.V. Disruption of sequential priming in organic and pharmacological amnesia: a role for the medial temporal lobes in implicit contextual learning. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2006;31:1768–1776. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300935. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Bosch S.E., Jehee J.F., Fernández G., Doeller C.F. Reinstatement of associative memories in early visual cortex is signaled by the hippocampus. J. Neurosci. 2014;34:7493–7500. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0805-14.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Turk-Browne N.B., Scholl B.J., Johnson M.K., Chun M.M. Implicit perceptual anticipation triggered by statistical learning. J. Neurosci. 2010;30:11177–11187. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0858-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Horton J.C., Hoyt W.F. The representation of the visual field in human striate cortex. A revision of the classic Holmes map. Arch. Ophthalmol. 1991;109:816–824. doi: 10.1001/archopht.1991.01080060080030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Hinds O.P., Rajendran N., Polimeni J.R., Augustinack J.C., Wiggins G., Wald L.L., Diana Rosas H., Potthast A., Schwartz E.L., Fischl B. Accurate prediction of V1 location from cortical folds in a surface coordinate system. Neuroimage. 2008;39:1585–1599. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.10.033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Fischl B. FreeSurfer. Neuroimage. 2012;62:774–781. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Benson N.C., Butt O.H., Datta R., Radoeva P.D., Brainard D.H., Aguirre G.K. The retinotopic organization of striate cortex is well predicted by surface topology. Curr. Biol. 2012;22:2081–2085. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.09.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Nissen M.J., Bullemer P. Attentional requirements of learning: Evidence from performance measures. Cognit. Psychol. 1987;19:1–32. [Google Scholar]
- 32.Censor N., Sagi D., Cohen L.G. Common mechanisms of human perceptual and motor learning. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2012;13:658–664. doi: 10.1038/nrn3315. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Cooke S.F., Bear M.F. Visual recognition memory: a view from V1. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2015;35:57–65. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2015.06.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Serences J.T., Yantis S. Selective visual attention and perceptual coherence. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2006;10:38–45. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.11.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Chubykin A.A., Roach E.B., Bear M.F., Shuler M.G. A cholinergic mechanism for reward timing within primary visual cortex. Neuron. 2013;77:723–735. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.12.039. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Karklin Y., Lewicki M.S. Emergence of complex cell properties by learning to generalize in natural scenes. Nature. 2009;457:83–86. doi: 10.1038/nature07481. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Craik F.I., Rose N.S., Gopie N. Recognition without awareness: Encoding and retrieval factors. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 2015;41:1271–1281. doi: 10.1037/xlm0000137. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Summerfield C., Egner T., Greene M., Koechlin E., Mangels J., Hirsch J. Predictive codes for forthcoming perception in the frontal cortex. Science. 2006;314:1311–1314. doi: 10.1126/science.1132028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Johnson J.D., McDuff S.G., Rugg M.D., Norman K.A. Recollection, familiarity, and cortical reinstatement: a multivoxel pattern analysis. Neuron. 2009;63:697–708. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.08.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Schacter D.L., Buckner R.L. Priming and the brain. Neuron. 1998;20:185–195. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80448-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Schacter D.L., Wig G.S., Stevens W.D. Reductions in cortical activity during priming. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2007;17:171–176. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2007.02.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Kouider S., Dehaene S. Levels of processing during non-conscious perception: a critical review of visual masking. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 2007;362:857–875. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2007.2093. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.López-Aranda M.F., López-Téllez J.F., Navarro-Lobato I., Masmudi-Martín M., Gutiérrez A., Khan Z.U. Role of layer 6 of V2 visual cortex in object-recognition memory. Science. 2009;325:87–89. doi: 10.1126/science.1170869. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Kirwan C.B., Wixted J.T., Squire L.R. Activity in the medial temporal lobe predicts memory strength, whereas activity in the prefrontal cortex predicts recollection. J. Neurosci. 2008;28:10541–10548. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3456-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Viskontas I.V., Knowlton B.J., Steinmetz P.N., Fried I. Differences in mnemonic processing by neurons in the human hippocampus and parahippocampal regions. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2006;18:1654–1662. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.10.1654. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Rutishauser U., Mamelak A.N., Schuman E.M. Single-trial learning of novel stimuli by individual neurons of the human hippocampus-amygdala complex. Neuron. 2006;49:805–813. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.02.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Brown M.W. Hippocampal and perirhinal functions in recognition memory. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2008;9:405. doi: 10.1038/nrn2154-c1. author reply 405. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Henke K. A model for memory systems based on processing modes rather than consciousness. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2010;11:523–532. doi: 10.1038/nrn2850. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.