Abstract
Cognitive impairment affects approximately half of people living with HIV (PLWH) in the United States, despite improved health outcomes and reduced mortality through widespread use of antiretroviral therapy (ART). Heavy alcohol use and cigarette smoking are potential contributors to neurocognitive impairment in PLWH, yet few studies have examined their influence concurrently. Here we investigated the effects of self-reported alcohol use and smoking on learning, memory, processing speed, verbal fluency, and executive function in 124 HIV-positive men who have sex with men [age (mean ± SD) = 42.8 ± 10.4 years], engaged with medical care. All participants were heavy drinkers. Duration of HIV infection averaged 9.9 ± 7.6 years, and 92.7% were on a stable ART regimen. Participants completed a neuropsychological battery and assessment of past 30-day substance use. Average number of drinks per drinking day (DPDD) was 5.6 ± 3.5, and 33.1% of participants were daily smokers. Rates of neurocognitive impairment were highest in learning (50.8%), executive function (41.9%), and memory (38.0%). Multiple regression models tested DPDD and smoking status as predictors of neurocognitive performance, controlling for age and premorbid intelligence. Smoking was significantly, negatively related to verbal learning (p = 0.046) and processing speed (p = 0.001). DPDD was a significant predictor of learning (p = 0.047) in a model that accounted for the interaction of DPDD and smoking status. As expected, premorbid intelligence significantly predicted all neurocognitive scores (p’s < 0.01), and older age was associated with slower processing speed (p’s < 0.01). In conclusion, smoking appears to be associated with neurocognitive functioning deficits in PLWH beyond the effects of heavy drinking, aging, and premorbid intelligence. Smoking cessation interventions have the potential to be an important target for improving functional outcomes in heavy drinking PLWH.
Keywords: smoking, alcohol, human immunodeficiency virus (HIV), cognition, processing speed
Introduction
Antiretroviral therapy (ART) has reduced mortality from human immunodeficiency virus (HIV) infection, yet cognitive impairment continues to affect people living with HIV (PLWH). HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders (HAND) are acquired impairments in cognitive functioning that present along a spectrum of severity and are not attributable to medical or psychiatric comorbidity (Antinori et al., 2007). In the era of ART, the prevalence of HAND is estimated at 44% in those without severe comorbidities (Heaton et al., 2010). Although rates of HIV-associated dementia have declined, milder forms of HAND persist despite viral suppression (Heaton et al., 2011). By definition, the mildest (“asymptomatic”) form of HAND entails minimal interference with daily functioning. However, lack of insight into deficits may lead to a self-report bias in prevalence estimates for more severe forms of HAND (Chiao et al., 2013). Recent studies have pointed to comorbidities, extent or timeline of viral suppression, and appropriateness of neuropsychological norms as factors that also may influence HAND prevalence estimates (Crum-Cianflone et al., 2013; Cysique & Brew, 2011; Nightingale et al., 2014).
Heavy alcohol use and smoking frequently co-occur with HIV infection, and may compound HAND. Among PLWH, 65% reported alcohol use in the past year, and 15% endorsed binge drinking in the past 30 days (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2014). HIV infection and alcohol use disorders (AUDs) both are associated with impairment in learning, memory, information processing, and executive functioning (Persidsky et al., 2011). At 41%, the rate of smoking in PLWH is twice that of the general population, and smoking is a major risk factor for cardiovascular disease, non-AIDS cancer, and opportunistic infection (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2014; Lifson et al., 2010). In a previous study, we found that smoking also was significantly associated with impaired learning and memory in PLWH (Bryant, Kahler, Devlin, Monti, & Cohen, 2013). Here we investigated associations of neurocognition with both alcohol use and smoking.
Method
Participants
Participants were 124 MSM living with HIV, recruited from a health center to take part in a clinical trial of a brief intervention for heavy drinking. Baseline, pre-randomization measures are presented here. Inclusion criteria were (1) male; (2) ≥ 18 years old; (3) heavy drinking (≥ 5 drinks) at least once per month, or >14 drinks per week; (4) confirmed diagnosis of HIV; and (5) self-reported sex (insertive or receptive, oral or anal) with a male partner in past 12 months. Participants on ART had to be stable on their current regimen for at least three months. Exclusion criteria were (1) current intravenous drug use; (2) current psychosis, suicidality, or mania, determined with the SCID-NP (First, Spitzer, Gibbon, & Williams, 1995); (3) treatment within the past three months for an HIV-related opportunistic infection; (4) current treatment for alcohol or drug problem; (5) score > 7 on the Clinical Institute Withdrawal Assessment for Alcohol (CIWA-Ar) (Sullivan, Sykora, Schneiderman, Naranjo, & Sellers, 1989). Participants were asked to abstain from alcohol for 24 hours before assessment, and zero breath alcohol content was confirmed with breath analysis. Participants provided informed consent in a format approved by the relevant Institutional Review Boards. Table 1 shows participant characteristics.
Table 1.
Mean (SD) or % | |
---|---|
Age (years) | 42.8 (10.4) |
Racea | |
White (%) | 73.4 |
African-American (%) | 32.0 |
Overweight (%) | 62.1 |
Education | |
Less than high school (%) | 1.6 |
High school (%) | 10.5 |
Some college or vocational training (%) | 41.1 |
College graduate or beyond (%) | 46.8 |
Unemployed (%) | 36.3 |
Duration of HIV infection (years) | 9.9 (7.6) |
CD4 count nadir (n = 115) | 343.5 (209.5) |
Hepatitis C virus coinfection (%) | 7.3 |
Undetectable viral load (%) | 91.1 |
Currently on ARTb (%) | 92.7 |
Current use of ART by drug classc | |
Nucleoside reverse transcriptase inhibitor (%) | 45.1 |
Nonnucleoside reverse transcriptase inhibitor (%) | 6.5 |
Protease inhibitor (%) | 50.8 |
HIV integrase strand transfer inhibitor (%) | 8.9 |
CCR5 co-receptor antagonist (%) | 1.6 |
Multi-class combination drug (%) | 41.9 |
Smoking status | |
Never smoker (%) | 15.0 |
Past smoker (%) | 31.5 |
Occasional smoker (%) | 20.2 |
Current daily smoker (%) | 33.1 |
Alcohol dependence diagnosis | |
Never (%) | 29.8 |
Past (%) | 32.3 |
Current (%) | 37.9 |
Number of drinking daysb | 16.9 (8.5) |
Average drinks per drinking dayb | 5.6 (3.5) |
Total number of drinksb | 93.3 (91.0) |
Number of heavy drinking daysb | 7.4 (6.9) |
Percentages reporting any use of other drugsb | |
Marijuana (%) | 44.4 |
Methamphetamine (%) | 8.9 |
Cocaine (%) | 23.4 |
Poppers/inhalants (%) | 18.5 |
None (%) | 34.7 |
Number of substance use daysb | |
Marijuana | 6.3 (10.4) |
Methamphetamine | 0.2 (0.9) |
Cocaine | 1.0 (3.3) |
Poppers/inhalants | 0.9 (2.4) |
Current cannabis dependence (%) | 6.5 |
Current non-cannabis substance dependence (%) | 8.1 |
Current depression diagnosis (n = 119) (%) | 9.7 |
Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression (CES-D) score | 6.2 (12.2) |
Three participants endorsed more than one racial category.
Assessed with Timeline Followback Interview covering past 30 days.
Sum of percentages exceeds 100% because participants could report taking multiples classes of ART.
Measures
Self-report measures
Alcohol and substance use disorders were assessed using the SCID-NP (First et al., 1995). The Timeline Followback Interview (Sobell & Sobell, 1992) was used to assess standard alcoholic drinks consumed (12 oz. beer, 5 oz. wine, or 1.5 oz. 80-proof liquor) and use of drugs in the past 30 days. Participants were designated daily smokers if they reported smoking ≥ 20 days. Depressive symptoms were assessed with the Center for Epidemiological Studies-Depression (CES-D) scale (Radloff, 1977).
Neurocognitive assessment
The battery assessed learning, memory, processing speed, executive function, and verbal fluency. The selected measures (Table 2) are sensitive to deficits associated with HIV and AUDs (Antinori et al., 2007; Cohen et al., 2001; Oscar-Berman et al., 2014). Standardized scores were calculated using published norms (Heaton, Miller, Taylor, & Grant, 2004). Impairment was defined as a score ≥ 1 standard deviation (SD) below the mean of standardized norms (Carey et al., 2004; Heaton et al., 1995).
Table 2.
Domain | Measure |
---|---|
Premorbid intelligence | Wechsler Test of Adult Reading (WTAR) score (Wechsler, 2001) |
Learning | Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised (HVLT) total recall (Benedict, Schretlen, Groninger, & Brandt, 1998) |
Memory | HVLT delayed recall |
Processing speed |
|
Executive function | Trails B test time (Reitan, 1992) |
Verbal fluency | Controlled Oral Word Association Test (COWAT) total score (Benton, Hamsher, & Sivan, 1994) |
Note: Higher score indicates better performance for all tests except Trails A and Trails B, for which higher time score indicates worse (i.e., slower) performance.
Statistical analysis
Preliminary analyses examined percentages of participants with impairment in each domain. Next, we examined bivariate correlations between drinking variables and raw cognitive scores. We chose DPDD as the primary alcohol use variable because research has shown that heavy episodic drinking is detrimental to brain function, overall health, and psychosocial functioning (Crews & Nixon, 2009; Holahan, Schutte, Brennan, Holahan, & Moos, 2014). The a priori decision to compare daily smokers to a combined group of never, past, and occasional smokers was supported by a preliminary analysis showing no differences in scores among the latter groups and by population-based research (Mons, Schottker, Muller, Kliegel, & Brenner, 2013).
Three sets of multiple linear regression models assessed the effects of selected predictors on cognitive functioning. We used raw scores because adjustments for demographic factors (i.e., age, education, race) were inconsistent across test norms. The preliminary analysis included age, WTAR, education, race, drug use (current/past cannabis dependence, any drug use, or any non-cannabis drug use), and CES-D score as covariates. The final models were chosen through a backward selection procedure, in which we removed non-significant covariates one step at a time. Education, race, drug use, and CES-D were non-significant and thus were not retained in subsequent models. The first set of regression models tested associations of DPDD and smoking status with cognitive scores, controlling for age. In the second set of regression models, we tested these associations controlling for age and WTAR. The third set of regression models included an interaction term for smoking and DPDD. Alpha level was set to .05 for statistical significance.
Results
Rates of cognitive impairment
Rates of impairment in learning (50.8%), executive function (41.9%), and memory (38.0%) exceeded the 16% expected on the basis of the normal distribution (Table 3).
Table 3.
Test and associated domain | Mean (SD) | % impaired |
---|---|---|
WTAR (premorbid intelligence) | 107.3 (15.0) | N/A |
HVLT total recall (learning) | 39.3 (11.0) | 50.8 |
HVLT delayed recall (memory)a | 42.4 (12.3) | 38.0 |
Digit Symbol (processing speed) | 48.0 (9.4) | 16.1 |
Trails A time (processing speed) | 49.4 (10.2) | 15.3 |
Trails B time (executive function) | 41. 5 (17.5) | 41.9 |
COWAT (verbal fluency)a | 52.1 (11.2) | 12.9 |
n = 123
Note: Impairment was defined as a score ≥ 1 SD below the mean for published norms.
Abbreviations: COWAT = Controlled Oral Word Association Test; HVLT = Hopkins Verbal Learning Test; WTAR = Wechsler Test of Adult Reading.
Contributions of smoking status and alcohol use to cognitive performance
Drinks per drinking day (DPDD) showed consistent associations with neurocognitive scores in bivariate correlations (Table 4). The first set of regression models (Table 5), controlling for age, showed a significant, negative association of current smoking with learning, memory, and processing speed. Age was a significant predictor of processing speed. Premorbid functioning was a significant predictor of all domains across models.
Table 4.
DPDD | Total number of drinks |
Number of heavy drinking days |
|
---|---|---|---|
Learning (HVLT total recall) |
−0.16 | −0.09 | −0.11 |
Memory (HVLT delayed recall) |
−0.24** | −0.17 | −0.12 |
Processing speed (Digit Symbol) |
−0.21* | −0.21* | −0.12 |
Processing speed (Trails A) |
0.17 | 0.13 | 0.04 |
Executive function (Trails B) |
0.00 | 0.02 | 0.02 |
Verbal fluency (COWAT) |
−0.21* | −0.17 | −0.17 |
significant at p < 0.01;
significant at p < 0.05.
Note: Higher scores correspond to better performance for HVLT, Digit Symbol, and COWAT measures. Lower scores indicate better performance for Trails A and Trails B.
Abbreviations: COWAT = Controlled Oral Word Association Test; DPDD = drinks per drinking day; HVLT = Hopkins Verbal Learning Test.
Table 5.
Domain and measure |
Predictors | Model 1a | Model 2b | Model 3c | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Estimate | p-value | Estimate | p-value | Estimate | p-value | ||
Premorbid intelligence (WTAR) |
Age | 0.05 | 0.542 | - | - | - | - |
DPDD | −0.65 | 0.011 | - | - | - | - | |
Daily Smoker | −4.06 | 0.035 | - | - | - | - | |
| |||||||
Learning (HVLT total recall) |
WTAR | - | - | 0.18 | < 0.001 | 4.86 | < 0.001 |
Age | −0.05 | 0.164 | −0.06 | 0.081 | −2.18 | 0.031 | |
DPDD | −0.11 | 0.365 | 0.01 | 0.934 | −2.01 | 0.047 | |
Daily Smoker | −2.48 | 0.007 | −1.73 | 0.046 | −3.19 | 0.002 | |
DPDD × Daily Smoker |
- | - | - | - | 2.47 | 0.015 | |
| |||||||
Memory (HVLT delayed recall) |
WTAR | - | - | 0.09 | < 0.001 | 4.42 | < 0.001 |
Age | −0.03 | 0.176 | −0.03 | 0.076 | −1.77 | 0.080 | |
DPDD | −0.10 | 0.087 | −0.04 | 0.443 | −0.47 | 0.640 | |
Daily Smoker | −1.19 | 0.008 | −0.81 | 0.056 | −1.09 | 0.278 | |
DPDD × Daily Smoker |
- | - | - | - | 0.05 | 0.959 | |
| |||||||
Processing speed (Digit Symbol) |
WTAR | - | - | 0.56 | < 0.001 | 4.33 | < 0.001 |
Age | −0.34 | 0.006 | −0.37 | 0.002 | −3.11 | 0.002 | |
DPDD | −0.46 | 0.223 | −0.10 | 0.783 | 0.17 | 0.867 | |
Daily Smoker | −11.65 | < 0.001 | −9.38 | 0.001 | −1.56 | 0.122 | |
DPDD × Daily Smoker |
- | - | - | - | −0.39 | 0.701 | |
| |||||||
Processing speed (Trails A) |
WTAR | - | - | −0.34 | < 0.001 | −3.22 | 0.002 |
Age | 0.30 | 0.002 | 0.32 | 0.001 | 3.41 | 0.001 | |
DPDD | 0.44 | 0.140 | 0.22 | 0.451 | 0.30 | 0.765 | |
Daily Smoker | 3.91 | 0.082 | 2.55 | 0.245 | 0.51 | 0.609 | |
DPDD × Daily Smoker |
- | - | - | - | 0.14 | 0.888 | |
| |||||||
Executive function (Trails B) |
WTAR | - | - | −4.51 | < 0.001 | −5.51 | < 0.001 |
Age | 0.58 | 0.470 | 0.81 | 0.265 | 1.07 | 0.286 | |
DPDD | −1.35 | 0.591 | −4.28 | 0.065 | −1.18 | 0.242 | |
Daily Smoker | 31.60 | 0.096 | 13.28 | 0.441 | 0.27 | 0.788 | |
DPDD × Daily Smoker |
- | - | - | - | 0.18 | 0.860 | |
| |||||||
Verbal fluency (COWAT) |
WTAR | - | - | 0.50 | < 0.001 | 3.78 | < 0.001 |
Age | −0.13 | 0.299 | −0.16 | 0.171 | −1.40 | 0.165 | |
DPDD | −0.74 | 0.054 | −0.41 | 0.270 | −0.84 | 0.403 | |
Daily Smoker | −2.65 | 0.357 | −0.51 | 0.854 | −0.33 | 0.743 | |
DPDD × Daily Smoker |
- | - | - | - | 0.27 | 0.786 |
Model 1: Regression analysis including all covariates in the table except WTAR
Model 2: Regression analysis including all covariates in the table
Model 3: Regression analysis including all covariates and the interaction of daily smoking and DPDD
Note: Higher scores indicate better performance for WTAR, HVLT, Digit Symbol, and COWAT measures. Lower scores indicate better performance for Trails A and Trails B.
Abbreviations: DPDD = drinks per drinking day; COWAT = Controlled Oral Word Association Test; HVLT = Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised; WTAR = Wechsler Test of Adult Reading.
In the second set of regression models, controlling for age and premorbid intelligence, smoking status was a significant predictor of learning and processing speed. Age remained a significant predictor of processing speed.
In the third set of models, which included the interaction of DPDD and smoking status, the interaction term was a significant predictor of learning, as were DPDD and smoking status individually. The direction for the interaction effect was opposite to individual effects for DPDD and smoking status. Post hoc follow-up analysis showed that the slope for DPDD on learning was negative for non-daily smokers (B = −0.06, p = 0.610) but positive for daily smokers (B = 0.22, p = 0.114).
Finally, the possibility that neurocognitive deficits associated with smoking might be related to lifetime substance exposure was explored in a post hoc chi-square test on binary (yes/no) variables for current smoking and lifetime substance use disorder. Current smokers were significantly more likely to report lifetime substance use disorders than non-smokers, χ2(1) = 9.59, p = 0.002.
Discussion
In this study, we found elevated rates of impairment in learning, executive function, and memory in heavy-drinking, HIV-positive MSM, consistent with previous studies on HIV infection and alcohol use (Persidsky et al., 2011). The main findings further our understanding of neurocognition in PLWH. First, daily smoking was significantly associated with decrements in verbal learning and processing speed, and these associations persisted after controlling for premorbid intelligence, age, and alcohol use. This finding replicates and extends key aspects of our earlier study showing that smoking predicted poorer learning, memory, and global cognition in an independent sample of PLWH (Bryant et al., 2013). Regarding clinical and functional implications, previous research identified the measure of processing speed associated with smoking status, the Digit Symbol test, as a strong independent predictor of quality of life in PLWH (Tozzi et al., 2003). Second, premorbid intelligence significantly predicted performance in all domains, and age significantly predicted processing speed. These results underscore the importance of using demographically appropriate norms and comparing an individual’s current functioning to his or her own baseline where possible. Third, DPDD showed correlations with memory, processing speed, and verbal fluency, but associations were not significant in adjusted models. A significant negative effect of DPDD on learning emerged when including the interaction of DPDD and smoking. However, the interaction effect, in which DPDD had a positive slope on learning for daily smokers and negative slope for non-daily smokers, is difficult to interpret and underscores the non-causal nature of findings in this observational study.
Generalizability is limited by the all-male sample and high average educational attainment. An important future direction is to compare HIV-positive and HIV-negative groups of MSM to quantify the direct influence of HIV infection on neurocognition after accounting for methodological, demographic, and behavioral factors.
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism under Grant P01AA019072, Grant K05AA019681, and Grant T32AA007459; and the National Institute of Allergy and Infectious Diseases under Grant P30AI042853-15.
Footnotes
Disclosure Statement
The authors have no financial interests or benefits to disclose in association with this work.
References
- Antinori A, Arendt G, Becker JT, Brew BJ, Byrd DA, Cherner M, Wojna VE. Updated research nosology for HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders. Neurology. 2007;69(18):1789–1799. doi: 10.1212/01.WNL.0000287431.88658.8b. doi: 10.1212/01.WNL.0000287431.88658.8b. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Benedict RHB, Schretlen A, Groninger L, Brandt J. Hopkins Verbal Learning Test Revised: Normative data and analysis of inter-form and test-retest reliability. The Clinical Neuropsychologist. 1998;12:43–55. doi: 10.1076/clin.12.1.43.1726. [Google Scholar]
- Benton AL, Hamsher K, Sivan AB. Multilingual aphasia examination. AJA Associates; Iowa City: 1994. [Google Scholar]
- Bryant VE, Kahler CW, Devlin KN, Monti PM, Cohen RA. The effects of cigarette smoking on learning and memory performance among people living with HIV/AIDS. AIDS Care. 2013;25(10):1308–1316. doi: 10.1080/09540121.2013.764965. doi: 10.1080/09540121.2013.764965. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carey CL, Woods SP, Rippeth JD, Gonzalez R, Moore DJ, Marcotte TD, Heaton RK. Initial validation of a screening battery for the detection of HIV-associated cognitive impairment. Clin Neuropsychol. 2004;18(2):234–248. doi: 10.1080/13854040490501448. doi: 10.1080/13854040490501448. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Centers for Disease Control and Prevention . Behavioral and clinical characteristics of persons receiving care for HIV infection--Medical Monitoring Project, United States, 2010. 2014. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chiao S, Rosen HJ, Nicolas K, Wendelken LA, Alcantar O, Rankin KP, Valcour V. Deficits in self-awareness impact the diagnosis of asymptomatic neurocognitive impairment in HIV. AIDS Res Hum Retroviruses. 2013;29(6):949–956. doi: 10.1089/aid.2012.0229. doi: 10.1089/aid.2012.0229. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen RA, Boland R, Paul R, Tashima KT, Schoenbaum EE, Celentano DD, Carpenter CC. Neurocognitive performance enhanced by highly active antiretroviral therapy in HIV-infected women. AIDS. 2001;15(3):341–345. doi: 10.1097/00002030-200102160-00007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crews FT, Nixon K. Mechanisms of neurodegeneration and regeneration in alcoholism. Alcohol Alcohol. 2009;44:115–127. doi: 10.1093/alcalc/agn079. doi: agn079 [pii] 10.1093/alcalc/agn079. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crum-Cianflone NF, Moore DJ, Letendre S, Poehlman Roediger M, Eberly L, Weintrob A, Hale BR. Low prevalence of neurocognitive impairment in early diagnosed and managed HIV-infected persons. Neurology. 2013;80(4):371–379. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e31827f0776. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e31827f0776. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cysique LA, Brew BJ. Prevalence of non-confounded HIV-associated neurocognitive impairment in the context of plasma HIV RNA suppression. J Neurovirol. 2011;17(2):176–183. doi: 10.1007/s13365-011-0021-x. doi: 10.1007/s13365-011-0021-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JBW. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders. New York State Psychiatric Institute; New York: 1995. [Google Scholar]
- Heaton RK, Clifford DB, Franklin DR, Woods SP, Ake C, Vaida F, Group, C. HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders persist in the era of potent antiretroviral therapy: CHARTER Study. Neurology. 2010;75(23):2087–2096. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e318200d727. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e318200d727. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heaton RK, Franklin DR, Ellis RJ, McCutchan JA, Letendre SL, Leblanc S, Group, H. HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders before and during the era of combination antiretroviral therapy: differences in rates, nature, and predictors. J Neurovirol. 2011;17(1):3–16. doi: 10.1007/s13365-010-0006-1. doi: 10.1007/s13365-010-0006-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heaton RK, Grant I, Butters N, White DA, Kirson D, Atkinson JH, et al. The HNRC 500--neuropsychology of HIV infection at different disease stages. HIV Neurobehavioral Research Center. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 1995;1(3):231–251. doi: 10.1017/s1355617700000230. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heaton RK, Miller W, Taylor M, Grant I. Revised comprehensive norms for an expanded Halstead-Reitan battery: demographically adjusted neuropsychological norms for African American and Caucasian adults. Psychological Assessment Resources; Lutz, FL: 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Holahan CJ, Schutte KK, Brennan PL, Holahan CK, Moos RH. Episodic heavy drinking and 20-year total mortality among late-life moderate drinkers. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2014;38(5):1432–1438. doi: 10.1111/acer.12381. doi: 10.1111/acer.12381. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lifson AR, Neuhaus J, Arribas JR, van den Berg-Wolf M, Labriola AM, Read TR, Group, I. S. S. Smoking-related health risks among persons with HIV in the Strategies for Management of Antiretroviral Therapy clinical trial. Am J Public Health. 2010;100(10):1896–1903. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2009.188664. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2009.188664. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mons U, Schottker B, Muller H, Kliegel M, Brenner H. History of lifetime smoking, smoking cessation and cognitive function in the elderly population. Eur J Epidemiol. 2013;28(10):823–831. doi: 10.1007/s10654-013-9840-9. doi: 10.1007/s10654-013-9840-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nightingale S, Winston A, Letendre S, Michael BD, McArthur JC, Khoo S, Solomon T. Controversies in HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders. Lancet Neurol. 2014;13(11):1139–1151. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(14)70137-1. doi: 10.1016/s1474-4422(14)70137-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oscar-Berman M, Valmas MM, Sawyer KS, Ruiz SM, Luhar RB, Gravitz ZR. Profiles of impaired, spared, and recovered neuropsychologic processes in alcoholism. Handb Clin Neurol. 2014;125:183–210. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-62619-6.00012-4. doi: 10.1016/b978-0-444-62619-6.00012-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Persidsky Y, Ho W, Ramirez SH, Potula R, Abood ME, Unterwald E, Tuma R. HIV-1 infection and alcohol abuse: neurocognitive impairment, mechanisms of neurodegeneration and therapeutic interventions. Brain Behav Immun. 2011;25(Suppl 1):S61–70. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2011.03.001. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2011.03.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Radloff LS. The CES-D scale: A self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement. 1977;1(3):385–401. [Google Scholar]
- Reitan RM. Trail making test. Reitan Neuropsychology Laboratory; Tucson, AZ: 1992. [Google Scholar]
- Sobell LC, Sobell MB. Timeline Followback: A technique for assessing self-reported alcohol consumption. In: Allen JP, Columbus M, editors. Assessing Alcohol Problems: A Guide for Clinicians and Researchers. National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism; Bethesda, MD: 1992. pp. 55–73. [Google Scholar]
- Sullivan JT, Sykora K, Schneiderman J, Naranjo CA, Sellers EM. Assessment of alcohol withdrawal: the revised clinical institute withdrawal assessment for alcohol scale (CIWA-Ar) Br J Addict. 1989;84:1353–1357. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.1989.tb00737.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tozzi V, Balestra P, Galgani S, Murri R, Bellagamba R, Narciso P, Wu AW. Neurocognitive performance and quality of life in patients with HIV infection. AIDS Res Hum Retroviruses. 2003;19(8):643–652. doi: 10.1089/088922203322280856. doi: 10.1089/088922203322280856. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wechsler D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III (WAIS-III) The Psychological Corporation; San Antonio, TX: 1997. [Google Scholar]
- Wechsler D. Wechsler Test of Adult Reading (WTAR) The Psychological Corporation; San Antonio, TX: 2001. [Google Scholar]