Abstract
The precise mechanisms underlying general anaesthesia pose important and still open questions. To address them, we have studied anaesthesia induced by the widely used (intravenous) propofol and (inhalational) sevoflurane anaesthetics, computing cross-frequency coupling functions between neuronal, cardiac and respiratory oscillations in order to determine their mutual interactions. The phase domain coupling function reveals the form of the function defining the mechanism of an interaction, as well as its coupling strength. Using a method based on dynamical Bayesian inference, we have thus identified and analysed the coupling functions for six relationships. By quantitative assessment of the forms and strengths of the couplings, we have revealed how these relationships are altered by anaesthesia, also showing that some of them are differently affected by propofol and sevoflurane. These findings, together with the novel coupling function analysis, offer a new direction in the assessment of general anaesthesia and neurophysiological interactions, in general.
Keywords: coupling function, cross-frequency coupling, anaesthesia, propofol, sevoflurane, brain–heart–respiration interactions
1. Introduction
General anaesthesia plays a crucial role in many surgical procedures, and it therefore has an enormous impact on human health. It is a drug-induced, reversible state characterized by unconsciousness, anti-nociception or analgesia, immobility and amnesia [1,2]. On rare occasions, however, the patient can remain unconscious longer than intended, or may regain awareness during surgery. There are no precise measures for maintaining the correct dose of anaesthetic, and there is currently no fully reliable instrument to monitor depth of anaesthesia. Although a number of devices for monitoring brain function or sympathetic output are commercially available [3], the anaesthetist also relies on clinical assessment and experience to judge anaesthetic depth. The undesirable consequences of overdose or unintended awareness might in principle be ameliorated by improved control if we could understand better the changes in function that occur during general anaesthesia, in particular the dynamical brain states, the dynamics of cardiovascular oscillations and their mutual interactions [4].
General anaesthesia can be induced by different anaesthetics which can affect different physiological regions, receptors and channels [5,6]. In this study, we used two of the most widely used anaesthetics—propofol and sevoflurane, i.e. we used one of the two in each anaesthesia measurement. Propofol is introduced intravenously, while sevoflurane is a sweet-smelling, non-flammable type of ether that is inhaled [7–10].
The central enigma in general anaesthesia is the nature of the unconscious state mediated in the brain. Neuronal states often manifest themselves as changes in brain electrophysiological activity, which emanates from the dynamics of large-scale cell ensembles oscillating synchronously [11,12] within characteristic frequency intervals. Individual ensembles communicate to integrate their local information flows into a common brain network. One way to describe such an integration or communication is through cross-frequency coupling, a method that has led to numerous studies elucidating the respective roles of cognition, attention, memory and anaesthesia [9,10,13–15]. Jirsa & Müller [13] recently identified different types of cross-frequency coupling based on use of the power, phase or frequency domains; in what follows, we focus on phase–phase cross-frequency couplings. Unlike earlier cross-frequency coupling methods, the approach that we will discuss assesses neuronal states through the computation of coupling functions describing the functional forms of individual cross-frequency interactions.
Coupling functions prescribe the physical rule specifying how the inter-oscillator interactions occur. They determine the possibility of qualitative transitions between the oscillations, e.g. routes into and out of phase synchronization [16]. Their decomposition can describe the functional contribution from each separate subsystem within a single coupling relationship. In this way, coupling functions offer a unique means of describing mechanisms in a unified and mathematically precise way. It is a fast growing field of research, with much recent progress on the theory [17,18] and especially towards being able to extract and reconstruct the coupling functions between interacting oscillations from data, leading to useful applications in cardiorespiratory interactions [19–21], chemistry [16], mechanics [22] and communications [23]. We will show that, in neuronal analysis, the cross-frequency coupling function describes much more than just a new way of measuring effects: it opens up a whole new perspective on the functional mechanisms underlying the functionality of the brain network.
The oscillatory processes of the brain are not only individually important to the function of the central nervous system, but they can also interact, both mutually and with other physiological oscillations. The latter comprise, e.g., the oscillatory processes of the cardiovascular system [24] including the heart and the lungs, which are closely associated because, working together, they provide the blood supply with oxygen and nutrients for the whole body including the brain. The brain's functional state is obviously of crucial importance in general anaesthesia and as such it provides the basis for number of measures [3] (including, e.g., the BIS (bispectral index) monitor by Medtronic (formerly aspect medical), the Entropy monitor by GE Healthcare, the Narcotrend index by MonitorTechnik, and others). However, although traditional anaesthetic monitoring includes only the on–off awake versus unconscious classification, indirect or surrogate measures of brain function, such as movement, blood pressure, heart rate, sweating and other anaesthesia-induced changes to the cardiovascular system [25–29], also provide valuable indicators [1]. Moreover, the two systems are connected in many ways, and some signatures of causal interaction have already been demonstrated [15]. For a comprehensive assessment of general anaesthesia one should therefore add a consideration of the (complex) interactions between the cardiovascular and brain oscillations [4], and the integration of their functions into what are interconnected physiological networks [4,15,30]. One may thus investigate the mechanisms and connections between the brain and the loss of consciousness [1,2] on the one hand, and, on the other hand, the cardiovascular system which is closely related to the function of the autonomous nervous system including anti-nociception, analgesia and the perception of pain [31–34].
In this paper, we seek to establish the functional laws defining the mutual interactions between the brain, heart and the lungs (figure 1) in general anaesthesia. The study is based on three complementary pillars: (i) anaesthesia with two of the most widely used anaesthetics, using the same experimental set-up; (ii) application of the novel methodology of cross-frequency coupling functions to determine phase-causal links and to probe the interaction mechanisms directly, and (iii) assessment of general anaesthesia based on the combined dynamics and interactions of the brain, lungs and heart oscillations.
2. Material and methods
(a). Inference of cross-frequency coupling functions
Cross-frequency couplings are usually inferred by methods based on the statistics of the coupled signals, such as the correlation and (bi-) coherence measures. Such approaches tell one about the functional connectivity [35], but they do not provide information about causality or about the form of the coupling functions. By contrast, however, we now show that inference of cross-frequency couplings based on a model of coupled phase oscillators [36,37] and dynamical Bayesian inference [19,38,39] enables us to infer the effective connectivity [35], i.e. to estimate the coupling functions and the underlying causality. We note that the effective connectivity was initially discussed in relation to the spatial segregation of brain functions [40] and is often used in this sense by the neuroscience community. In this work, we do not study spatial connectivity but, rather, we exploit the mathematical concept of effective connectivity in determining the influence that one oscillator exerts on another, under a particular model of causal dynamics [35,41]. With its ability to infer time-evolving coupled dynamics in the presence of noise, dynamical Bayesian inference is ideal for the calculation of effective connectivity from neuronal oscillations.
The signals derived from the chosen cross-frequency intervals are oscillatory, and their interactions can be studied effectively through their phase dynamics. We therefore consider a model of two coupled phase oscillators [36] described by the stochastic differential equation
2.1 |
with i≠j for i,j={1,2} and where ωi(t) is the parameter for the natural frequency. The deterministic part given by the base functions qi(ϕi,ϕj,t) describes the self and the interacting dynamics. The external stochastic dynamics ξi(t) is considered to be Gaussian white noise 〈ξi(t)ξj(τ)〉=δ(t−τ)Dij. Owing to the periodic nature of the deterministic dynamics, the base functions can be decomposed into infinite Fourier series . In practice, however, the dynamics is well described by a finite number of Fourier terms, so that one can rewrite the phase dynamics as , where , and the rest of Φi,k and are the K most important Fourier components. The Fourier components Φi,k act as base functions for the dynamical Bayesian inference, through which the parameters are evaluated. In the analysis, we used a second-order Fourier expansion (K=2). Two phase time-series and the order of expansion K act as inputs for the phase model which is inferred for each interaction (e.g. δ–α), from each subject.
Dynamical Bayesian inference [19,39] enables us to evaluate the model parameters , which give the time-evolving coupling functions and coupling strength in the presence of noise. From Bayes' theorem one can derive the minus log-likelihood function, which is of quadratic form. Assuming that the parameters are represented as a multivariate normal distribution (with mean , and covariance matrix Σ≡Ξ−1), and given such a distribution for the prior knowledge using the likelihood function, one can calculate recursively [19] the posterior distribution of the parameters using only the following four equations:
2.2 |
where summation over n=1,…,N is assumed, and summation over repeated indices k and w is implicit. We used informative priors and a special procedure for the propagation of information between consecutive data windows [19], which permitted the inference parameters that varied with time (for implementation and usage, see [42,43]). Given its ability to infer time-varying and noisy dynamics, our Bayesian method is especially well fitted for applications to EEG, ECG and respiration signals. A block diagram summarising the analysis procedure is provided in the electronic supplementary material.
Once we have the inferred parameters , we can calculate the coupling quantities and characteristics. The coupling functions are evaluated on a 2π×2π grid using the relevant base functions, i.e. Fourier components scaled by their inferred coupling parameters. The coupling strength is calculated as the Euclidean norm of the inferred parameters for a particular coupling [42]. The correlation ρ of the coupling parameters from two coupling functions gives the similarity of the forms of the coupling functions, irrespective of amplitude [20]. All coupling characteristics can be evaluated either for the net coupling, or for individual coupling components.
(b). Coupling decomposition model
The form of a coupling function depends on the differing contributions from individual oscillations. Changes in form may depend predominantly on only one of the phases (along one-axis), or they may depend on both phases, often resulting in a complicated and intuitively unclear dependance. This demonstrates the need for a model able to distinguish the individual functional contributions to a coupling. Accordingly, following the cardiorespiratory model [21], we present a generalized coupling decomposition model (figure 2).
Previous coupling treatments, including the cross-frequency coupling in neuroscience, have focused on the net coupling in one direction. Instead, we decompose the net coupling into two components depending on their functional roles: the direct and the indirect couplings (figure 2). Direct-coupling describes the influence of the direct (unidirectional) driving that one oscillator exerts on the other. Arguably, it is the most studied interaction in physiology, often linked to modulation mechanisms. We will see that direct-coupling is the dominant mechanism in most of the coupling functions. The second component, indirect-coupling, often called common-coupling, depends on the shared contributions of the two oscillators. The indirect coupling includes also the diffusive coupling given with the phase difference terms. The mechanism behind this coupling component (the small circle on the arrow in figure 2) can lie in some functional dependence from both of the current phase states, or it can be induced by a third system or process. Although we present the model in relation to phase dynamics, a similar functional decomposition of the couplings can also be applied to amplitude state dynamics.
In terms of the general theory of phase dynamics [36] (and equation (2.1)), the coupling function q1(ϕ1,ϕ2) can be expressed as the product of two functions
2.3 |
where Z1(ϕ1) is the phase response curve (PRC) of the first oscillator and shows how it responds to external perturbations, while I1(ϕ2) is the perturbation function through which the second oscillator acts on the first one. (The perturbation function is often given in a more general form like I1(ϕ1,ϕ2) [36]). In terms of equation (2.3), the direct-coupling component results from the existence of the constant part of the PRC Z1(ϕ1), while the common or indirect-coupling component results from the existence of the phase-dependent part of the PRC Z1(ϕ1) and the perturbation function I1(ϕ2).
(c). Subjects and protocol
We measured 25 awake and 29 anaesthetized healthy subjects, aged 18–60 years, who were about to undergo elective surgery, all of whom had given their informed consent in writing. The research was approved by the relevant research ethics committees in Norway and the UK. Of 29 anaesthetized subjects, 14 were anaesthetized with propofol and 15 with sevoflurane.
There are two sets of recordings for every subject: the first while the subject was awake and resting, and the second while anaesthetized with either propofol or sevoflurane by random choice. Propofol anaesthesia was induced by infusing propofol until a plasma target concentration of 6.0 μgm l−1 was reached [44]. A laryngeal mask airway was inserted 2 min after the start of the infusion. After insertion, the target concentration was reduced to 3.0 μgm l−1 and the infusion was maintained at this rate throughout the measurement period. Some of the propofol patients became restless during induction (while unconscious) and eight of them were given a small dose (50–100 μg) of the very short-lived (Thalf-life≃4 min.) opioid remifentanil during induction. Owing to the small dose and the short half-life, this would not have significantly affected the signals. The other group of subjects were asked to breathe 8% sevoflurane through a close-fitting facemask until an end-tidal concentration of 5% was reached. A laryngeal mask airway was inserted, and then the sevoflurane turned off until the end-tidal concentration fell to 2%. The sevoflurane was then reinstituted to maintain the end-tidal concentration at 2% throughout the measurement period. After a further stabilization period, the anaesthetized set of signal recordings took place. Subjects breathed spontaneously during both sets of recordings. The BIS EEG electrode was placed frontally on the forehead (similar to the FP1 electrode from the 10–20 international system). All data were recorded simultaneously using a Cardio & Brain Signals signal conditioning system (Jožef Stefan Institute, Ljubljana, Slovenia) specially designed for the BRACCIA study. Following 24-bit A/D conversion at 1200 Hz, the signals were stored on a computer for subsequent analysis. They included the three-lead ECG, and the respiration signal measured with a thorax-belt, as well as the frontal EEG signal. All were of 22–32 min duration. The analyses were performed on equal-length segments of 20 min.
(d). Signal processing and statistical analysis
The signals were first inspected visually, followed by automated artefact removal by interpolation. Data from subjects whose signals had many artefacts were disregarded and not analysed. The cross-frequency intervals were estimated by standard digital filtering procedures, including a FIR filter followed by a zero-phase digital filtering procedure (filtfilt in Matlab) to ensure that no time or phase lags were introduced by the filtering. The boundaries of the intervals extracted from the EEG signal were δ=0.8–4 Hz, θ=4–7.5 Hz, α=7.5–14 Hz, β=14–22 Hz and γ=22–100 Hz; the interval extracted from the respiration signal was r=0.145–0.6 Hz; and the extraction of the heart activity from the ECG signal was h=0.6–2 Hz. Wavelet power and coherence analyses, together with further clinical interpretation, will be presented elsewhere. For the EEG oscillations special care was taken in dealing with frequency spillage between intervals, heart artefacts and powerline artefacts [45].
The cardiac activity has been widely studied through heart rate variability (HRV) analysis [46]. Usually, the HRV signal is constructed by interpolation of the times of the R-peaks marked on an ECG signal, whence the variations in heart rate can be obtained up to a frequency of approximately 0.5 Hz, i.e. up to half of the main (fundamental) cardiac oscillation frequency at approximately 1 Hz. In this study, we focused on the coupled-oscillator approach [36,47], which meant that we required the cardiac main oscillation mode at approximately 1 Hz, which would of course get lost in an HRV estimation. By contrast, by band-filtration of the signal in the interval h=0.6–2 Hz around the main oscillation we were able to obtain well-defined phase estimates with intra-cycle resolution. Hence, we could analyse the phase interactions of the cardiac main oscillation mode, as required; we note, however, that this procedure would have led to the loss of some of the HRV variations, and especially those at the lower frequencies.
The phases of the filtered signals were estimated by use of the Hilbert transform [48], and the protophase-to-phase transformation [22] was then applied to the resultant protophases to obtain invariant observable-independent phases. To determine whether the coupling strength and coupling functions were not genuine, i.e. whether they happened by chance, the coupling of each of the relationships investigated was tested against intra- and intersubject surrogates [49]: the former were generated by randomizing the phase signals, and the latter by taking one of the phases from a different subject. In this way, the surrogates should be independent and any apparent coupling from the surrogate phases should be very low. From the large number of investigated relationships, only those exhibiting a statistically significant difference compared with their corresponding surrogates are discussed in the study. Similarly, for simplicity we present only the coupling in the predominant direction because that in the weaker direction was usually insignificant. To assess the statistical difference between groups of awake, propofol- and sevoflurane-anaesthetized subjects (and because of the non-normal distributions), we used the Wilcoxon statistical test, with p<0.05 considered as significant. The couplings were assessed independently; they did not form a statistical family and multiple comparison tests were not used. To present visually the differences between the distributions, we used standard boxplots which refer to the descriptive statistics (median, quartiles, maximum and minimum).
3. Results
(a). Cross-frequency coupling functions
The application of dynamical Bayesian inference to bivariate phase signals leads to the parameters of the coupled phase model, from which the coupling functions can then be reconstructed. For clarity, we first present in detail the coupling function for one relationship only—the delta–alpha coupling figure 3. Examples of the delta–alpha coupling function for single representative subjects in their awake and anaesthetized states are shown in figure 3a–c. In the averaged coupling function for all subjects (figure 3d–f) the inter-subject variations are averaged out, and the remaining coupling function signifies a functional form that represents a deterministic law for all of the subjects.
Comparison of the coupling function shapes for individual subjects (figure 3a–c) with the corresponding averages over all subjects (figure 3d–f) reveals considerable similarity between subjects. The coupling functions for awake resting [50], propofol and sevoflurane (figure 3a–f) are, however, quite different from each other, both in the form and strength of the coupling. The delta–alpha coupling function for the awake state has a relatively complex and varying form, and low amplitude. The coupling functions for propofol and sevoflurane are similar and they look significantly different from those for the awake state. The sevoflurane coupling function has the largest coupling amplitude. The qualitative form of the delta–alpha coupling function (figure 3f) has a sine-like wave form along the ϕδ-axis, while it is nearly constant along the ϕα-axis. This strongly implies that much of the delta–alpha coupling comes from the direct contribution of the delta oscillation. The specific form of the delta–alpha coupling function (e.g. figure 3f) reveals the underlying functional coupling mechanism, i.e. it shows that, when the delta oscillations are between π and 2π, the sine-wave coupling function is higher and the delta activity accelerates the alpha oscillations; similarly, when the delta oscillations are between 0 and π, the coupling function is decreased and delta decelerates the alpha oscillations.
In figure 4, we summarize our results for the coupling functions of all significant coupling relationships. They include the cross-frequency coupling functions that emerge within the brain, and between the brain, the lungs and the heart oscillations (figures with enhanced resolution are provided in the electronic supplementary material). The delta–alpha relationship is presented again for completeness and comparison. The theta–gamma coupling functions (figure 4e–h) have different forms, depending on the state of awakeness, with propofol and sevoflurane taking similar forms. The coupling amplitude of the propofol theta–gamma coupling (figure 4f) is lowest. The form of the functions looks like a second-order sine wave which changes predominantly along the ϕθ-axis. The alpha–gamma coupling functions (figure 4i–l) are of similar form, but their coupling amplitudes increase in anaesthesia, with the sevoflurane coupling function again being the highest. Interestingly, the qualitative form of the functions changes along both axes. This implies that the alpha–gamma coupling depends on both of the oscillations (alpha and gamma), or on the same indirect influence that affects them both.
The influence of respiration on brain theta oscillations is shown in figure 4m–p. There exist similarities in the form of the coupling functions between the awake and sevoflurane states, while the form of the propofol function seems qualitatively different. The direct influence of the phase ϕr of respiration is dominant in the awake and sevoflurane coupling functions. The coupling of the heart to theta oscillations is weak with a less-stable and time-varying form (figure 4q–t). The two anaesthetized heart–theta coupling functions are of similar form and are stronger than in the awake state. The strong coupling function between respiration and the heart oscillations (figure 4u–x) is the only one to have been studied previously, and our results confirm the earlier work [19–21]. More importantly, the propofol and sevoflurane anaesthesia made the form of the cardiorespiratory coupling function more time-varying and unstable—which is opposite to the effect of anaesthesia on the delta–alpha coupling (cf. figure 4a–d).
(b). Effect of anaesthesia on the coupling strength
In order to assess the influence of anaesthesia, we first quantify the coupling (amplitude) strength. The latter has been extensively studied in earlier work [13,15,45,51]: wherever reference was made to coupling causality and directionality, it was in fact the net coupling strength, or a measure proportional to it, that was being evaluated. Our coupling decomposition enables us to go beyond this by quantifying the coupling strengths of the individual components of the net coupling.
In figure 5, we summarize the changes of coupling strength induced by anaesthesia. The different effect on the separate coupling components is evident in the delta–alpha relationships shown in the top row of figure 5a–c. The net coupling with sevoflurane is significantly different from the awake and propofol states (figure 5a); for direct coupling all the states are different (figure 5b), while the indirect coupling for propofol was significantly the smallest (figure 5c). Note also that direct coupling is the dominant component of the net coupling. For the theta–gamma interaction, it is only the indirect coupling that differs between the awake and sevoflurane states (figure 5f). Anaesthesia increased significantly the net and indirect coupling strengths in alpha–gamma (figure 5g–i). This coupling is mostly defined by the indirect coupling component. The respiration–theta net coupling differed slightly between the two anaesthetics (figure 5j). Sevoflurane anaesthesia induced the strongest cardiorespiratory coupling strength, and this difference compared with other states is significant for all coupling types (figure 5m–o).
(c). The effect of anaesthesia on the form of coupling functions.
The other useful characterization of coupling functions is their functional form. It defines the functional law or mechanism and it is a specific feature of coupling functions. To quantify the forms of a given coupling relationship, we use a correlation measure that quantifies the similarity of the forms of two coupling functions, irrespective of their coupling strengths [20]. If the similarities of form for between the intersubject pairs for some interaction is high enough, it means that there exists a common deterministic functional form which underlies the mechanisms of that interaction. From the coupling decomposition model, we can investigate, separately, the similarity of form for each individual component of the coupling functions (figure 6).
The similarity in form of the delta–alpha coupling functions is shown in figure 6a–c. There is a large difference due to anaesthesia in the net and direct similarity (figure 6a,b), while the indirect similarity is different only for sevoflurane (figure 6c). The similarity of form is especially high for the direct component, while very low for the indirect component. The respiration–theta interaction had relatively small similarity of its functional forms, and there is only a small significant increase for sevoflurane in the net and direct similarities (figure 6d,e). The respiration-heart interaction also had all the significant differences seen in the net coupling, but now decreased with anaesthesia (figure 6g). The similarity of these interactions is mostly due to the high direct similarity (figure 6h), where awake is different from when under the two anaesthetics. We note that anaesthesia had opposite effects on the similarity of the functional forms for delta–alpha and respiration-heart—cf. figure 6a,g. This quantitative description is consistent with the observations of the coupling functions made in figure 4.
(d). The effect of anaesthesia on the noise strength
Dynamical Bayesian inference can decompose the dynamics into two parts: what is believed to be the deterministic part of the model; and a part originating from random (white) noise perturbations. The noise strength represents the level of random fluctuations relative to the frequency of the oscillation and its interactions with the other oscillations considered. So we also investigated whether and how anaesthesia affects the noise strength D of the brain and cardio-respiratory oscillations, with results as shown in figure 7. Correlated noise strengths, e.g. Dα,δ were found to be very small and not statistically different between the awakeness states, so they are not reported. Also, the noise strength for each of the intervals had (qualitatively) the same statistical difference when coupling was investigated with different intervals, e.g. Dα,α figure 7b was the same whether δ−α interactions or α−γ interactions were inferred.
The results in figure 7 demonstrate that the noise strength for some rhythms was unaffected by anaesthesia, Dδ,δ in figure 7a and Dh,h in figure 7f; for other rhythms anaesthesia made a significant difference, either increasing like Dθ,θ in figure 7c, or decreasing like Dα,α in figure 7b and Dγ,γ in figure 7d, with anaesthesia relative to the awake state; or the result was statistically different in all three states, like Dr,r in figure 7e.
4. Discussion
The present investigation relies on three complementary factors: (i) general anaesthesia under either intravenous and inhalational anaesthetics; (ii) the novel methodology of cross-frequency coupling functions to probe interaction mechanisms directly; and (iii) assessment of the combined dynamics and interactions of the cortical, respiratory and cardiac oscillations. We have thus been able to analyse the coupling functions between brain activity, which involves information processing and control of the human body, on the one side, and the cardiorespiratory systems, which take care of energy transport and the supplies of nutrients and oxygen, on the other.
While interactions have already been studied in lizards [52], mice [53], rats [15] and dogs [54], here we report the first insights into cardio-respiratory-cortical interactions in humans, in both the awake and anaesthetized states. Moreover, our extension of cross-frequency coupling to include the analysis of coupling functions has allowed us to investigate the interactions in greater depth by introducing the notion of the functional form, which represents a new dimension in the analysis of neuronal effective interactions. Thus, we have been able to present two quantitative dynamical properties of the phase interactions: the coupling strength and the form of the coupling function. The functional description of the couplings has enabled us to propose a coupling decomposition model that reveals the separate contributions, in turn providing deeper insight into the causality within a coupling. The model was strongly supported by the results (figures 5 and 6) where the effect of anaesthesia often differed for the individual coupling components.
Coupling functions describe the underlying mechanisms that gives rise to the qualitative states of the interacting systems, e.g. the phase synchronization state which is of great importance in neuronal [37] and cardiorespiratory [20,29] interactions. By knowing the form of the coupling function, one can predict the occurrence of phase synchronization for given parameters [16]. Although the discussion was only for large-scale cross-frequency couplings, the coupling functions presented have wide implications at different scales and levels of the heavily connected brain network [35]. Thus one can also describe the functional form of the edges, and can use the coupling decomposition model to investigate the separate contributions from the nodes.
One of the most prominent coupling relationships we identified is delta–alpha. It reflects how delta activity, associated with deep dreamless sleep [55], influences the alpha oscillations which are said to reduce the information processing [55,56] and play a key role in consciousness [57,58]. During the maintenance of general anaesthesia, the alpha and delta activities were increased [2,59]. The delta–alpha coupling has been linked to the coding mechanism of feedback valence information [60]. Even though the anaesthetized state differs from sleep and from the resting state generally, a strong delta–alpha link was observed during non-REM sleep [30] and recently it was suggested that delta–alpha coupling is mostly located within the frontal and the parieto-occipital regions when it is stronger during the eyes-closed state [13]. Our results are consistent with, and further extend and deepen, these findings. Namely, the form of the delta–alpha coupling functions (figures 3 and 6) indicates that the influence is direct modulation from delta to alpha, where the couplings are significantly stronger in anaesthesia than when awake. This shows that, once the subject is anaesthetized, delta activity influences the alpha oscillations by contributing to the reduction of information processing and integration.
Gamma activity, associated with attention, memory and sensory processing, is known to decrease in anaesthesia [61]. In seeking to reveal the underlying mechanisms, we identified two significant couplings: theta–gamma and alpha–gamma. They have been widely studied already, mostly with phase-to-power cross-frequency couplings and higher gamma intervals, and various functional roles have been attributed to them in different states and tasks [62,63]. It has been suggested that theta–gamma coupling plays a prominent role in memory tasks, whereas alpha–gamma interactions are more important for attention processing [64]. The coupling function analysis (figure 4) indicated that these two couplings are affected differently by anaesthesia. Namely, propofol decreased and sevoflurane increased the theta–gamma coupling, while both anaesthetics increased the alpha–gamma coupling (figures 4 and 5). These two couplings evidently have different functional mechanisms. The theta–gamma couplings in anaesthesia result from the direct influence of theta on gamma, while alpha–gamma is dominantly an indirect coupling, implying that there might be a third process which influences both of the oscillations.
We extended the analysis of cross-frequency neuronal couplings to include the interactions of two important parts of the cardiovascular system—the respiration and the heart [4,15]. We identified a coupling function from respiration to theta oscillations. The coupling function was of complex form, with strong direct component and relatively low intensity. The respiration–theta coupling was affected more by the sevoflurane than the propofol anaesthesia.
Of special interest are the brain–heart interactions as they have been linked to cardiac arrhythmias, psychophysiological coordination and vascular dementia [65–67]. Our analysis identified a coupling function from the heart to the brain theta oscillations. The form of the coupling function was relatively complex, its intensity was not very high, and the influence was predominantly with a direct component from the heart to the theta oscillations. This coupling function was not greatly affected by the onset of anaesthesia. The origin of the cardiac–theta couplings could be linked to the haemodynamic function of the heart in providing blood, together with oxygen and other metabolic substances, to the brain. Astrocytes and other glial cells might be responsible for mediation of these processes on the neural level [68,69].
The cardiorespiratory coupling function has been extensively studied [19–21] and its direct coupling component [21] and phase resetting curve [20] have been associated with respiratory sinus arrythmia. The functional connectivity of cardiorespiratory interactions was affected in different ways by propofol and sevoflurane anaesthesia [29]. Interestingly, we found that the effect of anaesthesia on the cardiorespiratory coupling functions showed that the coupling strength increased with anaesthesia, whereas the similarity of form decreased (cf. figures 5m and 6g). This indicates that the inter-subject similarity of forms becomes more varied with anaesthesia, while maintaining stable and strong interactions—perhaps reflecting the chronotaxic nature of the cardiorespiratory interactions [70].
These alterations of the cardiorespiratory coupling functions and their links to the theta brain oscillations (figures 5 and 6) may reflect partially the onset of analgesia and the reduced perception of pain [31,32], with possible links to consciousness. Therefore, such results could have implications for the quest of quantifying analgesia in the absence of consciousness [71,72].
The noise strength analysis in figure 7 shows that anaesthesia changes, not only the deterministic couplings, but also some of the random fluctuations acting on the oscillations. The decrease of the noise level in α, γ and respiratory oscillations (figure 7b,d,e) might be a consequence of the higher determinism associated with the onset of anaesthesia which induces, e.g. order, coupling and coherence of the oscillations ([2,15] and figure 5). More puzzling is the result that the noise strength for θ oscillations increased with anaesthesia, figure 7c. It may perhaps be linked to the origin of the θ oscillation and its role in the hippocampus [73]. These results are intriguing and invite further investigation using dynamical Bayesian inference, which has clearly demonstrated its potential for studies of this kind as well as for the analysis of (biological) experiments of a stochastic nature quite generally.
Unconsciousness is the most striking change in the state of a subject when general anaesthesia occurs [1,2,74]. The transition to unconsciousness and back can be traced through assessment of the cognitive EEG dynamics [9,10,75] and the recovery of consciousness has been found to differ in elderly subjects [76]. It has been noted that the standard clinical assessments of consciousness (motor, verbal and eye-opening responses [77]) are not sufficient and that there is a need for techniques which also assess the function and effective connectivity [1]. Our statistical comparisons of coupling functions in the awake and anaesthetized states demonstrate that there are significant differences, especially for the delta–alpha and alpha–gamma couplings (figures 3, 5 and 6). These coupling-induced changes of the phase advanced/delayed oscillations alter the attention and memory processes, and suppress information integration which is known for mediating the unconscious state [1].
The roles of propofol and sevoflurane in the induction of unconsciousness as a common mechanism was studied and power differences were outlined [78,75]. Our coupling function results have revealed that these anaesthetics often exhibit similar functional forms, perhaps implying similar mechanisms (figures 3 and 4), but that there are some quantitative differences (figures 5 and 6). In general, we observe similar forms of coupling function, but the strength and effect were significantly stronger for sevoflurane. This could be on account of the doses used. It is also possible that the molecular and neuronal processes associated with propofol and sevoflurane are largely similar, perhaps because both act on the same receptor (e.g. GABAA) but that there are minor differences in relation to the potassium channels affected [1,5,6].
In conclusion, coupling functions have enabled us to unveil a new perspective on how the neurophysiological mechanisms are affected by general anaesthesia. This initial application has been in a sense overwhelming in that we have identified six important and very illuminating coupling relationships. This was partly because we analysed not only neuronal oscillations, but also how the latter are affected by cardiorespiratory activity. The work has opened the door to a host of new questions and problems needing to be tackled. For example, can one apply coupling function analysis to assess spatial neuronal couplings using additional EEG electrodes, perhaps using different anaesthetics? The possibility of following time-evolving dynamics could lead to new insights based on studies of how the evolution of the coupling functions mechanisms lead to unconsciousness. Coupling functions can also be used to study the mechanisms of other neurophysiological perturbations, as well as to revisit known problems, states and diseases in order to reveal the underlying functional mechanisms. Needless to say, the findings and the methodology of this work also have wide implications for coupled oscillators in general, with the possibility of biomimetic [79] solutions to a diversity of difficult problems [80,23].
Acknowledgements
We thank the entire BRACCIA team for excellent collaboration and in particular to J. Petrovčič for designing the Cardio & Brain Signal Conditioning Unit, to B. Musizza for creating a LabView software platform to collect the data and to T. Draegni, S. A. Landswerk, P. Kvandal, D. A. Kenwright, L. W. Sheppard and M. Entwistle for their help in collecting the data.
Author' contributions
T.S. analysed the data, contributed to the interpretation and wrote the manuscript. S.P. analysed the time-frequency properties of the signals and helped define phases within each oscillatory interval. J.R. was a clinical group leader, managed the measurements in the Oslo University Hospital, and contributed to the physiological interpretation of the results. A.F.S. was a clinical group leader and managed the measurements in the Royal Lancaster Infirmary. P.V.E.Mc.C. helped in the execution and management of the research and editing the manuscript. A.S. conceived the study, was Coordinator of the EC FP6 grant BRACCIA and the EPSRC grant on Non-autonomous Dynamics that funded the research, was involved in discussion and interpretation of the results and edited the manuscript.
Competing interests
The author(s) declare they have no competing interests.
Funding
This work was supported by the European Union as a NEST (New and Emerging Science and Technology) Project, no. 517133, ‘Brain, Respiratory and Cardiac Causalities in Anaesthesia’ (BRACCIA), by the Engineering and Physical Sciences Research Council (EPSRC) UK (grant no. EP/100999X1) and in part by the ARRS Slovenia (program no. P20232).
References
- 1.Alkire MT, Hudetz AG, Tononi G. 2008. Consciousness and anesthesia. Science 322, 876–880. ( 10.1126/science.1149213) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Schwartz RS, Brown EN, Lydic R, Schiff ND. 2010. General anesthesia, sleep, and coma. New Engl. J. Med. 363, 2638–2650. ( 10.1056/NEJMra0808281) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Smith D, Andrzejowski J, Smith A. 2013. Certainty and uncertainty: NICE guidance on ‘depth of anaesthesia’ monitoring. Anaesthesia 68, 1000–1005. ( 10.1111/anae.12385) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Stefanovska A. 2007. Coupled oscillators: complex but not complicated cardiovascular and brain interactions. IEEE Eng. Med. Bio. Mag. 26, 25–29. ( 10.1109/EMB.2007.907088) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Rudolph U, Antkowiak B. 2004. Molecular and neuronal substrates for general anaesthetics. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 5, 709–720. ( 10.1038/nrn1496) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Franks NP. 2008. General anaesthesia: from molecular targets to neuronal pathways of sleep and arousal. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 370–386. ( 10.1038/nrn2372) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Katoh T, Suzuki A, Ikeda K. 1998. Electroencephalographic derivatives as a tool for predicting the depth of sedation and anesthesia induced by sevoflurane. Anesthesiology 88, 642–650. ( 10.1097/00000542-199803000-00014) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Hoymork S, Raeder J. 2005. Why do women wake up faster than men from propofol anaesthesia? Br. J. Anaesth. 95, 627–633. ( 10.1093/bja/aei245) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Purdon PL. et al. 2013. Electroencephalogram signatures of loss and recovery of consciousness from propofol. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 110, E1142–E1151. ( 10.1073/pnas.1221180110) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Blain-Moraes S, Tarnal V, Vanini G, Alexander A, Rosen D, Shortal B, Janke E, Mashour GA. 2015. Neurophysiological correlates of sevoflurane-induced unconsciousness. Anesthesiology 122, 307–316. ( 10.1097/ALN.0000000000000482) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Traub RD, Whittington MA, Colling SB, Buzsaki G, Jefferys JGR. 1996. Analysis of gamma rhythms in the rat hippocampus in vitro and in vivo. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 493, 471–484. ( 10.1113/jphysiol.1996.sp021397) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Klausberger T, Magill PJ, Márton LF, Roberts JDB, Cobden PM, Buzsáki G, Somogyi P. 2003. Brain-state-and cell-type-specific firing of hippocampal interneurons in vivo. Nature 421, 844–848. ( 10.1038/nature01374) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Jirsa V, Müller V. 2013. Cross-frequency coupling in real and virtual brain networks. Front. Comput. Neurosci. 7, 78 ( 10.3389/fncom.2013.00078) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Lakatos P, Karmos G, Mehta AD, Ulbert I, Schroeder CE. 2008. Entrainment of neuronal oscillations as a mechanism of attentional selection. Science 320, 110–113. ( 10.1126/science.1154735) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Musizza B, Stefanovska A, McClintock PVE, Paluš M, Petrovčič J, Ribarič S, Bajrović FF. 2007. Interactions between cardiac, respiratory, and EEG-δ oscillations in rats during anaesthesia. J. Physiol. (Lond.) 580, 315–326. ( 10.1113/jphysiol.2006.126748) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Kiss IZ, Zhai Y, Hudson JL. 2005. Predicting mutual entrainment of oscillators with experiment-based phase models. Phys. Rev. Lett. 94, 248301 ( 10.1103/PhysRevLett.94.248301) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Pereira T, Eroglu D, Bagci GB, Tirnakli U, Jensen HJ. 2013. Connectivity-driven coherence in complex networks. Phys. Rev. Lett. 110, 234103 ( 10.1103/PhysRevLett.110.234103) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Komarov M, Pikovsky A. 2013. Multiplicity of singular synchronous states in the kuramoto model of coupled oscillators. Phys. Rev. Lett. 111, 204101 ( 10.1103/PhysRevLett.111.204101) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Stankovski T, Duggento A, McClintock PVE, Stefanovska A. 2012. Inference of time-evolving coupled dynamical systems in the presence of noise. Phys. Rev. Lett. 109, 024101 ( 10.1103/PhysRevLett.109.024101) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Kralemann B. et al. 2013. In vivo cardiac phase response curve elucidates human respiratory heart rate variability. Nat. Commun. 4, 2418 ( 10.1038/ncomms3418) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Iatsenko D, Bernjak A, Stankovski T, Shiogai Y, Owen-Lynch PJ, Clarkson PBM, McClintock PVE, Stefanovska A. 2013. Evolution of cardiorespiratory interactions with age. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. A 371, 20110622 ( 10.1098/rsta.2011.0622) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Kralemann B, Cimponeriu L, Rosenblum M, Pikovsky A, Mrowka R. 2008. Phase dynamics of coupled oscillators reconstructed from data. Phys. Rev. E 77, 066205 ( 10.1103/PhysRevE.77.066205) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Stankovski T, McClintock PVE, Stefanovska A. 2014. Coupling functions enable secure communications. Phys. Rev. X 4, 011026 ( 10.1103/PhysRevX.4.011026) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Stefanovska A, Bračič M. 1999. Physics of the human cardiovascular system. Contemp. Phys. 40, 31–55. ( 10.1080/001075199181693) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Kanaya N, Hirata N, Kurosawa S, Nakayama M, Namiki A. 2003. Differential effects of propofol and sevoflurane on heart rate variability. Anesthesiology 98, 34–40. ( 10.1097/00000542-200301000-00009) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Chen Z, Purdon PL, Harrell G, Pierce ET, Walsh J, Brown EN, Barbieri R. 2011. Dynamic assessment of baroreflex control of heart rate during induction of propofol anesthesia using a point process method. Ann. Biomed. Eng. 39, 260–276. ( 10.1007/s10439-010-0179-z) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Dorantes Mendez G, Aletti F, Toschi N, Canichella A, Dauri M, Coniglione F, Guerrisi M, Signorini MG, Cerutti S, Ferrario M. 2013. Baroreflex sensitivity variations in response to propofol anesthesia: comparison between normotensive and hypertensive patients. J. Clin. Mon. Comput. 27, 417–426. ( 10.1007/s10877-012-9426-1) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Valenza G. 2014. Instantaneous monitoring of heart beat dynamics during anesthesia and sedation. J. Comput. Surg. 3, 1–18. ( 10.1186/s40244-014-0013-2)26702394 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Kenwright D. et al. 2015. The discriminatory value of cardiorespiratory interactions in distinguishing awake from anaesthetised states: a randomised observational study. Anaesthesia 70, 1356–1368. () [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Bashan A, Bartsch RP, Kantelhardt JW, Havlin S, Ivanov PC. 2012. Network physiology reveals relations between network topology and physiological function. Nat. Commun. 3, 702 ( 10.1038/ncomms1705) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Randich A, Maixner W. 1984. Interactions between cardiovascular and pain regulatory systems. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 8, 343–367. ( 10.1016/0149-7634(84)90057-5) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Bruehl S, Chung OY. 2004. Interactions between the cardiovascular and pain regulatory systems: an updated review of mechanisms and possible alterations in chronic pain. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 28, 395–414. ( 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.06.004) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Jeanne M, Logier R, De Jonckheere J, Tavernier B. 2009. Heart rate variability during total intravenous anesthesia: effects of nociception and analgesia. Autonom. Neurosci. 147, 91–96. ( 10.1016/j.autneu.2009.01.005) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Storm H, Støen R, Klepstad P, Skorpen F, Qvigstad E, Raeder J. 2013. Nociceptive stimuli responses at different levels of general anaesthesia and genetic variability. Acta Anaesth. Scand. 57, 89–99. ( 10.1111/aas.12017) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Park HJ, Friston K. 2013. Structural and functional brain networks: from connections to cognition. Science 342, 1238411 ( 10.1126/science.1238411) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Kuramoto Y. 1984. Chemical oscillations, waves, and turbulence. Berlin, Germany: Springer. [Google Scholar]
- 37.Varela F, Lachaux JP, Rodriguez E, Martinerie J. 2001. The brainweb: phase synchronization and large-scale integration. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2, 229–239. ( 10.1038/35067550) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.von Toussaint U. 2011. Bayesian inference in physics. Rev. Mod. Phys. 83, 943–999. ( 10.1103/RevModPhys.83.943) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Stankovski T, Ticcinelli V, McClintock PVE, Stefanovska A. 2015. Coupling functions in networks of oscillators. New J. Phys. 17, 035002 ( 10.1088/1367-2630/17/3/035002) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Friston KJ. 2011. Functional and effective connectivity: a review. Brain Connect. 1, 13–36. ( 10.1089/brain.2011.0008) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Kralemann B, Pikovsky A, Rosenblum M. 2014. Reconstructing effective phase connectivity of oscillator networks from observations. New J. Phys. 16, 085013 ( 10.1088/1367-2630/16/8/085013) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Stankovski T, Duggento A, McClintock PVE, Stefanovska A. 2014. A tutorial on time-evolving dynamical Bayesian inference. Eur. Phys. J. Spec. Top. 223, 2685–2703. ( 10.1140/epjst/e2014-02286-7) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Matlab toolbox for the dynamical Bayesian inference and the calculation of coupling functions is publicly available at http://py-biomedical.lancaster.ac.uk.
- 44.Marsh B, White M, Morton N, Kenny G. 1991. Pharmacokinetic model driven infusion of propofol in children. Br. J. Anaesth. 67, 41–48. ( 10.1093/bja/67.1.41) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Lehnertz K, Ansmann G, Bialonski S, Dickten H, Geier C, Porz S. 2014. Evolving networks in the human epileptic brain. Phys. D 267, 7–15. ( 10.1016/j.physd.2013.06.009) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology and others. 1996. Heart rate variability standards of measurement, physiological interpretation, and clinical use. Eur. Heart J. 17, 354–381. ( 10.1093/oxfordjournals.eurheartj.a014868) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Pikovsky A, Rosenblum M, Kurths J. 2001. Synchronization—a universal concept in nonlinear sciences. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 48.Gabor D. 1946. Theory of communication. J. IEEE 93, 429–457. [Google Scholar]
- 49.Schreiber T, Schmitz A. 2000. Surrogate time series. Phys. D 142, 346–382. ( 10.1016/S0167-2789(00)00043-9) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Deco G, Jirsa VK, McIntosh AR. 2010. Emerging concepts for the dynamical organization of resting-state activity in the brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 12, 43–56. ( 10.1038/nrn2961) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Canolty RT, Edwards E, Dalal SS, Soltani M, Nagarajan SS, Kirsch HE, Berger MS, Barbaro NM, Knight RT. 2006. High gamma power is phase-locked to theta oscillations in human neocortex. Science 313, 1626–1628. ( 10.1126/science.1128115) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.De Vera L, González J, Pereda E. 2000. Relationship between cortical electrical and cardiac autonomic activities in the awake lizard, Gallotia galloti. J. Exp. Zool. 287, 21–28. () [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Ito J. et al. 2014. Whisker barrel cortex delta oscillations and gamma power in the awake mouse are linked to respiration. Nat. Comm. 5, 3572 ( 10.1038/ncomms4572) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Lambertz M, Langhorst P. 1998. Simultaneous changes of rhythmic organization in brainstem neurons, respiration, cardiovascular system and EEG between 0.05 hz and 0.5 hz. J. Auton. Nerv. Syst. 68, 58–77. ( 10.1016/S0165-1838(97)00126-4) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Feinberg I, Floyd TC, March JD. 1987. Effects of sleep loss on delta (0.3–3 hz) EEG and eye movement density: new observations and hypotheses. Electroenceph. Clin. Neurophysiol. 67, 217–221. ( 10.1016/0013-4694(87)90019-8) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Pfurtscheller G, Lopes da Silva FH. 1999. Event-related EEG/MEG synchronization and desynchronization: basic principles. Clin. Neurophysiol. 110, 1842–1857. ( 10.1016/S1388-2457(99)00141-8) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Palva S, Palva JM. 2007. New vistas for α-frequency band oscillations. Trends Neurosci. 30, 150–158. ( 10.1016/j.tins.2007.02.001) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Klimesch W. 1999. Eeg alpha and theta oscillations reflect cognitive and memory performance: a review and analysis. Brain Res. Rev. 29, 169–195. ( 10.1016/S0165-0173(98)00056-3) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Li D, Li X, Hagihira S, Sleigh JW. 2013. Cross-frequency coupling during isoflurane anaesthesia as revealed by electroencephalographic harmonic wavelet bicoherence. Br. J. Anaesth. 110, 409–419. ( 10.1093/bja/aes397) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Cohen MX, Axmacher N, Lenartz D, Elger CE, Sturm V, Schlaepfer TE. 2009. Good vibrations: cross-frequency coupling in the human nucleus accumbens during reward processing. J. Cognit. Neurosci. 21, 875–889. ( 10.1162/jocn.2009.21062) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.John ER, Prichep LS, Kox W, Valds-Sosa P, Bosch-Bayard J, Aubert E, Tom M, diMichele F, Gugino LD. 2001. Invariant reversible QEEG effects of anesthetics. Conscious. Cogn. 10, 165–183. ( 10.1006/ccog.2001.0507) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Jensen O, Colgin LL. 2007. Cross-frequency coupling between neuronal oscillations. Trends Cogn. Sci. 11, 267–269. ( 10.1016/j.tics.2007.05.003) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Voytek B, Canolty RT, Shestyuk A, Crone NE, Parvizi J, Knight RT. 2010. Shifts in gamma phase–amplitude coupling frequency from theta to alpha over posterior cortex during visual tasks. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 4, ( 10.3389/fnhum.2010.00191). 191. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Ward LM. 2003. Synchronous neural oscillations and cognitive processes. Trends Cognit. Sci. 7, 553–559. ( 10.1016/j.tics.2003.10.012) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Zipes DP. 2008. Heart-brain interactions in cardiac arrhythmias: role of the autonomic nervous system. Clevel. Clin. J. Med. 75, S94–S96. ( 10.3949/ccjm.75.Suppl_2.S94) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.McCraty R, Atkinson M, Tomasino D, Bradley RT. 2009. The coherent heart: heart-brain interactions, psychophysiological coherence, and the emergence of system-wide order. Integral Rev. 5, 110–115. [Google Scholar]
- 67.Román GC. 2004. Brain hypoperfusion: a critical factor in vascular dementia. Neurol. Res. 26, 454–458. ( 10.1179/016164104225017686) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Zonta M. et al. 2003. Neuron-to-astrocyte signaling is central to the dynamic control of brain microcirculation. Nat. Neurosci. 6, 43–50. ( 10.1038/nn980) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Haydon PG, Carmignoto G. 2006. Astrocyte control of synaptic transmission and neurovascular coupling. Physiol. Rev. 86, 1009–1031. ( 10.1152/physrev.00049.2005) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Suprunenko YF, Clemson PT, Stefanovska A. 2013. Chronotaxic systems: a new class of self-sustained nonautonomous oscillators. Phys. Rev. Lett. 111, 024101 ( 10.1103/PhysRevLett.111.024101) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Price DD. 2015. Unconscious and conscious mediation of analgesia and hyperalgesia. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 112, 7624–7625. ( 10.1073/pnas.1508765112) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Jensen K, Kirsch I, Odmalm S, Kaptchuk TJ, Ingvar M. 2015. Classical conditioning of analgesic and hyperalgesic pain responses without conscious awareness. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 25, 7863–7867. ( 10.1073/pnas.1504567112) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Buzsáki G. 2002. Theta oscillations in the hippocampus. Neuron 33, 325–340. ( 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00586-X) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Mashour GA. 2004. Consciousness unbound: toward a paradigm of general anesthesia. Anesthesiology 100, 428–433. ( 10.1097/00000542-200402000-00035) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Gugino L, Chabot R, Prichep L, John E, Formanek V, Aglio L. 2001. Quantitative EEG changes associated with loss and return of consciousness in healthy adult volunteers anaesthetized with propofol or sevoflurane. Br. J. Anaesth. 87, 421–428. ( 10.1093/bja/87.3.421) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Canet J. et al. 2003. Cognitive dysfunction after minor surgery in the elderly. Acta Anaesth. Scand. 47, 1204–1210. ( 10.1046/j.1399-6576.2003.00238.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Teasdale G, Jennett B. 1974. Assessment of coma and impaired consciousness: a practical scale. Lancet 304, 81–84. ( 10.1016/S0140-6736(74)91639-0) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Harris RS, Lazar O, Johansen JW, Sebel PS. 2006. Interaction of propofol and sevoflurane on loss of consciousness and movement to skin incision during general anesthesia. Anesthesiology 104, 1170–1175. ( 10.1097/00000542-200606000-00011) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Vincent JFV, Bogatyreva OA, Bogatyrev NR, Bowyer A, Pahl A. 2006. Biomimetics: its practice and theory. J. R. Soc. Interface 3, 471–482. ( 10.1098/rsif.2006.0127) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Hopfield JJ. 1982. Neural networks and physical systems with emergent collective computational abilities. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 79, 2554–2558. ( 10.1073/pnas.79.8.2554) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]