Abstract
Background
Changes in reflexive emotional responses are hallmarks of depression, but how emotional reflexes make an impact on adaptive decision-making in depression has not been examined formally. Using a Pavlovian-instrumental transfer (PIT) task, we compared the influence of affectively valenced stimuli on decision-making in depression and generalized anxiety disorder compared with healthy controls; and related this to the longitudinal course of the illness.
Method
A total of 40 subjects with a current DSM-IV-TR diagnosis of major depressive disorder, dysthymia, generalized anxiety disorder, or a combination thereof, and 40 matched healthy controls performed a PIT task that assesses how instrumental approach and withdrawal behaviours are influenced by appetitive and aversive Pavlovian conditioned stimuli (CSs). Patients were followed up after 4–6 months. Analyses focused on patients with depression alone (n = 25).
Results
In healthy controls, Pavlovian CSs exerted action-specific effects, with appetitive CSs boosting active approach and aversive CSs active withdrawal. This action-specificity was absent in currently depressed subjects. Greater action-specificity in patients was associated with better recovery over the follow-up period.
Conclusions
Depression is associated with an abnormal influence of emotional reactions on decision-making in a way that may predict recovery.
Key words: Decision-making, emotional reactions, major depressive disorder, Pavlovian-instrumental transfer
Introduction
Computational theories of valuation provide a quantitative framework linking emotions to choices. They have carved out distinguishable but interacting decision-making systems (Daw & Dayan, 2014) including a reflective goal-directed system where choices are flexibly based on a prospective consideration of likely outcomes, and an ‘automatic’ Pavlovian system that inflexibly mandates evolutionarily ingrained reflex responses to emotional stimuli (Dayan et al. 2006; Guitart-Masip et al. 2011). In the context of depression, the interaction between these systems is particularly worth investigating as it might provide a quantitative handle on how reflexive emotional responses shape reflective cognitions, and vice versa (Huys et al. 2015).
Depression appears to alter reflexive emotional responses. In the appetitive domain, depression reduces reflexively evoked approach responses to a variety of appetitive stimuli such as films, images or monetary incentives (Rottenberg, 2005; Steele et al. 2007; Bylsma et al. 2008; Eshel & Roiser, 2010). If approach to positive situations or stimuli no longer occurs reflexively, but only after effortful reflection, then this might reduce the perceived ease of earning rewards and their experienced prevalence, both aspects of anhedonia. On the aversive side, an impairment in Pavlovian forms of behavioural inhibition (possibly mediated by serotonin: Dayan & Huys, 2008; Crockett et al. 2009; Geurts et al. 2013a) could suppress an automatic avoidance of potentially stressful situations and help explain why depression seems to both cause and be caused by stressful life events (Kendler et al. 1999, 2000).
Importantly, failures in reflexive Pavlovian approach or avoidance might have consequences for the internal working of other aspects of emotion (Huys et al. 2012): Because internal choices about what to think about are in many ways similar to external actions about what to do (Anderson & Oates, 2007; Huys et al. 2015), failures in internally directed Pavlovian approach might reduce positive emotion regulation strategies, such as the tendency to imagine positive events to up-regulate one's mood (Joormann & Vanderlind, 2014). A failure to consider positive explanations of events reflexively might contribute to negative cognitive distortions. Conversely, a reflexive sense of overwhelming failure might prevent reflective problem-solving (Elliott et al. 1997).
How reflexive Pavlovian responses interact with, and influence, other decision-making processes has yet to be studied explicitly in the setting of depression. To address this lacuna, we employed a Pavlovian-instrumental transfer (PIT) task and studied the influence of reflexive responses to incidental valued stimuli on ongoing deliberations about whether to approach and withdraw.
The task consisted of two blocks, each in three parts (Fig. 1 and online Supplement S1). One block was an instrumental approach block where subjects first learned whether or not to collect mushrooms. Specifically, in this instrumental training, they were presented with six mushrooms and rewarded or punished for either collecting three mushrooms or not collecting the other three by approaching or not approaching them (Fig. 1a). Next, Pavlovian compound conditioned stimuli (CSs) were trained (Fig. 1b). In the PIT component itself, subjects were told to continue to choose either to collect or leave the mushrooms, but now the background was tiled with task-irrelevant Pavlovian CSs and outcomes were no longer explicitly presented (Fig. 1c). The PIT effect in the approach block is a bias in instrumental performance due to the presence of the Pavlovian CS. Even though the Pavlovian CS was actually uninformative about whether the mushroom should be collected or not, control subjects approached (collected) more mushrooms when the Pavlovian CS was positively valued and fewer when the Pavlovian CS was negatively valued (Huys et al. 2011).
Exactly the opposite was observed in the withdrawal block (Huys et al. 2011), which had the same sequence of three parts, except that subjects were asked to decide whether or not to throw away a mushroom by withdrawing from it, or keeping it by doing nothing (Fig. 1e and f ). While in the approach block the active response involved movement towards the mushroom, in the withdrawal block it involved movement away from the mushroom. It was otherwise exactly the same. We previously found that, during the PIT part of the withdrawal block, appetitive CSs reduced the tendency to throw away mushrooms, while aversive CSs increased it (Huys et al. 2011). Hence the PIT effect was action-specific: when the active behaviour being modulated was approach, appetitive CSs promoted and aversive CSs inhibited it, while the opposite was true when withdrawal was being modulated.
We hypothesized that depression would reduce both appetitive and aversive PIT during approach. In addition, we examined whether reflexive Pavlovian processes might afford a less specific and more general guidance of instrumental choices by incidental valued stimuli by also testing whether action specificity is reduced. Finally, because we expected behaviourally observable PIT to parallel the internal biases of cognition by emotion, we predicted that these variables would be related to the longitudinal course of the disorder.
We recruited healthy controls and subjects with major depressive disorder (MDD) or dysthymia (DTH) in a naturalistic longitudinal follow-up study. In an attempt to examine the specificity of the findings with respect to depression, we also attempted to recruit patients with co-morbid anxiety (generalized anxiety disorder; GAD) and depression (MDD/DTH), and with anxiety (GAD) alone. All patients were re-contacted after 4 months to assess the state dependence of any effects that distinguished patients and controls at initial contact, as well as to examine whether these effects were related to future symptom course. As insufficient anxiety patients could be recruited, this report focuses on the depression dataset.
Method
Subjects and procedure
Patients were recruited from the Berlin area while in-patients at the Charité Hospital, via their community psychiatrists, or via self-referral through advertisements. Controls were recruited via advertisements and email alerts.
Prior to the experimental session, subjects completed the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR Axis I Disorders, Research Version, Patient Edition (SCID-I/P; First et al. 2002b) screening questionnaire, and the sections on mood disorder and GAD. Inclusion criteria were age 18–65 years and satisfying criteria for a current MDD or DTH or GAD. After the experimental session, all subjects underwent a full structured diagnostic interview performed by trained raters (First et al. 2002a, b ). Self- and observer-rated scales (Hamilton, 1960; Beck et al. 1996a, 1988) and intelligence quotient (IQ) (German vocabulary test Wortschatztest; Schmidt & Metzler, 1992) were obtained at this point. For patients, exclusion criteria included Axis I disorders other than GAD, MDD or DTH, and for controls any current or past Axis I diagnosis. Additional exclusion criteria for all subjects were neurological, endocrine and cardiac disorder or use of drugs of abuse in the past 6 months. Patients were re-contacted after 4 months, and retested at T2 between 4 and 6 months after initial testing at T1. All testing sessions including follow-up were performed by E.F. and M.G. between February 2009 and November 2010 (inter-rater reliability on 10 patients assessed by both: κ = 0.92). All participants gave written informed consent and ethical approval was obtained from the ethics committee of Charité – Universitätsmedizin Berlin.
A total of 45 patients were tested on the task (Fig. 2). Analyses focused on the 28 patients without anxiety (MDD n = 26 or DTH n = 2) because insufficient patients with pure GAD (n = 1) or co-morbid GAD + depression (GAD + MDD n = 10 and GAD + DTH n = 1) could be recruited. Two MDD subjects were excluded due to performance-related issues (online Supplement S3) and one DTH subject post-hoc due to low symptom scores [Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) score <11 at T1]. Two of the patients were unavailable for follow-up, resulting in a final sample of 25 depressed patients at T1, 23 depressed patients at T2, and 40 healthy controls. Results including patients with co-morbid anxiety are included in the online Supplementary material. A total of 61 healthy controls were tested. Data for six controls were lost (three technical errors, three incomplete). Of the controls, 46 were part of our previously published PIT study (Huys et al. 2011). To allow for matching, nine healthy males over 35 years had to be added. From the total pool of 55 controls we extracted the 40 that matched the 40 patients best for age, sex and education (online Supplement S2).
Analysis
Pavlovian and instrumental training
The number of correct choices on the Pavlovian free choice trials (Fig. 1c) was compared with chance level (0.5) for each subject individually using a binomial test.
Acquisition of instrumental training was assessed by examining the overall number of correct responses and comparing this with chance (50%). To examine the rate of learning, we computed a learning curve for each stimulus separately, averaged these within each subject and fitted a linear function of time to this learning curve. Tests were performed as in the Statistical tests section.
PIT behaviour
We computed the proportion of go responses in the presence of each of the five valenced Pavlovian CSs in the approach and withdrawal blocks separately. This analysis averaged over all instrumental stimuli within a block and was orthogonal to the relative value of go and no-go instrumental stimuli. Appetitive approach PIT was measured as the linear regression coefficient of the go probability across all instrumental stimuli for neutral and positive (0, +, ++) CSs. For aversive approach PIT, we regressed the go probability across all instrumental stimuli on neutral and negative (0, –,– –) CS values. For each action frame (approach and withdrawal), we then performed a single linear regression over all five CS values. Action specificity was calculated as the difference between the linear regression coefficients in approach and withdrawal blocks.
Planned analyses involved comparisons between controls and patients at T1, and between patients at T1 and T2 on action specificity, and on approach appetitive and approach aversive PIT.
Longitudinal course
The relationship of behavioural measures to longitudinal course was assessed by relating T1 behavioural measures to T2 depression scores. To control for baseline scores, we also regressed T2 scores onto T1 scores and asked whether behavioural measures were related to the residuals.
For categorical analyses, improvers were patients with a MDD or DTH diagnosis at T1 who achieved either a reduction in BDI scores greater than the median (a reduction of nine BDI points or more), or whose T2 BDI score was <50% of their T1 BDI score.
Statistical tests
Analyses were performed in Matlab. Outlier data points, i.e. data points >3 standard deviations from the relevant mean, were removed prior to performing any test. If there was a significant departure from Gaussianity (p < 0.05, Kolmogorov–Smirnoff test), we performed non-parametric tests (Wilcoxon signed-rank tests to test against null effects and Mann–Whitney U tests to compare populations) and otherwise t tests. For the effects of time we computed the statistics in a paired manner. Residual BDI scores at time T2 were computed by retaining the residuals after linearly regressing BDI T2 onto BDI T1 scores.
Results
Table 1 shows subject characteristics. Controls and patients were matched for age, sex, IQ and education (online Supplement S2). Roughly half the patients were medication-free. Patients were, on average, moderately depressed at T1 and only mildly depressed at T2 (Beck et al. 1996b). Patients and controls acquired instrumental and Pavlovian contingencies equally and well at T1 and patients learned the instrumental task on retesting at T2 faster than at T1 (online Supplement S3).
Table 1.
Controls | Patients T1 | Patients T2 | |
---|---|---|---|
n | 40 | 25 | 23 |
Male, % | 38 | 32 | 30 |
Age, years | 27.3 (7) | 28.3 (8) | |
Education, years | 12.8 (1.7) | 13.3 (1.6) | |
IQ | 114.2 (10.1) | 117.3 (10.6) | 117.8 (10.6) |
BDI-II | 2.8 (3.7) | 23.4 (8.6)* | 14.5 (10.7)† |
HAM-D | 0.8 (1.4) | 17.3 (5.6)* | 9.1 (7.0)† |
BAI | 4.3 (3.8) | 13.5 (7.8)* | 9.7 (7.4)‡ |
HAM-A | 0.6 (1.2) | 14.8 (5.1)* | 7.6 (6.8)† |
Medication status, n | |||
None | 40 | 13 | 13 |
SSRI | 0 | 3 | 2 |
5-HT | 0 | 3 | 3 |
Other | 0 | 6 | 5 |
Data are given as mean (standard deviation) unless otherwise indicated.
IQ, Intelligence quotient; BDI, Beck Depression Inventory-II; HAM-D, Hamilton Depression Rating Scale; BAI, Beck Anxiety Index; HAM-A, Hamilton Anxiety Rating Scale; SSRI, only selective serotonin inhibitor; 5-HT, on one mainly serotonergic medication; other, lithium, antipsychotic medication, benzodiazepines or combination of multiple treatments.
Significantly (p < 0.05) different from controls.
Significantly (p < 0.05) different from T1.
Trend difference from T1 (p < 0.1). All other comparisons are non-significant (p > 0.2).
Appetitive and aversive PIT during approach
We first examined valence-specific PIT effects during approach only. We regressed each subject's response probability, averaged across all instrumental stimuli, onto neutral and positive (0, +, ++) CSs to measure appetitive PIT, or onto neutral and negative (0, –, – –) CSs to measure aversive PIT. Comparison of the resulting linear regression coefficients yielded no group differences in appetitive or aversive approach PIT at T1, nor was there an effect of time in patients (all p > 0.1).
Action-specificity at T1
A central finding in one of our previous studies (Huys et al. 2011) was that the effects of Pavlovian CSs depended on the nature of the instrumental action, with positive v. negative CS valence promoting active approach v. active withdrawal, respectively. We computed approach and withdrawal PIT effects by regressing each subject's response probability during each of the two blocks on all five CS values (again averaging across instrumental stimuli). PIT action-specificity was the difference between the linear regression coefficients during approach and withdrawal.
At T1, PIT was action-specific in controls (p = 0.002, signed rank, Fig. 3a), but not in patients (p = 0.72, signed rank, Fig. 3b) and there was a trend-wise difference between patients and controls (p = 0.07, U test, Fig. 3c). Exploring approach and withdrawal blocks separately, there was a trend-wise difference between patients and controls during approach (p = 0.07, U test), but none during withdrawal (p = 0.8, U test), suggesting that any group difference in action specificity was driven more by approach than withdrawal effects at T1.
Action-specificity at T1 predicts the course of depression
Some patients improved more than others over the follow-up period. The size of the improvement was proportional to action-specificity at T1: action-specific PIT scores correlated with the BDI scores at T2 (ϱ = −0.47, p = 0.02, Spearman rank correlation) and with the residual BDI scores after correcting for (regressing out) the BDI scores at T1 (ϱ = −0.53, p = 0.009, Spearman rank correlation, Fig. 3d).
Improvers (n = 14) and non-improvers (n = 9) had similar numbers of previous episodes of depression and hospitalizations and did not differ in depression severity at T1, but did so at time T2 (Table 2). While improvers showed PIT action-specificity at T1, non-improvers did not (group difference: p = 0.04, U test, Fig. 3e– g ). Hence, patients who had intact action-specificity had a better outcome over the course of follow-up.
Table 2.
Improvers | Non-improvers | Statistics | |
---|---|---|---|
Previous episodes | 2.5 (3.1) | 1.3 (1.3) | p = 0.7, U test |
Hospitalizations | 1.1 (0.7) | 0.3 (0.5) | p = 0.6, U test |
BDI T1 | 23.3 (9.8) | 22.7 (5.8) | p = 0.9, t21 = 0.17 |
BDI T2 | 8.3 (6.2) | 24.2 (8.8) | p = 4.6 × 10−5, t21 = −5.1 |
Data are given as mean (standard deviation).
BDI, Beck Depression Inventory.
Exploring these effects further, we found that the difference in PIT action-specificity between improvers and non-improvers was driven by withdrawal effects. At T1, withdrawal was modulated by Pavlovian stimuli in improvers but not so in non-improvers (p = 0.02 and p = 0.2, respectively, both signed rank tests), and the groups differed in the withdrawal but not the approach condition (p = 0.02 and p = 0.5, respectively, U tests). Furthermore, withdrawal PIT correlated with residual BDI scores at T2 (ϱ = 0.55, p = 0.01, rank correlation), while approach PIT did not (p = 0.2). Finally, action-specificity correlated with improvement across symptom subdomains, but most strongly with changes in anhedonic symptoms (online Supplement S4).
Effects of medication, time and anxiety
There were no effects of medication, and the findings persisted when controlling for medication (online Supplement S5). In the depressed group, a trend for action specificity was present at T2 (p = 0.08, signed rank) but it did not interact with time and did not differentiate improvers from non-improvers (online Supplement S6).
All results held when including patients with co-morbid anxiety (online Supplement S7). At T1, there was no action specificity in patients p = 0.7. At T1, there was a significant difference between patients and controls (p = 0.04) and between improvers and non-improvers (p = 0.009). Action specificity at T1 also correlated negatively with residual BDI scores at T2 after correcting for scores at T1 (ϱ = −0.48, p = 0.005). Notably, there was a significant effect of time on withdrawal PIT when including GAD subjects (p = 0.04; online Supplementary Table S3).
Discussion
Advances in understanding decision-making have identified multiple separate and parallel, but interacting decision-making systems (Killcross & Coutureau, 2003; Daw et al. 2005; Huys et al. 2012; Dolan & Dayan, 2013; Lee et al. 2014). PIT is one paradigmatic examination of how incidental valued stimuli interact with and influence ongoing decisions about how to act. In the context of depression, this may tap into how unrelated but affectively salient events are coupled to current thought and behavioural selection processes.
Pavlovian stimuli exerted action-specific effects in healthy subjects. During depressive episodes, this was absent, but the relative preservation of action specificity predicted a better recovery. Thus, this first in-depth PIT examination suggests that in the depressed state the guidance of choices afforded by Pavlovian stimuli is reduced and lacks specificity. The finer differentiation between different types of behaviours seen in controls is lost during a clinical episode of depression.
Two facets of depression appeared to be differentially associated with the Pavlovian modulation of approach and withdrawal, which jointly make up action specificity. The distinction between improvers and non-improvers was driven by differences during withdrawal. Thus a more advantageous disease trajectory characterized those patients with relatively more intact Pavlovian guidance of withdrawal behaviours. Conversely, a distinction between patients and controls was more prominent during approach. A tempting, though emphatically tentative, interpretation is that the blunted modulation of approach might contribute more to a trait distinction between patients and controls. This contrasts with an alteration in the modulation of avoidance relating more to the trajectory and repair of depression itself.
The (marginally) blunted influence of both appetitive and aversive CSs on approach was broadly in keeping with reports of a symmetric blunting in responses to affective material (Bylsma et al. 2008) in depression. However, a straightforward insensitivity to rewards and losses should also have expressed itself in impairments in the acquisition of the instrumental or Pavlovian tasks, which was not the case. Hence, to the extent that a reduced PIT effect captures a similar underlying process as blunting, this might relate more to aspects of the expression than to the acquisition of value.
The fate of negative emotional reactions, i.e. the modulation of aversive behaviours in depression, is subject to much debate. Some emphasize a likely increase (Beck et al. 1979; Roiser et al. 2012) such that patients suffering from mood and anxiety disorders show more reactive aggression against themselves and others (Monahan et al. 2001). Experience sampling studies have shown that aversive events have greater affective consequences in never-depressed monozygotic twins with a depressed co-twin (Wichers et al. 2007). However, a meta-analysis showed that emotional reactions to negative stimuli are, overall, blunted in MDD (Bylsma et al. 2008). Akin to the finding that appetitive rather than aversive stimuli potentiate startle reflexes in MDD (Allen et al. 1999), our data suggest more subtle effects than a simple increase or decrease of PIT, specifically an alteration in the balance between approach and withdrawal. Our data also suggest that the affective control of aversive behaviours (by both appetitive and aversive expectations) is of particular importance to recovery. This hints that aversive influences, such as inappropriate inhibition of withdrawal, may have an unduly large effect on the maintenance of depression, for instance, by inhibiting rather than promoting withdrawal in dangerous situations. Maladaptive avoidance may in turn facilitate individuals’ self-selection into high-risk environments (Kendler et al. 1999) and thereby set up vicious depression–stress cycles.
At a neural level, we have shown that behavioural suppression due to aversive CSs is related to both serotonin (Geurts et al. 2013b) and to how aversive CSs modulate connectivity between the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (extending into the subgenual cortex, itself also predictive of the course of depressive episodes: Mayberg, 2009; Fu et al. 2013) and the caudate nucleus (Geurts et al. 2013a), raising the possibility that a lack of action specificity during depression might be related to neurobiological deficits previously described in MDD (Mayberg et al. 2005).
The generalization to severely depressed clinical samples requires verification as the present study contained a large fraction of moderately depressed students with relatively high average IQ. Second, we did not find significant medication effects, though the study was neither designed nor powered to detect these, and due to the small number of non-improvers we were unable to differentiate between subjects whose condition deteriorated v. remained stable. Third, we did not assess depressive episode length. Modulation of withdrawal responses at T1 was predictive of better recovery over the observation period. Hence it was surprising to see significant modulation of withdrawal even in non-improvers at T2. One possibility is that the recovery of withdrawal modulation is an early sign of improvement appearing before symptomatic improvement, and might predict recovery over a longer follow-up period even in the non-improvers. Conversely, improvers may have been at this advanced stage already when they were tested at T1. Finally, it should be noted that the number of subjects is relatively small for a longitudinal study, limiting both robustness and power, and hence the ability to detect reliable predictive relationships.
In conclusion, the current results provide a strong motivation to pursue computationally inspired decision-making tasks to examine the structure of emotional processes in depression. Aspects of decision-making that have predictive value may become useful for the guidance of treatment or for alternative (and complementary) classifications of psychiatric disorders and individual patients.
Acknowledgements
This work was funded by a Wellcome Trust Grant (Senior Investigator Award 098362/Z/12/Z) and a Max Planck Award to R.J.D. R.C. has received funding from the Human Frontiers Science Programme, the James McDonnel Foundation and the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research; and has been a consultant to AbbVie and Pfizer but is neither employee nor stockholder. P.D. is funded by the Gatsby Charitable Foundation and has received an unrestricted research gift from Google, Inc.; R.J.D. by the Wellcome Trust, the Mary Kinross Trust and the Max Planck Society. A.H. has received research funding from the German Research Foundation (Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft; Excellence Cluster) and the German Federal Ministry of Education and Research as well as unrestricted grants from Eli Lilly & Company, Janssen-Cilag, and Bristol-Myers Squibb. Q.J.M.H. and E.F. have received funding from the German Federal Ministry of Education and Research (DFG FOR 1617), and Q.J.M.H. additionally from the Swiss National Science Foundation (320030L_153449/1). The funders had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis and interpretation of the data; or preparation, review or approval of the manuscript; and decision to submit the manuscript for publication. We thank Jonathan P. Roiser for helpful comments on the manuscript. Q.J.M.H. had full access to all of the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.
Q.J.M.H., A.H., R.C., P.D. and R.J.D. conceived the study. Q.J.M.H., M.G., E.F., R.C. and P.D. designed the task; M.G. and E.F. collected the data; Q.J.M.H. analysed the data; Q.J.M.H., M.G., E.F., A.H., R.C., P.D. and R.J.D. interpreted the data and wrote the manuscript. All authors viewed the final version of the manuscript prior to submission and are accountable for all aspects of the work.
Declaration of Interest
None.
Supplementary material
For supplementary material accompanying this paper visit http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0033291715002597.
References
- Allen NB, Trinder J, Brennan C (1999). Affective startle modulation in clinical depression: preliminary findings. Biological Psychiatry 46, 542–550. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Anderson ML, Oates T (2007). A review of recent research in metareasoning and metalearning. AI Magazine 28, 12. [Google Scholar]
- Beck A, Steer R, Brown G (1996a). Manual for the Beck Depression Inventory-II. Psychological Corporation: San Antonio, TX. [Google Scholar]
- Beck AT, Epstein N, Brown G, Steer RA (1988). An inventory for measuring clinical anxiety: psychometric properties. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 56, 893–897. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beck AT, Rush AJ, Shaw BF, Emery G (1979). Cognitive Therapy of Depression, 1st edn. The Guilford Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy Series. Guilford Press: New York. [Google Scholar]
- Beck AT, Steer RA, Ball R, Ranieri W (1996b). Comparison of Beck Depression Inventories-IA and -II in psychiatric outpatients. Journal of Personality Assessment 67, 588–597. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bylsma LM, Morris BH, Rottenberg J (2008). A meta-analysis of emotional reactivity in major depressive disorder. Clinical Psychology Review 28, 676–691. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crockett MJ, Clark L, Robbins TW (2009). Reconciling the role of serotonin in behavioral inhibition and aversion: acute tryptophan depletion abolishes punishment-induced inhibition in humans. Journal of Neuroscience 29, 11993–11999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Daw ND, Dayan P (2014). The algorithmic anatomy of model-based evaluation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Science 369, 20130478. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Daw ND, Niv Y, Dayan P (2005). Uncertainty-based competition between prefrontal and dorsolateral striatal systems for behavioral control. Nature Neuroscience 8, 1704–1711. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dayan P, Huys QJM (2008). Serotonin, inhibition, and negative mood. PLoS Computational Biology 4, . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dayan P, Niv Y, Seymour B, Daw ND (2006). The misbehavior of value and the discipline of the will. Neural Networks 19, 1153–1160. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dolan RJ, Dayan P (2013). Goals and habits in the brain. Neuron 80, 312–325. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elliott R, Sahakian BJ, Herrod JJ, Robbins TW, Paykel ES (1997). Abnormal response to negative feedback in unipolar depression: evidence for a diagnosis-specific impairment. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry 63, 74–82. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eshel N, Roiser JP (2010). Reward and punishment processing in depression. Biological Psychiatry 68, 118–124. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JB (2002a). Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR Axis I Disorders, Research Version, Non-Patient Edition (SCID-I/NP). Biometrics Research, New York State Psychiatric Institute: New York. [Google Scholar]
- First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JB (2002b). Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR Axis I Disorders, Research Version, Patient Edition (SCID-I/P). Biometrics Research, New York State Psychiatric Institute: New York. [Google Scholar]
- Fu CHY, Steiner H, Costafreda SG (2013). Predictive neural biomarkers of clinical response in depression: a meta-analysis of functional and structural neuroimaging studies of pharmacological and psychological therapies. Neurobiology of Disease 52, 75–83. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Geurts DEM, Huys QJM, den Ouden HEM, Cools R (2013a). Aversive Pavlovian control of instrumental behavior in humans. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 25, 1428–1441. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Geurts DEM, Huys QJM, den Ouden HEM, Cools R (2013b). Serotonin and aversive Pavlovian control of instrumental behavior in humans. Journal of Neuroscience 33, 18932–18939. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guitart-Masip M, Fuentemilla L, Bach DR, Huys QJM, Dayan P, Dolan RJ, Duzel E (2011). Action dominates valence in anticipatory representations in the human striatum and dopaminergic midbrain. Journal of Neuroscience 31, 7867–7875. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hamilton M (1960). A rating scale for depression. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry 23, 56–62. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huys QJM, Cools R, Gölzer M, Friedel E, Heinz A, Dolan RJ, Dayan P (2011). Disentangling the roles of approach, activation and valence in instrumental and Pavlovian responding. PLoS Computational Biology 7, . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huys QJ, Daw ND, Dayan P (2015). Depression: a decision-theoretic account. Annual Review of Neuroscience 38, 1–23. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huys QJM, Eshel N, O'Nions E, Sheridan L, Dayan P, Roiser JP (2012). Bonsai trees in your head: how the Pavlovian system sculpts goal-directed choices by pruning decision trees. PLoS Computational Biology 8, . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Joormann J, Vanderlind WM (2014). Emotion regulation in depression: the role of biased cognition and reduced cognitive control. Clinical Psychological Science 2, 402–421. [Google Scholar]
- Kendler KS, Karkowski LM, Prescott CA (1999). Causal relationship between stressful life events and the onset of major depression. American Journal of Psychiatry 156, 837–841. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kendler KS, Thornton LM, Gardner CO (2000). Stressful life events and previous episodes in the etiology of major depression in women: an evaluation of the “kindling” hypothesis. American Journal of Psychiatry 157, 1243–1251. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Killcross S, Coutureau E (2003). Coordination of actions and habits in the medial prefrontal cortex of rats. Cerebral Cortex 13, 400–408. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee SW, Shimojo S, O'Doherty JP (2014). Neural computations underlying arbitration between model-based and model-free learning. Neuron 81, 687–699. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mayberg H, Lozano A, Voon V, McNeely H, Seminowicz D, Hamani C, Schwalb J, Kennedy S (2005). Deep brain stimulation for treatment-resistant depression. Neuron 45, 651–660. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mayberg HS (2009). Targeted electrode-based modulation of neural circuits for depression. Journal of Clinical Investigation 119, 717–725. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Monahan J, Steadman HJ, Silver E, Appelbaum PS, Robbins PC, Mulvey EP, Roth LH, Grisso T, Banks S (2001). Rethinking Risk Assessment: The MacArthur Study of Mental Disorder and Violence. Oxford University Press: New York. [Google Scholar]
- Roiser JP, Elliott R, Sahakian BJ (2012). Cognitive mechanisms of treatment in depression. Neuropsychopharmacology 37, 117–136. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rottenberg J (2005). Mood and emotion in major depression. Current Directions in Psychological Science 14, 167. [Google Scholar]
- Schmidt KH, Metzler P (1992). Wortschatztest (WST) (Vocabulary Test (WST)). Beltz Test GmbH: Weinheim. [Google Scholar]
- Steele JD, Kumar P, Ebmeier KP (2007). Blunted response to feedback information in depressive illness. Brain 130, 2367–2374. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wichers M, Myin-Germeys I, Jacobs N, Peeters F, Kenis G, Derom C, Vlietinck R, Delespaul P, Os JV (2007). Genetic risk of depression and stress-induced negative affect in daily life. British Journal of Psychiatry 191, 218–223. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
For supplementary material accompanying this paper visit http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0033291715002597.