Abstract
Neuroimaging studies have documented that aging can disrupt certain higher cognitive systems such as the default mode network (DMN), the salience network and the central executive network (CEN). The effect of cognitive training on higher cognitive systems remains unclear. This study used a 1-year longitudinal design to explore the cognitive training effect on three higher cognitive networks in healthy older adults. The community-living healthy older adults were divided into two groups: the multi-domain cognitive training group (24 sessions of cognitive training over a 3-months period) and the wait-list control group. All subjects underwent cognitive measurements and resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging scanning at baseline and at 1 year after the training ended. We examined training-related changes in functional connectivity (FC) within and between three networks. Compared with the baseline, we observed maintained or increased FC within all three networks after training. The scans after training also showed maintained anti-correlation of FC between the DMN and CEN compared to the baseline. These findings demonstrated that cognitive training maintained or improved the functional integration within networks and the coupling between the DMN and CEN in older adults. Our findings suggested that multi-domain cognitive training can mitigate the aging-related dysfunction of higher cognitive networks.
Keywords: aging, cognitive training, resting-state fMRI, functional connectivity, brain network
Introduction
As people age, they experience cognitive decline, which involves working memory, executive function and attention, among other functions, and this occurs with particular frequency in late life, resulting in an increasingly poor quality of life (Hedden and Gabrieli, 2004; Grady, 2012). To postpone the cognitive decline associated with aging, increasing numbers of studies have conducted interventions with older adults, focusing on cognitive training and physical exercise (Park and Bischof, 2013; Tanigawa et al., 2014). Results of neuropsychological studies have demonstrated benefits of cognitive functions after interventions in old adults, including improvements in working memory capacity, reasoning, and processing speed (Neely and Backman, 1995; Oswald et al., 2006; Bamidis et al., 2015; Leung et al., 2015). Our previous study has also demonstrated that the effects of interventions on cognition are maintained at 1 year after the training ended (Cheng et al., 2012). On the other hand, neuroimaging studies have proved that multi-domain training affects the spontaneous brain activity in healthy older adults, for example, altered regional homogeneity of spontaneous fluctuations in temporal gyrus and cerebellum, enhanced amplitude of low frequency fluctuations in frontal gyrus and cerebellum (Yin et al., 2014; Zheng et al., 2015). In another study, improved memory performance during memory task after training has been shown to associate with increased brain activation in the prefrontal, temporal, and parietal area in older adults (Kirchhoff et al., 2012). Also, the reduction in functional connection (FC) between the superior parietal cortex and inferior temporal lobe is observed after training in older adults, which is associated with training success (Strenziok et al., 2014). These findings suggest that the interventions bring about not only enhanced behavioral outcomes but improvement of the brain function in older adults (Kraft, 2012; Bamidis et al., 2014).
Recent studies suggest that functional brain networks, a higher level of organization of brain regions, are needed to better understand improved cognitive function after intervention (Langer et al., 2013; Bamidis et al., 2014). Using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), more than ten functional brain networks constituted of spatially distinct brain regions have been identified either at specific tasks (Calhoun et al., 2009) or at rest, including the default mode network (DMN), the salience network (SN) and the central executive network (CEN; Smith et al., 2009; Luo et al., 2012). Three (DMN, SN, and CEN) of these stable networks have been considered the ‘core’ neurocognitive networks for understanding higher cognitive functions and have received the most attention in cognitive studies (Sridharan et al., 2008; Bressler and Menon, 2010; Luo et al., 2011, 2014b). The DMN, which includes mainly the posterior cingulate cortex (PCC), medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC) and inferior parietal lobe (IPL), is involved in self-referential and autobiographical memory functions (Raichle et al., 2001; Buckner et al., 2008). The SN includes the bilateral anterior insula (AI) and dorsal anterior cingulate cortex, and it is associated with detecting internal and external events salient to homeostasis. In contrast, the CEN, composed of the dorsolateral frontal and parietal neocortices, is engaged in cognitively demanding tasks, such as decision making and working memory (Seeley et al., 2007). Many studies have indicated that these three networks, along with the disruption of FC within network and the anti-correlation between the DMN and CEN, are strongly affected by normal aging and Alzheimer’s disease, which is associated with cognitive impairments (Meunier et al., 2009; Wu et al., 2011; Onoda et al., 2012; Tomasi and Volkow, 2012; Cao et al., 2014).
Previous studies have shown that brain networks are affected by interventions, such as the DMN and CEN, associated with improved behavioral outcomes and cognitive performances (Patel et al., 2013; Bamidis et al., 2014; Luo et al., 2014a; Tang and Posner, 2014). For instance, intervention studies, including either cognitive or physical training, reveal changes in the resting-state FC within the DMN and CEN in healthy adults (Voss et al., 2010, 2013; Taylor et al., 2013). Interventions have also been conducted on older adults to investigate the changes in brain networks that subserve cognitive function (Voss et al., 2010; Bamidis et al., 2014; Luo et al., 2016). A study of multimodal intervention in older adults has shown improved resting-state FC between the MPFC and medial temporal lobe in the DMN after training, suggesting an association with better cognitive performance (Li et al., 2014). Increases in cerebral blood flow and resting-state FC within the DMN and CEN are also observed after complex cognitive training in healthy seniors (Chapman et al., 2015). In addition, a study also demonstrated the decreased FC between the DMN and CEN after working memory training in health adults (Takeuchi et al., 2013). These findings suggest that training can affect FC of certain brain networks, such as DMN, CEN. However, no study has investigated the effects of multi-domain cognitive training on resting-state FC within and among the three networks (DMN, SN, and CEN) in older adults.
The purpose of this study is to explore the effects of cognitive training on resting-state FCs within and between the three networks in old adults. We hypothesized that multi-domain cognitive training would result in changes of resting-state FC within and among the three networks in healthy older adults, which might be beneficial for counteracting decreased integration within network and impaired coupling between networks with aging. In our work, we investigated the resting-state FC of three networks using seed-based FC analysis at baseline and at 1 year after training ended. The relationship between cognitive improvements and the changes of FC was also analyzed.
Materials and Methods
Participants
The current work included 40 healthy older adults from two randomized groups [the multi-domain training group (n = 23) and the control group (n = 17)], who were recruited through a dispatched notice/broadcasting by the local institute of community service in three community centers around Tongji Hospital in Shanghai from March 2008 to April 2008. After a personal interview with professional interviewers, all participants then underwent cognitive measurements and fMRI scanning. The inclusion criteria included the following: age (65–75 years); educational year (more than 1 year); normal hearing, vision, or communication status; normal functional capacity; living independently in the community; score on the Chinese version of the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) of 19 or above [the lower cut-off point of the MMSE score is due to the lower educational level in China (Li et al., 2006)]; and being free of neurological or psychiatric disorders and of brain injury. Exclusion criteria included the following: obvious cognitive decline, a diagnosis of AD; serious neurological or psychiatric disorders such as major depressive disorder, brain cancer, and schizophrenia. One left-handed subject in the multi-domain training group was included due to the absence of a relationship between the training-related changes and handedness (Mackey et al., 2013). This study was approved by the Ethical Committee of the East China Normal University. All participants gave written informed consent, and they were not provided monetary compensation for their participation in this study.
Neuropsychological Measurements
Interventions
Cognitive training employed a randomized, controlled design. The multi-domain training group was divided into a small group, and the training procedure took place in a classroom in Tongji Hospital. All participants received 24 sessions of cognitive training over a 3-months period at a frequency of twice a week. The multi-domain cognitive training targeted memory, reasoning, and problem-solving strategies using approaches such as visual-spatial map reading, handcraft making, healthy living, and physical exercise. Each session lasted 60 min. A lecture about a common disease in aging people was presented during the first 15 min of each session. Then, the trainer taught the participants about a special cognitive strategy or technique via slide lecture during the second 30 min. The newly practiced skills were consolidated by dealing with some real-life problems during the last 15 min. The wait-list control group served as a match for the social contact associated with cognitive training. The multi-domain training group and the control group attended a lecture about healthy living every 2 months. More details about the training are provided in our previous study (Cheng et al., 2012).
Cognitive Measurements
To evaluate the effects of intervention on cognitive function, composite outcome measurements were created, including the Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status (RBANS, Form A), which shows good validity and reliability in a Chinese community-living elderly sample (Lim et al., 2010; Cheng et al., 2011), the trail-making test (TMT; Ashendorf et al., 2008), the visual reasoning test (Xiao et al., 2002) and the Color Word Stroop test (CWST; van Boxtel et al., 2001). All measurements were conducted at baseline and at 1 year after training.
To examine the effect of cognitive training on cognition, we compared the pre- and post-training changes between the multi-domain cognitive group and the control group using one-way analyses of covariance (ANCOVAs) with the differences between the 1-year post-test and baseline measurements as dependent variables and scores at baseline as covariates to exclude the possibility that any pre-existing difference in the measures between the groups affected the results of each measure (p < 0.05). In addition, two-sample t-tests were applied to investigate the differences in cognition measurements at baseline between the two groups. The confounding covariates, including age, gender and education years, were regressed in all statistical analyses.
Data Acquisition
All functional and structural imaging data were acquired using a Siemens 3T MRI scanner (Erlangen, Germany) at East China Normal University, Shanghai, China. All participants underwent scanning twice, at baseline and at 1 year after training. To minimize head motion, we fixed the subjects’ heads using foam pads. Resting-state functional images were collected using a single-shot, gradient-recalled echo planar imaging sequence [repetition time (TR) = 2000 ms, echo time (TE) = 25 ms and flip angle (FA) = 90°, field of view (FOV) = 240 mm × 240 mm, in-plane matrix = 64 × 64, slice thickness = 5 mm, voxel size = 3.75 mm × 3.75 mm × 5 mm], generating 32 slices. The functional scanning lasted for 310 s, yielding a total of 155 volumes. To ensure magnetic field stabilization, the first five volumes were discarded. During the resting-state scanning, the subjects were asked to lie with their eyes closed, not to fall asleep, and not to think of anything in particular. Axial T1-weighted anatomical images were acquired using a magnetisation-prepared rapid gradient-echo sequence, generating 160 slices (TR = 1900 ms, TE = 3.43 ms, FA = 90°, matrix size = 256 × 256, FOV = 240 mm × 240 mm, slice thickness = 1 mm, voxel size = 0.9375 mm × 0.9375 mm × 1 mm).
Data Preprocessing
Resting-State fMRI Data
Imaging data preprocessing was performed using the SPM8 software package1. First, all imaging data were corrected for the slice-timing and realigned for head movement correction. Accounting for the effect of head motion on the FC analysis, we excluded all the participants whose head movement exceeded 1 mm or 1°. Furthermore, we also calculated the framewise displacement (FD) given by
(1) |
where i is the i-th time point and
(2) |
The FD0 was set to zero and rotational displacements were converted from degrees to millimeters (Power et al., 2012). Then, the resulting functional data were normalized to the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) template using a 12-parameter affine transformation (resampled to 3 mm × 3 mm × 3 mm). Finally, the imaging data were spatially smoothed with a 6-mm full-width at half maximum (FWHM) Gaussian kernel. We applied a temporal band-pass filter (the frequency range of 0.01 and 0.08 Hz) to reduce the low frequency drift and high frequency physiological noise (Fox et al., 2005; Jiang et al., 2015; Luo et al., 2015). Several nuisance covariates including white matter (WM) signal, cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) signal, global signal, and six head motion parameters were also removed from the time course of all brain voxels using a multiple linear regression analysis.
Preprocessing of T1-Weighted Images
Previous studies provided the evidence of the loss of gray matter volume (GMV) with aging and of the potential effects on the functional results (Damoiseaux et al., 2008). In the present study, we increased the reliability of resting-state fMRI analysis by controlling for the GMV. The T1-weighted images were preprocessed using the voxel-based morphometry (VBM82) toolbox in SPM8. The native T1-weighted image was normalized to MNI space using an exponentiated lie algebra (DARTEL) approach. The resulting images were then segmented into gray matter (GM), WM and CSF. For each subject, the GMV of the whole brain was calculated.
Functional Connectivity Analysis
To evaluate our hypothesis, a seed-based FC analysis was performed to examine the training effect on FC within and between the DMN, SN, and CEN. Three seeds were selected based on previous studies: the PCC (0, -52, 20), the right AI (38, 26, -10) and the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex [right DLPFC (44, 36, 20)] to constitute the DMN, SN, and CEN, respectively (Fransson, 2006; Seeley et al., 2007; Luo et al., 2011). The average signal of spherical regions with nine-millimeter diameter centered at the three seeds was extracted. The FC map for each network was obtained by calculating Pearson correlation coefficient between the average time course of seed and each voxel in the whole brain for each subject. A Fisher z-transformation was used to convert the correlation coefficient of each voxel to a normal distribution (Shao et al., 2012; Chen et al., 2015). Thus, the individual z-score maps were obtained for each seed and subject. A one-sample t-test was used to identify the within-group FC map for each network. The significance level was set at p < 0.05 (FDR-corrected, cluster size > 621 mm3).
Statistical Analysis
To evaluate the training effect on FC within and between the three networks, a whole-brain voxel-wise 2 (between-subject factor: training and control groups) × 2 (within-subject factor: baseline and 1-year after training ending) repeated analysis of variance (ANOVA) was performed using the explicit mask from the union set of the one-sample t-test results of three networks, by controlling for the whole brain GMV, as well as age, gender, education years and FD as confounding covariates. This analysis was performed using SPM8, and the significance level was set at p < 0.05 (cluster size > 621 mm3). Post hoc comparisons (paired t-tests) were carried out for each significant interaction effect to clarify the effect of training on FC for each group.
Neural Correlate Analysis
To explore the relationship between FC and cognitive measurements, z-transformed connectivity values were extracted from regions that showed significant training-related effect, and the difference between the baseline and post-test was computed. Partial correlations were performed between the changes of resting-state FC and the pre- and post-training differences in the scores on the cognitive measures, while controlling for age, gender, and education years.
Results
Demographic and Neuropsychological Tests
At baseline, 23 subjects (one left-handed) from the multi-domain training group and 17 subjects from the control group underwent cognitive measurements and the fMRI scanning. Eighteen subjects in the multi-domain training group and 14 in the control group finished both the cognitive measurements and the fMRI scanning at 1 year after the intervention, and they were involved in the final analysis. A total of eight participants withdrew (one death, two cancers, one operation, and four rejecting scanning). No significant differences in age, gender, education years, FD and MMSE score were found between the multi-domain training group and the control group (p > 0.05; Table 1).
Table 1.
Multi-domain training group | Control group | p | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
Age (year) | Baseline | 70.61 ± 3.29 | 68.59 ± 3.24 | 0.838 |
(mean ± SD) | One-year post-test | 72.39 ± 3.43 | 70.85 ± 4.05 | 0.782 |
Gender (male) | Baseline | 23 (16) | 17 (9) | 0.283 |
One-year post-test | 18 (13) | 14 (9) | 0.631 | |
Education (year) | Baseline | 10.91 ± 3.65 | 10.64 ± 3.06 | 0.452 |
(mean ± SD) | One-year post-test | 11.11 ± 4.25 | 10.09 ± 3.34 | 0.383 |
MMSE | Baseline | 27.57 ± 2.57 | 28.17 ± 1.94 | 0.505 |
(mean ± SD) | One-year post-test | 27.72 ± 2.16 | 27.85 ± 2.31 | 0.900 |
FD | Baseline | 0.12 ± 0.04 | 0.14 ± 0.05 | 0.448 |
(mean ± SD) | One-year post-test | 0.12 ± 0.04 | 0.16 ± 0.07 | 0.068 |
FD, Framewise displacement; MMSE, the Mini-Mental State Examination; SD, standard deviation.
No significant differences in all cognitive measurements were found at baseline between the two groups (p > 0.05; Table 2). However, the multi-domain training group showed significant improvements in performance on language (p = 0.036) and delayed memory (p = 0.027) and marginally significant improvement on the RBANS total score (p = 0.058) at 1-year after training compared to the baseline (Table 2).
Table 2.
Multi-domain training |
Contorl |
|||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Baseline | One-year Post-test | Baseline | One-year Post-test | Pa | Pb | |
RBANS total score | 93.17 ± 16.10 | 106.94 ± 12.90 | 93.07 ± 15.00 | 99.57 ± 13.75 | 0.058 | 0.971 |
Immediate memory | 86.22 ± 16.45 | 103.17 ± 23.35 | 84.00 ± 14.34 | 100.5 ± 14.33 | 0.695 | 0.694 |
Visuospatial | 106.78 ± 12.86 | 103.9 ± 11.56 | 106.86 ± 19.05 | 103.00 ± 16.81 | 0.981 | 0.922 |
Language | 92.44 ± 12.92 | 101.00 ± 8.39 | 93.29 ± 7.12 | 95.86 ± 5.86 | 0.036∗ | 0.861 |
Attention | 90.67 ± 19.39 | 94.94 ± 15.17 | 91.5 ± 15.25 | 88.93 ± 15.24 | 0.22 | 0.776 |
Delayed memory | 98.28 ± 18.86 | 118.50 ± 12.41 | 99.00 ± 13.13 | 110.21 ± 14.80 | 0.027∗ | 0.999 |
The CWST | ||||||
Color interfere | 20.16 ± 13.95 | 27.33 ± 17.01 | 14.57 ± 7.62 | 23.85 ± 14.33 | 0.884 | 0.125 |
Word interfere | 38.61 ± 15.56 | 39.89 ± 17.35 | 39.71 ± 14.10 | 42.00 ± 19.00 | 0.669 | 0.642 |
The Visual Reasoning Test | 5.78 ± 1.35 | 6.56 ± 1.89 | 5.86 ± 2.74 | 5.93 ± 2.46 | 0.169 | 0.894 |
The TMT | ||||||
Trail A complete time | 79.50 ± 23.60 | 73.72 ± 33.93 | 87.21 ± 37.37 | 77.07 ± 29.22 | 0.739 | 0.381 |
Trail B complete time | 157.89 ± 55.41 | 138.5 ± 48.27 | 153.5 ± 68.11 | 142.57 ± 49.6 | 0.634 | 0.830 |
∗p<0.05. CWST, Color Word Stroop test; RBANS, Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status; TMT, Trail Making test. aOne-way ANCOVAs with differences between 1-year post-test and baseline in psychological measures as dependent variables, by controlling for scores of the psychological measures at baseline, age, gender and education years as covariates. bTwo sample t-test with scores of the psychological measures at baseline as dependent variables, by controlling for age, gender, and education years as covariates.
The Effect of Cognitive Training on Networks
Three networks were constituted by seed-based FC analysis, seeding at the PCC for the DMN, at the right AI for the SN and at the right DLPFC for the CEN. The DMN mainly involved the PCC/precuneus, VMPFC, DMPFC, bilateral IPL, bilateral temporal lobe, cerebellar lobule IX and crus II (Supplementary Figure S1). The SN mainly included the bilateral AI, DMPFC, dACC, and bilateral TPJ (Supplementary Figure S2), and the CEN was composed of the DLPFC, DMPFC, and IPL, all bilaterally (Supplementary Figure S3).
The 2 × 2 repeated measure ANOVA revealed significant interactions (p < 0.05) for FC within and between networks. In this study, this analysis was performed for the FCs that showed no significant difference between the training and control groups at baseline, because a training-related effect was the only possibility.
The ANOVA revealed the significant interactions for the FC within the DMN [PCC – left superior frontal gyrus (SFG) and PCC – cerebellar lobule IX]; within the SN (right AI – right middle/posterior insula, and right AI – left frontoinsula) and within the CEN (right DLPFC – bilateral DLPFC and right DLPFC – right SFG). Post hoc comparison demonstrated that some connections showed increased or maintained FC in the training group but decreased FC in the control group, including those between the PCC and the left SFG and between the PCC and cerebellar lobule IX within the DMN, between the right AI and left frontoinsula within the SN, and between the right DLPFC and the bilateral DLPFC and between the right DLPFC and the right SFG within the CEN (Figure 1; Table 3). In addition, we observed increased FC between the right AI and right middle/posterior insula in the control group but maintained FC after training in the training group (Figure 1; Table 3).
Table 3.
Seed | Brain region | BA | MNI coordinate |
F-score | Cluster size (mm3) | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
X | Y | Z | |||||
PCC | Left dorsolateral prefrontal lobe | 20 | -33 | 15 | 24 | 18.64 | 2187 |
Left cerebellar lobule IX | -6 | -51 | -54 | 16.2 | 2241 | ||
Right putamen | 9 | 18 | 15 | -9 | 13.17 | 1377 | |
Right dorsolateral prefrontal lobe | 51 | 0 | 30 | 12.86 | 1782 | ||
Left superior frontal gyrus | 20 | -24 | 36 | 39 | 11.66 | 1323 | |
Right AI | Right insula | 45 | 30 | -9 | -18 | 15.82 | 4644 |
Left frontoinsula | 45 | -51 | 33 | -12 | 11.67 | 2538 | |
Right DLPFC | Right superior frontal cortex | 11 | 15 | 27 | 54 | 13.30 | 3186 |
Left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex | 9 | -39 | 30 | 42 | 12.78 | 1647 | |
Left inferior parietal lobe | -30 | -63 | 30 | 13 | 1431 | ||
Right superior frontal gyrus | 42 | 27 | 42 | 12.12 | 2970 |
BA, Broadmann area; CEN, central executive network; DMN, default mode network; SN, salience network; DLPFC, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; AI, anterior insula; PCC, posterior cingulate cortex.
Our results also revealed significant interactions in the FCs between networks, including between DMN and CEN and between DMN and SN. Post hoc comparisons showed that the anti-correlations between the DMN and CEN were maintained or increased after training, including an increase in the anti-correlation between the PCC and the left DLPFC in the training group, while this correlation was maintained in control group; meanwhile, the anti-correlation between the PCC and the right DLPFC was maintained in training group and decreased in the control group. For the seed at the right DLPFC, the anti-correlation between the right DLPFC and left IPL was maintained in the training group and increased in the control group. For the connection between the DMN and SN, the connectivity between the PCC and the right putamen was maintained in the training group and decreased in the control group (Figure 2; Table 3).
The Correlation between Cognitive Measures and Resting-State fMRI
We found a significant relationship between the changes in FC (between the two fMRI sessions) and the improvements in cognitive performances (p < 0.05, uncorrected). As shown in Figure 3, the increased FC between the right DLPFC and the right SFG had a slim positive correlation with the RBANS test scores in the multi-domain training group (Figure 3A). The increased FC between the right AI and the right middle/posterior insula predicted poorer delayed memory and RBANS scores in the control group (Figures 3B,C).
Discussion
In the present study, we used resting-state fMRI analysis to examine the effects of multi-domain cognitive training on FC within and among three networks (DMN, SN, and CEN). Consistent with our a priori hypothesis, changes in resting-state FC within and among the three networks were found at the 1-year after multi-domain cognitive training ended in training group compared to the wait-list control group. After training, the maintained or increased anterior–posterior and interhemispheric FCs within network, including PCC – left SFG (DMN), PCC – cerebellar lobule IX (DMN), right AI – left AI (SN), and right DLPFC – left DLPFC (CEN), were observed in the training group, while these FCs decreased in the control group. Compared to the control group, the training group showed increased or maintained anti-correlated FC between DMN and CEN (PCC – DLPFC and DLPFC – IPL). The coupling between DMN and SN (PCC – putamen) was also maintained in training group, but decreased in the control group. These findings suggested that multi-domain cognitive training might counteract decreased integration within network and impaired coupling between networks with aging.
Training Effects on FC within Networks
This study found training-related enhanced integration of FC within the DMN, SN, and CEN. In particular, we observed that FCs between PCC and SFG and between PCC and cerebellar lobule IX were maintained in the multi-domain training group, but decreased in the control group. Previous studies have provided consistent evidence that healthy aging is accompanied by decreased FC within the DMN, particularly an anterior–posterior disruption of FC (Andrews-Hanna et al., 2007; Mevel et al., 2013). The decreased FC between the PCC and the SFG in the control group was consistent with previous findings, highlighting age-related disruption of connectivity at rest along the anterior–posterior axis of the DMN (Andrews-Hanna et al., 2007). The role of the cerebellum in cognitive function has been accepted. For example, lobule IX has been mentioned in association with DMN (Habas et al., 2009), and the FC between DMN nodes (PCC and MPFC) and cerebellar regions has been described as modified by aging and by diseases such as dementia (Bernard et al., 2013; Guo et al., 2015). Unlike the decreased FC with normal aging in the control group, multi-domain cognitive training maintained these FCs from the PCC to the SFG and to lobule IX of the cerebellum, suggesting that cognitive training might resist or postpone age-related disruption of FC within the DMN. In the CEN, we also observed increased FC between the right DLPFC and the frontal lobe (bilateral DLPFC and right SFG) at 1 year after training compared with baseline in the multi-domain training group, while these FCs decreased in the control group. These frontal regions have been found to be involved in executive and cognitive function (Ridderinkhof et al., 2004; Grady, 2012), such as working memory, and to be activated by tasks related to these function when the age-related effects were investigated (Grady, 2012). The frontal lobe is known to be affected by training (Park and Bischof, 2013). In addition, the change of FC between the right DLPFC and the right SFG was slight positively correlated with the improvement on the RBANS test in the training group. Thus, we presumed that the multi-domain training might have positive effects on improvement of behavior via enhancing the FC. However, a larger sample size should be considered to verify the relationship between the FC and cognitive function. A recent study of complex cognitive training in healthy seniors also found increased resting-state FC of the DMN and CEN, as well as increased cerebral blood flow (Chapman et al., 2015). Consistent with those reports, our results showed changes in intranetwork FC in the DMN and CEN after cognitive training. These findings might suggest that the cognitive training enhanced integration of FC within networks, resulting in the improvement of cognitive function.
Recent studies demonstrated that the decreased FC within the SN is an important feature of normal aging and is associated with the cognitive impairments (Onoda et al., 2012; He et al., 2013). Consistent with previous studies, we found decreased FC between the right AI and the left frontoinsula in the control group between the two timepoints scanned. However, the FC was maintained in the training group, as indicated by the absence of a difference between the FC values from before and after multi-domain cognitive training. In addition, we observed the FC between the right AI and right middle/posterior insula was maintained in the training group, but increase in the control group. The right AI, as a key node of the SN, is involved in cognitive function; by contrast, the middle/posterior insula serves in sensorimotor functions (Kurth et al., 2010). The increased FC between the right AI and right middle/posterior insula might indicate the disrupted function of the insula, likely reflecting an association with the cognitive decline in old adults. Here, we observed the negative correlation between the increase in the FC between the right anterior and right middle/posterior insula and the improvement of delayed memory and RBANS scores in the control group, probably implicating the relationship between the FC within the SN and the cognition. Thus, we speculated that cognitive training enhanced integration of the SN to retain at least some of the cognitive performance.
Training-Related Changes of FC between Networks
The triple network model of the DMN, SN, and CEN has been used to understand cognitive dysfunction in neurological and psychiatric disorders (Menon, 2011). A previous study provided evidence for a decrease in the anti-correlation between DMN and CEN in normal aging (Wu et al., 2011). Here, we found that the aging-sensitive anti-correlation between DMN and CEN was maintained after training. A higher anti-correlation between DMN and CEN was also found after working memory training in healthy adults (Takeuchi et al., 2013). In addition, the disruption of SN and DMN connectivity has been observed in dementia (Zhou et al., 2010). Here, the connection between the DMN and SN was maintained in the training group, but decreased in the control group. Our results might also suggest that the anti-correlation of DMN and CEN is relatively sensitive to cognitive training.
Limitations
Interpreting the present findings requires the consideration of a few key limitations. First, our demographic characteristics, such as sample size and educational level, might affect the generalisability of the results. A larger sample size and a more consistent educational level among the subjects are necessary to verify the effect of cognitive training on the resting-state FC of three networks. Second, because the imaging data were not acquired at intermediate stages and or immediately after the completion of training completion, the effects of training on FCs within and between networks at additional intermediate timepoints and immediately after the completion of training are not studied. Third, we considered the effects of brain atrophy on FC in this work. However, the other focal damage (i.e., vascular lesions) is prevalent in the population and may have contributed to our findings. Fourth, the effects of cognitive training on resting-state FCs of three networks were observed in our study. This result suggested that the training was valid in the current population; however, further work should be performed to verify these conclusions in other populations. Finally, another control group of younger subjects should be considered in future work to determine whether the effects of cognitive training are specific to older adults.
Conclusion
We found changes in resting-state FC within networks and between the three networks at 1 year post-training, perhaps reflecting that cognitive training enhanced integration within network and maintenance of segregation between DMN and CEN. Our findings provide evidence that multi-domain cognitive training could mitigate the age-related functional alterations involving DMN, SN and CEN, thereby helping older adults maintain brain health.
Author Contributions
Conceived and designed the work: WC, Cheng Luo, Chunbo Li, DY. Acquired the data: YC, XC, TL, LJ. Analyzed the data: WC, CH, XC, YC. Wrote the paper: WC, Cheng Luo. All authors revised the work for important intellectual content. All of the authors have read and approved the manuscript.
Conflict of Interest Statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
This project was funded by the National Nature Science Foundation of China (81330032, 81371505, 91232725, 81271547, and 30770769), the Science and Technology Commission of Shanghai Municipality (134119a2501, 13dz2260500) and SHSMU-ION Research Center for Brain Disorders. This study was also sponsored by the ‘111’ project of China (No. B12027).
Footnotes
Supplementary Material
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: http://journal.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fnagi.2016.00070
References
- Andrews-Hanna J. R., Snyder A. Z., Vincent J. L., Lustig C., Head D., Raichle M. E., et al. (2007). Disruption of large-scale brain systems in advanced aging. Neuron 56 924–935. 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.10.038 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ashendorf L., Jefferson A. L., O’Connor M. K., Chaisson C., Green R. C., Stern R. A. (2008). Trail Making Test errors in normal aging, mild cognitive impairment, and dementia. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 23 129–137. 10.1016/j.acn.2007.11.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bamidis P. D., Fissler P., Papageorgiou S. G., Zilidou V., Konstantinidis E. I., Billis A. S., et al. (2015). Gains in cognition through combined cognitive and physical training: the role of training dosage and severity of neurocognitive disorder. Front. Aging Neurosci. 7:152 10.3389/fnagi.2015.00152 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bamidis P. D., Vivas A. B., Styliadis C., Frantzidis C., Klados M., Schlee W., et al. (2014). A review of physical and cognitive interventions in aging. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 44 206–220. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.03.019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bernard J. A., Peltier S. J., Wiggins J. L., Jaeggi S. M., Buschkuehl M., Fling B. W., et al. (2013). Disrupted cortico-cerebellar connectivity in older adults. Neuroimage 83C 103–119. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.06.042 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bressler S. L., Menon V. (2010). Large-scale brain networks in cognition: emerging methods and principles. Trends Cogn Sci. 14 277–290. 10.1016/j.tics.2010.04.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buckner R. L., Andrews-Hanna J. R., Schacter D. L. (2008). The brain’s default network: anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1124 1–38. 10.1196/annals.1440.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Calhoun V. D., Eichele T., Pearlson G. (2009). Functional brain networks in schizophrenia: a review. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 3:17 10.3389/neuro.09.017.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cao W., Luo C., Zhu B., Zhang D., Dong L., Gong J., et al. (2014). Resting-state functional connectivity in anterior cingulate cortex in normal aging. Front. Aging Neurosci. 6:280 10.3389/fnagi.2014.00280 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chapman S. B., Aslan S., Spence J. S., Hart J. J., Jr., Bartz E. K., Didehbani N., et al. (2015). Neural mechanisms of brain plasticity with complex cognitive training in healthy seniors. Cereb. Cortex 25 396–405. 10.1093/cercor/bht234 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chen X., Duan M., Xie Q., Lai Y., Dong L., Cao W., et al. (2015). Functional disconnection between the visual cortex and the sensorimotor cortex suggests a potential mechanism for self-disorder in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 166 151–157. 10.1016/j.schres.2015.06.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cheng Y., Wu W., Feng W., Wang J., Chen Y., Shen Y., et al. (2012). The effects of multi-domain versus single-domain cognitive training in non-demented older people: a randomized controlled trial. BMC Med. 10:30 10.1186/1741-7015-10-30 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cheng Y., Wu W., Wang J., Feng W., Wu X., Li C. (2011). Reliability and validity of the repeatable battery for the assessment of neuropsychological status in community-dwelling elderly. Arch. Med. Sci. 7 850–857. 10.5114/aoms.2011.25561 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Damoiseaux J. S., Beckmann C. F., Arigita E. J., Barkhof F., Scheltens P., Stam C. J., et al. (2008). Reduced resting-state brain activity in the “default network” in normal aging. Cereb. Cortex 18 1856–1864. 10.1093/cercor/bhm207 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fox M. D., Snyder A. Z., Vincent J. L., Corbetta M., Van Essen D. C., Raichle M. E. (2005). The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 102 9673–9678. 10.1073/pnas.0504136102 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fransson P. (2006). How default is the default mode of brain function? Further evidence from intrinsic BOLD signal fluctuations. Neuropsychologia 44 2836–2845. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.06.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grady C. (2012). The cognitive neuroscience of ageing. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 13 491–505. 10.1038/nrn3256 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guo W., Liu F., Zhang Z., Liu G., Liu J., Yu L., et al. (2015). Increased cerebellar functional connectivity with the default-mode network in unaffected siblings of schizophrenia patients at rest. Schizophr. Bull. 41 1317–1325. 10.1093/schbul/sbv062 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Habas C., Kamdar N., Nguyen D., Prater K., Beckmann C. F., Menon V., et al. (2009). Distinct cerebellar contributions to intrinsic connectivity networks. J. Neurosci. 29 8586–8594. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1868-09.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- He X., Qin W., Liu Y., Zhang X., Duan Y., Song J., et al. (2013). Abnormal salience network in normal aging and in amnestic mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Hum. Brain Mapp. 35 3446–3464. 10.1002/hbm.22414 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hedden T., Gabrieli J. D. (2004). Insights into the ageing mind: a view from cognitive neuroscience. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 5 87–96. 10.1038/nrn1323 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jiang S., Luo C., Liu Z., Hou C., Wang P., Dong L., et al. (2015). Altered local spontaneous brain activity in juvenile myoclonic epilepsy: a preliminary resting-state fMRI study. Neural Plast. 2015:635196 10.1155/2016/3547203 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kirchhoff B. A., Anderson B. A., Barch D. M., Jacoby L. L. (2012). Cognitive and neural effects of semantic encoding strategy training in older adults. Cereb. Cortex 22 788–799. 10.1093/cercor/bhr129 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kraft E. (2012). Cognitive function, physical activity, and aging: possible biological links and implications for multimodal interventions. Neuropsychol. Dev. Cogn. B Aging Neuropsychol. Cogn. 19 248–263. 10.1080/13825585.2011.645010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kurth F., Zilles K., Fox P. T., Laird A. R., Eickhoff S. B. (2010). A link between the systems: functional differentiation and integration within the human insula revealed by meta-analysis. Brain Struct. Funct. 214 519–534. 10.1007/s00429-010-0255-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Langer N., von Bastian C. C., Wirz H., Oberauer K., Jancke L. (2013). The effects of working memory training on functional brain network efficiency. Cortex 49 2424–2438. 10.1016/j.cortex.2013.01.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leung N. T., Tam H. M., Chu L. W., Kwok T. C., Chan F., Lam L. C., et al. (2015). Neural plastic effects of cognitive training on aging brain. Neural Plast. 2015:535618 10.1155/2015/535618 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li C., Wu W., Jin H., Zhang X., Xue H., He Y., et al. (2006). Successful aging in Shanghai, China: definition, distribution and related factors. Int. Psychogeriatr. 18 551–563. 10.1017/S1041610205002966 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li R., Zhu X., Yin S., Niu Y., Zheng Z., Huang X., et al. (2014). Multimodal intervention in older adults improves resting-state functional connectivity between the medial prefrontal cortex and medial temporal lobe. Front. Aging Neurosci. 6:39 10.3389/fnagi.2014.00039 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lim M. L., Collinson S. L., Feng L., Ng T. P. (2010). Cross-cultural application of the repeatable battery for the assessment of neuropsychological status (rbans): performances of elderly Chinese Singaporeans. Clin. Neuropsychol. 24 811–826. 10.1080/13854046.2010.490789 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luo C., Li Q., Lai Y., Xia Y., Qin Y., Liao W., et al. (2011). Altered functional connectivity in default mode network in absence epilepsy: a resting-state fMRI study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 32 438–449. 10.1002/hbm.21034 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luo C., Qiu C., Guo Z., Fang J., Li Q., Lei X., et al. (2012). Disrupted functional brain connectivity in partial epilepsy: a resting-state fMRI study. PLoS ONE 7:e28196 10.1371/journal.pone.0028196 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luo C., Tu S., Peng Y., Gao S., Li J., Dong L., et al. (2014a). Long-term effects of musical training and functional plasticity in salience system. Neural Plast. 2014:180138 10.1155/2014/180138 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luo C., Yang T., Tu S., Deng J., Liu D., Li Q., et al. (2014b). Altered intrinsic functional connectivity of the salience network in childhood absence epilepsy. J. Neurol. Sci. 339 189–195. 10.1016/j.jns.2014.02.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luo C., Zhang X., Cao X., Gan Y., Li T., Cheng Y., et al. (2016). The lateralization of intrinsic networks in the aging brain implicates the effects of cognitive training. Front. Aging Neurosci. 8:32 10.3389/fnagi.2016.00032 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luo C., Zhang Y., Cao W., Huang Y., Yang F., Wang J., et al. (2015). Altered structural and functional feature of striato-cortical circuit in benign epilepsy with centrotemporal spikes. Int. J. Neural. Syst. 25:1550027 10.1142/S0129065715500276 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mackey A. P., Miller Singley A. T., Bunge S. A. (2013). Intensive reasoning training alters patterns of brain connectivity at rest. J. Neurosci. 33 4796–4803. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4141-12.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Menon V. (2011). Large-scale brain networks and psychopathology: a unifying triple network model. Trends Cogn. Sci. 15 483–506. 10.1016/j.tics.2011.08.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meunier D., Achard S., Morcom A., Bullmore E. (2009). Age-related changes in modular organization of human brain functional networks. Neuroimage 44 715–723. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.09.062 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mevel K., Landeau B., Fouquet M., La Joie R., Villain N., Mezenge F., et al. (2013). Age effect on the default mode network, inner thoughts, and cognitive abilities. Neurobiol. Aging 34 1292–1301. 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2012.08.018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Neely A. S., Backman L. (1995). Effects of multifactorial memory training in old age: generalizability across tasks and individuals. J. Gerontol. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 50 134–140. 10.1093/geronb/50B.3.P134 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Onoda K., Ishihara M., Yamaguchi S. (2012). Decreased functional connectivity by aging is associated with cognitive decline. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 24 2186–2198. 10.1162/jocn_a_00269 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oswald W. D., Gunzelmann T., Rupprecht R., Hagen B. (2006). Differential effects of single versus combined cognitive and physical training with older adults: the SimA study in a 5-year perspective. Eur. J. Ageing 3 179–192. 10.1007/s10433-006-0035-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Park D. C., Bischof G. N. (2013). The aging mind: neuroplasticity in response to cognitive training. Dialogues Clin. Neurosci. 15 109–119. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Patel R., Spreng R. N., Turner G. R. (2013). Functional brain changes following cognitive and motor skills training: a quantitative meta-analysis. Neurorehabil. Neural Repair 27 187–199. 10.1177/1545968312461718 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Power J. D., Barnes K. A., Snyder A. Z., Schlaggar B. L., Petersen S. E. (2012). Spurious but systematic correlations in functional connectivity MRI networks arise from subject motion. Neuroimage 59 2142–2154. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.10.018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raichle M. E., MacLeod A. M., Snyder A. Z., Powers W. J., Gusnard D. A., Shulman G. L. (2001). A default mode of brain function. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 98 676–682. 10.1073/pnas.98.2.676 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ridderinkhof K. R., van den Wildenberg W. P., Segalowitz S. J., Carter C. S. (2004). Neurocognitive mechanisms of cognitive control: the role of prefrontal cortex in action selection, response inhibition, performance monitoring, and reward-based learning. Brain Cogn. 56 129–140. 10.1016/j.bandc.2004.09.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seeley W. W., Menon V., Schatzberg A. F., Keller J., Glover G. H., Kenna H., et al. (2007). Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control. J. Neurosci. 27 2349–2356. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5587-06.2007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shao J., Myers N., Yang Q., Feng J., Plant C., Bohm C., et al. (2012). Prediction of Alzheimer’s disease using individual structural connectivity networks. Neurobiol. Aging 33 2756–2765. 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2012.01.017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith S. M., Fox P. T., Miller K. L., Glahn D. C., Fox P. M., Mackay C. E., et al. (2009). Correspondence of the brain’s functional architecture during activation and rest. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 106 13040–13045. 10.1073/pnas.0905267106 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sridharan D., Levitin D. J., Menon V. (2008). A critical role for the right fronto-insular cortex in switching between central-executive and default-mode networks. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 105 12569–12574. 10.1073/pnas.0800005105 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Strenziok M., Parasuraman R., Clarke E., Cisler D. S., Thompson J. C., Greenwood P. M. (2014). Neurocognitive enhancement in older adults: comparison of three cognitive training tasks to test a hypothesis of training transfer in brain connectivity. Neuroimage 85(Pt 3) 1027–1039. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.07.069 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Takeuchi H., Taki Y., Nouchi R., Hashizume H., Sekiguchi A., Kotozaki Y., et al. (2013). Effects of working memory training on functional connectivity and cerebral blood flow during rest. Cortex 49 2106–2125. 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.09.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tang Y. Y., Posner M. I. (2014). Training brain networks and states. Trends Cogn. Sci. 18 345–350. 10.1016/j.tics.2014.04.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tanigawa T., Takechi H., Arai H., Yamada M., Nishiguchi S., Aoyama T. (2014). Effect of physical activity on memory function in older adults with mild Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive impairment. Geriatr. Gerontol. Int. 14 758–762. 10.1111/ggi.12159 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taylor V. A., Daneault V., Grant J., Scavone G., Breton E., Roffe-Vidal S., et al. (2013). Impact of meditation training on the default mode network during a restful state. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 8 4–14. 10.1093/scan/nsr087 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tomasi D., Volkow N. D. (2012). Aging and functional brain networks. Mol. Psychiatry 17 549–458. 10.1038/mp.2011.81 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Boxtel M. P., ten Tusscher M. P., Metsemakers J. F., Willems B., Jolles J. (2001). Visual determinants of reduced performance on the Stroop color-word test in normal aging individuals. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 23 620–627. 10.1076/jcen.23.5.620.1245 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Voss M. W., Erickson K. I., Prakash R. S., Chaddock L., Kim J. S., Alves H., et al. (2013). Neurobiological markers of exercise-related brain plasticity in older adults. Brain Behav. Immun. 28 90–99. 10.1016/j.bbi.2012.10.021 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Voss M. W., Prakash R. S., Erickson K. I., Basak C., Chaddock L., Kim J. S., et al. (2010). Plasticity of brain networks in a randomized intervention trial of exercise training in older adults. Front. Aging Neurosci. 2:32 10.3389/fnagi.2010.00032 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wu J. T., Wu H. Z., Yan C. G., Chen W. X., Zhang H. Y., He Y., et al. (2011). Aging-related changes in the default mode network and its anti-correlated networks: a resting-state fMRI study. Neurosci. Lett. 504 62–67. 10.1016/j.neulet.2011.08.059 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Xiao S., Yao P., Li X., Zhang M. (2002). Neuropsychological testing profiles of patients with Alzheimer’s Disease and mild cognitive impairment: a case-control study. Hong Kong J. Psychiatry 12 2–5. [Google Scholar]
- Yin S., Zhu X., Li R., Niu Y., Wang B., Zheng Z., et al. (2014). Intervention-induced enhancement in intrinsic brain activity in healthy older adults. Sci. Rep. 4:7309 10.1038/srep07309 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zheng Z., Zhu X., Yin S., Wang B., Niu Y., Huang X., et al. (2015). Combined cognitive-psychological-physical intervention induces reorganization of intrinsic functional brain architecture in older adults. Neural Plast 2015:713104 10.1155/2015/713104 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhou J., Greicius M. D., Gennatas E. D., Growdon M. E., Jang J. Y., Rabinovici G. D., et al. (2010). Divergent network connectivity changes in behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Brain 133 1352–1367. 10.1093/brain/awq075 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.