Abstract
The potential for selective harvests to induce rapid evolutionary change is an important question for conservation and evolutionary biology, with numerous biological, social and economic implications. We analyze 39 years of phenotypic data on horn size in bighorn sheep (Ovis canadensis) subject to intense trophy hunting for 23 years, after which harvests nearly ceased. Our analyses revealed a significant decline in genetic value for horn length of rams, consistent with an evolutionary response to artificial selection on this trait. The probability that the observed change in male horn length was due solely to drift is 9.9%. Female horn length and male horn base, traits genetically correlated to the trait under selection, showed weak declining trends. There was no temporal trend in genetic value for female horn base circumference, a trait not directly targeted by selective hunting and not genetically correlated with male horn length. The decline in genetic value for male horn length stopped, but was not reversed, when hunting pressure was drastically reduced. Our analysis provides support for the contention that selective hunting led to a reduction in horn length through evolutionary change. It also confirms that after artificial selection stops, recovery through natural selection is slow.
Keywords: conservation biology, contemporary evolution, quantitative genetics
Introduction
Human activities such as habitat modifications, expanding road networks, overexploitation and climate change affect animal populations. While the demographic impacts of humans on wild species are clear, their evolutionary impacts are debated (Loehr et al. 2007; Hard et al. 2008). Intense exploitation by humans may outpace (Darimont et al. 2009) or oppose (Carlson et al. 2007) the selective effects of natural predators, potentially leading to evolutionary changes in behaviour, phenotype or life history (Hard et al. 2008; Devine et al. 2012). van Wijk et al. (2013) showed that selective harvesting of guppies (Poecilia reticulata) led to changes in size and in the frequency of alleles associated with size in just two generation. Human‐induced evolution may also impair population persistence or prevent recovery (Swain et al. 2007; Uusi‐Heikkilä et al. 2015). While numerous studies of fishes report evidence of evolution induced by intense harvest (reviewed in Hutchings and Fraser 2008), evidence for evolution through selective harvest in terrestrial species remains scarce and controversial (Coltman et al. 2003; Garel et al. 2007; Mysterud 2011; Traill et al. 2014), partly because the statistical techniques used to quantify evolutionary changes using pedigrees in earlier studies have been questioned (Postma 2006; Hadfield et al. 2010).
Trophy hunting can be an important component of many conservation programs (Leader‐Williams et al. 2001), and its economic revenues are partly driven by expectation of large trophy size (Festa‐Bianchet and Lee 2009; Crosmary et al. 2013). In most of Canada, sport harvest of mountain sheep (Ovis canadensis and O. dalli) rams is based on a phenotypic definition of minimum horn curl that establishes whether or not a ram can be shot, with an unlimited number of permits available to resident hunters (Festa‐Bianchet et al. 2014). In wild sheep, horn size is a key determinant of success in male‐male competition over breeding opportunities (Coltman et al. 2002). Artificial selection favoring shorter horns through hunting mortality, however, sets in 2–3 years before natural selection favoring longer horns through reproductive success (Coltman et al. 2002). Multiple studies report that males with fast‐growing horns, that would enjoy high mating success at 8–10 years of age, are harvested at 4–7 years, conferring a reproductive advantage to small‐horned males that, in the absence of size‐selective harvests, would normally be outcompeted (Festa‐Bianchet et al. 2004, 2014; Loehr et al. 2007; Hengeveld and Festa‐Bianchet 2011; Douhard et al. In Press).
One approach to study evolution in nature, often referred to as the animal model, involves mixed models combining a pedigree with data on phenotype and environmental conditions to estimate genetic parameters (Kruuk 2004). Using this approach, Coltman et al. (2003) used a pedigree up to six generation deep to report a decline in estimated breeding values (EBV) of horn length and body mass in bighorn rams over 30 years, suggesting an evolutionary response to size‐selective harvests. Their analyses, however, were criticised for not adequately accounting for environmental effects on phenotype, for the error in estimation of breeding values and for the effect of drift; possibly leading to exaggerated estimates of evolutionary change (Postma 2006; Hadfield et al. 2010). Hence, the importance of evolution in the observed change in phenotype following selective harvesting is still debated.
A recent paper used data from the individually monitored population of bighorn sheep of Ram Mountain to parameterise an Integral Projection Model and show a decline in body mass, but argued that the phenotypic response to harvest was only demographic (Traill et al. 2014). The statistical criticisms and alternative analyses listed above cast doubt on the conclusion that selective hunting could lead to evolutionary changes. Coltman et al. (2003) drew that conclusion after analysing data for the only sport‐hunted population of ungulates for which a pedigree and horn measurements are available (Pelletier et al. 2012). By extension, these criticisms also question phenotype‐based studies that reported long‐term trends consistent with an evolutionary impact of selective hunting (Garel et al. 2007; Hengeveld and Festa‐Bianchet 2011). A clear understanding of the importance of evolutionary change due to selective harvesting is of critical importance to those responsible for managing harvested wild populations (Allendorf and Hard 2009). A reanalysis of the Ram Mountain data is therefore warranted, particularly because the 10‐fold decline in harvests after 1996 provides an opportunity to test the impacts of changes in harvest pressure on trait evolution (Douhard et al. In Press).
Here, we use a Bayesian animal model to analyse an expanded database on bighorn sheep from Ram Mountain, adding 9 years of data to those available to Coltman et al. (2003) and taking into account subsequent statistical criticisms (Postma 2006; Hadfield et al. 2010). We also compare a period of intense harvest with a period when harvest was first dramatically reduced, then stopped. This allowed us to compare temporal trends in genetic values under heavy and very light artificial selection. To maximise the use of phenotypic information, we considered data on male and female traits using a multivariate model. Genetic correlations have already been established among some of these traits (Poissant et al. 2012), and proper estimation of breeding values must account for genetic covariance (Wolak et al. 2015). By including phenotypic data on females we could also compare temporal changes in traits that are (male horn base and female horn length) and are not (female horn base) genetically correlated to male horn length (Poissant et al. 2012). We expected to see temporal changes in EBV in male horn length only under heavy harvest. Male horn base circumference is particularly interesting because it is correlated with horn length and likely affects male‐male competition by contributing to horn mass, but is not a direct target of selective hunting. We expected strong selective effects on male horn length, the trait most directly related to the legal definition of harvestable ram (Festa‐Bianchet et al. 2014). We expected a response similar to male horn length for male horn base and female horn length given their strong genetic correlations with male horn length, and no response in female horn base, which has a weak genetic correlation with male horn length (genetic correlations of 0.72, 1 and −0.28 respectively; Poissant et al. 2012). To compare our results with previous studies on this population, we also built animal models using univariate one‐sex (with phenotypic data on males only) and two‐sex (phenotypic data from both sexes) approach. Univariate models are also less prone to problems when fitted with limited data given their simpler structure (Wilson et al. 2010).
Material and methods
Study population and phenotypic data
Bighorn sheep at Ram Mountain, Alberta, Canada are intensively monitored. The study area is 30 km east of the Rockies (52°8′N, 115°8′W, elevation 1082–2173 m), on a mountainous outcrop dominated by cliffs, rock scree and alpine meadows. Since 1972, sheep have been marked with ear tags and collars. Each year, between May and September, sheep were repeatedly captured in a corral trap baited with salt. Rams were captured on average 2.6 times per year. At each capture, horn length in cm was measured along the outside curvature with a flexible tape. To reduce the potential measurement error caused by horn wear or breakage, we used the longest horn in analyses. Horn base circumference was also measured in cm, and we analysed the mean of the left and right measurements. Nearly all individuals (95%) were first captured as lambs or yearlings, so their exact age was known. For the others, age was determined using horn annuli.
The study population was hunted until 2011 based on a morphological definition of ‘legal’ ram. From late August to October, rams were at a risk of being shot only if they met that definition, which specified a minimum degree of horn curl and was correlated with horn length (Festa‐Bianchet et al. 2014). Artificial selection through hunting, however, changed over time. In 1996, the minimum horn curl of a ‘legal’ ram was increased from 4/5 to full curl (Fig. S1). This change, implemented at a time when horn size had declined (Coltman et al. 2003), drastically decreased the harvest, with only four rams shot in the following 15 years. Mean harvest was 2.26 rams/year in 1973–1995 and only 0.27 rams/year in 1996–2010. Hunting was closed in 2011. Therefore, we compared trends in the EBV of morphological traits for cohorts of 1973–1996 (referred to as the hunted period) and 1996–2011 (non‐hunted period; see below). Based on the average age of fathers at Ram Mountain (7.3 years), we monitored 3.3 generations under strong artificial selection followed by 2.2 generations under natural selection.
We first adjusted all traits to September 15 using a mixed model approach (Martin and Pelletier 2011). As adult females and adult males display different growth curves, we used sex‐specific linear models to account for capture date and fitted one model per year to allow for environmental variability. Trait was fitted as a function of the square root of Julian date, considering May 25th as day 1. With this modelling approach, individual identity can be used to estimate an individual intercept and slope, providing a more accurate standardization than classical least square regression (Martin and Pelletier 2011). The procedure was used for horn length and horn base. A total of 2295 adjusted phenotypic measurements where obtained from 510 females and 497 males.
Pedigree reconstruction
Since 1972, maternities were assigned from observation of suckling behaviour. Since 1988, DNA samples have allowed the assignation of paternities based on 26 microsatellite loci with a confidence threshold of 95% using CERVUS (Coltman et al. 2005). The pedigree in 2014 contained 864 maternal links involving 254 dams and 528 paternal links involving 79 sampled and 37 unsampled sires, the latter identified using COLONY (Jones and Wang 2010). Unsampled sires include rams that died before we began sampling for DNA and immigrants that are on Ram Mountain only for the rut.
Quantitative genetic analyses
Analyses of horn base include phenotypic data of individuals aged 2–10 years between 1975 and 2013. For horn length, however, we only included data for sheep aged 2–4 years. Horns frequently break, and the chance of horn damage increases with age. Many old males have broken horns, missing up to the first 2 years of growth. Our data suggest that by 4 years of age ewes have reached 97% of the horn length they will have at age 8 (including the effect of breakage), and rams 73%. In addition, after age 4 the sample of rams is biased because those with longer horns are removed by hunters (Coltman et al. 2002). To reduce the importance of maternal effects on phenotypes (Wilson et al. 2005), analyses excluded phenotypic data of lambs and yearling (Réale et al. 1999; Wilson, Kruuk, and Coltman 2005).
The multivariate animal model was fitted using four traits: male horn length, female horn length, male horn base and female horn base. Phenotypic variance was then partitioned into its components, including additive genetic variance. The model also included sheep identity, year of measurement and year of birth as random effects to assess the amount of variance due to permanent, yearly and cohort environmental effects respectively. Including year of measurement and year of birth as random effects accounts for both short‐ and long‐term environmental effects, including changes in density, weather and forage quality. The year effect is necessary to obtain unbiased estimates of breeding value but it may also partly absorb temporal genetic trends, making this analysis conservative. Maternal identity was not included since the exclusion of lambs and yearlings minimized maternal effects. Age was included as a categorical fixed effect. To compare our results with previously published studies on this population we also examined univariate animal models (see supplementary material). We tested univariate models using male phenotype only (SI 2) to obtain results comparable to Coltman et al. (2003). We also fitted univariate models including both male and female phenotype to increase power (SI 3). These models are further described and their results presented in the supplementary material (SI 2–3).
The animal model estimates the breeding value of each individual. To correctly estimate breeding values and their associated error (Hadfield et al. 2010), the model was fitted using a Bayesian method with MCMCglmm version 2.21. We used a multivariate inverse‐Wishart prior to obtain the most objective results possible. Models were run using two chains for 8 500 000 iterations, with a thinning of 75 000 and a burn‐in of 1 000 000 iterations. A sensitivity analysis evaluated the robustness of the model to different prior specifications (Fig. S2).
Temporal change in estimated breeding value
We compared temporal trends in EBV to those obtained based on different models of evolutionary change. For each realization of the MCMC chain of the animal model, we calculated mean EBV by cohort and the slope in mean EBV as a function of cohort (ße) for both the hunted and not‐hunted periods to obtain a posterior distribution of slopes. We compared this distribution to the posterior distribution of slopes of alternative models, which included no change, drift, stasis and expected evolutionary response. To compare the posterior distributions of slopes, we subtracted each realization of the posterior distribution of the alternative model to that of the distribution ße, obtaining a distribution of differences. From this distribution, we can obtain the mean difference between the expected and observed distributions as well as the confidence interval of the difference.
First, as done previously by Coltman et al. (2003), we compared slopes in EBV to 0. Second, following Hadfield et al. (2010), we compared the slopes in EBV to those obtained from simulated drift. To do so, we simulated random breeding values down the pedigree for each of the 1000 posterior samples of the animal model based on the estimated additive genetic variance. We then fitted a linear regression to the cohort mean of these random breeding values to obtain the slopes due to drift for each posterior sample. Third, we compared observed change in estimated breeding value to stasis (Hunt 2007), a pattern likely to occur under stabilizing selection. To simulate stasis, mean cohort breeding values were randomly drawn from a normal distribution with a mean of 0 and a variance equal to the observed variance in mean cohort EBV.
We also compared observed change in EBV to the response to selection predicted by the secondary theorem of selection (Morrissey et al. 2012). This theorem states that change should be equal to the additive genetic covariance between the trait of interest and relative fitness. We used longevity as a fitness measure, which we divided by mean cohort longevity to obtain relative fitness. We used longevity rather than reproductive success because molecular assignments of paternities only began in 1988. We then fitted separate bivariate animal models of trait and fitness for each of the studied traits. The predicted response to selection was then extracted from the G matrix and divided by the mean generation time (7.3 years, the average age of fathers in our population) to obtain a predicted change per year (Table S1). Predicted response to selection could only be estimated for the hunted period due to the limited number of individuals of known longevity born after hunting pressure was reduced. The proportion of iterations for which the slope for the estimated breeding value (ße) is lower than that of the random breeding value (ßr) was also calculated to estimate the probability that the trend was not caused solely by drift.
Results
Animal model analyses
Estimates of variance components and heritability for horn length and base (Table 1) showed that heritability was >0 for all traits. The trait with the highest posterior mode for heritability was male horn length, followed by male horn base, female horn length and female horn base. Permanent environmental effects explained much of the variance in female but not in male traits. Cohort always explained a significant part of phenotypic variance, while the effects of year and permanent environment varied among traits (Table 1). Confirming previous analyses (Poissant et al. 2012), genetic correlations between male horn length, female horn length and male horn base were high while female horn base had low genetic correlation with other traits (Table 2).
Table 1.
Horn length male | Horn length female | Horn base male | Horn base female | |
---|---|---|---|---|
h 2 | 0.397 (0.203–0.534) | 0.223 (0.090–0.446) | 0.250 (0.119–0.413) | 0.265 (0.148–0.335) |
ID | 0.025 (0.003–0.211) | 0.376 (0.203–0.540) | 0.098 (0.016–0.268) | 0.171 (0.110–0.265) |
yr | 0.110 (0.039–0.168) | 0.022 (0.010–0.052) | 0.193 (0.109–0.289) | 0.161 (0.112–0.268) |
Cohort | 0.363 (0.211–0.528) | 0.149 (0.071–0.286) | 0.203 (0.097–0.354) | 0.212 (0.107–0.291) |
h 2 refers to the narrow‐sense heritability, ID refers to the proportion of phenotypic variance explained by permanent environment (identity of the sheep), yr refers to the proportion of phenotypic variance explained by year of measurement and cohort refers to the proportion of phenotypic variance explained by year of birth.
Table 2.
Hl‐M | Hl‐F | Hb‐M | Hb‐F | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Hl‐M | 17.884 (9.82–25.881) | 0.921 (0.557–0.981) | 0.878 (0.729–0.959) | 0.189 (−0.285–0.538) |
Hl‐F | 5.345 (1.928–8.144) | 1.622 (0.748–3.963) | 0.799 (0.275–0.939) | 0.368 (−0.063–0.610) |
Hb‐M | 5.435 (2.797–9.666) | 1.274 (0.318–2.881) | 2.915 (1.124–4.485) | 0.286 (−0.203–0.656) |
Hb‐F | 0.070 (−0.542–1.059) | 0.187 (−0.062–0.481) | 0.182 (−0.164–0.508) | 0.183 (0.119–0.270) |
Values below the diagonal are the posterior modes of genetic covariance between traits: male horn length (HL‐M), female horn length (HL‐F), male horn base (HB‐M) and female horn base (HB‐F). Values above the diagonal are the posterior modes of genetic correlations. Values in parentheses represent the 95% Bayesian posterior interval of highest density.
Temporal changes in EBV
Temporal changes in mean phenotypic values over 39 years differed between traits (Fig. 1). A temporal change in EBV was also observed (Fig. 2). During the hunted period from 1973 to 1996, the EBV of male horn length declined significantly (ß = −0.119; CI = −0.248, −0.006). Similarly, genetically correlated traits also appeared to decline. Female horn length breeding value declined with a slope of −0.027 (CI = −0.063, 0.013), while EBV for male horn base had a slope of −0.030 (CI = −0.076, 0.019). Unlike male horn length, the breeding value of female horn base appeared to increase, with a slope of 0.005 (CI = −0.008, 0.016). We then compared observed changes in EBV for male horn length to those expected under various models of evolutionary change (Table S2; Fig. 3). The observed temporal change in estimated breeding value differed significantly from 0 (Pr[ße < 0] = 0.974). While observed EBV did not differ significantly from that predicted by other models, the probability of declining more than expected by drift alone (Pr[ße < ßr]) was 0.901. The observed temporal change in EBV was most similar to that predicted by the secondary theorem of selection (expected change per generation of −0.76, Fig. 3) with a posterior difference of 0.016, while the posterior differences of other models of evolution ranged from 0.117 to 0.120 (Table S2). Similarly, for female horn length, observed trends were most similar to those predicted by the secondary theorem of selection (expected change per generation of −0.10) with a posterior difference of 0.013. Other models of evolution all had similar differences of 0.027. The probability of declining more than expected by drift alone (Pr[ße < ßr]) was 0.816. For male horn base, observed trends were also most similar to those predicted by the secondary theorem of selection (expected change per generation of −0.13) with a posterior difference of 0.013. Other models of evolution all had similar differences of 0.030. The probability of declining more than expected by drift alone (Pr[ße < ßr]) was 0.796. Finally, for female horn base, all models were similar. The predicted response according to the secondary theorem of selection (expected change per generation of 0.005) had a difference of −0.004. Other models had differences of −0.005.
After the near‐cessation of hunting in 1996, average EBV remained stable or showed a weak tendency to increase, with slopes of 0.053 (CI: −0.174, 0.282) and 0.021 (CI: −0.054, 0.104) for horn length of males and females respectively. For horn base, EBVs after the change in regulations had slopes of 0.032 (CI: −0.056, 0.137) and −0.006 (CI: −0.029, 0.018) for males and females respectively. The probabilities that the slope in EBV of male and female horn length increased after the change in hunting regulations were 0.894 and 0.847 respectively. Similarly, the probabilities that the slope in EBVs of male and female horn base increased after the change in regulations were 0.866 and 0.226. Unfortunately, we could not compare observed changes in EBVs for male horn length to those expected under various models of evolutionary change for the not‐hunted period. Because of the shorter period and a smaller population size, we did not have adequate statistical power to estimate predicted responses to selection. A comparison of observed temporal trends in EBV to alternative models of evolutions such as drift or stasis suggested that all models were quite similar (Table S2).
Results for the univariate animal models were qualitatively similar to the multivariate model presented here. For the male‐only model, the posterior probabilities of declining more than expected by drift (Pr[ße < ßr]) were 0.874 and 0.629 for horn length and horn base. Posterior probabilities of declining more than expected by drift changed to 0.560, and 0.637 respectively after the change in hunting regulations (for complete results, see Table S3). For the two‐sex model, the posterior probabilities that breeding values declined more than expected by drift (Pr[ße < ßr]) were 0.985 and 0.503 for horn length of males and females, respectively. These probabilities were 0.745 and 0.582 for horn base of males and females (Table S4). Changes in breeding value had a low probability of being steeper than expected from drift after 1996 (0.560 and 0.637 for horn length, and horn base of males; 0.543 and 0.555 for the same traits in females).
Discussion
We assessed whether temporal genetic trends in wild bighorn sheep were consistent with evolutionary changes expected from selective pressures acting on traits targeted or not targeted by trophy hunting. Using a 39‐year dataset, we expand upon previous results (Coltman et al. 2003), using a statistical approach (Hadfield et al. 2010) that is robust to biases likely affecting earlier estimates of breeding values. A model including a term for the random effect of year, as suggested by Postma (2006) confirms a statistically significant negative trend in EBV for male horn length during a period of intense harvest. Hadfield et al. (2010) suggested an even more conservative test, comparing the observed change in breeding value to simulated changes that may occur through genetic drift. The observed decline in male horn length breeding value had a probability of 90.1% of being greater than expected from drift alone, although this probability varied when using simpler univariate models (87.4% and 98.5% depending on the univariate animal model used; Tables S1–S2). The decline in breeding value had a very high probability of being greater than that expected under genetic drift in a univariate model that included information on phenotype and pedigree from both sexes but did not include genetic covariance with other traits (Table S2).
The decline in male horn length breeding values appeared to stop when hunting pressure was greatly reduced. While horn length declined during the hunting period, female horn base, a trait not subjected to trophy hunting and with low genetic correlation (0.189) to male horn length (Table 2), did not decline, supporting the contention that the decline in horn length was partly due to artificial selection. Further, female horn length and male horn base, traits genetically correlated to male horn length but not under selection, showed responses similar to male horn length. Overall, these results provide compelling evidence of a response to artificial selection while refuting the hypothesis that the observed changes were entirely caused by changes in environment. Our study population is small (average of 28.5 adult rams, yearly range 8–61) and after the hunting regulations were changed it declined partly through cougar (Puma concolor) predation (Festa‐Bianchet et al. 2006), averaging 17 rams. Therefore, drift may play a substantial role in changes in allele frequencies and fluctuations in breeding values over time.
Traill et al. (2014) suggested that all phenotypic changes in mass observed at Ram Mountain were due to demographic changes in response to hunting. Our analyses of horn length, however, support the result of Coltman et al. (2003) and suggest that observed changes in horn length were due to an evolutionary response to artificial selection. The difference between these studies can be explained in two ways. First, the simulations presented by Traill et al. (2014) were based on body mass. Although horn length and body mass have a moderate genetic correlation (0.48, Poissant et al. 2012), mass is not a direct target of trophy hunting. More importantly, the inheritance function in Traill et al. (2014) links parent and offspring phenotype solely upon the relationship between parental mass at conception and offspring mass at weaning: it does not allow large fathers to produce offspring that grow to become large adults (Hedrick et al. 2014; Chevin 2015) despite strong heritability of adult mass in this population (Poissant et al. 2012). The ‘inheritance’ function is nearly zero for father‐offspring, while the mother‐offspring function explains only about 5% of the variance in weaning mass (Festa‐Bianchet and Jorgenson 1998; Réale et al. 1999).
Between 1973 and 1996, the horn length of bighorn rams on Ram Mountain declined by nearly 30% (Coltman et al. 2003). It has since recovered by about 13%. When the artificial selection stopped, EBV did not increase, but there was a phenotypic increase in horn length. The very low population density in the last 15 years may have contributed to the non‐genetic increase in mean age‐corrected horn length, which remains smaller than 30–40 years ago (Fig. 1). Environmental factors such as population density and weather play important roles in horn growth (Jorgenson et al. 1993; Festa‐Bianchet et al. 2014). For example, a doubling of population size at Ram Mountain contributed to a decline in ram horn length, which, however, remained stable during an earlier period of experimental population control through ewe removals (Jorgenson et al. 1998). Therefore, it is important to adequately partition environmental and genetic phenotypic changes. Including both cohort and year in the animal model should control for both long‐ and short‐term effects of these variables on the phenotype (Wilson et al. 2010).
It seems reasonable to expect that strong artificial selection on heritable traits may lead to evolutionary changes (Garland and Rose 2009). A study on 74 domestic sheep (Ovis aries) breeds found strong genetic signals of selection for the absence of horns and for other traits such as body size, reproduction and pigmentation (Kijas et al. 2012). Evidence for the evolutionary effects of selective hunting in wild terrestrial species, however, remains scarce and controversial (Mysterud 2011). We suggest that evidence is scarce partly because it requires detailed long‐term data on genotypes, phenotypes, vital rates, population fluctuations, harvest pressure and environmental changes in harvested populations. Most longitudinal studies of wild vertebrates that have collected these data have been conducted on unharvested populations. There is abundant support for artificial selection in commercially‐exploited fish and recent studies provide evidence of a genetic response to that selection over a few generations (Swain et al. 2007; van Wijk et al. 2013). Therefore, it should not be surprising to find an effect of artificial selection over about 3–4 generations of bighorn sheep, given that rams with 4/5‐curl horn faced a 40% yearly probability of being shot and that the negative selective pressure through hunting started 2–3 years before large‐horned rams could achieve high reproductive success (Coltman et al. 2002). Long‐term phenotypic data from harvested rams support this contention by showing temporal declines in horn length in populations subject to high harvest pressure. Age‐specific horn size of Rocky Mountain bighorn rams declined in Alberta (Festa‐Bianchet et al. 2014) but not in the neighbouring province of British Columbia, where a more conservative definition of ‘legal’ ram reduces harvest pressure (Hengeveld and Festa‐Bianchet 2011). Similarly, in Stone's rams (Ovis dalli), early horn growth declined under intense selective harvest, but not under lower hunting pressure (Douhard et al. In Press).
Using detailed monitoring of a harvested population, we provide evidence that horn length – a trait directly targeted by trophy hunting – declined in response to intense artificial selection. The lack of evolutionary recovery in mean horn length breeding values after harvest stopped supports the hypothesis that recovery from potentially maladaptive human‐induced evolution is slow, likely because natural selective pressures are weaker than artificial ones (Swain et al. 2007; Allendorf and Hard 2009). Given the substantial economic importance of trophy hunting (Foote and Wenzel 2009) and its potential role in conservation (Leader‐Williams et al. 2001), it is critical to assess what levels of selective harvest can drive evolution in game species.
Data archiving statement
The data used in this paper were collected over 39 years and are the infrastructure for several ongoing and planned research programs. They will be available in 10 years from the Dryad Digital Repository: http://dx.doi.org/10.5061/dryad.41d7q. They are also available upon request to anyone who wishes to collaborate with us or repeat our analysis.
Supporting information
Acknowledgements
We are grateful to Bill Wishart, Anne Hubbs, Chiarastella Feder, and Jon Jorgenson for their support of the Ram Mountain research program, to Jack Hogg for initiating tissue sample collections and analyses, to Dany Garant and Alastair Wilson for insightful discussions, and to all assistants and students who worked on this program over decades. M.F.B., D.W.C., and F.P. are funded by NSERC Discovery Grants. F.P. holds the Canada Research Chair in Evolutionary Demography and Conservation. Our research was also supported by the Government of Alberta, the Université de Sherbrooke and an Alberta Conservation Association Challenge Grant in Biodiversity. The authors declare no conflicts of interest.
Literature cited
- Allendorf, F. W. , and Hard J. J. 2009. Human‐induced evolution caused by unnatural selection through harvest of wild animals. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 106(Suppl 1):9987–9994. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carlson, S. M. , Edeline E., Asbjørn Vøllestad L., Haugen T. O., Winfield I. J., Fletcher J. M., Ben James J. et al. 2007. Four decades of opposing natural and human‐induced artificial selection acting on Windermere pike (Esox lucius). Ecology Letters 10:512–521. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chevin, L.‐M. 2015. Evolution of adult size depends on genetic variance in growth trajectories: a comment on analyses of evolutionary dynamics using integral projection. Methods in Ecology and Evolution 6:981–986. [Google Scholar]
- Coltman, D. W. , Festa‐Bianchet M., Jorgenson J. T., and Strobeck C. 2002. Age‐dependent sexual selection in bighorn rams. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 269:165–172. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coltman, D. W. , O'Donoghue P., Jorgenson J. T., Hogg J. T., Strobeck C., and Festa‐Bianchet M. 2003. Undesirable evolutionary consequences of trophy hunting. Nature 426:655–658. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coltman, D. W. , O'Donoghue P., Hogg J. T., and Festa‐Bianchet M. 2005. Selection and genetic (co)variance in bighorn sheep. Evolution 59:1372–1382. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crosmary, W. G. , Loveridge A. J., Ndaimani H., Lebel S., Booth V., Côté S. D., and Fritz H. 2013. Trophy hunting in Africa: long‐term trends in antelope horn size. Animal Conservation 16:648–660. [Google Scholar]
- Darimont, C. T. , Carlson S. M., Kinnison M. T., Paquet P. C., Reimchen T. E., and Wilmers C. C.. 2009. Human predators outpace other agents of trait change in the wild. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences United States of America 106:952–954. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Devine, J. A. , Wright P. J., Pardoe H. E., Heino M., and Fraser D. J. 2012. Comparing rates of contemporary evolution in life‐history traits for exploited fish stocks. Canadian Journal of Fisheries and Aquatic Sciences 69:1105–1120. [Google Scholar]
- Douhard, M. , Festa‐Bianchet M., Pelletier F., Gaillard J.‐M., and Bonenfant C. In Press. Changes in horn size of Stone's sheep over four decades correlate with trophy hunting pressure. Ecological Applications. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Festa‐Bianchet, M. , and Jorgenson J. T. 1998. Selfish mothers: reproductive expenditure and resource availability in bighorn ewes. Behavioral Ecology 9:144–150. [Google Scholar]
- Festa‐Bianchet, M. , and Lee R. 2009. Guns, sheep, and genes: when and why trophy hunting may be a selective pressure In Dickson B., Hutton J., and Adams W. M., eds. Recreational Hunting. Conservation and Rural Livelihoods, pp. 94–107. Wiley‐Blackwell, Oxford. [Google Scholar]
- Festa‐Bianchet, M. , Coltman D. W., Turelli L., and Jorgenson J. T. 2004. Relative allocation to horn and body growth in bighorn rams varies with resource availability. Behavioral Ecology 15:305–312. [Google Scholar]
- Festa‐Bianchet, M. , Coulson T., Gaillard J. M., Hogg J. T., and Pelletier F. 2006. Stochastic predation events and population persistence in bighorn sheep. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 273:1537–1543. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Festa‐Bianchet, M. , Pelletier F., Jorgenson J. T., Feder C., and Hubbs A. 2014. Decrease in horn size and increase in age of trophy sheep in Alberta over 37 years. The Journal of Wildlife Management 78:133–141. [Google Scholar]
- Foote, L. , and Wenzel G. W. 2009. Polar bear conservation hunting in Canada: economics, culture and unintended consequences In Freeman M. M. R., and Foote L., eds. Inuit, Polar Bear and Sustainable Use, pp. 13–24. CCI Press, Edmonton. [Google Scholar]
- Garel, M. , Cugnasse J. M., Maillard D., Gaillard J. M., Hewison A. J., and Dubray D. 2007. Selective harvesting and habitat loss produce long‐term life history changes in a mouflon population. Ecological Applications 17:1607–1618. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garland, T. , and Rose M. R. 2009. Experimental Evolution: concepts, Methods, and Applications of Selection Experiments. University of California Press, Berkeley, 752 pp. [Google Scholar]
- Hadfield, J. D. , Wilson A. J., Garant D., Sheldon B. C., and Kruuk L. E. 2010. The misuse of BLUP in ecology and evolution. The American Naturalist 175:116–125. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hard, J. J. , Gross M. R., Heino M., Hilborn R., Kope R. G., Law R., and Reynolds J. D. 2008. SYNTHESIS: Evolutionary consequences of fishing and their implications for salmon. Evolutionary Applications 1:388–408. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hedrick, P. W. , Coltman D. W., Festa‐Bianchet M., and Pelletier F.. 2014. Not surprisingly, no inheritance of a trait results in no evolution. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences United States of America 111:E4810. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hengeveld, P. E. , and Festa‐Bianchet M. 2011. Harvest regulations and artificial selection on horn size in male bighorn sheep. The Journal of Wildlife Management 75:189–197. [Google Scholar]
- Hunt, G . 2007. The relative importance of directional change, random walks, and stasis in the evolution of fossil lineages. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences U.S.A. 104:18404–18408. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hutchings, J. A. , and Fraser D. J. 2008. The nature of fisheries‐ and farming‐induced evolution. Molecular Ecology 17:294–313. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jones, O. R. , and Wang J. 2010. COLONY: a program for parentage and sibship inference from multilocus genotype data. Molecular Ecology Resources 10:551–555. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jorgenson, J. T. , Festa‐Bianchet M., Lucherini M., and Wishart W. D. 1993. Effects of body size, population density, and maternal characteristics on age at first reproduction in bighorn ewes. Canadian Journal of Zoology 71:2509–2517. [Google Scholar]
- Jorgenson, J. T. , Festa‐Bianchet M., and Wishart W. D. 1998. Effects of population density on horn development of bighorn rams. Journal of Wildlife Management 62:1011–1020. [Google Scholar]
- Kijas, J. W. , Lenstra J. A., Hayes B., Boitard S., Porto Neto L. R., San Cristobal M., Servin B. et al. 2012. Genome‐wide analysis of the world's sheep breeds reveals high levels of historic mixture and strong recent selection. PLoS Biology 10:e1001258. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kruuk, L. E. 2004. Estimating genetic parameters in natural populations using the “animal model”. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London: Series B, Biological Sciences 359:873–890. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leader‐Williams, N. , Smith R. J., and Walpole M. J. 2001. Elephant hunting and conservation. Science 293:2203–2204. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Loehr, J. , Carey J., Hoefs M., Suhonen J., and Ylonen H. 2007. Horn growth rate and longevity: implications for natural and artificial selection in thinhorn sheep (Ovis dalli). Journal of Evolutionary Biology 20:818–828. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martin, J. G. A. , and Pelletier F. 2011. Measuring growth patterns in the field: effects of sampling regime and methods on standardized estimates. Canadian Journal of Zoology 89:529–537. [Google Scholar]
- Morrissey, M. L. B. , Parker D. J., Korsten P. R., Pemberton J. M., Kruuk L. E. B., and Wilson A. J. 2012. The prediction of adaptive evolution: empirical application of the secondary theorem of selection and comparison to the Breeder's equation. Evolution 66:2399–2410. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mysterud, A. 2011. Selective harvesting of large mammals: how often does it result in directional selection? Journal of Applied Ecology 48:827–834. [Google Scholar]
- Pelletier, F. , Festa‐Bianchet M., and Jorgenson J. T. 2012. Data from selective harvests underestimate temporal trends in quantitative traits. Biology Letters 8:878–881. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Poissant, J. , Davis C. S., Malenfant R. M., Hogg J. T., and Coltman D. W. 2012. QTL mapping for sexually dimorphic fitness‐related traits in wild bighorn sheep. Heredity 108:256–263. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Postma, E. 2006. Implications of the difference between true and predicted breeding values for the study of natural selection and micro‐evolution. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 19:309–320. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Réale, D. , Festa‐Bianchet M., and Jorgenson J. T. 1999. Heritability of body mass varies with age and season in wild bighorn sheep. Heredity 83:526–532. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Swain, D. P. , Sinclair A. F., and Mark Hanson J. 2007. Evolutionary response to size‐selective mortality in an exploited fish population. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 274:1015–1022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Traill, L. W. , Schindler S., and Coulson T. 2014. Demography, not inheritance, drives phenotypic change in hunted bighorn sheep. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences United States of America 111:13223–13228. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Uusi‐Heikkilä, S. , Whiteley A. R., Kuparinen A., Matsumura S., Venturelli P. A., Wolter C., Slate J. et al. 2015. The evolutionary legacy of size‐selective harvesting extends from genes to populations. Evolutionary Applications 8:597–620. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Wijk, S. J. , Taylor M. I., Creer S., Dreyer C., Rodrigues F. M., Ramnarine I. W., van Oosterhout C. et al. 2013. Experimental harvesting of fish populations drives genetically based shifts in body size and maturation. Frontiers in Ecology and the Environment 11:181–187. [Google Scholar]
- Wilson, A. J. , Coltman D. W., Pemberton J. M., Overall A. D., Byrne K. A., and Kruuk L. E. 2005. Maternal genetic effects set the potential for evolution in a free‐living vertebrate population. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 18:405–414. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson, A. J. , Kruuk L. E. B., and Coltman D. W. 2005. Ontogenetic patterns in heritable variation for body size: using random regression models in a wild ungulate population. The American naturalist 166:E177–E192. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilson, A. J. , Réale D., Clements M. N., Morrissey M. M., Postma E., Walling C. A., Kruuk L. E. et al. 2010. An ecologist's guide to the animal model. The Journal of Animal Ecology 79:13–26. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolak, M. E. , Roff D. A., and Fairbairn D. J. 2015. Are we underestimating the genetic variances of dimorphic traits? Ecology and Evolution 5:590–597. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.