Abstract
Early-life caregiving shapes the architecture and function of the developing brain. The fact that the infant-caregiver relationship is critically important for infant functioning across all altricial species, and that the anatomical circuits supporting emotional functioning are highly preserved across different species, suggests that the results of studies examining the role of early adversity and emotional functioning should be translatable across species. Here we present findings from 4 different research laboratories, using 3 different species, which have converged on a similar finding: adversity accelerates the developmental trajectory of amygdala-prefrontal cortex (PFC) development and modifies emotional behaviors. First, a rodent model of attachment learning associated with adversity is presented showing precocial disruption of attachment learning and emergence of heightened fear learning and emotionality. Second, a model of infant-mother separation is presented in which early adversity is shown to accelerate the developmental emergence of adult-like fear retention and extinction. Third, a model of early life adversity in Rhesus monkeys is presented in which a naturally occurring variation in maternal-care (abuse) is shown to alter the functioning of emotion circuits. Finally, a human model of maternal deprivation is presented in which children born into orphanages and then adopted abroad exhibit aberrant development of emotion circuits. The convergence of these cross-species studies on early life adversity suggests that adversity targets the amygdala and PFC and has immediate impact on infant behaviour with the caregiver, and emotional reactions to the world. These results provide insight into mechanisms responsible for caregiver induced mental health trajectory alterations.
Introduction
Early-life experiences shape the architecture and function of the developing brain (Levine, 1962; Suomi et al., 1970). Amongst the most important of early-life experiences is the relationship between the primary caregiver and the infant. In both human and non-human primates, as well as other mammals, the quality of the infant-caregiver relationship plays an important role in the development of behavior and the brain (Callaghan & Richardson, 2013; Kuhn & Schanberg, 1998; Ladd et al., 2004; Plotsky & Meaney, 1993; Plotsky et al., 2005; Raineki et al., 2010; Ruppenthal et al., 1976; Sanchez et al., 2001a; Tottenham & Sheridan, 2009). For example, individuals reared in institutional settings characterized by high child to caregiver ratios have been shown to exhibit deficits in emotion regulation, attachment to primary caregivers, and cognitive development (Kreppner et al., 2007; see Nelson, 2007, for a review of these findings; O’Conner et al., 2003; Rutter, 1998; Rutter et al., 2001; Tottenham et al., 2010; Zeanah et al., 2009).
While the importance of early experiences on an individual’s emotional and cognitive functioning has been recognized for some time, many of the models which have been created to probe this link are underutilized because they do not focus on early developmental time points (Thompson & Levitt, 2010). It is well accepted that many mental health problems have their onset early in life (e.g., Bradley et al., 2008; Cartwright-Hatton et al., 2006; Caspi et al., 2003; Juster et al., 2011; Polanczyk et al., 2009; Spataro et al., 2004; Taylor et al., 2011). Indeed, in one cohort, over 70% of individuals with a diagnosable anxiety disorder at age 21 had first received a diagnosis in childhood or adolescence (Newman et al., 1996). Also, for adult onset disorders (e.g., schizophrenia) the developmental trajectory eventually leading to end-state functioning is established in early life (Lewis & Levitt, 2002). For these reasons it has been suggested that clinical advances in the understanding and treatment of mental health problems will emerge from research which attempts to link the timing of adverse events to the onset of circuit disruption (Thompson & Levitt, 2010). Hence, studies investigating the outcomes of stress at early developmental time points will be critical in determining the initial steps in developmental trajectories which are established by stress and adversity, as well as how its early emerging manifestations might accumulate across the lifespan of an individual to result in end-state mental health dysfunction.
Models of disrupted infant-caregiver relationship have already shown much promise in uncovering some of the factors linking early adversity to poor mental health in adulthood, with similar findings being observed across species. For example, in infant rats, low levels of active maternal care are associated with greater methylation of a glucocorticoid receptor (GR) gene promoter in the hippocampus of adult offspring (Weaver et al., 2004). Similar epigenetic changes to GR gene promoters were recently found in the hippocampus of adult suicide victims that had been abused early in life; these changes were not observed in adult suicide victims not exposed to such early abuse (McGowan et al., 2009). These studies clearly demonstrate the benefits of utilising a ‘translational’ approach in investigating the effects of early-life adversity, with many findings in rodents appearing to be applicable to primates. Continuing with that approach, here we present findings from 4 different research laboratories, using 3 different species, which have converged on a similar finding: adversity alters the early trajectory of emotion regulation behaviors and the neural circuits which support those behaviors. These findings provide strong support for the view that translational models of disrupted infant-caregiver relationship are critical in our understanding of the establishment of mental health trajectories. We start this review by briefly outlining the development of the stress response system in rodents, primates and humans, contrasting this against the maturation of emotion behaviors to suggest that early postnatal life may be an especially sensitive period for the input of stress signals to regulate emotional development.
Development of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis
The hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) neuroaxis is one of the main systems activated by stress and is thought to mediate some of the alterations that have been reported in studies of early adversity. Briefly, when a stressor is detected specific pathways are activated, resulting in parvocellular neurons of the paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus (PVN) being activated and releasing corticotropin-releasing hormone (CRH), and other neuropeptides, such as arginine vasopressin (AVP), into the portal vasculature of the median eminence, which then act on the anterior pituitary to cause release of adrenocorticotropic hormone (ACTH) into the general circulation. ACTH acts upon cells in the adrenal cortex to cause the synthesis and release of glucocorticoids (GCs), cortisol in primates and corticosterone in rodents (together referred to as CORT; Charmandari et al., 2005; Levine, 2001). CORT is a highly catabolic steroid hormone whose main function is to mobilize energy substrates to provide fuel to organs such as the heart and skeletal musculature to respond to threat. The HPA axis has a negative feedback mechanism to shut down its activity via GCs, so that CORT, for example, turns off axis activation by inhibiting ACTH synthesis at the level of the pituitary, and CRH at the level of the hypothalamus, and also by acting on extrahypothalamic structures, including limbic regulatory regions such as the hippocampus and PFC, that inhibit the axis (Charmandari et al., 2005; Herman & Cullinan, 1997; Ulrich-Lai & Herman, 2009). Stress-induced increases in circulating CORT levels are superimposed on circadian rhythms of cortisol, with the highest levels occurring in the morning around waking, and the lowest occurring at night around the onset of sleep, a result of changes in pulsatile release of CRH and AVP from the hypothalamus (Charmandari et al., 2005). Although CORT can have fast, nongenomic effects (Dallman, 2005), glucocorticoids are mainly slow-acting and act via two types of intracellular receptors: (1) glucocorticoid receptors (GR) which have low affinity for GCs and are primarily responsible for negative feedback of the HPA axis in both basal circadian function and the stress response, and (2) mineralocorticoid receptors (MR) which have high affinity for GCs and are primarily responsible for maintaining basal HPA axis activity (Sanchez, 2006). Both of these receptors translocate to the nuclei of cells once CORT is bound, affecting gene transcription by acting as transcription factors (Charmandari et al., 2005; Ulrich-Lai & Herman, 2009).
The HPA neuroaxis undergoes dramatic development across the early postnatal period in both rodents as well as non-human and human primates. One of the distinguishing features of this period is the relative quiescence of the HPA axis, either at baseline or in response to parental presence. In rodents, few stressors are capable of mounting a strong release of CORT early in development, leading to the characterisation of this developmental period as the ‘stress hyporesponsive period’ (Dallman, 2000). In rodents the SHRP extends from about postnatal day (P) 4–14. At the termination of the SHRP, P14 rats can mount a corticosterone response to stress, however, other components of the HPA axis continue to develop after this time, e.g., diurnal patterns of corticosterone do not mature until approximately P26 (Levin & Levine, 1975). In humans, activity of the developing HPA axis is strongly socially buffered, particularly by maternal cues in securely attached dyads (Hostinar et al., 2014). For example, compared to insecurely attached infants, children with secure attachments to the mother exhibited lower cortisol levels when mothers accompanied them to the first few days of childcare (Ahnert et al., 2004). In older children too, even access to maternal vocalisations have a powerful cortisol buffering effect. Specifically, 7–12 year old girls exposed to the Trier Social Stress Test for children and then given physical or telephone access to their mother exhibited a much faster return of cortisol to baseline levels than girls not given any access to the mother (Seltzer et al., 2010). Another important feature of HPA axis development in humans is the rise in baseline cortisol levels at the transition from childhood to adolescence, which corresponds with pubertal status (Kiess et al., 1995). Similar, to studies demonstrating social buffering of the HPA axis in humans, parental care also appears to buffer the developing HPA system in rhesus monkeys, with absence of parental care leading to increases in CORT (Sanchez, 2006).
Neurodevelopment of amygdala-prefrontal circuitry and fear
Interestingly, the time course of maturation for many fear behaviors (e.g., conditioned fear responses in the rat and separation anxiety in the human), and the neural circuitry which supports those behaviours, appears to map very well on to the trajectory of HPA axis maturation. For example, infant rats are markedly different from adults both in how fear is learned and expressed: amygdala dependent cued fear learning and innate fear to predator odor does not functionally emerge until P10 (Camp & Rudy, 2004; Haroutunian & Campbell, 1979; Sullivan et al., 2000; Takahashi et al., 2005). The delayed emergence of fear is not atypical. Indeed, for many altricial species, fear expression is developmentally delayed, presumably emerging when it becomes ecologically significant and beneficial for survival. For rodents, fear emerges at a time when pups begin to leave the protection of the nest by taking brief trips outside the nest. We suggest that the emergence of the neural circuits supporting fear provides animals with a nervous system and behavioral repertoire adapted to their changing ecological niche as they transition from the nest to independence, which occurs over a brief 3 week period from birth to weaning.
Indeed, the first few weeks of life in the rodent also represent a period of developmental change in retention and extinction of conditioned fear. Specifically, while adult rats exhibit excellent memory retention following just a single conditioning episode (Gale et al., 2004; Paschall & Davis, 2002), infant rats rapidly forget fear associations across intervals as short as one week (a phenomenon known as infantile amnesia; Campbell & Campbell, 1962; Campbell & Spear, 1972). In addition, adult rats use an extinction system which is prone to fear relapse (e.g., Bouton, 2002; Bouton & Bolles, 1979), yet extinction in infancy is relapse-resistant (for a review see Kim & Richardson, 2010). In other words, after extinction training in adulthood, rats will exhibit fear again if the context changes (renewal), if they are given a brief reminder treatment (reinstatement), or merely following the passage of time (spontaneous recovery). On the other hand, fear that is extinguished in infancy is permanently suppressed, regardless of the post-extinction manipulation (Kim & Richardson, 2007a; 2007b; Yap & Richardson, 2007). In addition to these behavioral dissociations in fear expression and extinction, there are also developmental changes in the neural circuits which support these behaviors. For example, the expression of fear in adult rats is dependent upon neural activity in the prelimbic prefrontal cortex (PL-PFC; Burgos-Robles et al., 2009; Corcoran & Quirk, 2007; Quirk et al., 2003; Sierra-Mercado et al., 2010; Vidal-Gonzalez et al., 2006). However, fear expression in infancy is PL-PFC independent (Li et al., 2012) and it was recently suggested that the exaggerated forgetting seen in young rats may be due to the apparent lack of coordinated activity between the mPFC and basolateral amygdala (BLA) early in life (Kim et al., 2012). Similar, to fear expression, infant rats also use a different neural circuit to adults when fear is extinguished. While extinction in adult rats is dependent upon the infralimbic (IL) region of the PFC (Milad & Quirk, 2002), infant extinction is IL independent (Kim et al., 2009). Taken together these data suggest that fear conditioning and extinction in infancy are likely supported by a very simple circuit, involving only the amygdala and probably downstream targets involved in the expression of specific fear responses (e.g., the periaqueductal gray).
Non-human primates also exhibit substantial change in emotion circuits across the infancy to juvenile period of development. For example in rhesus monkeys amygdala afferents from portions of temporal cortex that relay visual information mature at week 3 when an animal first begins to respond appropriately to social cues, while efferents from these same temporal regions to orbital PFC do not become mature until 2 months when curiosity and frustration become apparent (Machado & Bachevalier, 2003). It is also during this time that the amygdala undergoes the fastest changes in volume (Chareyron et al., 2012; Payne et al., 2010). PFC development occurs over a much longer period, not becoming mature until approximately 3–4 years (Machado & Bachevalier, 2003). An example of how maturation and myelination of the PFC is reflected behaviorally is in the transition from infancy to early childhood, when the capacity for effortful behavioral control becomes apparent (Dawson et al., 1992; Maestripieri & Carroll, 1998).
Finally, significant maturation of emotional networks also occurs over the childhood to adolescent period in humans. Specifically, typically raised children show an immature pattern of task-based connectivity between amygdala and mPFC (Gee et al., 2013b; i.e., positive coupling, such that when activity of the amygdala increased, so did activity of the mPFC). This immature pattern was paralleled by high amygdala reactivity and immature fear behaviors (i.e., developmentally normative high separation anxiety). This amygdala-mPFC connectivity phenotype switched to an adult-like phenotype around 10 years old, such that like adults, adolescents showed the mature pattern of amygdala-mPFC connectivity (i.e., negative coupling, such that when mPFC activity increased, amygdala reactivity decreased). The mature pattern was paralleled by decreased amygdala reactivity and lower levels of separation anxiety.
Together these data suggest that there exist both immature and mature patterns of fear responding and neural connectivity in emotion circuits and that the natural developmental transition between such behaviours/circuits may correspond with maturation of the HPA axis. Specifically, as the animal ages and maternal care decreases, HPA axis activity matures and there is a corresponding development of fear behaviours and supporting neural circuitry. Considering that the caregiver is a species-expected stimulus in altricial species, such as rodents and primates, then premature separation or near complete deprivation in the infant period should have enormous consequences on the developmental trajectory of systems that require maternal presence. In fact, it can be hypothesised that in the presence of early adversity the transition from immature to mature fear/stress circuitry would be accelerated. The following sections outline recently collected data supporting that hypothesis in rodent, monkey and human models of early adversity.
Rat models of early life adversity within attachment and the developmental trajectory of affiliation and fear
As mentioned earlier, amygdala dependent fear learning and innate fear to predator odor does not functionally emerge until P10 (Camp & Rudy, 2004; Haroutunian & Campbell, 1979; Sullivan et al., 2000; Takahashi et al., 2005). Here we briefly review the initial developmental emergence of cue (odor) fear learning in rodents. In very young rats, cue fear (the stimulus that predicts the shock) is not expressed or learned until around P10, which corresponds with the functional emergence of the amygdala, a brain area well-documented as critical for fear. Hippocampal-dependent fear learning does not functionally emerge until pups are weaning age (Sullivan & Holman, 2010). Second, we describe how early life fear cue learning and expression is uniquely dependent on the presence of amygdala CORT and the important role of the mother in controlling her pups’ CORT levels, thus controlling her pups’ fear. Finally, we describe the benefits of a reduced fear system in early infancy when considered within the context of attachment to the mother.
Delayed development of olfactory fear learning and its ecological significance
Prior to the emergence of amygdala-dependent fear, pairing an odor and pain (moderate intensity shock, tailpinch, or rough handling by the caregiver) in rat pups results in a strong learned approach to the odor present during the pain (Camp & Rudy, 2004; Sullivan et al., 2000). The paradoxical odor preference learning induced by odor-pain pairings (i.e. a fear conditioning paradigm) occurs despite pups’ robust response to pain and minor changes in pain threshold (Barr, 1995). Young pups fail to show odor avoidance learning due to their suppressed fear system, and particularly, the failure of the young amygdala to exhibit learning-associated plasticity. We have demonstrated this phenomenon in controlled classical fear conditioning outside the nest and within natural learning paradigms where an abusive mother delivers aversive stimuli within the nest, with all manipulations resulting in a failure to recruit the amygdala and preference learning (Raineki et al., 2012)
The suppression of fear learning and the production of preference learning by odor shock conditioning appears paradoxical. However, when these results are placed within the context of the pup’s ecological niche, a cohesive picture of ecological significance emerges. The main function of odor learning in rat pups is to learn the maternal odor, which the deaf and blind pup relies on to support feeding and huddling with the mother. Finally, the maternal odor changes its characteristics as the mother eats new foods, suggesting pups are required to learn and relearn the maternal odor. Decades of research has shown us that pups readily learn new maternal odors simply by pairing a novel odor with presumably pleasant stimuli (i.e. milk, warmth, tactile stimulation – called stroking) but also painful stimuli (0.5mA shock, tail pinch) (Camp & Rudy, 2004; Haroutunian & Campbell, 1979; Roth & Sullivan, 2005; Sullivan et al., 2000; Takahashi et al., 2005; Upton & Sullivan, 2010). Within the nest, mothers frequently interact with their pups in a gentle manner, but also inflict pain through stepping on them when entering or leaving the nest. We speculate that, since pups in the sensitive period for attachment are confined to the nest and the mother remains with her pups about 80% of the time, a broad system to support odor preference learning and suppress fear learning could optimize pups learning to approach the mother, rather than avoid the mother (Sullivan & Holman, 2010).
It should be noted that pups can learn to avoid odors if that odor is paired with malaise, such as that produced by a LiCl injection or strong 1.0 mA shock, although this learning does not depend upon the amygdala, unlike the adult, (Shionoya et al., 2006; Smotherman, 1982) and does not produce paradoxical approach/preference learning, despite pups experiencing endogenous malaise and pain.
Maternal control of the newly emerging fear system
After amygdala-dependent fear learning functionally emerges, this system has the unique function of being turned off if the mother is present. As is discussed here, it is the mother’s ability to suppress pups’ shock induced CORT release via social buffering, combined with the infant amygdala’s unique dependence on CORT plasticity, that appears to underlie the maternal effect on fear at this age. Clues to the importance of the stress hormone CORT’s critical role in fear have been known for decades. As previously stated, pups’ fear expression to predator odor emerges at P10, yet increasing CORT in younger pups permits the precocious expression of fear, while reducing CORT in older pups prevents fear expression (Takahashi, 1994). Our pharmacological manipulations of pups’ CORT levels also demonstrated its critical role in fear learning and its ability to switch on amygdala plasticity to support fear learning (Moriceau et al., 2006).
As noted above, CORT has a unique function in early life: it controls fear to predator odor and learned fear (Sullivan & Holman, 2010; Takahashi et al., 2005). Specifically, at about P10, shock begins to increase pups’ CORT levels sufficiently to provide enough CORT to support fear expression and amygdala plasticity required for fear learning. Microinfusions of amygdala CORT can rapidly permit amygdala plasticity in pups during the SHRP, while infusions of receptor blockers prevent amygdala plasticity in post-SHRP pups. There is ecological significance to the ability of CORT to control pups’ fear learning: mothers can alter pups’ CORT levels though her milk or through rough handling of pups. Specifically, stress-induced rough maternal care (induced by providing insufficient bedding for nest building) produces a premature elevation in pups’ CORT (Gilles et al., 1996; Raineki et al., 2012). The source of CORT seems to be a combination of that delivered through the mother’s milk and pups’ premature activation of the adrenal gland, which releases CORT (Raineki et al., 2012). Prematurely increasing pups’ CORT, produces premature emergence of fear learning and termination of the sensitive period for attachment. Mothers can also suppress pups’ cortiocsterone levels, although this only occurs after the SHRP has terminated and pups can mount a CORT response to stressors, which more recent data suggests occurs around P10 (VanOers et al., 1998). Indeed, when pups receive shock during the ‘Transitional Sensitive Period’, maternal presence “socially buffers” their CORT response, by completely blocking the shock-induced CORT response. Maternal presence suppresses the infant CORT response by blocking norepinephrine release, which is typically induced by shock, and which is necessary to activate the hypothalamic-piuitary-adrenal axis, causing the release of CORT from the adrenal gland (Shionoya et al., 2006). As maternal presence blocks pups’ CORT, pups’ amygdala plasticity and fear learning is blocked, allowing the reinstatement of pups’ attachment learning (Moriceau & Sullivan, 2006; Shionoya et al., 2006).
Taken together, these data support an acceleration hypothesis of emotion learning by early life stress, suggesting that adult-like fear behaviours and the underlying neural circuitry which produces those behaviours can be elicited prematurely by adversity which causes early HPA stimulation. Further, they highlight the important role of the mother in buffering the infants cortisol response and regulating the expression of fear in transitional periods of development.
Rat models of early-life stress: Maternal-separation and the developmental trajectory of fear and extinction systems
The developmental transition in attachment learning is not the only change to take place in the fear system across early life. As mentioned earlier fear conditioning and extinction in rats have each been shown to function differently in infancy than in adulthood (infants exhibit infantile amnesia and relapse resistant extinction whereas adults exhibit good fear retention and relapse-prone extinction learning). Importantly, Pavlovian fear conditioning and extinction are two aspects of emotional learning that have high cross-species validity and clinical relevance. For example, there is high convergence between rats and humans in the neural structures which support fear and extinction learning. Also, disruptions in the processes of fear and extinction learning are believed to underlie numerous anxiety disorders (e.g., post-traumatic stress disorder [PTSD] and specific phobias; Coles & Heimberg, 2002; Milad et al., 2008; Milad et al., 2009; Monk et al., 2008). Further, the cognitive and behavioral treatments for anxiety disorders (i.e., exposure therapy) are modeled on the process of extinction (Milad et al., 2006). Hence, understanding the processes of fear learning and fear inhibition in animals across development, as well as how these processes are affected by early-life stress, will be critical for enhancing our understanding of the establishment and progression of anxiety disorders, as well as how such disorders can be avoided through preventative intervention and treatment across the lifespan.
Much of the research which has examined the effects of early adversity on fear and extinction learning has focused on adult outcomes. For example, disruptions in the infant-caregiver relationship have been shown to lead to impaired context and cued fear conditioning (Kosten et al., 2006; Kosten et al., 2005; Stevenson et al., 2009) as well as extinction recall (Wilber et al., 2009) in adult rodents. Until very recently, only a small number of studies had investigated the early-life outcomes of infant-caregiver disruption on fear and extinction learning, however, both of those studies support an acceleration hypothesis. Specifically, parental separation was reported to enhance active avoidance learning in juvenile rodents (Abraham & Gruss, 2010) and early-life handling enhanced contextual conditioning in P18 rats (Beane et al., 2002); both those types of fear learning are typically impaired early in development. We recently investigated the effect of early adversity on cued fear and extinction learning in infancy.
Consistent with previous studies examining the effect of stress on developmental trajectories in fear behaviour we also found that early stress accelerated the emergence of adult-like forms of cued fear retention and extinction. Specifically, we exposed rats to maternal separation (MS) rearing and then examined fear retention and extinction behaviours following conditioning in infancy. Maternal-separation is a potent stressor which typically involves removal of the pups from the mother for a moderate period of time (usually three hours), every day, across the first two weeks of life (e.g., Sanchez et al., 2001a). The procedure has been designed to model abnormal disruptions in maternal-infant contact, based upon the observation that dams often leave the nest to forage for food for periods of only 15–30 minutes (Sanchez et al., 2001a). In addition, MS is sufficient to evoke a CORT response in pups within the SHRP (Huot et al., 2002), and appears to disrupt maternal behaviors, with MS dams exhibiting delays in retrieving the pups as well as initiating licking and grooming upon reunion (Sanchez et al., 2001a). In the first demonstration that MS affected emotional learning in infant rats, Callaghan and Richardson (2011) exposed pups to 3 hour bouts of MS from post-natal days (P) 2–14. These rats were then trained, extinguished, and tested for extinction retention on consecutive days in infancy (i.e., beginning at P17). MS rats were compared to standard reared (SR) rats of the same age. Across three experiments it was shown that MS had no effect on the initial amount of fear learned or the rate of within-session extinction, with both MS and SR infant rats showing high levels of fear at the start of extinction and low levels of fear by the end of extinction. However, when MS rats were tested for their fear to the CS in a different context than extinction-training, or when rats were tested the day after a brief reminder of conditioning (a single mild footshock), their fear responses returned. Standard-reared infant rats, on the other hand, exhibited low levels of fear regardless of these post-extinction manipulations. In other words, similar to adult rats, P17 MS rats exhibited the renewal and reinstatement effects, whereas SR infant rats did not. In addition, those experiments showed that the expression of extinction at test was dependent upon activity of the neurotransmitter GABA (gamma amino butyric acid) in MS infants but not in SR infants; GABA is critical for the expression of extinction memories in adult rats but not in typically developing infant rats (Harris & Westbrook, 1998; Kim & Richardson, 2007b). Together, the results reported by Callaghan and Richardson (2011) show that disrupting the infant-caregiver relationship through maternal-separation results in an early transition from the infant to the adult extinction systems. That is, after MS, rats appear to exhibit adult-like extinction earlier in development.
Because early-life stress has been shown to cause early developmental transitions in both the extinction system and in the fear learning system, we recently asked whether early life stress would also cause an early transition in a different component of the fear system – retention (Callaghan & Richardson, 2012). In those experiments rats were exposed either to MS or SR and were then trained in infancy (P17). Using a between-subjects design rats were tested for their fear to the CS at one of two intervals, either 1 day later (an interval where young rats would normally remember) or 10 days later (an interval where young rats would normally forget; Callaghan & Richardson, 2012). The performance of the SR animals was consistent with the typical profile of infantile amnesia – memory was good one day after training but was dramatically reduced 10 days after training. The performance of the MS animals on the other hand was surprising because memory expression was high at both the one and 10 day intervals, suggesting that no forgetting had occurred across that time period. In a follow-up experiment we examined how long infant MS rats could retain a memory before it was forgotten. In that experiment the performance of MS paired rats was compared to a control group of MS rats that was given unpaired presentations of the CS and US at intervals 7, 14, 30, or 55 days after training. While MS unpaired rats expressed low freezing to the CS at all intervals, MS paired rats expressed fear to the CS for as long as 30 days post conditioning. In a final experiment of that study, we examined whether the effects of MS could be mimicked by exposing mothers to the stress hormone CORT in their drinking water, rather than putting them through an MS procedure. In that experiment, pups that were nursed by CORT-treated dams (CORT-nursed) exhibited the same enhanced retention of fear associations as did the MS pups in the earlier experiments. On the other hand, pups nursed by vehicle-treated mothers exhibited infantile amnesia. Those results suggest that exposure to a stress hormone alone was sufficient to cause the early transition in fear retention systems.
Taken together, the studies reviewed here suggest that experiences which occur in the first few weeks of the rats’ life are vital in establishing the developmental trajectory of emotion regulation systems. While adult-like forms of fear retention and fear extinction are typically slow to develop, these forms of emotion learning come online much faster following exposure to stress and stress hormones. These findings contribute to the burgeoning literature which points to stress as a general developmental switch, stimulating developmental transitions in many forms of emotional learning.
Rhesus monkey models of early life stress: phylogenetic bridge between rodents and humans
As demonstrated by the extensive work described above, rodents are indispensable translational models of the effects of early life stress on the development of fear and emotion systems. However, due to their phylogenetic distance to humans, it is difficult to know how these findings translate to primates. Non-human primate models are crucial when studying the neural mechanisms of the developmental effects of early life stress because they help to bridge the phylogenetic gap between rodents and humans (Gibbs et al., 2007). Rhesus monkeys, an Old World species, in particular have been widely used to study outcomes following disruption of the mother-infant relationship in early life due to their similarity to humans in both behavioral and neural development. Rhesus monkeys show complex social behaviors and mother-infant relationships that develop in ways similar to humans. For example, rhesus monkeys have single offspring and infants spend the first month of life in close physical contact with their mothers, who limits the infant’s social interactions to immediate family members (Hinde & Spencer-Booth, 1967), much like the infant phase in humans. Another example are the rapid changes in the mother-infant relationship that occur between 3 and 6 months of age, a time of increased infant independence, exploration and social interactions, when social fear emerges, corresponding to the toddler age in humans. Rhesus monkeys have been instrumental in providing evidence of the neurodevelopmental changes that parallel these behavioral changes in primates, both under species typical conditions and during disruption of the mother-infant relationship.
Several rhesus monkey models of mother-infant relationship disruption have been used to study the effects of this experience on the brain and behavior (Sanchez et al., 2001b). Rearing monkeys without a mother in partial social isolation (as done by Dr. Harry Harlow and colleagues in the 1960’s and 70’s) resulted in severe behavioral and social deficits including stereotypies, self-injurious behavior, inability to read social cues or develop normal social relationships, and hyper aggressiveness, while rearing in complete social isolation led to similar outcomes, but with increased severity, and also included increased fear and anxiety (Seay & Gottfried, 1975; Suomi et al., 1971). Maternal separation has also been used in non-human primates to model early life stress and its effects on the brain. Separation from the mother is a potent stressor in infant monkeys, just as in rodents, that leads to increased motor activity, vocalizations, and increased HPA axis function (Bayart et al., 1990; Harlow et al., 1971). Increased acoustic startle reactivity and alterations in HPA axis function have been reported in maternally separated monkeys (Sanchez et al., 2005), effects thought to be mediated by exposure to elevated levels of stress hormones (Sanchez et al., 2001a).
These alterations parallel those discussed above in rodents and are thought to be due to underlying alterations in the brain, particularly regions involved in fear and emotion such as PFC-amygdala circuits. It is important to highlight that rhesus monkeys have well-developed brains that closely resemble humans’ in respect to organization and neurochemistry (Barbas, 2000; Croxson et al., 2005; Reep, 1984; Thiebaut de Schotten et al., 2012), particularly regarding the PFC, which is very different in primates versus rodents (Öngür & Price, 2000; Preuss, 1995; Preuss & Goldman-Rakic, 1991a; 1991b), making them strong model organisms to study developmental effects of experience on PFC-amygdala circuits. The rhesus brain also develops in similar temporal and anatomical patterns as in humans, i.e. maturational processes occur in similar region specific patterns during development, although over a condensed time period in the case of monkeys (approximately 1:4 years, monkey to human time; Diamond, 1991; Gibson, 1991; Huttenlocher & Dabholkar, 1997).
As mentioned earlier, PFC-amygdala circuits undergo rapid developmental changes early in life in both humans and rhesus monkeys, one of the primary reasons these regions are thought to be especially vulnerable to early life stress. For example in rhesus monkeys afferents and efferents between the amygdala and portions of temporal cortex continue to mature across the first 2 months of life (Machado & Bachevalier, 2003) and amygdala volume increases dramatically across this time (Chareyron et al., 2012; Payne et al., 2010). On the other hand, maturation and myelination of the PFC occurs over a much longer period, not becoming mature until approximately 3–4 years (Machado & Bachevalier, 2003).
Evidence in rhesus monkeys corroborates the role of cortisol in the alterations of PFC-amygdala circuits. In primates glucocorticoid and mineralocorticoid receptors (the two main types of receptors to which cortisol binds) are expressed in most cortical regions, including the PFC (Sanchez, 2006; Sanchez et al., 2000). It is through these receptors that stress-induced elevations of GCs can affect the PFC and other emotion regulatory regions, particularly during development. Functional changes in metabolism in PFC that correlate with cortisol have been demonstrated in juvenile rhesus monkeys during maternal separations (Rilling et al., 2001). Interestingly, enlargement of these stress sensitive areas, including the dorsomedial PFC and dorsal anterior cingulate cortex, has been reported in juvenile monkeys reared in partial social isolation (Spinelli et al., 2009), which, given that volumes of these regions increase during and beyond the juvenile period (Knickmeyer et al., 2010) is consistent with the hypothesis that this adverse early experience increases the rate of maturation in these regions. Alterations in the HPA axis have also been reported in non-human primate models of social isolation (Boyce et al., 1995; Ladd et al., 2005). It is difficult to determine whether or not these changes are consistent with an increase in maturational rate of the HPA axis because this system is one of the primary responses to stressors, and may change its function in response to stress, especially during development. It is also important to recognize the role of heritable factors in the complex interplay of early experience and development of these systems. Recently several studies provided evidence for several gene x environment interactions and epigenetic mechanisms for the effects of early adversity (Barr et al., 2004; Provençal et al., 2012), highlighting the importance of genetic contributions to the effects of early experience.
In conclusion, nonhuman primate models have been invaluable in the study of development of the brain and behaviour in response to early life stress. To best understand the effects of mother-infant relationship disruption in primates we need to first understand normative development in primates. There is still much to learn about normative brain development before we can accurately interpret and understand the many findings described above. It is also important to recognize the importance of rodent studies in informing nonhuman primate research on both typical and atypical development. In order to further our understanding of alterations in human development, studies in monkeys can be designed using information gained from the important work being done in rodents. An example are the current studies being undertaken in the laboratory of Dr. Mar Sanchez, in which the role of the experience of maternal infant maltreatment is being investigated using a cross-fostering design in rhesus monkeys, much like the studies done by Michael Meaney and others in rodents (Caldji et al., 2011). Using this design we hope to disentangle the role of the experience, independent of heritable factors, on the brain and behavior in primates, with the ultimate goal being to apply this knowledge to humans.
Early-life adversity in the human: Developmental trajectory of amygdala circuits in previously institutionalised children
Despite the many important differences between humans and the other non-human species discussed in this paper, there are nonetheless important early-life processes that share an ecological similarity across rodents and primates. For example, although the species differ in the duration of gestation and subsequent age at birth, the regulatory needs of the infant are highly similar regardless of species. The infant is born in an altricial state; that is, the infant could not survive without caregiving (as discussed in Tottenham, 2012). The critical value of this relationship for survival qualifies the caregiver as a species-expected stimulus for the infant. Therefore, the infant, regardless of rodent, non-human primate, or human, has the same goal, which is to ensure proximity to the caregiver (Ainsworth, 1969; Bowlby, 1982; Moriceau & Roth, 2010).
If the caregiver is a species-expected stimulus, then premature separation or near complete deprivation in the infant period should have devastating consequences on the developmental trajectory of systems that require maternal presence. As is discussed in this paper, the amygdala and prefrontal cortex have shown aberrant developmental trajectories following early caregiving adversity in the monkey and the rodent. In particular, the rodent findings provide evidence that maternal absence acts to accelerate amygdala and prefrontal development. This acceleration may be a developmental adaptation of the infant to meet the unexpected needs elicited by a severely altered caregiving environment. This section discusses development of the human amygdala following early maternal deprivation.
The hypotheses driving research with human samples have been largely motivated and constrained by the animal findings, such as those presented in this paper. Until the advent of functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) technology, a non-invasive technique that provides both structural information and an indirect measure of neural activity, there was very little opportunity to address neurodevelopment in healthy and awake humans. Structural or functional MRI is a particularly valuable technique for examining the development of deep structures in the brain, such as the amygdala. With the advent of fMRI, the field of research on human brain development has exploded. For the first time in history, scientists are able to obtain information about normative brain development in awake and performing child and adolescent participants. The first fMRI study on normative brain development was published by Dr. BJ Casey (1995), and since then, the field has grown rapidly.
Early-life stress exposure is an experimental variable that can be controlled in many animal models. That is, to make claims about the timing of early-life stress, researchers can restrict the timing and duration of adversity to a particular developmental period (e.g., the early post-natal period). However, this temporal specificity is not commonly available in most human populations, where adversity tends to remain a fairly stable characteristic across the lifetime (e.g., psychosocial adversity at one age has a high likelihood of being followed by subsequent adversity/ies). Therefore, making claims about the effects of early adversity on human amygdala development is typically not possible. The sample that we have been closely following is a very special group of children and adolescents who experienced maternal deprivation (in the form of institutional rearing, such as orphanages abroad) and were subsequently adopted (via international adoption) into families in the United States. Families that adopt internationally tend to come from very high socio-economic backgrounds. As demonstrated by their efforts to adopt internationally (which is arduous), families that adopt internationally have a great desire to provide care, a critical characteristic in children’s mental health outcomes (Hodges & Tizard, 1989). Therefore, this population of children provide two unusual factors: the environment of early-life (pre-adoption) and that after adoption contrast starkly; and the end-date of the maternally deprived environment is known.
Consistent with animal models that have shown alterations in amygdala development following early-life stress, examination of human amygdala development has also demonstrated altered phenotypes. In a group of children (mean age = 8 years old), those with a history of institutional care (previously-institutionalized; PI) exhibited larger amygdala volumes relative to a typically-raised comparison group of peers (Tottenham et al., 2010). Longer stays in the institutional care setting (i.e., later age of adoption) were associated with larger amygdala volumes. Similar findings have been observed in a group of children adopted from Romania into the United Kingdom (Mehta & Golembo, 2009; although see Sheridan & Fox, 2012). Larger amygdala volumes have been observed in less extreme forms of maternal deprivation as well; namely, children of depressed mothers also exhibit enlarged amygdala volumes and elevated levels of glucocorticoids (Lupien & Parent, 2011). These findings suggest that the human amygdala may be particularly sensitive to variations in maternal care early in life. We have observed behavioral correlates of amygdala phenotype such that larger amygdala volumes were associated with more errors on a behavioral regulation task in the context of highly arousing emotional information and greater parent reported internalizing problems and trait anxiety (Tottenham et al., 2010), phenotypes commonly identified in this sample of children (Goff et al., 2012; Wiik & Loman, 2011; Zeanah et al., 2009). It is unclear what larger volume as assessed by structural MRI reflects at the cellular level, but rodent work has shown that early weaning results in premature myelination of cells in the basolateral amygdala (Ono et al., 2008). If the same occurs in humans, structural differences measured at the level of MRI may reflect increased speed and efficiency within the amygdala following early caregiving adversity.
Activity of the amygdala and its connections with medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) are also altered following institutional rearing. We have observed amygdala hyperactivity in PI youth across two separate samples (Weill Cornell Medical College & UCLA) in response to highly arousing emotional faces (Tottenham et al., 2011). Amygdala hyperactivity was associated with poorer social competence with peers as reported by parents, and hyperactivity also mediated group differences in laboratory-measured social behaviors. Specifically, children in the post-institutionalized group exhibited decreased eye-contact relative to typically-raised peers, and this decreased contact was mediated by heightened amygdala response during the scanning session.
We also observed a pattern of functional connections between amygdala and mPFC that was highly consistent with the rat models presented above. Typically raised children show an immature pattern of task-based connectivity between amygdala and mPFC (Gee et al., 2013) (i.e., positive coupling, such that when activity of the amygdala increased, so did activity of the mPFC). This immature pattern was paralleled by high amygdala reactivity and immature fear behaviors (i.e., developmentally normative high separation anxiety). This amygdala-mPFC connectivity phenotype switched to an adult-like phenotype around 10 years old, such that like adults, adolescents showed the mature pattern of amygdala-mPFC connectivity (i.e., negative coupling, such that when mPFC activity increased, amygdala reactivity decreased). The mature pattern was paralleled by decreased amygdala reactivity and lower levels of separation anxiety. In contrast to the typical group, PI children showed evidence of the mature amygdala-mPFC connectivity phenotype such that their connectivity resembled an adolescent’s (Gee et al., 2013a). These group differences between typically raised and PI children were mediated by group differences in cortisol production, consistent with the notion that stress can accelerate amygdala-mPFC circuit development. One interpretation of this timing effect is that the shift in connectivity towards the adult-state is the cascading result of early and/or elevated amygdala activity, which could have instantiated this change in connections with mPFC.
It is unclear at this point whether aberrant amygdala development in the post-institutionalized group reflects accelerated development of the human amygdala as it does in the rodent. To make this claim, much more needs to be understood about the normative course of human amygdala development. The age of human amygdala functional onset is not yet known, and this information is critical for understanding the timing effects of early adversity. In the rodent, the amygdala remains functionally dormant for a period (e.g., approximately 10 days) after birth. This level of specific knowledge has not been established in the human, although it is a goal in the human research and progress is being made. We have shown that the human amygdala is responsive to fear-relevant stimuli by 4 years old (Gee et al., 2013b). The amygdala was highly reactive at this young age and showed attenuated responses in older ages. This finding suggests that amygdala functional onset emerges earlier than early childhood and may occur in the preschool period (if not earlier). Until those data are collected, it is not possible to know whether heightened amygdala response following early life caregiving adversity reflects acceleration or not. In fact, the heightened amygdala reactivity to fear faces observed in typically developing groups may mask any group differences in amygdala hyperactivity during early childhood. Therefore, a prerequisite for understanding the effects of early caregiving adversity is to characterize normative development of the human amygdala.
Conclusions
The models of early-life stress discussed here (maternal-deprivation/separation, abuse) are frequently used, across species, to empirically examine the effect of exposure to adverse events on early emotional functioning. The fact that many emotional disorders emerge during development and are associated with early-life adversity affords high clinical importance to the research being done in this area and support the use of a translational approach. Indeed, there are many features of early stress models that make them amenable to translation across species. Specifically, neural circuits that are affected by early stress, and those which support fear learning and attachment, are highly preserved across different species (e.g., rodents and primates; Lissek et al., 2005; Milad et al., 2008; Milad et al., 2009; Monk et al., 2008). Also, the infant-caregiver relationship is critical for infant survival in all altricial mammals (due to complete dependence upon the caregiver at birth), suggesting that maternal-deprivation/separation and abuse represent ecologically valid models of disruption in exposure to a species-expected stimulus.
In the symposium, and now in this review, we have highlighted the remarkable similarity in research findings to emerge from current models of early-life adversity across 3 different species – rodent, monkey, and human. The four research groups that contributed to this symposium had reported that early-life adversity affected the maturation of fear circuitry (e.g., amygdala and prefrontal cortex), as well as associated behaviours of the fear network (e.g., attachment learning, fear conditioning, extinction, and anxiety). It is striking that many of these outcomes appear to reflect accelerated maturation of emotion circuits and behaviours following early-life stress. Indeed, early maturation of fear systems following adversity could be advantageous, promoting independent survival at an earlier than expected time point. However, the interpretation of these findings as either accelerated maturation, or aberrant development, will be greatly enhanced by further studies which map typical development of emotion networks in the brain, especially in monkeys and humans (see Gee et al., 2013b; Qin et al., 2012, for examples). If these outcomes are indeed reflective of accelerated maturation it will be interesting for future studies to determine the functional ‘cost’ of such premature development in emotional systems as well as the short-term benefits and the likely evolutionary significance of this effect. We hope that these findings will encourage greater research into the development of fear circuits and the environmental cues which regulate their maturation across species.
Footnotes
About the symposium
The findings discussed in this paper were presented at the Sackler Special Interest Symposium at the Internatio0nal Society for Developmental Psychobiology annual meeting in 2012. The symposium documented emerging data from the following labs across the world: Regina Sullivan at New York University School of Medicine; Bridget Callaghan (who was a PhD candidate in the laboratory of Professor Rick Richardson at The University of New South Wales in Australia at the time of the symposium and presented a complement of studies from her doctoral thesis); Brittany Howell (who was a PhD candidate in the laboratory of Dr. Mar Sanchez in the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, School of Medicine, Emory University and the Yerkes National Primate Research Center, Atlanta, GA at the time of the symposium and presented a combination of previous results from Dr. Sanchez’s group and preliminary results from her dissertation research); Nim Tottenham from the Department of Psychology at the University of California, Los Angeles (who presented data from both from her work at UCLA as well as findings from her dissertation research with Dr. BJ Casey’s laboratory in the Sackler Institute for Developmental Psychobiology at Weill Cornell Medical College in New York City).
References
- Abraham A, Gruss M. Stress inoculation facilitates active avoidance learning of the semi-precocial rodent Octodon degus. Behavioural Brain Research. 2010;213:293–303. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.05.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ahnert L, Gunnar MR, Lamb ME, Barthel M. Transition to child care: Associations with infant–mother attachment, infant negative emotion, and cortisol elevations. Child development. 2004;75:639–650. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00698.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ainsworth MD. Object relations, dependency, and attachment: a theoretical review of the infant-mother relationship. Child Development. 1969;40:969–1025. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barbas H. Connections underlying the synthesis of cognition, memory, and emotion in primate prefrontal cortices. Brain Res Bull. 2000;52:319–330. doi: 10.1016/s0361-9230(99)00245-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barr CS, Newman TK, Shannon C, Parker C, Dvoskin RL, Becker ML, et al. Rearing condition and rh5-HTTLPR interact to influence limbic-hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis response to stress in infant macaques. Biol Psychiatry. 2004;55:733–738. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2003.12.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barr GA. Ontogeny of nociception and antinociception. NIDA research monograph. 1995;158:172–201. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bayart F, Hayashi KT, Faull KF, Barchas JD, Levine S. Influence of maternal proximity on behavioral and physiological responses to separation in infant rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) Behav Neurosci. 1990;104:98. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beane ML, Cole MA, Spencer RL, Rudy JW. Neonatal handling enhances contextual fear conditioning and alters corticosterone stress responses in young rats. Hormones and Behavior. 2002;41:33–40. doi: 10.1006/hbeh.2001.1725. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bouton ME. Context, ambiguity, and unlearning: Sources of relapse after behavioral extinction. Biological Psychiatry. 2002;52:976–986. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(02)01546-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bouton ME, Bolles RC. Contextual control of the extinction of conditioned fear. Learning and Motivation. 1979;10:445–466. [Google Scholar]
- Bowlby J. Attachment and loss: retrospect and prospect. Am J Orthopsychiatry. 1982;52:664–678. doi: 10.1111/j.1939-0025.1982.tb01456.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boyce W, Champoux M, Suomi S, Gunnar M. Salivary cortisol in nursery-reared rhesus monkeys: reactivity to peer interactions and altered circadian activity. Dev Psychobiol. 1995;28:257. doi: 10.1002/dev.420280502. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bradley RG, Binder EB, Epstein MP, Tang Y, Nair HP, Liu W, et al. Influence of child abuse on adult depression: moderation by the corticotropin-releasing hormone receptor gene. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2008;65:190–200. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2007.26. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burgos-Robles A, Vidal-Gonzalez I, Quirk GJ. Sustained conditioned responses in prelimbic prefrontal neurons are correlated with fear expression and extinction failure. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2009;29:8474–8482. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0378-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Caldji C, Hellstrom IC, Zhang TY, Diorio J, Meaney MJ. Environmental regulation of the neural epigenome. FEBS letters. 2011;585:2049–2058. doi: 10.1016/j.febslet.2011.03.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Callaghan B, Richardson R. The effect of adverse rearing environments on persistent memories in young rats: removing the brakes on infant fear memories. Translational Psychiatry. 2012;2:e138. doi: 10.1038/tp.2012.65. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Callaghan BL, Richardson R. Maternal separation results in early emergence of adult-like fear and extinction learning in infant rats. Behavioral Neuroscience. 2011;125:20–28. doi: 10.1037/a0022008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Callaghan BL, Richardson R. Early experiences and the development of emotional learning systems in rats. Biology of Mood & Anxiety Disorders. 2013;3 doi: 10.1186/2045-5380-3-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Camp LL, Rudy JW. Changes in the categorization of appetitive and aversive events during postnatal development of the rat. Developmental psychobiology. 2004;21:25–42. doi: 10.1002/dev.420210103. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Campbell BA, Campbell EH. Retention and extinction of learned fear in infant and adult rats. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology. 1962;55:1–8. doi: 10.1037/h0049182. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Campbell BA, Spear NE. Ontogeny of memory. Psychological Review. 1972;79:215–236. doi: 10.1037/h0032690. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cartwright-Hatton S, McNicol K, Doubleday E. Anxiety in a neglected population: Prevalence of anxiety disorders in pre-adolescent children. Clinical Psychology Review. 2006;26:817–833. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2005.12.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Casey BJ, JD, Cohen, et al. Activation of prefrontal cortex in children during a nonspatial working memory task with functional MRI. Neuroimage. 1995;2:221–229. doi: 10.1006/nimg.1995.1029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Caspi A, Sugden K, Moffitt TE, Taylor A, Craig IW, Harrington HL, et al. Influence of life stress on depression: moderation by a polymorphism in the 5-HTT gene. Science. 2003;301:386–389. doi: 10.1126/science.1083968. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chareyron LJ, Lavenex PB, Amaral DG, Lavenex P. Postnatal development of the amygdala: A stereological study in macaque monkeys. Journal of Comparative Neurology. 2012;520:1965–1984. doi: 10.1002/cne.23023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Charmandari E, Tsigos C, Chrousos G. Endocrinology of the stress response 1. Annu Rev Physiol. 2005;67:259–284. doi: 10.1146/annurev.physiol.67.040403.120816. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Coles ME, Heimberg RG. Memory biases in the anxiety disorders: Current status. Clinical Psychology Review. 2002;22:587–627. doi: 10.1016/s0272-7358(01)00113-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corcoran KA, Quirk GJ. Activity in prelimbic cortex is necessary for the expression of learned, but not innate, fears. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2007;27:840–844. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5327-06.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Croxson PL, Johansen-Berg H, Behrens TE, Robson MD, Pinsk MA, Gross CG, et al. Quantitative investigation of connections of the prefrontal cortex in the human and macaque using probabilistic diffusion tractography. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2005;25:8854–8866. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1311-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dallman MF. Editorial: moments in time—the neonatal rat hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal axis. Endocrinology. 2000;141:1590–1592. doi: 10.1210/endo.141.5.7527. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dallman MF. Fast glucocorticoid actions on brain: back to the future. Front Neuroendocrinol. 2005;26:103–108. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2005.08.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dawson G, Panagiotides H, Klinger LG, Hill D. The role of frontal lobe functioning in the development of infant self-regulatory behavior. Brain Cognition. 1992;20:152–175. doi: 10.1016/0278-2626(92)90066-u. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diamond A. Frontal lobe involvement in cognitive changes during the first year of life. In: KRG, Petersen AC, editors. Brain maturation and cognitive development: Comparative and cross-cultural perspectives. New York: Social Science Research Council; 1991. pp. 127–180. [Google Scholar]
- Gale GD, Anagnostaras SG, Godsil BP, Mitchell S, Nozawa T, Sage JR, et al. Role of the basolateral amygdala in the storage of fear memories across the adult lifetime of rats. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2004;24:3810–3815. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4100-03.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gee D, Gabard-Durnam LJ, Flannery J, Goff B, Humphreys KL, Telzer EH, et al. Early Developmental Emergence of Mature Human Amygdala-Prefrontal Phenotype following Maternal Deprivation: Evidence of Stress-Induced Acceleration. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2013a;110:15638–15643. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1307893110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gee DG, Humphreys KL, Flannery J, Goff B, Telzer EH, Shapiro M, et al. A Developmental Shift from Positive to Negative Connectivity in Human Amygdala–Prefrontal Circuitry. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2013b;33:4584–4593. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3446-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gibbs RA, Rogers J, Katze MG, Bumgarner R, Weinstock GM, Mardis ER, et al. Evolutionary and biomedical insights from the rhesus macaque genome. Science. 2007;316:222–234. doi: 10.1126/science.1139247. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gibson KR. Myelination and behavioral development: a comparative perspective on questions of neotony, altriciality, and intelligence. In: Gibson KR, Petersen AC, editors. Brain maturation and cognitive development: comparative and cross-cultural perspectives. Hawthorne, NY: Aldine de Gruyter; 1991. pp. 29–63. [Google Scholar]
- Gilles EE, Schultz L, Baram BM. Abnormal corticosterone regulation in an immature rat model of continuous chronic stress. Pediatric Neurology. 1996;15:114–119. doi: 10.1016/0887-8994(96)00153-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goff B, Gee DG, Telzer EH, Humphreys KL, Gabard-Durnam L, Flannery J, et al. Reduced Nucleus Accumbens Reactivity and Adolescent Depression following Early-life Stress. Neuroscience. 2012;249 doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2012.12.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harlow HF, Harlow MK, Suomi SJ. From thought to therapy: lessons from a primate laboratory. American Scientist. 1971;59:538–549. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haroutunian V, Campbell BA. Emergence of interoceptive and exteroceptive control of behavior in rats. Science. 1979;205:927–929. doi: 10.1126/science.472715. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harris JA, Westbrook RF. Evidence that GABA transmission mediates context-specific extinction of learned fear. Psychopharmacology. 1998;140:105–115. doi: 10.1007/s002130050745. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herman JP, Cullinan WE. Neurocircuitry of stress: central control of the hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenocortical axis. Trends Neurosci. 1997;20:78–84. doi: 10.1016/s0166-2236(96)10069-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hinde RA, Spencer-Booth Y. The behaviour of socially living rhesus monkeys in their first two and a half years. Animal Behaviour. 1967;15:169–196. doi: 10.1016/s0003-3472(67)80029-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hodges J, Tizard B. Social and family relationships of ex-institutional adolescents. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1989;30:77–97. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1989.tb00770.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hostinar CE, Sullivan RM, Gunnar MR. Psychobiological mechanisms underlying the social buffering of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenocortical axis: A review of animal models and human studies across development. Psychological bulletin. 2014;140:256. doi: 10.1037/a0032671. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huot RL, Plotsky PM, Lenox RH, McNamara RK. Neonatal maternal separation reduces hippocampal mossy fiber density in adult Long Evans rats. Brain Research. 2002;950:52–63. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(02)02985-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huttenlocher PR, Dabholkar AS. Regional differences in synaptogenesis in human cerebral cortex. Journal of comparative Neurology. 1997;387:167–178. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1096-9861(19971020)387:2<167::aid-cne1>3.0.co;2-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Juster RP, Bizik G, Picard M, Arsenault-Lapierre G, Sindi S, Trepanier L, et al. A transdisciplinary perspective of chronic stress in relation to psychopathology throughout life span development. Development and Psychopathology. 2011;23:725–776. doi: 10.1017/S0954579411000289. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kiess W, Meidert A, Dressendörfer R, Schriever K, Kessler U, Köunig A, et al. Salivary cortisol levels throughout childhood and adolescence: relation with age, pubertal stage, and weight. Pediatric Research. 1995;37:502–506. doi: 10.1203/00006450-199504000-00020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim JH, Hamlin AS, Richardson R. Fear extinction across development: the involvement of the medial prefrontal cortex as assessed by temporary inactivation and immunohistochemistry. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2009;29:10802–10808. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0596-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim JH, Li S, Hamlin AS, McNally GP, Richardson R. Phosphorylation of mitogen-activated protein kinase in the medial prefrontal cortex and the amygdala following memory retrieval or forgetting in developing rats. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory. 2012;97:59–68. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2011.09.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim JH, Richardson R. A developmental dissociation in reinstatement of an extinguished fear response in rats. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory. 2007a;88:48–57. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2007.03.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim JH, Richardson R. A developmental dissociation of context and GABA effects on extinguished fear in rats. Behavioral Neuroscience. 2007b;121:131–139. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.121.1.131. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim JH, Richardson R. New findings on extinction of conditioned fear early in development: theoretical and clinical implications. Biological Psychiatry. 2010;67:297–303. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.09.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Knickmeyer RC, Styner M, Short SJ, Lubach GR, Kang C, Hamer R, et al. Maturational trajectories of cortical brain development through the pubertal transition: unique species and sex differences in the monkey revealed through structural magnetic resonance imaging. Cerebral cortex. 2010;20:1053–1063. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp166. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kosten TA, Lee HJ, Kim JJ. Early life stress impairs fear conditioning in adult male and female rats. Brain Research. 2006;1087:142–150. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.03.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kosten TA, Miserendino MJ, Bombace JC, Lee HJ, Kim JJ. Sex-selective effects of neonatal isolation on fear conditioning and foot shock sensitivity. Behavioural Brain Research. 2005;157:235–244. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2004.07.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kreppner JM, Rutter M, Beckett C, Castle J, Colvert E, Groothues C, et al. Normality and impairment following profound early institutional deprivation: A longitudinal follow-up into early adolescence. Developmental Psychology. 2007;43:931–946. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.43.4.93. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuhn CM, Schanberg SM. Responses to maternal separation: mechanisms and mediators. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience. 1998;16:261–270. doi: 10.1016/s0736-5748(98)00034-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ladd CO, Huot RL, Thrivikraman KV, Nemeroff CB, Plotsky PM. Long-term adaptations in glucocorticoid receptor and mineralocorticoid receptor mRNA and negative feedback on the hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal axis following neonatal maternal separation. Biological Psychiatry. 2004;55:367–375. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2003.10.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ladd CO, Thrivikraman KV, Huot RL, Plotsky PM. Differential neuroendocrine responses to chronic variable stress in adult Long Evans rats exposed to handling-maternal separation as neonates. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2005;30:520–533. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2004.12.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levin R, Levine S. Development of circadian periodicity in base and stress levels of corticosterone. American Journal of Physiology--Legacy Content. 1975;229:1397–1399. doi: 10.1152/ajplegacy.1975.229.5.1397. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levine S. Plasma-free corticosteroid response to electric shock in rats stimulated in infancy. Science. 1962;135:795–796. doi: 10.1126/science.135.3506.795-a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levine S. Primary social relationships influence the development of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis in the rat. Physiology & Behavior. 2001;73:255–260. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(01)00496-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lewis DA, Levitt P. Schizophrenia as a disorder of neurodevelopment. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 2002;25:409–432. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.25.112701.142754. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li S, Kim JH, Richardson R. Differential involvement of the medial prefrontal cortex in the expression of learned fear across development. Behavioral Neuroscience. 2012;126:217–225. doi: 10.1037/a0027151. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lissek S, Powers AS, McClure EB, Phelps EA, Woldehawariat G, Grillon C, et al. Classical fear conditioning in the anxiety disorders: a meta-analysis. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2005;43:1391–1424. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2004.10.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lupien SJS Parent e. a. Larger amygdala but no change in hippocampal volume in 10-year-old children exposed to maternal depressive symptomatology since birth. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011;108:14324–14329. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1105371108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Machado CJ, Bachevalier J. Non-human primate models of childhood psychopathology: the promise and the limitations. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2003;44:64–87. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00103. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maestripieri D, Carroll KA. Risk factors for infant abuse and neglect in group-living rhesus monkeys. Psychological Sciences. 1998;9:143–145. [Google Scholar]
- McGowan PO, Sasaki A, D’Alessio AC, Dymov S, Labonté B, Szyf M, et al. Epigenetic regulation of the glucocorticoid receptor in human brain associates with childhood abuse. Nature Neuroscience. 2009;12:342–348. doi: 10.1038/nn.2270. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mehta MA, NI, Golembo, et al. Amygdala, hippocampal and corpus callosum size following severe early institutional deprivation: the English and Romanian Adoptees study pilot. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry. 2009;50:943–951. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02084.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Milad MR, Orr SP, Lasko NB, Chang Y, Rauch SL, Pitman RK. Presence and acquired origin of reduced recall for fear extinction in PTSD: results of a twin study. Journal of Psychiatric Research. 2008;42:515–520. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2008.01.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Milad MR, Pitman RK, Ellis CB, Gold AL, Shin LM, Lasko NB, et al. Neurobiological basis of failure to recall extinction memory in posttraumatic stress disorder. Biological Psychiatry. 2009;66:1075–1082. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.06.026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Milad MR, Quirk GJ. Neurons in medial prefrontal cortex signal memory for fear extinction. Nature. 2002;420:70–74. doi: 10.1038/nature01138. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Milad MR, Rauch SL, Pitman RK, Quirk GJ. Fear extinction in rats: implications for human brain imaging and anxiety disorders. Biological Psychology. 2006;73:61–71. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2006.01.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Monk CS, Telzer EH, Mogg K, Bradley BP, Mai X, Louro H, et al. Amygdala and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex activation to masked angry faces in children and adolescents with generalized anxiety disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2008;65:568–576. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.65.5.568. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moriceau S, TL, Roth T, et al. Rodent model of infant attachment learning and stress. Dev Psychobiol. 2010;52:651–660. doi: 10.1002/dev.20482. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moriceau S, Wilson DA, Levine S, Sullivan RM. Dual circuitry for odor–shock conditioning during infancy: Corticosterone switches between fear and attraction via amygdala. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2006;26:6737–6748. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0499-06.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nelson CA. A neurobiological perspective on early human deprivation. Child Development Perspectives. 2007;1:13–18. [Google Scholar]
- Newman DL, Moffitt TE, Caspi A, Magdol L, Silva PA, Stanton WR. Psychiatric disorder in a birth cohort of young adults: Prevalence, comorbidity, clinical significance, and new case incidence from ages 11–21. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1996;64:552–562. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O’Conner TG, Marvin RS, Rutter M, Olrick JT, Britner PA. Child–parent attachment following early institutional deprivation. Development and Psychopathology. 2003;15:19–38. doi: 10.1017/s0954579403000026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Öngür D, Price J. The organization of networks within the orbital and medial prefrontal cortex of rats, monkeys and humans. Cerebral cortex. 2000;10:206–219. doi: 10.1093/cercor/10.3.206. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ono M, Kikusui T, Sasaki N, Ichikawa M, Mori Y, Murakami-Murofushi K. Early weaning induces anxiety and precocious myelination in the anterior part of the basolateral amygdala of male Balb/c mice. Neuroscience. 2008;156:1103–1110. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2008.07.078. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paschall GY, Davis M. Olfactory-mediated fear-potentiated startle. Behavioral Neuroscience. 2002;116:4. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.116.1.4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Payne C, Machado CJ, Bliwise NG, Bachevalier J. Maturation of the hippocampal formation and amygdala in Macaca mulatta: a volumetric magnetic resonance imaging study. Hippocampus. 2010;20:922–935. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20688. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Plotsky PM, Meaney MJ. Early, postnatal experience alters hypothalamic corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF) mRNA, median eminence CRF content and stress-induced release in adult rats. Molecular Brain Research. 1993;18:195–200. doi: 10.1016/0169-328x(93)90189-v. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Plotsky PM, Thrivikraman KV, Nemeroff CB, Caldji C, Sharma S, Meaney MJ. Long-term consequences of neonatal rearing on central corticotropin-releasing factor systems in adult male rat offspring. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2005;30:2192–2204. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300769. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Polanczyk G, Caspi A, Williams B, Price TS, Danese A, Sugden K, et al. Protective effect of CRHR1 gene variants on the development of adult depression following childhood maltreatment: replication and extension. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2009;66:978–985. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Preuss TM. Do rats have prefrontal cortex? The Rose-Woolsey-Akert program reconsidered. J Cogn Neurosci. 1995;7:1–24. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1995.7.1.1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Preuss TM, Goldman-Rakic PS. Ipsilateral cortical connections of granular frontal cortex in the strepsirhine primate Galago, with comparative comments on anthropoid primates. Journal of comparative Neurology. 1991a;310:507–549. doi: 10.1002/cne.903100404. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Preuss TM, Goldman-Rakic PS. Myelo-and cytoarchitecture of the granular frontal cortex and surrounding regions in the strepsirhine primate Galago and the anthropoid primate Macaca. Journal of comparative Neurology. 1991b;310:429–474. doi: 10.1002/cne.903100402. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Provençal N, Suderman MJ, Guillemin C, Massart R, Ruggiero A, Wang D, et al. The signature of maternal rearing in the methylome in rhesus macaque prefrontal cortex and T cells. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2012;32:15626–15642. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1470-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Qin S, Young CB, Supekar K, Uddin LQ, Menon V. Immature integration and segregation of emotion-related brain circuitry in young children. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2012;109:7941–7946. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1120408109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Quirk GJ, Likhtik E, Pelletier JG, Paré D. Stimulation of medial prefrontal cortex decreases the responsiveness of central amygdala output neurons. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2003;23:8800–8807. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-25-08800.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raineki C, Cortes MR, Belnoue L, Sullivan RM. Effects of Early-Life Abuse Differ across Development: Infant Social Behavior Deficits Are Followed by Adolescent Depressive-Like Behaviors Mediated by the Amygdala. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2012;32:7758–7765. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5843-11.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raineki C, Moriceau S, Sullivan R. Developing a neurobehavioral animal model of infant attachment to an abusive caregiver. Biological Psychiatry. 2010;67:1137–1145. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.12.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reep R. Relationship between prefrontal and limbic cortex: a comparative anatomical review. Brain Behavior Evolution. 1984;25:5–80. doi: 10.1159/000118849. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rilling JK, Winslow JT, O’Brien D, Gutman DA, Hoffman JM, Kilts CD. Neural correlates of maternal separation in rhesus monkeys. Biological Psychiatry. 2001;49:146–157. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(00)00977-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Roth TL, Sullivan RM. Memory of early maltreatment: neonatal behavioral and neural correlates of maternal maltreatment within the context of classical conditioning. Biological Psychiatry. 2005;57:823–831. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.01.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ruppenthal GC, Arling GL, Harlow HF, Sackett GP, Suomi SJ. A 10-year perspective of motherless-mother monkey behavior. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1976;85:341–349. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.85.4.341. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rutter M. Developmental catch-up, and deficit, following adoption after severe global early privation. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 1998;39:465–476. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rutter ML, Kreppner JM, O’Conner TG. Specificity and heterogeneity in children’s responses to profound institutional privation. The British Journal of Psychiatry. 2001;179:97–103. doi: 10.1192/bjp.179.2.97. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sanchez M. The impact of early adverse care on HPA axis development: nonhuman primate models. Hormones & Behavior. 2006;50:623–631. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2006.06.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sanchez MM, Ladd CO, Plotsky PM. Early adverse experience as a developmental risk factor for later psychopathology: evidence from rodent and primate models. Development and Psychopathology. 2001a;13:419–449. doi: 10.1017/s0954579401003029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sanchez MM, Ladd CO, Plotsky PM. Early adverse experience as a developmental risk factor for later psychopathology: evidence from rodent and primate models. Dev Psychopathol. 2001b;13:419–449. doi: 10.1017/s0954579401003029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sanchez MM, Noble PM, Lyon CK, Plotsky PM, Davis M, Nemeroff CB, et al. Alterations in diurnal cortisol rhythm and acoustic startle response in nonhuman primates with adverse rearing. Biological Psychiatry. 2005;57:373–381. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.11.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sanchez MM, Young LJ, Plotsky PM, Insel TR. Distribution of corticosteroid receptors in the rhesus brain: relative absence of glucocorticoid receptors in the hippocampal formation. Journal of Neuroscience. 2000;20:4657–4668. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-12-04657.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seay B, Gottfried NW. A phylogenetic perspective for social behavior in primates. The Journal of general psychology. 1975;92:5–17. doi: 10.1080/00221309.1975.9711323. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seltzer LJ, Ziegler TE, Pollak SD. Social vocalizations can release oxytocin in humans. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2010;277:2661–2666. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2010.0567. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sheridan MA, NA, Fox, et al. Variation in neural development as a result of exposure to institutionalization early in childhood. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2012;109:12927–12932. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1200041109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shionoya K, Moriceau S, Lunday L, Miner C, Roth TL, Sullivan RM. Development switch in neural circuitry underlying odor-malaise learning. Learn Mem. 2006;13:801–808. doi: 10.1101/lm.316006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sierra-Mercado D, Padilla-Coreano N, Quirk GJ. Dissociable roles of prelimbic and infralimbic cortices, ventral hippocampus, and basolateral amygdala in the expression and extinction of conditioned fear. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;36:529–538. doi: 10.1038/npp.2010.184. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smotherman WP. Odor aversion learning by the rat fetus. Physiology & Behavior. 1982;29:769–771. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(82)90322-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spataro J, Mullen PE, Burgess PM, Wells DL, Moss SA. Impact of child sexual abuse on mental health. The British Journal of Psychiatry. 2004;184:416–421. doi: 10.1192/bjp.184.5.416. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spinelli S, Chefer S, Suomi SJ, Higley JD, Barr CS, Stein E. Early-life stress induces long-term morphologic changes in primate brain. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2009;66:658–665. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.52. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stevenson CW, Meredith JP, Spicer CH, Mason R, Marsden CA. Early life programming of innate fear and fear learning in adult female rats. Behavioural Brain Research. 2009;198:51–57. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2008.10.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sullivan RM, Holman PJ. Transitions in sensitive period attachment learning in infancy: the role of corticosterone. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2010;34:835–844. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.11.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sullivan RM, Landers M, Yeaman B, Wilson DA. Neurophysiology: Good memories of bad events in infancy. Nature. 2000;407:38–39. doi: 10.1038/35024156. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Suomi SJ, Harlow HF, Domek CJ. Effect of repetitive infant-infant separation of young monkeys. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1970;76:161. doi: 10.1037/h0029809. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Suomi SJ, Harlow HF, Kimball SD. Behavioral effects of prolonged partial social isolation in the rhesus monkey. Psychological Reports. 1971;29:1171–1177. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1971.29.3f.1171. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Takahashi LK. Organizing action of corticosterone on the development of behavioral inhibition in the preweanling rat. Developmental Brain Research. 1994;81:121–127. doi: 10.1016/0165-3806(94)90074-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Takahashi LK, Nakashima BR, Hong H, Watanabe K. The smell of danger: a behavioral and neural analysis of predator odor-induced fear. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2005;29:1157–1167. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.04.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taylor SE, Way BM, Seeman TE. Early adversity and adult health outcomes. Development and Psychopathology. 2011;23:939–954. doi: 10.1017/S0954579411000411. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thiebaut de Schotten M, Dell’Acqua F, Valabregue R, Catani M. Monkey to human comparative anatomy of the frontal lobe association tracts. Cortex. 2012;48:82–96. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2011.10.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thompson BL, Levitt P. The clinical-basic interface in defining pathogenesis in disorders of neurodevelopmental origin. Neuron. 2010;67:702–712. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.08.037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tottenham N. Human amygdala development in the absence of species-expected caregiving. Developmental Psychobiology. 2012;54:598–611. doi: 10.1002/dev.20531. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tottenham N, Hare T, Millner A, Gilhooly T, Zevin J, Casey B. Elevated amygdala response to faces following early deprivation. Developmental Science. 2011;14:190–204. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00971.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tottenham N, Hare TA, Quinn BT, McCarry TW, Nurse M, Gilhooly T, et al. Prolonged institutional rearing is associated with atypically large amygdala volume and difficulties in emotion regulation. Developmental Science. 2010;13:46–61. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00852.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tottenham N, Sheridan MA. A review of adversity, the amygdala and the hippocampus: a consideration of developmental timing. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2009;3:Article 68. doi: 10.3389/neuro.09.068.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ulrich-Lai YM, Herman JP. Neural regulation of endocrine and autonomic stress responses. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2009;10:397–409. doi: 10.1038/nrn2647. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Upton KJ, Sullivan RM. Defining age limits of the sensitive period for attachment learning in rat pups. Developmental Psychobiology. 2010;52:453–464. doi: 10.1002/dev.20448. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- VanOers H, Kloet ED, Whelan T, Levine S. Maternal deprivation effect on the infant’s neural stress markers is reversed by tactile stimulation and feeding but not by suppressing corticosterone. Neuroscience. 1998;18:10171–10179. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-23-10171.1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vidal-Gonzalez I, Vidal-Gonzalez B, Rauch SL, Quirk GJ. Microstimulation reveals opposing influences of prelimbic and infralimbic cortex on the expression of conditioned fear. Learning & Memory. 2006;13:728–733. doi: 10.1101/lm.306106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weaver ICG, Cervoni N, Champagne FA, D’Alessio AC, Sharma S, Seckl JR, et al. Epigenetic programming by maternal behavior. Nature Neuroscience. 2004;7:847–854. doi: 10.1038/nn1276. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wiik KL, MM, Loman, et al. Behavioral and emotional symptoms of post-institutionalized children in middle childhood. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2011;52:56–63. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02294.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wilber AA, Southwood CJ, Wellman CL. Brief neonatal maternal separation alters extinction of conditioned fear and corticolimbic glucocorticoid and NMDA receptor expression in adult rats. Developmental Neurobiology. 2009;69:73–87. doi: 10.1002/dneu.20691. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yap CSL, Richardson R. Extinction in the developing rat: An examination of renewal effects. Developmental Psychobiology. 2007;49:565–575. doi: 10.1002/dev.20244. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zeanah C, Egger H, Smyke A, Nelson C, Fox N, Marshall P, et al. Institutional rearing and psychiatric disorders in Romanian preschool children. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2009;166:777–785. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.08091438. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]