Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2017 Apr 1.
Published in final edited form as: Curr Opin Behav Sci. 2016 Apr;8:214–219. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2016.02.009

Recent advances in understanding emotion-driven temporal distortions

Jessica I Lake 1
PMCID: PMC4834897  NIHMSID: NIHMS758464  PMID: 27104212

Abstract

Emotions are powerful drivers of distortions in time perception. Recent work continues to support arousal and attentional mechanisms of emotion-driven temporal distortions. A possible memory-related mechanism and various modulatory factors, such as age, gender, and psychopathology, have also been implicated in such distortions. Beyond the rich behavioral literature on this topic, neurobiological substrates associated with emotion-driven temporal distortions have begun to be identified and represent an important next step for research within this domain. The study of emotion-driven temporal distortions holds great promise for advancing our understanding of this perceptual phenomenon and how it may play a functional role in mediating changes in cognition, behavior, and emotion.

Graphical Abstract

graphic file with name nihms758464f2.jpg

Introduction

Emotions are dynamic, unfolding over time. Despite this temporal dependence, it is well known that the way in which we perceive time during emotional experiences does not always conform to reality. Over the past two decades, increased interest in this perceptual phenomenon has greatly advanced an understanding of how emotions distort time perception and the various factors that modulate these distortions. In more recent years, research within this domain has extended in several exciting new directions. This review will summarize the current state of the literature on emotion-driven temporal distortions in humans, highlighting the psychological and neural mechanisms associated with emotional influences on time perception, the factors that modulate these influences, the translation of emotional timing paradigms into the clinical domain, and the available evidence that emotion-driven temporal distortions shape decision-making and possibly emotion itself (see Figure 1). Throughout the review, promising directions for future research will be emphasized.

Figure 1. The relationship between emotion and time perception.

Figure 1

Extension of (a part of) the pacemaker-accumulator model of time perception [1,2,8] to include the mechanisms of emotion-driven temporal distortions and the processes influenced by emotion-driven manipulations of perceived duration.

Psychological Mechanisms

Across investigations of emotion-driven temporal distortions, a general picture has emerged of the predominant mechanisms underlying such distortions, supported by pacemaker-accumulator models of time perception [1,2]. Specifically, emotions appear to alter time perception via changes in arousal and attention. According to pacemaker-accumulator models, arousal speeds the rate of an internal timekeeping pacemaker, such that an individual perceives more time to have elapsed than the objective duration of the emotional event, resulting in the overestimation of duration. However, in many initial studies of emotion-driven temporal distortions, effects of arousal may have been confounded by factors such as the complexity of visual stimuli [3]. Recent investigations have more clearly linked arousal to duration overestimation by demonstrating a positive relationship between overestimations and arousal ratings [4,5] and by using conditioned stimuli [6,7]. The advantage of using conditioned stimuli is that these stimuli can be simple, as their emotionality is derived from their association with an emotional event, thus deconfounding visual stimulus properties from the effects of arousal.

In addition to evidence for arousal-mediated effects of emotion on time perception, emotion-driven changes in attention have also been shown to modulate time perception. Changes in attention are conceptualized as affecting the activity of a switch or gate that influences the number of pulses emitted from the pacemaker and subsequently collected in an accumulator, representing the perceived duration of the stimulus being timed [2,8]. If attention is directed away from timing, such as in the case of attentional capture by emotion-laden stimuli [9,10], the functionality of the switch/gate changes, resulting in fewer pulses being passed to the accumulator and the underestimation of duration. Such effects have been found by experimentally manipulating emotion, via emotional pictures [11], faces [12], drug-induced euphoria [13], and conditioned stimuli [14].

In addition to the empirical support for the mechanistic roles of arousal and attention in emotion-driven temporal distortions, a recent investigation suggests that the impact of emotion on memory could influence time perception by changing the internal reference to which an interval is compared. Grondin and colleagues [15] showed participants pictures of mutilation and neutral and disgusted faces. Participants were asked to decide if each presented image matched previously learned ‘short’ or ‘long’ target durations. Mutilation images were more frequently overestimated in duration than neutral and disgusted faces. However, the relative overestimation of mutilation images appeared to be driven by a bias to respond ‘short’ more frequently for the neutral/disgusted faces, rather than a bias to respond ‘long’ more frequently for mutilation images. The authors suggested that this bias might be the result of an effect of emotion on memory, such that the durations of emotional images were given more weight in forming a representation of the durations to which the presented stimuli were compared. Further work can help to establish the relative contributions of memory representations for durations versus pacemaker and switch/gate mechanisms to emotion-driven temporal distortions.

Neural Mechanisms

Only in the past few years have human studies begun to address the neural regions associated with emotion-driven temporal distortions. Theories have focused on the anterior insula as a key potential region involved in the subjective experience of time and the integration of emotional experiences into an internal representation of time [16,17]. Functional MRI investigations of emotion-driven temporal distortions have provided support for the engagement of regions implicated in timing networks, including the anterior insula, as well as the right inferior frontal gyrus (rIFG) and supplementary motor area (SMA), and emotionally relevant regions, such as the amygdala [4,18,19]. A particularly compelling finding was that a rIFG region, defined by a timing localizer task, showed greater activation for emotional versus neutral images during an emotional timing task [19] and the magnitude of this contrast correlated with an emotion-driven overestimation bias.

A promising novel approach to investigating emotion-driven temporal distortions was taken by Tipples [20] who used drift diffusion modeling for a bisection timing task, wherein spider phobics and healthy controls were tasked with judging whether pictures of spider and birds were presented for a duration that more closely matched a previously learned ‘short’ or ‘long’ duration. The findings supported an increase in temporal accumulation in spider phobics for images of spiders versus birds, resulting in changes in the rate at which a decision threshold was met, with this decision threshold representing the time point at which a presented image switches from being judged as closer in duration to the ‘short’ duration to being judged as closer in duration to the ‘long’ duration. This finding dovetails with the results of a recent fMRI investigation [18] in which a bisection timing task was used to demonstrate that activation in a rIFG/anterior insula cluster and the SMA were modulated by emotion at the stimulus duration associated with the slowest response times, indicative of maximal temporal discrimination difficulty, and thus, the duration nearest the decision threshold. These findings suggest a role for the rIFG/anterior insula and SMA in the affective modulation of temporal decision-making.

Neuroimaging studies have begun to identify regions that may plausibly be implicated in emotion-driven temporal distortions, with encouraging consistency in reported regions across investigations. Nevertheless, the specific mechanistic contributions of these regions to distortions in time perception remain unclear. Investigating functional connectivity measures will be an important avenue for future research to understand the likely communication between emotion detection regions, such as the amygdala, and regions implicated in timing networks, such as the rIFG, insula, and SMA.

Modulatory Factors

Beyond attempts to clarify mechanisms, much of the work on emotion-driven temporal distortions has focused on identifying factors that modulate these distortions. Findings for valence-related modulations have varied [2125] and a better understanding of these discrepancies across studies is necessary. Recent evidence has shown that declarative knowledge—what information people were told about the expected direction of emotional biases on time perception—enhanced existing biases if the information was accurate and decreased existing biases if the information was inaccurate [26]. Participant gender [27] and age [28], perceived control over emotional events [29,30], as well as trait measures of fearfulness [29,31] and anxiety [32] have all been shown to modulate the magnitude of emotion-driven temporal distortions. These findings point to the ability of various factors to impact the magnitude of temporal distortions. At the same time, Van Volkinburg and Balsam [25] demonstrated that contextual factors may modulate not just the magnitude of an emotion-driven temporal distortions, but also the underlying mechanisms of distortion. In a temporal reproduction task, these researchers found that emotion-driven temporal distortions were consistent with an enhanced arousal effect when emotional stimuli were presented during duration encoding, but were more consistent with an attentional distraction effect when an emotional stimulus was presented during the reproduction of a temporal duration. The growing number of factors reported to modulate emotion-driven temporal distortions are a testament to the flexibility of time perception.

Clinical Populations

The field of emotion-driven temporal distortions has seen a recent increase in translation into the clinical domain [3335]. Whereas baseline time perception appears to be intact in emotion-related disorders [36,37], exaggerations of emotion-driven temporal distortions have been observed, particularly in individuals with anxiety and specific phobias [20,38,39]. Manic, but not euthymic, bipolar patients also demonstrate altered emotion-driven temporal distortions, suggesting that such distortions are state, rather than trait, dependent features of this disorder [33]. Intriguingly, individuals with autism spectrum disorders, known to have problems with face processing and social interaction, do not differ from healthy controls in demonstrating the canonical overestimation of emotional relative to neutral faces [35], suggesting that implicit emotional processing may be intact in these individuals. The translation of emotion-driven temporal distortion paradigms for use in clinical populations seems a particularly fruitful area for further research to better understand how timing during emotional experiences can go awry and what that might tell us about the nature of timing as well as the perceptual experiences and behaviors of clinical populations [40].

Consequences for Cognition, Behavior, and Emotion

The relationship between emotion and time begs the question of whether emotion-driven temporal distortions are merely a byproduct of emotional processing or instead, play a functional role. Although little research has attempted to address this question directly, available evidence suggests that temporal distortions may mediate certain consequences of emotion. For example, time perception has been implicated in decision-making processes and, as emotions contribute to time perception, the emotional modulation of time perception may have important consequences for such processes. One decision-making domain that might be particularly sensitive to emotion-driven temporal distortions is the phenomenon of temporal discounting [4146]. Temporal discounting is defined as the devaluation of future rewards in favor of more imminent rewards and follows a hyperbolic response distribution, such that the subjective value of future rewards decreases with the duration of the delay. The steepness of this function is sensitive to individual difference factors, such as impulsivity; more impulsive individuals demonstrate greater discounting of future rewards in favor of temporally proximal rewards [4750]. Theoretical work suggests that biases in time perception may mediate the relationship between impulsivity and temporal discounting [44]. A tendency to overestimate the duration of time between a decision and its anticipated outcome may explain the greater tendency to favor more imminent rewards. In support of such theories, empirical studies have supported a relationship between impulsivity and temporal biases [51,52] and between time perception and temporal discounting [42,43,46,53].

Given the available evidence pointing to the role of time perception in influencing temporal discounting, emotions may have a considerable impact on the role of time perception in temporal discounting [44]. If decision-making occurs during or after an emotionally arousing event, an arousal-driven temporal overestimation bias may increase the steepness of the hyperbolic temporal discounting curve, resulting in an increased bias towards imminent versus delayed rewards. Such an influence would be similar to that of impulsivity. This premise is consistent with recent evidence that emotional priming with negative images, relative to priming with neutral or positive images, induces greater temporal discounting in favor of smaller but more temporally proximal rewards [54]. Furthermore, this temporal discounting effect was correlated with time perception during a temporal reproduction task, such that greater temporal discounting after negative priming was associated with greater temporal overestimation of negative versus neutral images. This is just one example of how time perception may mediate the effects of emotion on cognition and behavior. Other aspects of decision-making, such as when decisions are made under time pressure, might also be affected by emotion-driven temporal distortion.

Although it is clear that emotion influences the perception of time, preliminary evidence suggests that the relationship between emotion and time perception may be bidirectional. In a study of pain, Pomares and colleagues [55] delivered painful thermal stimulation while participants viewed a clock that indicated the duration of the painful stimulation. In one condition, the clock indicated that the duration of the painful stimulation was shorter than the duration of stimulation in the other condition, but the actual durations of painful stimulation were identical in both conditions. Participants generally rated the stimulation as less painful during the condition in which they were led to believe that the duration of stimulation was shorter, suggesting that perceived time influenced affective responses irrespective of the actual duration of painful stimulation. Given the positive relationship between arousal and temporal overestimation, it is possible that perceived time may serve as a proxy for emotional intensity. It would be fruitful to consider if such an effect generalizes to highly arousing emotions, such as fear or excitement. If time perception serves a feedback function, it could have important implications in understanding exaggerated emotional responses, such as in individuals with heightened fear and anxiety who tend to show exacerbated temporal biases during emotionally arousing events [29,31,32]. These exaggerated overestimation biases could increase the perceived intensity of an emotionally arousing event, perpetuating fearful and anxious responding. Additionally, individuals with anxiety often generalize fear to nonthreatening cues [5658]. It could be worthwhile to assess whether such generalizations are mediated by distortions in time perception that are interpreted as evidence of threat. Preliminary evidence has supported a possible relationship between threat discrimination and time perception [14].

Conclusions

Investigations of emotional influences on time perception have made considerable progress in furthering our understanding of the psychological mechanisms underlying the plasticity of our sense of time and the factors that modulate emotion-driven temporal distortions (Figure 1). Investigations into the neural correlates of this perceptual phenomenon are still in their infancy, but further consideration of neural mechanisms holds great promise for advancing an understanding of emotion-driven temporal distortions, particularly given criticisms of the biological plausibility of pacemaker-accumulator models typically relied on for interpreting emotional timing data [59, 60]. The recent translation of emotional timing paradigms into the clinical domain may have important consequences for understanding the perceptual experiences of individuals with emotion-related disorders. At the same time, investigating how emotion-driven time perception may mediate emotional effects on cognition and behavior, and even feedback to influence emotion itself is an especially attractive area for further research. If these distortions do play a functional role, future work should consider how the malleability of time perception could be leveraged to affect behavioral, cognitive, and emotional changes. There is great promise that the study of emotion-driven temporal distortions can tell us more about how time perception shapes how we experience the world [61].

Highlights.

  • Emotions influence timing via changes in arousal, attention, and possibly memory.

  • Emotion-driven time distortions are modulated by trait and state dependent factors.

  • Evidence implicates the insula, rIFG, SMA, and amygdala in these distortions.

  • Emotion effects on timing may impact cognition, behavior, and emotion itself.

Acknowledgments

I would like to thank Dr. Cindy Yee for helpful comments on this manuscript. This work was supported by T32 MH096682.

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

References

  • 1.Treisman M. Temporal discrimination and the indifference interval. Implications for a model of the "internal clock". Psychol Monogr. 1963;77:1–31. doi: 10.1037/h0093864. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Gibbon J, Church RM, Meck WH. Scalar timing in memory. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1984;423:52–77. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1984.tb23417.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Folta-Schoofs K, Wolf OT, Treue S, Schoofs D. Perceptual complexity, rather than valence or arousal accounts for distracter-induced overproductions of temporal durations. Acta Psychol (Amst) 2014;147:51–59. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2013.10.001. *This study found that the complexity of images, rather than ratings of arousal and valence, was responsible for modulating timing in a temporal reproduction task. These findings highlight the necessity of controlling for potential perceptual confounds in assessing the effects of emotion on time perception and may speak to discrepancies in the literature.
  • 4.Dirnberger G, Hesselmann G, Roiser JP, Preminger S, Jahanshahi M, Paz R. Give it time: neural evidence for distorted time perception and enhanced memory encoding in emotional situations. NeuroImage. 2012;63:591–599. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.06.041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Gil S, Droit-Volet S. Emotional time distortions: the fundamental role of arousal. Cogn Emot. 2012;26:847–862. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2011.625401. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Kliegl KM, Watrin L, Huckauf A. Duration perception of emotional stimuli: using evaluative conditioning to avoid sensory confounds. Cogn Emot. 2015:1–18. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2014.978841. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Fayolle S, Gil S, Droit-Volet S. Fear and time: fear speeds up the internal clock. Behav Processes. 2015;120:135–140. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2015.09.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Zakay D. Subjective time and attentional resource allocation: an integrated model of time estimation. In: Levin I, Zakay D, editors. Time and human cognition: a life span perspective. North Holland: 1989. pp. 365–397. [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Ohman A, Flykt A, Esteves F. Emotion drives attention: detecting the snake in the grass. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2001;130:466–478. doi: 10.1037//0096-3445.130.3.466. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Mogg K, Bradley BP. Some methodological issues in assessing attentional biases for threatening faces in anxiety: a replication study using a modified version of the probe detection task. Behav Res Ther. 1999;37:595–604. doi: 10.1016/s0005-7967(98)00158-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Lui MA, Penney TB, Schirmer A. Emotion effects on timing: attention versus pacemaker accounts. PLoS One. 2011;6:e21829. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0021829. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Gil S, Droit-Volet S. Time perception in response to ashamed faces in children and adults. Scand J Psychol. 2011;52:138–145. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9450.2010.00858.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Lake JI, Meck WH. Differential effects of amphetamine and haloperidol on temporal reproduction: dopaminergic regulation of attention and clock speed. Neuropsychologia. 2013;51:284–292. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.09.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Lake JI, Meck WH, LaBar KS. Discriminative fear learners are resilient to temporal distortions during threat anticipation. Timing Time Percept. doi: 10.1163/22134468-00002063. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Grondin S, Laflamme V, Gontier É. Effect on perceived duration and sensitivity to time when observing disgusted faces and disgusting mutilation pictures. Atten Percept Psychophys. 2014;76:1522–1534. doi: 10.3758/s13414-014-0682-7. * Pictures of mutilation were found to be overestimated in duration relative to images of neutral and disgusted faces but interestingly, within the context of a bisection procedure, this bias appeared to be driven by an underestimation of the duration of neutral/disgusted faces, rather than an overestimation of mutilation pictures. This finding suggests that emotion may bias memory representations for interval duration, which is an important potential mechanism of emotion-driven temporal distortion to further explore.
  • 16.Wittmann M. The inner experience of time. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2009;364:1955–1967. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2009.0003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Craig AD. Emotional moments across time: a possible neural basis for time perception in the anterior insula. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2009;364:1933–1942. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2009.0008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Tipples J, Brattan V, Johnston P. Facial emotion modulates the neural mechanisms responsible for short interval time perception. Brain Topogr. 2015;28:104–112. doi: 10.1007/s10548-013-0350-6. * This investigation demonstrated that emotions modulate neural activity in regions associated with time perception, specifically at the time point when temporal judgments are most difficult, suggesting a role for these regions in temporal decision-making. This study represent an important first step in understanding the neural mechanisms of emotion-driven temporal distortions.
  • 19. Pfeuty M, Dilharreguy B, Gerlier L, Allard M. fMRI identifies the right inferior frontal cortex as the brain region where time interval processing is altered by negative emotional arousal. Hum Brain Mapp. 2015;36:981–995. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22680. * Activity in the rIFG was found to be modulated by a timing localizer task and by emotion in an emotional timing task. The magnitude of the contrast between the emotional and neutral condition in this region was correlated with the degree of overestimation in the emotional condition.
  • 20. Tipples J. Rapid temporal accumulation in spider fear: evidence from hierarchical drift diffusion modelling. Emotion. 2015;15:742–751. doi: 10.1037/emo0000079. ** This study used drift diffusion modelling to demonstrate that temporal information accumulates more rapidly in spider fearful individuals for phobic images as compared to images of birds. A similar effect was not found for nonfearful individuals. This new approach of using response times, in addition to temporal judgments, to inform emotion-driven temporal distortions holds great promise for novel inferences to be made about the mechanisms of such distortions that are not possible using traditional response analyses alone.
  • 21.Smith SD, McIver TA, Di Nella MS, Crease ML. The effects of valence and arousal on the emotional modulation of time perception: evidence for multiple stages of processing. Emotion. 2011;11:1305–1313. doi: 10.1037/a0026145. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Noulhiane M, Mella N, Samson S, Ragot R, Pouthas V. How emotional auditory stimuli modulate time perception. Emotion. 2007;7:697–704. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.7.4.697. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Angrilli A, Cherubini P, Pavese A, Mantredini S. The influence of affective factors on time perception. Percept Psychophys. 1997;59:972–982. doi: 10.3758/bf03205512. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Droit-Volet S, Tourret S, Wearden J. Perception of the duration of auditory and visual stimuli in children and adults. Q J Exp Psychol A. 2004;57:797–818. doi: 10.1080/02724980343000495. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Van Volkinburg H, Balsam P. Effects of emotional valence and arousal on time perception. Timing Time Percept. 2014;2:360–378. doi: 10.1163/22134468-00002034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Droit-Volet S, Lamotte M, Izaute M. The conscious awareness of time distortions regulates the effect of emotion on the perception of time. Conscious Cogn. 2015;38:155–164. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2015.02.021. * An interesting example of how emotion-driven temporal distortions can be modulated, in this case, by giving participants correct or incorrect information about the anticipated effects of emotional stimuli on their perception of time. This study is a good example of the flexibility of temporal distortions.
  • 27.Laflamme V, Grondin S, Bienvenue P, Labonté K, Roy M-L. Sex effect in the temporal perception of faces expressing anger and shame. Int J Comp Psychol. 2015;28:1–11. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Nicol JR, Tanner J, Clarke K. Perceived duration of emotional events: evidence for a positivity effect in older adults. Exp Aging Res. 2013;39:565–578. doi: 10.1080/0361073X.2013.839307. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Buetti S, Lleras A. Perceiving control over aversive and fearful events can alter how we experience those events: an investigation of time perception in spider-fearful individuals. Front Psychol. 2012;3:337. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00337. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Mereu S, Lleras A. Feelings of control restore distorted time perception of emotionally charged events. Conscious Cogn. 2013;22:306–314. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2012.08.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Tipples J. When time stands still: fear-specific modulation of temporal bias due to threat. Emotion. 2011;11:74–80. doi: 10.1037/a0022015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Bar-Haim Y, Kerem A, Lamy D, Zakay D. When time slows down: the influence of threat on time perception in anxiety. Cogn Emot. 2010;24:255–263. [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Ryu V, Kook S, Lee SJ, Ha K, Cho H-S. Effects of emotional stimuli on time perception in manic and euthymic patients with bipolar disorder. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2015;56:39–45. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2014.07.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Mioni G, Meligrana L, Grondin S, Perini F, Bartolomei L, Stablum F. Effects of emotional facial expression on time perception in patients with Parkinson’s disease. J Intl Neuropsychol Soc. 2015;21:1–10. doi: 10.1017/S1355617715000612. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Jones CRG, Gaigg SB, Lambrechts A. Using time perception to explore sensitivity to emotional stimuli in autism spectrum disorder. Procedia Soc Behav Sci. 2014;126:102–103. [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Thönes S, Oberfeld D. Time perception in depression: a meta-analysis. J Affect Disord. 2015;175:359–372. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2014.12.057. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Åström E, Wiberg B, Sircova A, Wiberg M, Carelli MG. Insights into features of anxiety through multiple aspects of psychological time. Integr Psychol Ther. 2014;2:3. http://dx.doi.org/10.7243/2054-4723-2-3. [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Watts FN, Sharrock R. Fear and time estimation. Percept Motor Skill. 1984;59:597–598. doi: 10.2466/pms.1984.59.2.597. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Yoo J-Y, Lee J-H. The effects of valence and arousal on time perception in individuals with social anxiety. Front Psychol. 2015;6:1208. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01208. * This study demonstrated that individuals with high social anxiety demonstrate exaggerated emotion-driven temporal distortions for highly arousing, negative faces relative to highly arousing, positive pictures. This is the first study to show that an overestimation bias in time perception for highly anxious individuals is specific to highly arousing negative emotions, suggesting that the interaction between valence and arousal may be important for understanding this perceptual bias.
  • 40.Lake JI, Labar KS. Unpredictability and uncertainty in anxiety: a new direction for emotional timing research. Front Integr Neurosci. 2011;5:55. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2011.00055. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Klapproth F. Time and decision making in humans. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2008;8:509–524. doi: 10.3758/CABN.8.4.509. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Han RK, Takahashi T. Psychophysics of time perception and valuation in temporal discounting of gain and loss. Physica A. 2012;391:6568–6576. [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Zauberman G, Kim BK, Malkoc SA, Bettman JR. Discounting time and time discounting: subjective time perception and intertemporal preferences. J Marketing Res. 2009;46:543–556. [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Wittmann M, Paulus MP. Decision making, impulsivity and time perception. Trends Cogn Sci. 2008;12:7–12. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.10.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Takahashi T. Loss of self-control in intertemporal choice may be attributable to logarithmic time-perception. Med Hypotheses. 2005;65:691–693. doi: 10.1016/j.mehy.2005.04.040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Takahashi T, Oono H, Radford MHB. Psychophysics of time perception and intertemporal choice models. Physica A. 2008;387:2066–2074. [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Alessi SM, Petry NM. Pathological gambling severity is associated with impulsivity in a delay discounting procedure. Behav Process. 2003;64:345–354. doi: 10.1016/s0376-6357(03)00150-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Bickel WK, Odum AL, Madden GJ. Impulsivity and cigarette smoking: delay discounting in current, never, and ex-smokers. Psychopharmacology. 1999;146:447–454. doi: 10.1007/pl00005490. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Ostaszewski P. The relation between temperament and rate of temporal discounting. Eur J Pers. 1996;10:161–172. [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Barkley RA, Edwards G, Laneri M, Fletcher K, Metevia L. Executive functioning, temporal discounting, and sense of time in adolescents with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and oppositional defiant disorder (ODD) J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2001;29:541–556. doi: 10.1023/a:1012233310098. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Wittmann M, Leland DS, Churan J, Paulus MP. Impaired time perception and motor timing in stimulant-dependent subjects. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2007;90:183–192. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2007.03.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Berlin HA, Rolls ET. Time perception, impulsivity, emotionality, and personality in self-harming borderline personality disorder patients. J Pers Disord. 2004;18:358–378. doi: 10.1521/pedi.18.4.358.40349. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Baumann AA, Odum AL. Impulsivity, risk taking, and timing. Behav Processes. 2012;90:408–414. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2012.04.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54. Guan S, Cheng L, Fan Y, Li X. Myopic decisions under negative emotions correlate with altered time perception. Front Psychol. 2015;6:468. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00468. ** This study found that negative emotional priming enhances temporal discounting and this bias in intertemporal choice was correlated with the magnitude of overestimation of negative emotions, suggesting that emotion-driven distortions may play a functional role in affecting decision-making and behavior.
  • 55.Pomares FB, Creac'h C, Faillenot I, Convers P, Peyron R. How a clock can change your pain? The illusion of duration and pain perception. Pain. 2011;152:230–234. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2010.10.047. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Lissek S, Kaczkurkin AN, Rabin S, Geraci M, Pine DS, Grillon C. Generalized anxiety disorder is associated with overgeneralization of classically conditioned fear. Biol Psychiatry. 2014;75:909–915. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.07.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Lissek S, Rabin SJ, McDowell DJ, Dvir S, Bradford DE, Geraci M, Pine DS, Grillon C. Impaired discriminative fear-conditioning resulting from elevated fear responding to learned safety cues among individuals with panic disorder. Behav Res Ther. 2009;47:111–118. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2008.10.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Gazendam FJ, Kamphuis JH, Kindt M. Deficient safety learning characterizes high trait anxious individuals. Biol Psychol. 2013;92:342–352. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.11.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Matell MS, Meck WH. Neuropsychological mechanisms of interval timing behavior. BioEssays. 2000;22:94–103. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1521-1878(200001)22:1<94::AID-BIES14>3.0.CO;2-E. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Merchant H, Harrington DL, Meck WH. Neural basis of the perception and estimation of time. Ann Rev Neurosci. 2013;36:313–336. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-062012-170349. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Cheng R-K, Tipples J, Narayanan NS, Meck WH. Clock speed as a window into dopaminergic control of emotion and time perception. Timing Time Percept. in press. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES