Summary
Mesolimbic dopamine encodes the benefits of a course of action. However, the value of an appetitive reward depends strongly on an animal’s current state. To investigate the relationship between dopamine, value, and physiological state, we monitored sub-second dopamine release in the nucleus accumbens core while rats made choices between food and sucrose solution following selective satiation on one of these reinforcers. Dopamine signals reflected preference for the reinforcers in the new state, decreasing to the devalued reward and, after satiation on food, increasing for the valued sucrose solution. These changes were rapid and selective, with dopamine release returning to pre-satiation patterns when the animals were re-tested in a standard food-restricted state. Such rapid and selective adaptation of dopamine-associated value signals could provide an important signal to promote efficient foraging for a varied diet.
Graphical Abstract
Highlights
-
•
Dopamine reward prediction errors are shaped by physiological state
-
•
Both choices and dopamine signals rapidly update after selective satiation
-
•
In a new state, dopamine signals mainly only update with experience
-
•
When returning to a familiar state, dopamine immediately signals stored values
Dopamine signals information about reward value used for learning and decision making. Papageorgiou et al. show that mesolimbic dopamine coding of reward prediction errors rapidly updates to reflect current state-dependent values.
Introduction
The phasic activity of midbrain dopamine neurons and dopamine release in regions such as the nucleus accumbens (NAc) signal predictions of future reward and discrepancies between such predictions and received reward (Gan et al., 2010, Montague et al., 1996, Schultz et al., 1997, Syed et al., 2016). These signals appear encoded on a common value scale, integrated across different reward attributes, that reflects individuals’ subjective preference for particular outcomes rather than the objective properties of reward (Lak et al., 2014). However, such preferences are not fixed but, instead, depend on an organism’s current nutritional needs, particularly in comparison with recent consumption. Several studies have shown that dopamine levels in the presence of reward are influenced by current physiological state, as well as the nutritional content of reinforcers (Ahn and Phillips, 1999, Bassareo and Di Chiara, 1999, Beeler et al., 2012, de Araujo et al., 2013, McCutcheon et al., 2012). Nonetheless, to date, the relationship between phasic dopamine, reward prediction errors, nutritional needs, and reward-guided choice remains poorly understood. Here, we investigated this issue by recording dopamine release while rats made choices between food and sucrose solution either in a baseline food-restricted state or after selective satiation on one of the two reinforcers (Rolls et al., 1983). Thus, by monitoring how patterns of dopamine release updated between the sessions, we could investigate how dopamine prediction errors are influenced by selective changes in subjective value and how value predictions and behavioral preferences updated with experience of the reinforcers in a new state.
Results
Behavioral Performance before and after Selective Satiation
Food-restricted rats were trained to perform a two-option operant decision-making task where the selection of each option was associated with a particular type of reward (food pellet or sucrose solution) (Figure 1A). Sessions consisted of trials where only one reward type was available (“forced” trials) and others where rats could choose between the two (“choice” trials). After acquiring the task, the rats (n = 8) performed four sessions: two baseline sessions (A and B), each of which preceded a devaluation session (the devalue food session and the devalue sucrose solution session, order counterbalanced across animals) that was identical to the baseline sessions, except that the rats had free access to one of the rewards for an hour before the test session.
In the first pre-devaluation baseline session (baseline A), the group of rats overall displayed no overall preference in general for either reward type on choice trials (t test against 50% for food choices: t(7) = 0.168, p = 0.87), no difference in response latencies to the two options on forced trials (t(6) = 0.94, p = 0.38; the data from one animal was lost because of a computer error), and no differences in the numbers of wrong-lever choices or missed trials (both <2% of trials, t(7) < 1.60, p > 0.15).
Prior to the devaluation sessions, the rats consumed, on average, either 11.5 g (SEM, ±1.24 g) of pellets or 24.75 ml (SEM, ±1.81 ml) of sucrose solution. This manipulation reliably altered the animals’ preference for the reward types (t test against 50% for food choices: devalue food, t(7) = −2.84, p = 0.01; devalue sucrose solution, t(7) = 4.13, p < 0.01) (Figure 1B). There was no difference in the magnitude of this change following satiation with either the food or the sucrose solution (t(7) = 0.29, p = 0.78). There was also a significant increase in the number of missed trials and wrong-lever choices on forced trials in the devaluation sessions compared to baseline sessions (main effect of devaluation, both Fs(1, 7) > 7.02, p < 0.034), an effect driven by a selective increase on the devalued option (interaction between reward type and devaluation session: wrong choices, F(1, 7) = 9.75, p = 0.02; missed trials, F(1, 7) = 5.53, p = 0.051).
These changes in subjective valuation were temporary and specific to the devaluation session. Preference returned to indifference in the baseline B session run in between the counterbalanced devaluation sessions (t(7) = 1.08, p = 0.32), and there was no change in choices from the pre-devaluation baseline session (t(7) = 1.18, p = 0.28).
Dopamine Release at Reward Delivery following Sensory-Specific Satiation Procedures
We monitored dopamine release in the NAc core (Figure S1) using fast-scan cyclic voltammetry while rats performed this reward identity decision paradigm. On 80% of trials, the choice of one lever resulted in the delivery of the standard amount of the expected reward type (“standard” trials). However, on the remaining subset of trials, the animals received either (1) an increased quantity of the expected reward type (value surprise “MORE” trials) or (2) the standard amount of the other reward type (identity surprise “SWITCH” trials) (Figures 1A, 1C, and 1D). Note that, until the reward is dispensed, surprise trials are otherwise identical to standard trials. While we recorded dopamine release in both baseline and selective satiety sessions, here we will mainly focus on patterns of dopamine release in the latter.
To examine the effect of selective satiety and, consequently, a selective change in the subjective value of one of the options, on value-related dopamine signals at the time of reward delivery, we ran a linear regression on the two devaluation sessions (Figures 2A and 2B). There was a strong influence of MORE trials on dopamine release, as well as a significant interaction between MORE trials and reward type. Importantly, the sign of the interaction term switched depending on whether food or sucrose solution was devalued (Figure 2C). This demonstrates reinforcer-specific satiety effects on value surprise trials. The same influence of selective satiety was also observed on SWITCH trials. In sucrose solution devaluation sessions, there was a transient increase in dopamine following the surprise delivery of a valued food pellet after a response on the sucrose solution lever (Figures 2A, 2D, 2F, and 2G). These signals were significantly more discriminable than during the baseline session (paired t test on the dopamine discrimination index: t(7) = 2.88, p = 0.028). The opposite pattern was observed in the food devaluation sessions: now, it was the surprising delivery of the valued sucrose solution that caused a selective increase in dopamine release, whereas there was no observable increase following a surprise pellet delivery (Figures 2B and 2E–2G). Therefore, surprise-evoked dopamine release can also be modulated by the current state-based value of the reinforcers, demonstrating that the pattern of dopamine is distinct from the physical properties of the reward.
It was also evident that dopamine release differed even on the standard trials for the valued and devalued options in a new state, even though the anticipated type of reward was always delivered (p < 0.05; Figures 2D and 2E). To investigate what might be influencing this, we directly contrasted dopamine time locked to reward delivery in the devaluation sessions against an equivalent period recorded in the baseline session (Figure 2H). Surprisingly, there was no consistent difference in the change in average dopamine levels after receipt of the devalued reward when compared to receiving that same reward in the baseline session (p > 0.05). Instead, there was a small but significant increase in dopamine when receiving the valued option for both reinforcers when compared to the same situation during baseline testing (Figure 2H; Figure S2). Moreover, when the session was divided into five blocks, this difference was found to be, on average, largest at the beginning of the session and diminished linearly as the session progressed (linear main effect of block: F(1, 7) = 5.88, p = 0.046) (Figure 2I). Therefore, receipt of the valued reward following selective satiety procedures appeared to produce a small positive prediction error that updated as the animals gained more experience of the reward in the new state.
Rapid Updating of Cue-Elicited Dopamine Release after Changes in State
The pattern of dopamine release at reward delivery suggests that value predictions are shaped by current motivational state in a reinforcer-specific manner. If so, state-based modulations of value predictions should also be observable at cue onset.
As can be observed in Figure 3, this is exactly what we found. While food cues elicited significantly greater dopamine release than sucrose solution cues in the sucrose solution devaluation session (Figure 3A), this reversed after food devaluation, with dopamine release for food cues now lower than after sucrose solution cues (Figure 3B). This was borne out by a linear regression that showed a significant effect of reward type on cue-evoked dopamine in both sessions, but with the sign modulated by the identity of the pre-fed reinforcer (Figures 3C and 3D). To further investigate this change in cue-elicited release, dopamine levels either on forced food or forced sucrose solution trials were analyzed across five equally sized blocks in the session. This showed that these effects occurred rapidly, being evident within the first block of the session (analysis of valued or devalued cue dopamine: F(1, 6) = 11.93, p = 0.014; no main effect or interaction with reward type: both Fs < 1.39, p > 0.28; n = 7, as one animal was excluded for having ≤5% responses on the devalued option) (Figure 3E).
Interestingly, although selective satiation uniformly decreased dopamine after devalued cues across the session, there was an asymmetric effect on valued cue-elicited dopamine (Figures 3F and 3G). Specifically, after food devaluation, dopamine levels were, on average, significantly greater after sucrose solution cues compared to baseline sessions (p < 0.05). By contrast, there was no statistically reliable change in either direction in response to valued food cues after sucrose solution devaluation. In other words, after eating to satiety, the predicted value of the sweet liquid option increased. Nonetheless, while the selective satiety procedures reliably biased choice behavior and modulated dopamine release, there was no observable consistent relationship between the size of cue-elicited signals in a particular session for a particular animal and its preference for one reinforcer over the other on choice trials (Figures 3C–3E and 3H).
Although these analyses show a rapid influence of selective satiety on dopamine release, it is not clear whether this is purely an experience-dependent effect based on learning the value of the options in the new state or whether dopamine cue signals can update even before the devalued reward is consumed during the session. To examine this, we analyzed dopamine release elicited by the first presentation in the session of both the valued and devalued options (Figure 4A). This revealed an overall attenuation of dopamine release on the initial trial of the devaluation sessions compared to the preceding baseline session (main effect of session type: F(1, 6) = 12.44, p = 0.012, including trial order as a between-subjects factors). However, this reduction was not significantly greater after first presentation of the cue associated with the currently devalued option than after that associated with the currently valued option (interactions including Session Type × Devaluation: all Fs < 0.27, p > 0.62). This implies that selective satiety induces an immediate general, rather than stimulus-specific, reduction in cue-elicited dopamine signals but that rats need experience of the reinforcer in the new state to fully update learned cue associations.
Nonetheless, inspection of Figure 4A suggests that dopamine levels were, on average, lower on the first devalued trial compared to cue presentations in valued states, particularly in the later period between lever extension and reward delivery. Therefore, we also directly compared average dopamine levels during the lever extension/response period, prior to reward delivery, on the first valued and devalued trials. This confirmed that dopamine levels were significantly attenuated after presentation of the devalued lever, compared to after the valued lever, even though the reinforcers had yet to be directly experienced in the new state (main effect of devaluation: F(1, 6) = 12.63, p = 0.012) (Figure 4B). This occurred in spite of the fact that there were no differences in lever press latency between the first valued or devalued trial (mean ± SEM: valued, 0.42 s ± 0.09 s; devalued, 0.60 s ± 0.20 s; t(7) = 1.05, p = 0.33).
Importantly, although selective satiation strongly modulated dopamine levels in the devaluation session, this did not have a lasting influence over patterns of dopamine release. The first presentation of the previously devalued option in baseline B immediately elicited comparable levels of dopamine as when that same cue had been presented during the first baseline A session (comparison between first trial dopamine in baseline A and baseline B, separated by which option was devalued during devaluation A: all Fs < 0.96, p > 0.36) (Figure 4C). Therefore, while dopamine signals update with experience of the reinforcer in the new state, they immediately revert to the original learned values once animals return to a baseline food-restricted state.
Discussion
These results demonstrate that mesolimbic dopamine flexibly encodes reward prediction error signals shaped by the specific properties of a reward to satisfy a current need. Midbrain dopamine neurons in primates tested for multiple days in a similar state have been shown to encode reward prediction errors that reflect the animals’ subjective preference for different reward types (Lak et al., 2014). Here, we observed a rapid, experience-driven updating of NAc core dopamine signals, both to predictive cues and reward delivery, to reflect the subjective value of stimuli following selective satiation.
Several of our results, therefore, appear consistent with key predictions of model-free temporal difference learning models. Dopamine release on SWITCH trials in the devaluation sessions principally encoded surprising changes in reward identity based on discrepancies between expected and received value rather than the sensory surprise of receiving the alternative reinforcer. This does not rule out that coding of reward identity prediction errors may exist in other contexts, where the value difference between the options is less prominent or when a change in identity is more relevant for behavior. For instance, the SWITCH trials here occurred as rare fluctuations in an otherwise stable task, but in other paradigms, such as unblocking or reversal learning, a change in reward identity can be more long lasting and of more significance for behavior (McDannald et al., 2014, Stalnaker et al., 2014). Equally, it is possible that distinct dopamine pathways might contain additional information about reward identity or other aspects of reward (Huetteroth et al., 2015). The current data were collected from the NAc core, as dopamine release in this structure has been shown to signal discrepancies from expectation (Day et al., 2007, Syed et al., 2016). However, in rodents, the NAc shell rather than the core—and, specifically, the D1-receptor-expressing medium spiny neurons in this region—has been associated with the ability of specific reinforcers to motivate and invigorate responding and promote feeding (Corbit and Balleine, 2011, Laurent et al., 2014, O’Connor et al., 2015).
We observed a strong modulation of both food- and sucrose-solution-elicited dopamine by the amount of reinforcer consumed within and prior to the session. Such selective modulation of stored value signals by specific satiety may be important to promote efficient and varied foraging behaviors. Cue-elicited dopamine release rapidly updated, with significant differences between the valued and devalued signals being evident within the first block of trials following selective satiation. However, this appeared to be predominantly shaped by direct incentive learning in the new state. While there was a general reduction in dopamine on the first trials of the devaluation sessions, compared to the preceding baseline sessions, this was not selective for the devalued option. This is in line with studies showing a general activating role for the NAc core, and dopamine transmission in this region, in the presence of reward-associated cues to motivate and invigorate available actions (Corbit and Balleine, 2011, du Hoffmann and Nicola, 2014).
However, it is notable that there were already selective differences in dopamine levels in the period after lever extension while the rat was making a response and waiting for either the valued or devalued reward. Therefore, some aspects of dopamine signaling can partially update without direct experience of the outcome (Bromberg-Martin et al., 2010). Moreover, cue-elicited dopamine returned to pre-devaluation patterns by the start of the subsequent baseline session run in a food-restricted state, in spite of the large difference between release elicited by the valued and devalued cues at the end of the devaluation session. This implies that mesolimbic dopamine systems have access to stored memories of learned incentive values when returning to a familiar state.
Together, our data add to the evidence indicating a close link between mesolimbic dopamine and physiological state (de Araujo et al., 2012, McCutcheon, 2015, Sclafani et al., 2011). Ventral tegmental area dopamine neurons receive excitatory inputs from the lateral hypothalamus (Watabe-Uchida et al., 2012) and dopamine cell activity, and NAc core dopamine release is influenced by physiological state (Branch et al., 2013) and by peptides involved in appetite (Cone et al., 2014). In the present experiment, decisions will be made based not only on the objective sensory qualities of a food pellet versus a bolus of sucrose solution but also on their subjective value in a given state. The rapid adaptation of mesolimbic dopamine signals following a change in state would potentially allow it to play an important role in prioritizing behaviors based on the available opportunities.
Experimental Procedures
Animals
17 male Sprague-Dawley rats were used for this experiment, of which 8 contributed data reported here (see the Supplemental Experimental Procedures). During the training and testing periods, access to food was restricted so that rats’ weights were kept between 85% and 90% of their free-feeding body weight. Water was continuously available in the home cages. All procedures were in compliance with the United Kingdom Animals Scientific Procedures Act of 1986 and the University of Oxford Policy on the Use of Animals in Scientific Research. All experiments were approved by the University of Oxford Animal Welfare and Ethical Review Board.
Behavioral Paradigm
We used fast-scan cyclic voltammetry to record dopamine release from chronically implanted carbon fiber electrodes in the NAc, as described previously (Clark et al., 2010, Syed et al., 2016), as animals performed a two-option/two-reward decision-making task. Sessions consisted of 120 trials, broken down into blocks of eight forced trials (four to each lever in a pseudorandom order) followed by four free-choice trials. One option was consistently associated with one reward type (45-mg food pellet), and the other was associated with a bolus of sucrose liquid (95 μl 20% sucrose solution), both delivered to the same food cup. On 80% of trials, animals received the reinforcer associated with the selected option. However, on 10% of the forced trials and 5% of the choice trials, the animals unexpectedly received the reward associated with the other lever (“SWITCH”). On another 5% of the forced trials, the animals received four times more reward than expected, although of the expected identity (“MORE”). These surprise trials occurred pseudorandomly throughout the session.
Data Analysis
As in previous studies, dopamine signals were extracted using principal-component analysis (Heien et al., 2004, Syed et al., 2016). To quantify which factors affected dopamine levels, regression coefficients were estimated for each animal at each time point around an event of interest. A linear model was used with a constant term, representing an ordinary least-squares fit of the given regressors to the data over trials (see the Supplemental Experimental Procedures). The discriminability of dopamine signals in pairs of different trial types was analyzed in each individual animal at each time point using the area under the receiver operating characteristic curve (auROC) (Syed et al., 2016). All data are reported as significant based on permutation tests when p < 0.05, corrected for multiple comparisons (i.e., p < 0.001). To calculate a dopamine discriminability index, we calculated the auROC using the average dopamine in the 5-s window after reward delivery for a particular trial type. In situations where there were insufficient numbers of trials to calculate an auROC (i.e., when examining changes in bins of trials across the session), we extracted the average dopamine levels instead, within a 3-s window after reward delivery, a 5-s window between cue onset and lever extension, or a 2-s window between lever extension and reward delivery, and performed a repeated-measures ANOVA.
Author Contributions
G.K.P. and M.E.W. conceived the study; G.K.P. and M.B. performed surgeries; G.K.P. collected the data with the assistance of F.C.; M.E.W. and G.K.P. analyzed the data; and M.E.W. and G.K.P. wrote the manuscript.
Acknowledgments
This work was funded by a Wellcome Trust Research Career Development Fellowship (WT090051MA to M.E.W.). G.K.P. received support from the A.G. Leventis Foundation. We would like to thank Emilie Syed for advice about data analysis, Greg Daubney for histology, and Matthew Rushworth for initial discussions about paradigm design.
Published: March 31, 2016
Footnotes
Supplemental Information includes Supplemental Experimental Procedures and two figures and can be found with this article online at http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.celrep.2016.03.031.
Contributor Information
Georgios K. Papageorgiou, Email: georgios.papageorgiou@psy.ox.ac.uk.
Mark E. Walton, Email: mark.walton@psy.ox.ac.uk.
Supplemental Information
References
- Ahn S., Phillips A.G. Dopaminergic correlates of sensory-specific satiety in the medial prefrontal cortex and nucleus accumbens of the rat. J. Neurosci. 1999;19:RC29. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-19-j0003.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bassareo V., Di Chiara G. Modulation of feeding-induced activation of mesolimbic dopamine transmission by appetitive stimuli and its relation to motivational state. Eur. J. Neurosci. 1999;11:4389–4397. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.1999.00843.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beeler J.A., McCutcheon J.E., Cao Z.F., Murakami M., Alexander E., Roitman M.F., Zhuang X. Taste uncoupled from nutrition fails to sustain the reinforcing properties of food. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2012;36:2533–2546. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2012.08167.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Branch S.Y., Goertz R.B., Sharpe A.L., Pierce J., Roy S., Ko D., Paladini C.A., Beckstead M.J. Food restriction increases glutamate receptor-mediated burst firing of dopamine neurons. J. Neurosci. 2013;33:13861–13872. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5099-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bromberg-Martin E.S., Matsumoto M., Hong S., Hikosaka O. A pallidus-habenula-dopamine pathway signals inferred stimulus values. J. Neurophysiol. 2010;104:1068–1076. doi: 10.1152/jn.00158.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clark J.J., Sandberg S.G., Wanat M.J., Gan J.O., Horne E.A., Hart A.S., Akers C.A., Parker J.G., Willuhn I., Martinez V. Chronic microsensors for longitudinal, subsecond dopamine detection in behaving animals. Nat. Methods. 2010;7:126–129. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.1412. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cone J.J., McCutcheon J.E., Roitman M.F. Ghrelin acts as an interface between physiological state and phasic dopamine signaling. J. Neurosci. 2014;34:4905–4913. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4404-13.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Corbit L.H., Balleine B.W. The general and outcome-specific forms of Pavlovian-instrumental transfer are differentially mediated by the nucleus accumbens core and shell. J. Neurosci. 2011;31:11786–11794. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2711-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Day J.J., Roitman M.F., Wightman R.M., Carelli R.M. Associative learning mediates dynamic shifts in dopamine signaling in the nucleus accumbens. Nat. Neurosci. 2007;10:1020–1028. doi: 10.1038/nn1923. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Araujo I.E., Ferreira J.G., Tellez L.A., Ren X., Yeckel C.W. The gut-brain dopamine axis: a regulatory system for caloric intake. Physiol. Behav. 2012;106:394–399. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2012.02.026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Araujo I.E., Lin T., Veldhuizen M.G., Small D.M. Metabolic regulation of brain response to food cues. Curr. Biol. 2013;23:878–883. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.04.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- du Hoffmann J., Nicola S.M. Dopamine invigorates reward seeking by promoting cue-evoked excitation in the nucleus accumbens. J. Neurosci. 2014;34:14349–14364. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3492-14.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gan J.O., Walton M.E., Phillips P.E. Dissociable cost and benefit encoding of future rewards by mesolimbic dopamine. Nat. Neurosci. 2010;13:25–27. doi: 10.1038/nn.2460. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heien M.L., Johnson M.A., Wightman R.M. Resolving neurotransmitters detected by fast-scan cyclic voltammetry. Anal. Chem. 2004;76:5697–5704. doi: 10.1021/ac0491509. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huetteroth W., Perisse E., Lin S., Klappenbach M., Burke C., Waddell S. Sweet taste and nutrient value subdivide rewarding dopaminergic neurons in Drosophila. Curr. Biol. 2015;25:751–758. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.01.036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lak A., Stauffer W.R., Schultz W. Dopamine prediction error responses integrate subjective value from different reward dimensions. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2014;111:2343–2348. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1321596111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Laurent V., Bertran-Gonzalez J., Chieng B.C., Balleine B.W. δ-opioid and dopaminergic processes in accumbens shell modulate the cholinergic control of predictive learning and choice. J. Neurosci. 2014;34:1358–1369. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4592-13.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McCutcheon J.E. The role of dopamine in the pursuit of nutritional value. Physiol. Behav. 2015;152(Pt B):408–415. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2015.05.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McCutcheon J.E., Beeler J.A., Roitman M.F. Sucrose-predictive cues evoke greater phasic dopamine release than saccharin-predictive cues. Synapse. 2012;66:346–351. doi: 10.1002/syn.21519. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McDannald M.A., Esber G.R., Wegener M.A., Wied H.M., Liu T.L., Stalnaker T.A., Jones J.L., Trageser J., Schoenbaum G. Orbitofrontal neurons acquire responses to ‘valueless’ Pavlovian cues during unblocking. eLife. 2014;3:e02653. doi: 10.7554/eLife.02653. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Montague P.R., Dayan P., Sejnowski T.J. A framework for mesencephalic dopamine systems based on predictive Hebbian learning. J. Neurosci. 1996;16:1936–1947. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-05-01936.1996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- O’Connor E.C., Kremer Y., Lefort S., Harada M., Pascoli V., Rohner C., Lüscher C. Accumbal D1R Neurons Projecting to Lateral Hypothalamus Authorize Feeding. Neuron. 2015;88:553–564. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rolls E.T., Rolls B.J., Rowe E.A. Sensory-specific and motivation-specific satiety for the sight and taste of food and water in man. Physiol. Behav. 1983;30:185–192. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(83)90003-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schultz W., Dayan P., Montague P.R. A neural substrate of prediction and reward. Science. 1997;275:1593–1599. doi: 10.1126/science.275.5306.1593. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sclafani A., Touzani K., Bodnar R.J. Dopamine and learned food preferences. Physiol. Behav. 2011;104:64–68. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.04.039. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stalnaker T.A., Cooch N.K., McDannald M.A., Liu T.L., Wied H., Schoenbaum G. Orbitofrontal neurons infer the value and identity of predicted outcomes. Nat. Commun. 2014;5:3926. doi: 10.1038/ncomms4926. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Syed E.C., Grima L.L., Magill P.J., Bogacz R., Brown P., Walton M.E. Action initiation shapes mesolimbic dopamine encoding of future rewards. Nat. Neurosci. 2016;19:34–36. doi: 10.1038/nn.4187. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Watabe-Uchida M., Zhu L., Ogawa S.K., Vamanrao A., Uchida N. Whole-brain mapping of direct inputs to midbrain dopamine neurons. Neuron. 2012;74:858–873. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.03.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.