Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2016 May 1.
Published in final edited form as: J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2016 Feb 5;45(3):361–382. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2015.1110823

Future Directions in Childhood Adversity and Youth Psychopathology

Katie A McLaughlin 1
PMCID: PMC4837019  NIHMSID: NIHMS759215  PMID: 26849071

Abstract

Despite long-standing interest in the influence of adverse early experiences on mental health, systematic scientific inquiry into childhood adversity and developmental outcomes has emerged only recently. Existing research has amply demonstrated that exposure to childhood adversity is associated with elevated risk for multiple forms of youth psychopathology. In contrast, knowledge of developmental mechanisms linking childhood adversity to the onset of psychopathology—and whether those mechanisms are general or specific to particular kinds of adversity—remains cursory. Greater understanding of these pathways and identification of protective factors that buffer children from developmental disruptions following exposure to adversity is essential to guide the development of interventions to prevent the onset of psychopathology following adverse childhood experiences. This article provides recommendations for future research in this area. In particular, use of a consistent definition of childhood adversity, integration of studies of typical development with those focused on childhood adversity, and identification of distinct dimensions of environmental experience that differentially influence development are required to uncover mechanisms that explain how childhood adversity is associated with numerous psychopathology outcomes (i.e., multifinality) and identify moderators that shape divergent trajectories following adverse childhood experiences. A transdiagnostic model that highlights disruptions in emotional processing and poor executive functioning as key mechanisms linking childhood adversity with multiple forms of psychopathology is presented as a starting point in this endeavour. Distinguishing between general and specific mechanisms linking childhood adversity with psychopathology is needed to generate empirically informed interventions to prevent the long-term consequences of adverse early environments on children’s development.


The lasting influence of early experience on mental health across the lifespan has been emphasized in theories of the etiology of psychopathology since the earliest formulations of mental illness. In particular, the roots of mental disorder have often been argued to be a consequence of adverse environmental experiences occurring in childhood. Despite this long-standing interest, systematic scientific inquiry into the effects of childhood adversity on health and development has emerged only recently. Prior work on childhood adversity focused largely on individual types of adverse experiences—such as death of a parent, divorce, sexual abuse, or poverty—and research on these topics evolved as relatively independent lines of inquiry. The transition to considering these types of adversities as indicators of the same underlying construct was prompted, in part, by the findings of a seminal study examining childhood adversity as a determinant of adult physical and mental health and advances in theoretical conceptualizations of stress. Specifically, findings from the Adverse Childhood Experiences (ACE) Study documented high levels of co-occurrence of multiple forms of childhood adversity and strong associations of exposure to adverse childhood experiences with a wide range of adult health outcomes (Dong et al., 2004; Edwards, Holden, Felitti, & Anda, 2003; Felitti et al., 1998). Around the same time, the concept of allostatic load was introduced as a comprehensive neurobiological model of the effects of stress (McEwen, 1998, 2000). Allostatic load provided a framework for explaining the neurobiological mechanisms linking a variety of adverse social experiences to health. Together, these discoveries sparked renewed interest in the childhood determinants of physical and mental health. Since that time there has been a veritable explosion of research into the impact of childhood adversity on developmental outcomes, including psychopathology.

CHILDHOOD ADVERSITY AND PSYCHOPATHOLOGY

Over the past two decades, hundreds of studies have examined the associations between exposure to childhood adversity and risk for psychopathology (Evans, Li, & Whipple, 2013). Here, I briefly review this evidence, focusing specifically on findings from epidemiological studies designed to allow inferences to be drawn at the population level. These studies have documented five general patterns with regard to childhood adversity and the distribution of mental disorders in the population. First, despite differences across studies in the prevalence of specific types of adversity, all population-based studies indicate that exposure to childhood adversity is common. The prevalence of exposure to childhood adversity is estimated at about 50% in the U.S. population across numerous epidemiological surveys (Green et al., 2010; Kessler, Davis, & Kendler, 1997; McLaughlin, Conron, Koenen, & Gilman, 2010; McLaughlin, Green et al., 2012). Remarkably similar prevalence estimates have been documented in other high-income countries, as well as in low- and middle-income countries worldwide (Kessler et al., 2010). Second, individuals who have experienced childhood adversity are at elevated risk for developing a lifetime mental disorder compared to individuals without such exposure, and the odds of developing a lifetime mental disorder increase as exposure to adversity increases (Edwards et al., 2003; Green et al., 2010; Kessler et al., 1997; Kessler et al., 2010; McLaughlin, Conron, et al., 2010; McLaughlin, Green, et al., 2012). Third, exposure to childhood adversity confers vulnerability to psychopathology that persists across the life-course. Childhood adversity exposure is associated not only with risk of mental disorder onset in childhood and adolescence (McLaughlin, Green, et al., 2012) but also with elevated odds of developing a first onset mental disorder in adulthood, which persists after adjustment for mental disorders beginning at earlier stages of development (Green et al., 2010; Kessler et al., 1997; Kessler et al., 2010). Fourth, the associations of childhood adversity with different types of commonly occurring mental disorders are largely nonspecific. Individuals who have experienced childhood adversity experience greater odds of developing mood, anxiety, substance use, and disruptive behavior disorders, with little meaningful variation in the strength of associations across disorder classes (Green et al., 2010; Kessler et al., 1997; Kessler et al., 2010; McLaughlin, Green, et al., 2012). Recent epidemiological findings suggest that the associations of child maltreatment —a commonly measured form of adversity—with lifetime mental disorders operate entirely through a latent liability to experience internalizing and externalizing psychopathology with no direct effects on specific mental disorders that are not explained by this latent vulnerability (Caspi et al., 2014; Keyes et al., 2012). Finally, exposure to childhood adversity explains a substantial proportion of mental disorder onsets in the population, both in the United States and cross-nationally (Afifi et al., 2008; Green et al., 2010; Kessler et al., 2010; McLaughlin, Green, et al., 2012). This reflects both the high prevalence of exposure to childhood adversity and the strong association of childhood adversity with the onset of psychopathology.

Together, findings from epidemiological studies indicate clearly that exposure to childhood adversity powerfully shapes risk for psychopathology in the population. As such, it is time for the field to move beyond these types of basic descriptive studies to research designs aimed at identifying the underlying developmental mechanisms linking childhood adversity to psychopathology. Although ample research has been conducted examining mechanisms linking individual types of adversity to psychopathology (e.g., sexual abuse; Trickett, Noll, & Putnam, 2011), far less is known about which of these mechanisms are common across different types of adversity versus specific to particular types of experiences. Greater understanding of these pathways, as well as the identification of protective factors that buffer children from disruptions in emotional, cognitive, social, and neurobiological development following exposure to adversity, is essential to guide the development of interventions to prevent the onset of psychopathology in children exposed to adversity, a critical next step for the field. However, persistent issues regarding the definition and measurement of childhood adversity must be addressed before meaningful progress on mechanisms, protective factors, and prevention of psychopathology following childhood adversity will be possible.

FUTURE DIRECTIONS IN CHILDHOOD ADVERSITY AND YOUTH PSYCHOPATHOLOGY

This article has two primary goals. The first is to provide recommendations for future research on childhood adversity and youth psychopathology. These recommendations relate to the definition and measurement of childhood adversity, the integration of studies of typical development with those on childhood adversity, and the importance of distinguishing between general and specific mechanisms linking childhood adversity to psychopathology. The second goal is to provide a transdiagnostic model of mechanisms linking childhood adversity and youth psychopathology that incorporates each of these recommendations.

Defining Childhood Adversity

Childhood adversity is a construct in search of a definition. Despite the burgeoning interest and research attention devoted to childhood adversity, there is a surprising lack of consistency with regard to the definition and measurement of the construct. Key issues remain unaddressed in the literature regarding the definition of childhood adversity and the boundary conditions of the construct. To what does the construct of childhood adversity refer? What types of experiences qualify as childhood adversity and what types do not? Where do we draw the line between normative experiences of stress and those that qualify as an adverse childhood experience? How does the construct of childhood adversity differ from other constructs that have been linked to psychopathology risk, including stress, toxic stress, and trauma? It will be critical to gain clarity on these definitional issues before more complex questions regarding mechanisms and protective factors can be systematically examined. Even in the seminal ACE Study that spurred much of the recent research into childhood adversity, a concrete definition of adverse childhood experience is not provided. The original article from the study argues for the importance of understanding the lasting health effects of child abuse and “household dysfunction,” the latter of which is never defined specifically (Felitti et al., 1998). The CDC website for the ACE Study (http://www.cdc.gov/violenceprevention/acestudy/findings.html) indicates that the ACE score, a count of the total number of adversities experienced, is designed to assess “the total amount of stress experienced during childhood.”

Why has a concrete definition of childhood adversity remained elusive? As I see it, there is a relatively simple explanation for this notable gap in the literature. Childhood adversity is difficult to define but fairly obvious to most observers, making the construct an example of the classic standard of you know it when you see it. Although this has allowed a significant scientific knowledge base on childhood adversity to emerge within a relatively short period, the lack of an agreed-upon definition of the construct represents a significant impediment to future progress in the field.

How can we begin to build scientific consensus on the definition of childhood adversity? Critically, we must come to an agreement about what childhood adversity is and what it is not. Adversity is defined as “a state or instance of serious or continued difficulty or misfortune; a difficult situation or condition; misfortune or tragedy” (“Adversity,” 2015). This provides a reasonable starting point. Adversity is an environmental event that must be serious (i.e., severe) or a series of events that continues over time (i.e., chronic). Building on Scott Monroe’s (2008) definition of life stress and models of experience-expectant brain development (Baumrind, 1993; Fox, Levitt, & Nelson, 2010), I propose that childhood adversity should be defined as experiences that are likely to require significant adaptation by an average child and that represent a deviation from the expectable environment. The expectable environment refers to a wide range of species-typical environmental inputs that the human brain requires to develop normally. These include sensory inputs (e.g., variation in patterned light information that is required for normal development of the visual system), exposure to language, and the presence of a sensitive and responsive caregiver (Fox et al., 2010). As I have argued elsewhere (McLaughlin, Sheridan, & Lambert, 2014; Sheridan & McLaughlin, 2014), deviations from the expectable environment often take two primary forms: an absence of expected inputs (e. g., limited exposure to language or the absence of a primary caregiver), or the presence of unexpected inputs that represent significant threats to the physical integrity or well-being of the child (e.g., exposure to violence). A similar approach to classifying key forms of child adversity has been articulated by others as well (Farah et al., 2008; Humphreys & Zeanah, 2015). These experiences can either be chronic (e.g., prolonged neglect) or involve single events that are severe enough to represent a deviation from the expectable environment (e.g., sexual abuse). Together, this provides a working definition of childhood adversity: exposure during childhood or adolescence to environmental circumstances that are likely to require significant psychological, social, or neurobiological adaptation by an average child and that represent a deviation from the expectable environment.

This definition provides some clarity about what childhood adversity is not. The clearest boundary condition involves the developmental timing of exposure; experiences classified as childhood adversity must occur prior to adulthood, either during childhood or adolescence. Most research on childhood adversity has taken a broad definition of childhood, including events occurring during either childhood or adolescence. Although the demarcation between adolescence and adulthood is itself a point of debate, relative consensus exists regarding the onset of adult roles as the end of adolescence (Steinberg, 2014). Second, childhood adversity refers to an event or ongoing events in the environment. Childhood adversity thus refers only to specific environmental circumstances or events and not to an individual child’s response to those circumstances. Third, childhood adversity refers to environmental conditions that are likely to require significant psychological, social, or neurobiological adaptation by an average child; therefore, events that represent transient or minor hassles should not qualify. What types of events should be considered severe enough to warrant classification as adversity? Although there is no absolute rule or formula that can be used to distinguish circumstances or events requiring significant adaptation from those that are less severe or impactful, childhood adversity should include conditions or events that are likely to have a meaningful and lasting impact on developmental processes for most children who experience them. In other words, experiences that could alter fundamental aspects of development in emotional, cognitive, social, or neurobiological domains are the types of experiences that should qualify as adversity. Studies of childhood adversity should clearly define the study-specific decision rules used to distinguish between adversity and more normative stressors. Finally, environmental circumstances or stressors that do not represent deviations from the expectable environment should not be classified as childhood adversity. In other words, childhood adversity should not include any and all stressors that occur during childhood or adolescence. Two examples of childhood stressors that would likely not qualify as childhood adversity based on this definition, because they do not meet the condition of representing a deviation from the expectable environment, are moving to a new school and death of an elderly grandparent. Each of these childhood stressors should require adaptation by an average child, and could influence mental health and development. However, neither represents a deviation from the expectable childhood environment and therefore does not meet the proposed definition of childhood adversity.

A key question for the field is whether the definition of childhood adversity should be narrow or broad. This question will determine whether other common forms of adversity or stress should be considered as indicators of childhood adversity. For example, many population-based studies have included parental psychopathology and divorce as forms of adversity (Felitti et al., 1998; Green et al., 2010). Given the high prevalence of psychopathology and divorce in the population, consideration of any form of parental psychopathology or any type of divorce as a form of adversity results in a fairly broad definition of adversity; certainly, not all cases of parental psychopathology or all divorces result in significant adversity for children. A more useful approach might be to consider only those cases of parental psychopathology or divorce that result in parenting behavior that deviates from the expectable environment (i. e., consistent unavailability, unresponsiveness, or insensitive care) or that generate other types of significant adversity for children (e.g., economic adversity, emotional abuse, etc.) as meeting the threshold for childhood adversity. Providing these types of boundary conditions is important to prevent the construct of childhood adversity from meaning everything and nothing at the same time.

Finally, how does childhood adversity differ from related constructs, including stress, toxic stress, and trauma that can also occur during childhood? What is unique about the construct of childhood adversity that is not captured in definitions of these similar constructs? First, how is childhood adversity different from stress? The prevailing conceptualization of life stress defines the construct as the adaptation of an organism to specific circumstances that change over time (Monroe, 2008). This definition includes three primary components that interact with one another: environment (the circumstance or event that requires adaptation by the organism), organism (the response to the environmental stimulus), and time (the interactions between the organism and the environment over time; Monroe, 2008). In contrast, childhood adversity refers only to the first of these three components—the environmental aspect of stress. Second, how is adversity different from toxic stress, a construct recently developed by Jack Shonkoff and colleagues (Shonkoff & Garner, 2012)? Toxic stress refers to the second component of stress just described—the response of the organism. Specifically, toxic stress refers to exaggerated, frequent, or prolonged activation of physiological stress response systems in response to an accumulation of multiple adversities over time in the absence of protection from a supportive caregiver (Shonkoff & Garner, 2012). The concept of toxic stress is conceptually similar to the construct of allostatic load as defined by McEwen (2000) and focuses on a different aspect of stress than childhood adversity. Finally, how is childhood adversity distinct from trauma? Trauma is defined as exposure to actual or threatened death, serious injury, or sexual violence, either by directly experiencing or witnessing such events or by learning of such events occurring to a close relative or friend (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Traumatic events occurring in childhood represent one potential form of childhood adversity, but not all types of childhood adversity are traumatic. Examples of adverse childhood experiences that would not be considered traumatic are neglect; poverty; and the absence of a stable, supportive caregiver.

The first concrete recommendation for future research is that the field must utilize a consistent definition of childhood adversity (see Table 1). A useful definition must have clarity about what childhood adversity is and what it is not, provide guidance about decision rules for applying the definition in specific contexts, and increase consistency in the measurement and application of childhood adversity across studies. The definition proposed here—that childhood adversity involves experiences that are likely to require significant adaptation by an average child and that represent a deviation from the expectable environment—represents a starting point in this endeavor, although consideration of alterative definitions and scholarly debate about the relative merits of different definitions is encouraged.

TABLE 1.

Recommendations for Future Research on Childhood Adversity and Youth Psychopathology

Develop and utilize a consistent definition of childhood adversity
Integrate studies of typical development with those focused on understanding the impact of childhood adversity; in particular, research that can shed light on sensitive periods in emotional, social, cognitive, and neurobiological development is needed
Identify developmental mechanisms linking adverse environmental experiences in childhood to the onset of multiple forms of psychopathology (i.e., mechanisms that explain multifinality)
Identify distinct dimensions of environmental experience that might differentially influence developmental mechanisms
Measure multiple dimensions of environmental experience in studies of childhood adversity to distinguish between common and specific underlying mechanisms linking different forms of adversity to psychopathology
Identify protective factors that buffer children from the negative consequences of adversity at two levels: (a) factors that modify the association between childhood adversity and the maladaptive patterns of emotional, cognitive, social, and neurobiological development that serve as intermediate phenotypes (i.e., mechanisms) linking adversity with psychopathology, and (b) factors that moderate the influence of intermediate phenotypes on the emergence of psychopathology, leading to divergent trajectories of adaptation across children

Integrating Studies of Typical and Atypical Development

A developmental psychopathology perspective emphasizes the reciprocal and integrated nature of our understanding of normal and abnormal development (Cicchetti, 1996; Cicchetti & Lynch, 1993; Lynch & Cicchetti, 1998). Normal developmental patterns must be characterized to identify developmental deviations, and abnormal developmental outcomes shed light on the normal developmental processes that lead to maladaptation when disrupted (Cicchetti, 1993; Sroufe, 1990). Maladaptive outcomes—including psychopathology—are considered to be the product of developmental processes (Sroufe, 1997, 2009). This implies that in order to uncover mechanisms linking childhood adversity to psychopathology, the developmental trajectory of the candidate emotional, cognitive, social, or neurobiological process under typical circumstances must first be characterized before examining how exposure to an adverse environment alters that trajectory. This approach has been utilized less frequently than would be expected in the literature on childhood adversity.

Recent work from Nim Tottenham’s lab on functional connectivity between the amygdala and medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) highlights the utility of this strategy. In an initial study, Gee, Humphreys, et al. (2013) demonstrated age-related changes in amygdala–mPFC functional connectivity in a typically developing sample of children during a task involving passive viewing of fearful and neutral faces. Specifically, they observed a developmental shift from a pattern of positive amygdala–mPFC functional connectivity during early and middle childhood to a pattern of negative connectivity (i.e., higher mPFC activity, lower amygdala activity) beginning in the prepubertal period and continuing throughout adolescence (Gee, Humphreys, et al., 2013). Next, they examined how exposure to institutional rearing in infancy influenced these age-related changes, documenting a more mature pattern of negative functional connectivity among young children with a history of institutionalization (Gee, Gabard-Durnam, et al., 2013).

Utilizing this type of approach is important not only to advance knowledge of developmental mechanisms underlying childhood adversity–psychopathology associations but also to leverage research on adverse environmental experiences to inform our understanding of typical development. Specifically, as frequently argued by Cicchetti (Cicchetti & Toth, 2009), research on atypical or aberrant developmental processes can provide a window into typical development not available through other means. This is particularly relevant in studies of some forms of childhood adversity that involve an absence of expected inputs from the environment, such as institutional rearing and child neglect (McLaughlin, Sheridan, & Lambert, 2014; Sheridan & McLaughlin, 2014). Examining the developmental consequences associated with deprivation in a particular type of input from the environment (e.g., the presence of an attachment figure, exposure to complex language) can provide insights into the types of environmental inputs that are required for a system or set of competencies to develop normally.

Evidence on the developmental trajectories of children raised in institutional settings provides an illustrative example. Institutions for abandoned and orphaned children vary widely, but a common feature across them is the absence of an attachment figure who provides sensitive and responsive care for each child (Smyke et al., 2007; Tottenham, 2012; Zeanah et al., 2003). Developmental research on children raised in institutional settings has provided ample evidence about the importance of the attachment relationship in early development for shaping numerous aspects of development. Unsurprisingly, most children raised in institutions fail to develop a secure attachment relationship to a caregiver; this is particularly true if children remain in institutional care past the age of 2 years (Smyke, Zeanah, Fox, Nelson, & Guthrie, 2010; Zeanah, Smyke, Koga, Carlson, & The Bucharest Early Intervention Project Core Group, 2005). Children reared in institutional settings also exhibit social skills deficits, delays in language development, lasting disruptions in executive functioning skills, decrements in IQ, and atypical patterns of emotional processing (Almas et al., 2012; Bos, Fox, Zeanah, & Nelson, 2009; Nelson et al., 2007; Tibu et al., 2016; Tottenham et al., 2011; Windsor et al., 2011). Institutional rearing also has wide-ranging impacts on patterns of brain development, including neural structure and function (Gee et al., 2013; McLaughlin, Fox, Zeanah, & Nelson, 2011; McLaughlin, Sheridan, Winter, et al., 2014; Sheridan, Fox, Zeanah, McLaughlin, & Nelson, 2012; Tottenham et al., 2011). Although children raised in institutional settings often experience deprivation in environmental inputs of many kinds, it is likely that the absence of a primary attachment figure in early development explains many of the downstream consequences of institutionalization on developmental outcomes. Indeed, recent evidence suggests that disruptions in attachment may be a causal mechanism linking institutional rearing with the onset of anxiety and depression in children. Specifically, in a randomized controlled trial of foster care as an intervention for orphaned children in Romania, improvements in attachment security were a mechanism underlying the preventive effects of the intervention on the onset of anxiety and depression in children (McLaughlin, Zeanah, Fox, & Nelson, 2012). By examining the developmental consequences of the absence of an expected input from the environment—namely, the presence of a primary attachment figure—studies of institutional rearing provide strong evidence for the centrality of the early attachment relationship in shaping numerous aspects of development.

Sensitive Periods

The integration of studies on typical and atypical development may be particularly useful in the identification of sensitive periods. Developmental psychopathology emphasizes the cumulative and hierarchical nature of development (Gottlieb, 1991a, 1991b; Sroufe, 2009; Sroufe, Egeland, & Kreutzer, 1990; Werner & Kaplan, 1963). Learning and acquisition of competencies at one point in development provide the scaffolding upon which subsequent skills and competencies are built, such that capabilities from previous periods are consolidated and reorganized in a dynamic, unfolding process across time. The primary developmental tasks occurring at the time of exposure to a risk factor are thought to be the most likely to be interrupted or disrupted by the experience. Developmental deviations from earlier periods are then carried forward and have consequences for children’s ability to successfully accomplish developmental tasks in a later period (Cicchetti & Toth, 1998; Sroufe, 1997). In other words, early experiences constrain future learning of patterns or associations that represent departures from those that were previously learned (Kuhl, 2004).

This concept points to a critical area for future research on childhood adversity involving the identification of sensitive periods of emotional, cognitive, social, and neurobiological development when inputs from the environment are particularly influential. Sensitive periods have been identified both in sensory development and in the development of complex social–cognitive skills, including language (Hensch, 2005; Kuhl, 2004). Emerging evidence from cognitive neuroscience also suggests the presence of developmental periods when specific regions of the brain are most sensitive to the effects of stress and adversity (Andersen et al., 2008). However, identification of sensitive periods has remained elusive in other domains of emotional and social development, potentially reflecting the fact that sensitive periods exist for fewer processes in these domains. However, determining how anomalous or atypical environmental inputs influence developmental processes differently based on the timing of exposure provides a unique opportunity to identify sensitive periods in development; in this way, research on adverse environments can inform our understanding of typical development by highlighting the environmental inputs that are necessary to foster adaptive development.

Identifying sensitive periods of emotional and social development requires detailed information on the timing of exposure to atypical or adverse environments, which is challenging to measure. To date, studies of institutional rearing have provided the best opportunity for studying sensitive periods in human emotional and social development, as it is straightforward to determine the precise period during which the child lived in the institutional setting. Studies of institutional rearing have identified a sensitive period for the development of a secure attachment relationship at around 2 years of age; the majority of children placed into stable family care before that time ultimately develop secure attachments to a caregiver, whereas the majority of children placed after 2 years fail to develop secure attachments (Smyke et al., 2010). Of interest, a sensitive period occurring around 2 years of age has also been identified for other domains, including reactivity of the autonomic nervous system and hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis to the environment and a neural marker of affective style (i.e., frontal electroencephalogram asymmetry; McLaughlin et al., 2011; McLaughlin, Sheridan, et al., 2015), suggesting the importance of the early attachment relationship in shaping downstream aspects of emotional and neurobiological development.

The second concrete recommendation for future research is to integrate studies of typical development with those focused on understanding the impact of childhood adversity; in particular, research that can shed light on sensitive periods in emotional, social, cognitive, and neurobiological development is needed. Identifying the developmental processes that are disrupted by exposure to particular types of adverse environments will be facilitated by first characterizing the typical developmental trajectories of the processes in question. In turn, studies of atypical or adverse environments should be leveraged to inform our understanding of the types of environmental inputs that are required—and when—for particular systems to develop normally. Given the inherent problems in retrospective assessment of timing of exposure to particular environmental experiences, longitudinal studies with repeated measurements of environmental experience and acquisition of developmental competencies are likely to be most informative. Alternatively, the occurrence of exogenous events like natural disasters, terrorist attacks, and changes in policies or the availability of resources (e.g., the opening of the casino on a Native American reservation; Costello, Compton, Keeler, & Angold, 2003) provides additional opportunities to study sensitive periods of development. Identifying sensitive periods is likely to yield critical insights into the points in development when particular capabilities are most likely to be influenced by environmental experience, an issue of central importance for understanding both typical and atypical development. Such information can be leveraged to inform decisions about the points in time when psychosocial interventions for children exposed to adversity are likely to be maximally efficacious.

Explaining Multifinality

The principle of multifinality is central to developmental psychopathology (Cicchetti, 1993). Multifinality refers to the process by which the same risk and/or protective factors may ultimately lead to different developmental outcomes (Cicchetti & Rogosch, 1996). It is has been repeatedly demonstrated that most forms of childhood adversity are associated with elevated risk for the onset of virtually all commonly occurring mental disorders (Green et al., 2010; McLaughlin, Green, et al., 2012). As noted earlier, recent evidence suggests that child maltreatment is associated with a latent liability for psychopathology that explains entirely the associations of maltreatment with specific mental disorders (Caspi et al., 2014; Keyes et al., 2012). However, the mechanisms that explain how child maltreatment, or other forms of adversity, influence a generalized liability to psychopathology have not been specified. To date, there have been few attempts to articulate a model explaining how childhood adversity leads to the diversity of mental disorders with which it is associated (i. e., multifinality). What are the mechanisms that explain this generalized vulnerability to psychopathology arising from adverse early experiences? Are these mechanisms shared across multiple forms of childhood adversity, or are they specific to particular types of adverse experience?

Identifying general versus specific mechanisms will require changes in the way we conceptualize and measure childhood adversity. Prior research has followed one of two strategies. The first involves studying individual types of childhood adversity, such as parental death, physical abuse, neglect, or poverty (Chase-Lansdale, Cherlin, & Kiernan, 1995; Dubowitz, Papas, Black, & Starr, 2002; Fristad, Jedel, Weller, & Weller, 1993; Mullen, Martin, Anderson, Romans, & Herbison, 1993; Noble, McCandliss, & Farah, 2007; Wolfe, Sas, & Wekerle, 1994). However, most individuals exposed to childhood adversity have experienced multiple adverse experiences (Dong et al., 2004; Finkelhor, Ormrod, & Turner, 2007; Green et al., 2010; McLaughlin, Green, et al., 2012). This presents challenges for studies focusing on a single type of adversity, as it is unclear if any observed associations represent the downstream effects of the focal adversity in question (e.g., poverty) or the consequences of other co-occurring experiences (e.g., exposure to violence) that might have different developmental consequences. Increasing recognition of the co-occurring nature of adverse childhood experiences has resulted in a shift from focusing on single types of adversity to examining the associations between number of adverse childhood experiences and developmental outcomes—the core strategy of the ACE approach (Arata, Langhinrichsen-Roling, Bowers, & O’Brien, 2007; Dube et al., 2003; Edwards et al., 2003; Evans et al., 2013). There has been a proliferation of research utilizing this approach in recent years, and it has proved useful in documenting the importance of childhood adversity as a risk factor for a wide range of negative mental health outcomes. However, this approach implicitly assumes that very different kinds of experiences ranging from violence exposure to material deprivation (e.g., food insecurity) to parental loss influence psychopathology through similar mechanisms. Although there is likely to be some overlap in the mechanisms linking different forms of adversity to psychopathology, the count approach oversimplifies the boundaries between distinct types of environmental experience that may have unique developmental consequences.

An alternative approach that is likely to meet with more success involves identifying dimensions of environmental experience that underlie multiple forms of adversity and are likely to influence development in similar ways. In recent work, my colleague Margaret Sheridan and I have proposed two such dimensions that cut across multiple forms of adversity: threat and deprivation (McLaughlin, Sheridan, & Lambert, 2014; Sheridan & McLaughlin, 2014). Threat involves exposure to events involving harm or threat of harm, consistent with the definition of trauma in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (5th ed.; American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Threat is a central dimension underlying multiple commonly studied forms of adversity, including physical abuse, sexual abuse, some forms of emotional abuse (i.e., that involve threats of physical violence and coercion), exposure to domestic violence, and other forms of violent victimization in home, school, or community settings. Deprivation, in contrast, involves the absence of expected cognitive and social inputs from the environmental stimuli, resulting in reduced opportunities for learning. Deprivation in expected environmental inputs is common to multiple forms of adversity including emotional and physical neglect, institutional rearing, and poverty. Critically, we do not propose that exposure to deprivation and threat occurs independently for children, as these experiences are highly co-occurring, or that these are the only important dimensions of experience involved in childhood adversity. Instead we propose, first, that these are two important dimensions that can be measured separately and, second, that the mechanisms linking these experiences to the onset of psychopathology are likely to be at least partially distinct (McLaughlin, Sheridan, & Lambert, 2014; Sheridan & McLaughlin, 2014). I describe some of these key mechanisms in the transdiagnostic model presented later. Recently, others have argued for the importance taking this type of dimensional approach as well (Hamby & Grych, 2013; Humphreys & Zeanah, 2015).

Specific recommendations are for future research to (a) identify key dimensions of environmental experience that might differentially influence developmental outcomes and (b) measure multiple such dimensions in studies of childhood adversity to distinguish between general and specific underlying mechanisms linking different forms of adversity to psychopathology. Fine-grained measurement of the dimensions of threat and deprivation has often not been conducted within the same study. Studies focusing on specific types of exposure (e.g., abuse) without measuring or adjusting for co-occurring exposures (e.g., neglect) are unable to distinguish between common and specific mechanisms linking different dimensions of adverse experiences to psychopathology. The only way to determine whether such specificity exists is to measure and model these dimensions of experience together in future studies.

Characterizing the Interplay of Risk and Protective Factors

Although psychopathology is common among children exposed to a wide range of adverse environments, many children exhibit adaptation and resilience following adversity (Masten, 2001; Masten, Best, & Garmezy, 1990). For example, studies of resilience suggest that children who have a positive relationship with a caring and competent adult; are good at learning, problem solving, and self-regulation; are socially engaging; and have positive self-image are more likely to exhibit positive adaptation after exposure to adversity than children without these characteristics (Luthar, Cicchetti, & Becker, 2000; Masten, 2001; Masten et al., 1990). However, in contrast to the consistent pattern of associations between childhood adversity and psychopathology, evidence for protective factors varies widely across studies, and in most cases children exposed to adversity exhibit adaptive functioning in some domains but not others; even within a single domain, children may be functioning well at one point in time but not at others (Luthar et al., 2000). This is not surprising given that the degree to which a particular factor is protective depends heavily upon context, including the specific risk factors with which it is interacting (Cicchetti & Lynch, 1993; Sameroff, Gutman, & Peck, 2003). For example, authoritative parenting has been shown to be associated with adaptive outcomes for children raised in stable contexts that are largely free of significant adversity (Steinberg, Elmen, & Mounts, 1989; Steinberg, Lamborn, Dornbusch, & Darling, 1992; Steinberg, Mounts, Lamborn, & Dornbusch, 1991); in contrast, authoritarian parenting appears to be protective for children being raised in environments characterized by low resources and/or high degrees of violence and other threats (Flouri, 2007; Gonzales, Cauce, Friedman, & Mason, 1996). The degree to which variation in specific genetic polymorphisms moderates the impact of childhood adversity on development outcomes is also highly variable across studies; although genetic variation clearly contributes to developmental trajectories of adaptation and maladaptation following childhood adversity, this topic has been reviewed extensively elsewhere (Heim & Binder, 2012; McCrory, De Brito, & Viding, 2010; Uher & McGuffin, 2010) and is not discussed further. This complexity has contributed to the widely variable findings regarding protective factors and resilience.

Progress in identifying protective factors that buffer children from maladaptive outcomes following childhood adversity might be achieved by shifting the focus from downstream outcomes to more proximal mechanisms known to underlie the relationship between adverse childhood experiences and psychopathology. Research on resiliency has often focused on distal outcomes, such as the absence of psychopathology, the presence of high-quality peer relationships, or good academic performance as markers of adaptive functioning in children with exposure to adversity (Bolger, Patterson, & Kupersmidt, 1999; Collishaw et al., 2007; Fergusson & Lynskey, 1996; Luthar, 1991). Just as there are numerous mechanisms through which exposure to adverse environments lead to psychopathology and other downstream outcomes, there are likely to be a wide range of mechanisms through which protective factors buffer children from maladaptation following childhood adversity. Indeed, modern conceptualizations of resilience describe it as a developmental process that unfolds over time as an ongoing transaction between a child and the multiple contexts in which he or she is embedded (Luthar et al., 2000). Rather than examining protective factors that buffer children from developing psychopathology following adverse childhood experiences, an alternative approach is to focus on factors that moderate the association of childhood adversity with the developmental processes that serve as mechanisms linking adversity with psychopathology (e.g., emotion regulation, executive functioning) or that moderate the link between these developmental processes and the onset of psychopathology. Deconstructing the pathways linking childhood adversity to psychopathology allows moderators to be examined separately at different stages of these pathways and may yield greater information about how protective factors ultimately exert their effects on downstream outcomes, including psychopathology.

Accordingly, a fourth recommendation is that future research should focus on identifying protective factors that buffer children from the negative consequences of adversity at two levels: (a) factors that modify the association between childhood adversity and the maladaptive patterns of emotional, cognitive, social, and neurobiological development that serve as intermediate phenotypes linking adversity with psychopathology; and (b) factors that moderate the influence of intermediate phenotypes on the emergence of psychopathology, leading to divergent trajectories of adaptation across children. To understand resilience, we first need to understand the developmental processes that are disrupted following exposure to adversity and how certain characteristics either prevent or compensate for those developmental disruptions or reduce their impact on risk for psychopathology.

A TRANSDIAGNOSTIC MODEL OF CHILDHOOD ADVERSITY AND PSYCHOPATHOLOGY

The remainder of the article outlines a transdiagnostic model of mechanisms linking childhood adversity with youth psychopathology. Two core developmental mechanisms are proposed that, in part, explain patterns of multifinality: emotional processing and executive functioning. The model builds on a framework described by Nolen-Hoeksema and Watkins (2011) for identifying transdiagnostic processes. Of importance, the model is not intended to be comprehensive in delineating all mechanisms linking childhood adversity with psychopathology but rather focuses on two candidate mechanisms linking childhood adversity to multiple forms of psychopathology. At the same time, these mechanisms are also specific in that each is most likely to emerge following exposure to specific dimensions of adverse early experience. The model is specific with regard to the underlying dimensions of adverse experience considered and identifies several key moderators that might explain divergent developmental trajectories among children following exposure to adversity. Future research is needed to expand this framework to incorporate other key dimensions of the adverse environmental experience, developmental mechanisms linking those dimensions of adversity with psychopathology, and moderators of those associations.

Distal Risk Factors

Within the proposed model, core dimensions of environmental experience that underlie multiple forms of adversity are conceptualized as distal risk factors for psychopathology. Specifically, experiences of threat and deprivation constitute the first component of the proposed transdiagnostic model of childhood adversity and psychopathology (see Figure 1). Experiences of threat and deprivation meet each of Nolen-Hoeksema and Watkins’s (2011) criteria for a distal risk factor. They represent environmental conditions largely outside the control of the child that are linked to the onset of psychopathology only through intervening causal mechanisms that represent more proximal risk factors. Although they are probabilistically related to psychopathology, exposure to threat and deprivation do not invariably lead to mental disorders. These experiences influence proximal risk factors primarily through learning mechanisms that ultimately shape patterns of information processing, emotional responses to the environment, and higher order control processes that influence both cognitive and emotional processing.

FIGURE 1.

FIGURE 1

A transdiagnostic model of childhood adversity and youth psychopathology. Note. Dashed lines represent pathways that are less well established and may represent indirect associations or associations that emerge only in interaction with other processes. For example, children exposed to threatening early environments may exhibit problems with working memory and inhibitory control only in emotionally arousing situations or when the stimuli to be remembered or to which a response must be inhibited are emotional in nature. Greater research is needed to understand these pathways and the conditions under which they operate.

Proximal Risk Factors

The developmental processes that are altered following exposure to adverse environmental experiences represent proximal risk factors, or intermediate phenotypes, linking them to the onset of psychopathology. These proximal risk factors represent the second component of the proposed transdiagnostic model. Nolen-Hoeksema and Watkins (2011) argued that proximal risk factors are within-person factors that mediate the relationship between distal risk factors—including aspects of environmental context that are difficult to modify, such as childhood adversity—and the emergence of psychopathology. Proximal risk factors directly influence symptoms and are temporally closer to symptom onset and often easier to modify than distal risk factors (Nolen-Hoeksema & Watkins, 2011). Identifying modifiable within-person factors that link adverse environmental experiences with the onset of symptoms is the key to developing interventions to prevent the onset of psychopathology in children who have experienced adversity.

The model includes two primary domains of proximal risk factors: emotional processing and executive functioning. Emotional processing refers to information processing of emotional stimuli (e.g., attention, memory), emotional reactivity, and both automatic (e.g., habituation, fear extinction) and effortful (e.g., cognitive reappraisal) forms of emotion regulation. These processes all represent responses to emotional stimuli, and many involve interactions of cognition with emotion. Executive functions comprise a set of cognitive processes that support the ability to learn new knowledge and skills; hold in mind goals and information; and create and execute complex, future-oriented plans. Executive functioning comprises the ability to hold information in mind and focus on currently relevant information (working memory), inhibit actions and information not currently relevant (inhibition), and switch flexibly between representations or goals (cognitive flexibility; Miyake & Friedman, 2012; Miyake, Friedman, Rettinger, Shah, & Hegarty, 2001). Together these skills allow the creation and execution of future oriented plans and the inhibition of behaviors that do not serve these plans, providing the foundation for healthy decision making and self-regulation. Many of the diverse mechanisms linking childhood adversity to psychopathology are subsumed within these two broad domains.

Emotional processing

Stable patterns of emotional processing, emotional responding to the environment, and emotion regulation represent the first core domain of proximal risk factors. Experiences of uncontrollable threat are associated with strong learning of specific contingencies and overgeneralization of that learning to novel contexts, which facilitates the processing of salient emotional cues in the environment (e.g., biased attention to threat). Given the importance of quickly identifying potential threats in the environment for children growing up in environments characterized by legitimate danger, these learning processes should produce information processing biases that promote rapid identification of potential threats. Indeed, evidence suggests that children with abuse histories—an environment characterized by high levels of threat—exhibit attention biases toward facial displays of anger, identify anger with little perceptual information, have difficulty disengaging from angry faces, and display anticipatory monitoring of the environment following interpersonal displays of anger (Pollak, Cicchetti, Hornung, & Reed, 2000; Pollak & Sinha, 2002; Pollak & Tolley-Schell, 2003; Pollak, Vardi, Putzer Bechner, & Curtin, 2005; Shackman, Shackman, & Pollak, 2007). Given the relevance of anger as a signal of potential threat, these findings suggest that exposure to threatening environments results in stable patterns of information processing that facilitate threat identification and maintenance of attention to threat cues. These attention biases are specific to children who have experienced violence; for example, children who have been neglected (i.e., an environment characterized by deprivation in social and cognitive inputs) experience difficulty discriminating facial expressions of emotion but do not exhibit attention biases toward threat (Pollak, Klorman, Thatcher, & Cicchetti, 2001; Pollak et al., 2005). In addition to attention biases, children who have been the victims of violence are also more likely to generate attributions of hostility to others in socially ambiguous situations (Dodge, Bates, & Pettit, 1990; Dodge, Pettit, Bates, & Valente, 1995; Weiss, Dodge, Bates, & Petit, 1992), a pattern of social information processing tuned to be overly sensitive to potential threats in the environment. Finally, some evidence suggests that exposure to threatening environments is associated with memory biases for overgeneral autobiographical memories in both children and adults (Crane et al., 2014; Williams et al., 2007).

Children with trauma histories also exhibit meaningful differences in patterns of emotional responding that are consistent with these patterns of information processing. For example, children who have experienced interpersonal violence exhibit greater activation in the amygdala and other nodes of the salience network (e.g., anterior insula, putamen, thalamus) to a wide range of negative emotional stimuli (McCrory et al., 2013; McCrory et al., 2011; McLaughlin, Peverill, Gold, Alves, & Sheridan, 2015), suggesting heightened salience of information that could predict threat. These findings build on earlier work using evoked response potentials documenting amplified neural response to angry faces in children who were physically abused (Pollak, Cicchetti, Klorman, & Brumaghim, 1997; Pollak et al., 2001) and suggests that exposure to threatening experiences heightens the salience of negative emotional information, due to the potential relevance for detecting novel threats. Heightened amygdala response to negative emotional cues could also reflect fear learning processes, whereby previously neutral stimuli that have become associated with traumatic events begin to elicit conditioned fear responses, or the result of deficits in automatic emotion regulation processes like fear extinction and habituation, which are mediated through connections between the ventromedial prefrontal cortex and amygdala. Recent findings of poor resting-state functional connectivity between the ventromedial prefrontal cortex and amygdala among female adolescents with abuse histories provide some evidence for this latter pathway (Herringa et al., 2013).

In addition to heightened neural responses in regions involved in salience processing, consistent associations between exposure to threatening environments and elevations in self-reported emotional reactivity to the environment have been observed in our lab and elsewhere (Glaser, Van Os, Portegijs, & Myin-Germeys, 2006; Heleniak, Jenness, Van Der Stoep, McCauley, & McLaughlin, in press; McLaughlin, Kubzansky et al., 2010). Atypical physiological responses to emotional cues have also been documented consistently among children who have experienced trauma, although the specific pattern of findings has varied across studies depending on the specific physiological measures and emotion eliciting paradigms employed. We recently applied a theoretical model drawn from social psychology on adaptive and maladaptive responses to stress to examine physiological responses to stress among maltreated youths. We observed a pattern of increased vascular resistance and blunted cardiac output reactivity among youths who had been physically or sexually abused relative to participants with no history of violence exposure (McLaughlin, Sheridan, Alves, & Mendes, 2014). This pattern of autonomic nervous system reactivity reflects an inefficient cardiovascular response to stress that has been shown in numerous studies to occur when individuals are in a state of heightened threat and is associated with threat appraisals and maladaptive cognitive and behavioral responses to stress (Jamieson, Mendes, Blackstock, & Schmader, 2010; Jamieson, Nock, & Mendes, 2012; Mendes, Blascovich, Major, & Seery, 2001; Mendes, Major, McCoy, & Blascovich, 2008). Using data from a large population-based cohort of adolescents, we recently replicated the association between childhood trauma exposure and blunted cardiac output reactivity during acute stress (Heleniak, Riese, Ormel, & McLaughlin, 2016).

Together, converging evidence across multiple levels of analysis indicates that exposure to trauma is associated with a persistent pattern of information processing involving biased attention toward potential threats in the environment, heightened neural and subjective responses to negative emotional cues, and a pattern of autonomic nervous system reactivity consistent with heightened threat perception. This heightened reactivity to negative emotional cues may make it more difficult for children who have been exposed to threatening environments to regulate emotional responses. Indeed, a recent study from my lab found that when trying to regulate emotional responses using cognitive reappraisal, children who had been abused recruited regions of the prefrontal cortex involved in effortful control to a greater degree than children who had never experienced violence (McLaughlin, Peverill, et al., 2015). This pattern suggests that attempts to modulate emotional responses to negative cues require more cognitive resources for children with abuse histories, meaning that effective regulation may break down more easily in the face of stress. Evidence that the negative emotional effects of stressful events are heightened among those with maltreatment histories is consistent with this possibility (Glaser et al., 2006; McLaughlin, Conron, et al., 2010).

In addition to alterations in patterns of emotional reactivity to environmental cues, child trauma has been associated with maladaptive patterns of responding to distress. For example, exposure to threatening environments early in development is associated with habitual engagement in rumination, a response style characterized by passive focus on feelings of distress along with their causes and consequences without attempts to actively resolve the causes of distress (Nolen-Hoeksema, Wisco, & Lyubomirsky, 2008). High reliance on rumination as a strategy for responding to distress has been observed in adolescents and adults who were abused as children (Conway, Mendelson, Giannopoulos, Csank, & Holm, 2005; Heleniak et al., in press; Sarin & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2010), adolescents who experienced victimization by peers (McLaughlin, Hatzenbuehler, & Hilt, 2009), and both adolescents and adults exposed to a wide range of negative life events (McLaughlin & Hatzenbuehler, 2009; Michl, McLaughlin, Shepherd, & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2013), although the latter findings are not specific to threat per se.

Although evidence for disruptions in emotional processing come primarily from studies examining children exposed to environments characterized by high degrees of threat, deprived environments are also likely to have downstream effects on emotional development that are at least partially unique from those associated with threat. As noted previously, children who have been neglected experience difficulties discriminating facial displays of emotion (Pollak et al., 2001; Pollak et al., 2005), although some studies of neglected children have found few differences in neural responses to facial emotion in early childhood (Moulson, Fox, Zeanah, & Nelson, 2009; Slopen, McLaughlin, Fox, Zeanah, & Nelson, 2012). However, recent work suggests that children raised in deprived early environments exhibit elevated amygdala response to facial emotion and a mature pattern of functional connectivity between the amygdala and mPFC during emotional-processing tasks (Gee et al., 2013; Tottenham et al., 2011). Finally, children who were neglected or raised in deprived institutions tend to exhibit blunted physiological responses to stress, including in the autonomic nervous system and HPA axis (Gunnar, Frenn, Wewerka, & Van Ryzin, 2009; McLaughlin, Sheridan, et al., 2015).

Much of the existing work on childhood adversity and emotional responding has focused on responses to negative emotional cues. However, a growing body of evidence also suggests that responses to appetitive and rewarding cues are disrupted in children exposed to adversity. For example, children raised in deprived early environments exhibit blunted ventral striatal response to the anticipation of reward (Mehta et al., 2010), and a similar pattern has been observed in a sample of adults exposed to abuse during childhood (Dillon et al., 2009). In a recent study, an increase in ventral striatum response to happy emotional faces occurred from childhood to adolescence in typically developing children but not in children reared in deprived institutions (Goff et al., 2013). In recent work in our lab, we have also observed blunted reward learning among children exposed to institutional rearing (Sheridan, McLaughlin, et al., 2016). Although the mechanisms underlying the link between diverse forms of childhood adversity and responsiveness to reward have yet to be clearly identified, it has been suggested that repeated activation of the HPA axis in early childhood can attenuate expression of brain-derived neurotrophic factor, which in turn regulates the mesolimbic dopamine system that underlies reward learning (Goff & Tottenham, 2014). These reductions in brain-derived neurotrophic factor expression may contribute to a pattern of blunted ventral striatum response to reward anticipation or receipt. Alternatively, given the central role of the mesolimbic dopamine system in attachment-related behavior (Strathearn, 2011), the absence or unpredictability of an attachment figure in early development may reduce opportunities for learning about the rewarding nature of affiliative interactions and social bonds; the absence of this type of stimulus-reward learning early in development, when sensitive and responsive caregiving from a primary attachment figure is an expected environmental input, may ultimately contribute to biased processing of rewarding stimuli later in development. If social interactions in early life are either absent or unrewarding, expectations about the hedonic value of social relationships and other types of rewards might be altered in the long term, culminating in attenuated responsiveness to anticipation of reward. Future research is needed to identify the precise mechanisms through which adverse early environments ultimately shape reward learning and responses to rewarding stimuli.

Links between emotional processing and psychopathology

An extensive and growing body of work suggests that disruptions in emotional processing, emotional responding, and emotion regulation represent transdiagnostic factors associated with virtually all commonly occurring forms of psychopathology (Aldao, Nolen-Hoeksema, & Schweizer, 2010). Specifically, attention biases to threat and overgeneral autobiographical memory biases have been linked to anxiety and depression, respectively, in numerous studies (Bar-Haim, Lamy, Bakermans-Kranenburgh, Pergamin, & Van Ijzendoorn, 2007; Williams et al., 2007), and attributions of hostility and other social information processing biases associated with trauma exposure are associated with risk for the onset of conduct problems and aggression (Dodge et al., 1990; Dodge et al., 1995; Weiss et al., 1992). Heightened emotional responses to negative environmental cues are associated with both internalizing and externalizing psychopathology in laboratory-based paradigms examining self-reported emotional and physiological responses to emotional stimuli (Boyce et al., 2001; Carthy, Horesh, Apter, Edge, & Gross, 2010; Hankin, Badanes, Abela, & Watamura, 2010; McLaughlin, Kubzansky, et al., 2010; McLaughlin, Sheridan, Alves, et al., 2014; Rao, Hammen, Ortiz, Chen, & Poland, 2008), fMRI studies examining neural response to facial emotion (Sebastian et al., 2012; Siegle, Thompson, Carter, Steinhauer, & Thase, 2007; Stein, Simmons, Feinstein, & Paulus, 2007; Suslow et al., 2010; Thomas et al., 2001), and experience sampling studies that measure emotional responses in real world situations (Myin-Germeys et al., 2003; Silk, Steinberg, & Morris, 2003). Habitual engagement in rumination has also been linked to heightened risk for anxiety, depression, eating disorders, and problematic substance use (McLaughlin & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2011; Nolen-Hoeksema, 2000; Nolen-Hoeksema, Stice, Wade, & Bohon, 2007). Together, evidence from numerous studies examining emotional processing at multiple levels of analysis suggests that disruptions in emotional processing are a key transdiagnostic factor in psychopathology that may explain patterns of multifinality following exposure to threatening early environments.

Executive functioning

Disruptions in executive functioning represent the second key proximal risk factor in the model. A growing body of evidence suggests that environmental deprivation is associated with lasting alterations in executive functioning skills. Poor executive functioning—including problems with working memory, inhibitory control, planning ability, and cognitive flexibility—has consistently been documented among children raised in deprived environments ranging from institutional settings to low socioeconomic status (SES) families. Children raised in institutional settings exhibit a range of deficits in cognitive functions including general intellectual ability (Nelson et al., 2007; O’Connor, Rutter, Beckett, Keaveney, & Kreppner, 2000), expressive and receptive language (Albers, Johnson, Hostetter, Iverson, & Miller, 1997; Windsor et al., 2011), and executive function skills (Bos et al., 2009; Tibu et al., 2016). In contrast to other domains of cognitive ability, however, deficits in executive functioning and marked elevations in the prevalence of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)—which is characterized by executive functioning problems—are persistent over time even after placement into a stable family environment (Bos et al., 2009; Tibu et al., 2016; Zeanah et al., 2009). Similar patterns of executive functioning deficits have also been observed among children raised in low SES families, including problems with working memory, inhibitory control, and cognitive flexibility (Blair, 2002; Farah et al., 2006; Noble et al., 2007; Noble, Norman, & Farah, 2005; Raver, Blair, Willoughby, & The Family life Project Key Investigators, 2013), as well as deficits in language abilities (Fernald, Marchman, & Weisleder, 2013; Weisleder & Fernald, 2013). Poor cognitive flexibility among children raised in low SES environments has been observed as early as infancy (Clearfield & Niman, 2012). Relative to children who have been abused, children exposed to neglect are at greater risk for cognitive deficits (Hildyard & Wolfe, 2002) similar to those observed in poverty and institutionalization (Dubowitz et al., 2002; Spratt et al., 2012).

The lateral PFC is recruited during a wide variety of executive functioning tasks, including working memory (Wager & Smith, 2003), inhibition (Aron, Robbins, & Poldrack, 2004), and cognitive flexibility (Rougier, Noelle, Braver, Cohen, & O’Reilly, 2005), and is one of the brain regions most centrally involved in executive functioning. In addition to exhibiting poor performance on executive functioning tasks, children from low SES families also have different patterns of lateral PFC recruitment during these tasks as compared to children from middle-class families (Kishiyama, Boyce, Jimenez, Perry, & Knight, 2009; Sheridan, Sarsour, Jutte, D’Esposito, & Boyce, 2012). A similar pattern of poor inhibitory control and altered lateral PFC recruitment during an inhibition task has also been observed in children raised in institutional settings (Mueller et al., 2010).

These studies provide some clues about where to look with regards to the types of environmental inputs that might be necessary for the development of adaptive executive functions. In particular, environmental inputs that are absent or atypical among children raised in institutional settings, as well as among children raised in poverty, are promising candidates. Institutional rearing is associated with an absence of environmental inputs of numerous kinds, including the presence of an attachment figure, variation in daily routines and activities, access to age-appropriate enriching cognitive stimulation from books, toys, and interactions with adults, and complex language exposure (Smyke et al., 2007; Zeanah et al., 2003). Some of these dimensions of environmental experience have also been shown to be deprived among children raised in poverty, including access to cognitively enriching activities—including access to books, toys, and puzzles; learning opportunities outside the home (e.g., museums) and within the context of the parent–child relationship (e.g., parental encouragement of learning colors, words, and numbers, reading to the child); and variation in environmental complexity and stimulation—as well as the amount and complexity of language input (Bradley, Convyn, Burchinal, McAdoo, & Coll, 2001; Bradley, Corwyn, McAdoo, & Coll, 2001; Dubowitz et al., 2002; Garrett, Ng’andu, & Ferron, 1994; Hart & Risley, 1995; Hoff, 2003; Linver, Brooks-Gunn, & Kohen, 2002).

Together, these distinct lines of research suggest that enriching cognitive activities and exposure to complex language might provide the scaffolding that children require to develop executive functions. Some indirect evidence supports this notion. For example, degree of environmental stimulation in the home and amount and quality of maternal language each predict the development of language skills in early childhood (Farah et al., 2008; Hoff, 2003), and children raised in both institutional settings and low SES families exhibit deficits in expressive and receptive language (Albers et al., 1997; Hoff, 2003; Noble et al., 2007; Noble et al., 2005; Windsor et al., 2011) in addition to problems with executive functioning skills. Moreover, a recent study found that atypical patterns of PFC activation during executive function tasks among children from low SES families is explained by degree of complex language exposure in the home (Sheridan et al., 2012). Finally, children raised in bilingual environments appear to have improved performance on executive function tasks (Carlson & Meltzoff, 2008). These findings suggest that the environmental inputs that are required for language development (i.e., complex language directed at the child) may also be critical for the development of executive function skills. Language provides an opportunity to develop multiple such skills ranging from working memory (e.g., holding in mind the first part of a sentence as you wait for the speaker to finish), inhibitory control (e.g., waiting your turn in a conversation), and cognitive flexibility (e.g., switching between grammatical and syntactic rules).

Lack of consistent rules, routines, structure, and parental scaffolding behaviors may be another mechanism explaining deficits in executive functioning among children from low SES families. This lack of environmental predictability is more common among low SES than middle-class families (Deater-Deckard, Chen, Wang, & Bell, 2012; Evans, Gonnella, Mareynyszyn, Gentile, & Salpekar, 2005; Evans & Wachs, 2009). The absence of consistent rules, routines, and contingencies in the environment may interfere with children’s ability to learn abstract rules and to develop the capacity for self-regulation. Indeed, higher levels of parental scaffolding—or provision of support to allow the child to solve problems autonomously—has been prospectively linked with the development of better executive function skills in early childhood (Bernier, Carlson, & Whipple, 2010; Hammond, Muller, Carpendale, Bibok, & Liebermann-Finestone, 2012; Landry, Miller-Loncar, Smith, & Swank, 2002). These findings suggest that environmental unpredictability is an additional mechanism linking low SES environments to poor executive functioning in children. However, given the highly structured and routinized nature of most institutional settings, environmental unpredictability is an unlikely explanation for executive functioning deficits among institutionally reared children.

Deficits in executive functioning skills have sometimes been observed in children with exposure to trauma (DePrince, Weinzierl, & Combs, 2009; Mezzacappa, Kindlon, & Earls, 2001) as well as children with high levels of exposure to stressful life events (Hanson et al., 2012), although some studies have found associations between trauma exposure and working memory but not inhibition or cognitive flexibility (Augusti & Melinder, 2013). There are two possible explanations for these findings. First, for children exposed to threat, it may be that deficits in executive functions emerge primarily in emotional contexts, such that the heightened perceptual sensitivity and reactivity to emotional stimuli in children exposed to threat draws attention to emotional stimuli (Shackman et al., 2007), making it more difficult to hold other stimuli in mind, effectively inhibit responses to emotional stimuli, or flexibly allocate attention to nonemotional stimuli. Indeed, in a recent study in my lab, we observed that exposure to trauma (both maltreatment and community violence) was associated with deficits in inhibitory control only in the context of emotional stimuli (i.e., a Stroop task involving emotional faces) and not when stimuli were neutral (i.e., shapes), and had no association with cognitive flexibility (Lambert, King, Monahan, & McLaughlin, 2016). In contrast, deprivation exposure was associated with deficits in inhibition to both neutral and emotional stimuli and poor cognitive flexibility. Although this suggests there may be specificity in the association of trauma exposure with executive functions, greater research is needed to understand these links. Second, studies examining exposure to trauma seldom measure indicies of deprivation, nor do they adjust for deprivation exposure (just as studies of deprivation rarely assess or control for trauma exposure). Disentangling the specific effects of these two types of experiences on executive functioning processes is a critical goal for future research.

Links between executive functioning and psychopathology

Executive functioning deficits are a central feature of ADHD (Martinussen, Hayden, Hogg-Johnson, & Tannock, 2005; Sergeant, Geurts, & Oosterlaan, 2002; Willcutt, Doyle, Nigg, Faraone, & Pennington, 2005). Problems with executive functions have also been observed in children with externalizing psychopathology, including conduct disorder and oppositional defiant disorder, even after accounting for comorbid ADHD (Hobson, Scott, & Rubia, 2011). They are also associated with elevated risk for the onset of substance use problems and other types of risky behavior (Crews & Boettiger, 2009; Patrick, Blair, & Maggs, 2008), including criminal behavior (Moffitt et al., 2011) and the likelihood of becoming incarcerated (Yechiam et al., 2008). Although executive functioning deficits figure less prominently in theoretical models of the etiology of internalizing psychopathology, when these deficits emerge in the context of emotional processing (e.g., poor inhibition of negative emotional information) they are more strongly linked to internalizing problems, including depression (Goeleven, De Raedt, Baert, & Koster, 2006; Joorman & Gotlib, 2010). Executive functioning deficits also contribute to other proximal risk factors, such as rumination (Joorman, 2006), that are well-established risk factors for depression and anxiety disorders. Patterns of executive functioning in childhood have lasting implications for health and development beyond effects on psychopathology. Recent work suggests that executive functioning measured in early childhood predicts a wide range of outcomes in adulthood in the domains of health, SES, and criminal behavior, over and above the effects of IQ (Moffitt et al., 2011).

Mechanisms Linking Distal Risk Factors to Proximal Risk Factors

How do experiences of threat and deprivation come to influence proximal risk factors? Learning mechanisms are the most obvious pathways linking these experiences with changes in emotional processing and executive functioning, although other mechanisms (e.g., the development of stable beliefs and schemas) are also likely to play an important role. Specifically, the impact of threatening and deprived early environments on the development of patterns of emotional processing and emotional responding may be mediated, at least in part, through emotional learning pathways. The associative learning mechanisms and neural circuitry underlying fear learning and reward learning have been well characterized in both animals and humans and reviewed elsewhere (Delgado, Olsson, & Phelps, 2006; Flagel et al., 2011; Johansen, Cain, Ostroff, & LeDoux, 2011; O’Doherty, 2004). Exposure to threatening or deprived environments early in development results in the presence (i.e., in the case of threats) or absence (i.e., in the case of deprivation) of opportunities for emotional learning; these learning experiences, in turn, have lasting downstream effects on emotional processing. Specifically, early learning histories can influence the salience of environmental stimuli as either potential threats or incentives, shape the magnitude of emotional responses to environmental stimuli—particularly those that represent either threat or reward, and alter motivation to avoid threats or pursue rewards. Thus, fear learning mechanisms and their downstream consequences explain, in part, the association of threatening environments with alterations in emotional processing (McLaughlin et al., 2014; Sheridan & McLaughlin, 2014). Similarly, the effects of deprived early environments on emotional processing are likely to be partially explained through reward learning pathways. Pathways linking threatening early environments to habitual patterns of responding to distress, such as rumination, may also involve learning mechanisms including both observational (e.g., modeling responses utilized by caregivers) and instrumental (e.g., reinforcement of passive responses to distress when emotional displays are met with dismissive or punishing reactions from caregivers) learning.

Learning mechanisms may also be a central mechanism in the association between deprived early environments and the development of executive functioning. In particular, deprived environments such as institutional rearing, neglect, and poverty are characterized by the absence of learning opportunities, which is thought to directly contribute to later difficulties with complex higher order cognition. Specifically, reduced opportunities for learning due to the absence of complex and varied stimulus-response contingencies or the presence of consistent rules, routines, and structures that allow children to learn concrete and abstract rules may influence the development of both cognitive and behavioral aspects of self-regulation.

Moderators of the Link Between Distal and Proximal Risk Factors

Children vary markedly in their sensitivity to environmental context. Advances in theoretical conceptualizations of individual differences in sensitivity to context can be leveraged to understand variability in developmental processes among children exposed to adverse environments. A growing body of evidence suggests that certain characteristics make children particularly responsive to environmental influences; such factors confer not only vulnerability in the context of adverse environments but also benefits in the presence of supportive environments (Belsky, Bakermans-Kranenburg, & Van Ijzendoorn, 2007; Belsky & Pluess, 2009; Boyce & Ellis, 2005; Ellis, Essex, & Boyce, 2005). Highly reactive temperament, vagal tone, and genetic polymorphisms that regulate the dopaminergic and serotonergic system have been identified as markers of plasticity and susceptibility to both negative and positive environmental influences (Belsky & Pluess, 2009). These plasticity markers represent potential moderators of the link between childhood adversity and disruptions in emotional processing and executive functioning.

Developmental timing of exposure to adversity also plays a meaningful role in moderating the impact of childhood adversity on emotional processing and executive functioning. For example, in recent work we have shown that early environmental deprivation has a particularly pronounced impact on the development of stress response systems during the first 2 years of life (McLaughlin et al., 2015). These findings suggest the possibility of an early sensitive period during which the environment exerts a disproportionate effect on the development of neurobiological systems that regulate responses to stress. As noted in the beginning of this article, additional research is needed to identify developmental periods of heightened plasticity in specific subdomains of emotional processing and executive functioning and to determine the degree to which disruptions in these domains vary as a function of the timing of exposure to childhood adversity.

Moderators of Trajectories From Proximal Risk Factors to Psychopathology

A key component of Nolen-Hoeksema and Watkins’s (2011) transdiagnostic model of psychopathology involves moderators that determine the specific type of psychopathology that someone with a particular proximal risk factor will develop. Specifically, their model argues that ongoing environmental context and neurobiological factors can moderate the impact of proximal risk factors on psychopathology by raising concerns or themes that are acted upon by proximal risk factors and by shaping responses to and altering the reinforcement value of particular types of stimuli. For example, the nature of ongoing environmental experiences might determine whether someone with an underlying vulnerability (e.g., neuroticism) develops anxiety or depression. Specifically, a person with high neuroticism who experiences a stressor involving a high degree of threat or danger (e.g., a mugging or a car accident) might develop an anxiety disorder, whereas a person with high neuroticism who experiences a loss (e.g., an unexpected death of a loved one) might develop major depression (Nolen-Hoeksema & Watkins, 2011). Neurobiological factors that influence the reinforcement value of certain stimuli (e.g., alcohol and other substances, food, social rejection) can also serve as moderators. For example, individual differences in rejection sensitivity might determine whether a child who is bullied develops an anxiety disorder. Although a review of these factors is beyond the scope of the current article, greater understanding of the role of ongoing environmental context as a moderator of the link between proximal risk factors and the emergence of psychopathology has relevance for research on childhood adversity. In particular, environmental factors that buffer against the emergence of psychopathology in children with disruptions in emotional processing and executive functioning can point to potential targets for preventive interventions for children exposed to adversity.

CONCLUSION

Exposure to childhood adversity represents one of the most potent risk factors for the onset of psychopathology. Recognition of the strong and pervasive influence of childhood adversity on risk for psychopathology throughout the life course has generated a burgeoning field of research focused on understanding the links between adverse early experience, developmental processes, and mental health. This article provides recommendations for future research in this area (see Table 1). In particular, future research must develop and utilize a consistent definition of childhood adversity across studies, as it is critical for the field to agree upon what the construct of childhood adversity represents and what types of experiences do and do not qualify. Progress in identifying developmental mechanisms linking childhood adversity to psychopathology requires integration of studies of typical development with those focused on childhood adversity in order to characterize how experiences of adversity disrupt developmental trajectories in emotion, cognition, social behavior, and the neural circuits that support these processes, as well as greater efforts to distinguish between distinct dimensions of adverse environmental experience that differentially influence these domains of development. Greater understanding of the developmental pathways linking childhood adversity to the onset of psychopathology can inform efforts to identify protective factors that buffer children from the negative consequences of adversity by allowing a shift in focus from downstream outcomes like psychopathology to specific developmental processes that serve as intermediate phenotypes (i.e., mechanisms) linking adversity with psychopathology.

Progress in these domains will generate clinically useful knowledge regarding the mechanisms that explain how childhood adversity is associated with a wide range of psychopathology outcomes (i.e., multifinality) and identify moderators that shape divergent trajectories following adverse childhood experiences. This knowledge can be leveraged to develop and refine empirically informed interventions to prevent the long-term consequences of adverse early environments on children’s development. Greater understanding of modifiable developmental processes underlying the associations of diverse forms of childhood adversity with psychopathology will provide critical information regarding the mechanisms that should be specifically targeted by intervention. Determining whether these mechanisms are general or specific is essential, as it is unlikely that a one-size-fits-all approach to intervention will be effective for preventing the onset of psychopathology following all types of childhood adversity. Identifying processes that are disrupted following specific forms of adversity, but not others, will allow interventions to be tailored to address the developmental mechanisms that are most relevant for children exposed to particular types of adversity. Identification of moderators that buffer children either from disruptions in core developmental domains or from developing psychopathology in the presence of developmental disruptions—for example, among children with heightened emotional reactivity or poor executive functioning—will provide additional targets for intervention. Finally, uncovering sensitive periods when emotional, cognitive, and neurobiological processes are most likely to be influenced by the environment will provide key information about when interventions are most likely to be successful. Together, these advances will help the field to generate innovative new approaches for preventing the onset of psychopathology among children who have experienced adversity.

Acknowledgments

FUNDING

This research was supported by grants from the National Institutes of Health (K01-MH092526 and R01-MH103291).

References

  1. Adversity. Merriam-Webster’s online dictionary. 2015 Retrieved from http://www.merriam-webster.com/dictionary/adversity.
  2. Afifi TO, Enns MW, Cox BJ, Asmundson GJG, Stein MB, Sareen J. Population attributable risk fractions of psychiatric disorders and suicde ideation and attempts associated with adverse childhood experiences. American Journal of Public Health. 2008;98:946–952. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2007.120253. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Albers LH, Johnson DE, Hostetter MK, Iverson S, Miller LC. Health of children adopted from the former Soviet Union and Eastern Europe. Comparison with preadoptive medical records. Journal of the American Medical Association. 1997;278:922–924. doi: 10.1001/jama.1997.03550110060037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Aldao A, Nolen-Hoeksema S, Schweizer S. Emotion-regulation strategies across psychopathology: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review. 2010;30:217–237. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Almas AN, Degnan KA, Radulescu A, Nelson CA, Zeanah CH, Fox NA. Effects of early intervention and the moderating effects of brain activity on institutionalized children’s social skills at age 8. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2012;109:17228–17231. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1121256109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 5. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press; 2013. (DSM-5) [Google Scholar]
  7. Andersen SL, Tomada A, Vincow ES, Valente E, Polcari A, Teicher MH. Preliminary evidence for sensitive periods in the effect of childhood sexual abuse on regional brain development. The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences. 2008;20:292–301. doi: 10.1176/jnp.2008.20.3.292. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Arata CM, Langhinrichsen-Rohling J, Bowers D, O’Brien N. Differential correlates of multi-type maltreatment among urban youth. Child Abuse & Neglect. 2007;31:393–415. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2006.09.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Aron AR, Robbins TW, Poldrack RA. Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2004;8(4):170–177. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.02.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Augusti EM, Melinder A. Maltreatment is associated with specific impairments in executive functions: A pilot study. Journal of Traumatic Stress. 2013;26:780–783. doi: 10.1002/jts.2013.26.issue-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Bar-Haim Y, Lamy D, Bakermans-Kranenburgh MJ, Pergamin L, Van Ijzendoorn MH. Threat-related attentional bias in anxious and non-anxious individuals: A meta-analytic study. Psychological Bulletin. 2007;133:1–24. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.133.1.1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Baumrind D. The average expectable environment is not good enough: A response to Scarr. Child Development. 1993;64:1299–1317. doi: 10.2307/1131536. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Belsky J, Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ, Van Ijzendoorn MH. For better and for worse: Differential susceptibility to environmental influences. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2007;16:300–304. doi: 10.1111/cdir.2007.16.issue-6. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  14. Belsky J, Pluess M. Beyond diathesis stress: Differential susceptibility to environmental influences. Psychological Bulletin. 2009;135:885–908. doi: 10.1037/a0017376. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Bernier A, Carlson S, Whipple N. From external regulation to self-regulation: Early parenting precursors of young children’s executive functioning. Child Development. 2010;81:326–339. doi: 10.1111/cdev.2010.81.issue-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Blair C. School readiness: Integrating cognition and emotion in a neurobiological conceptualization of children’s functioning at school entry. The American Psychologist. 2002;57(2):111–127. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.57.2.111. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Bolger KE, Patterson C, Kupersmidt JB. Peer relationships and self-esteem among children who have been maltreated. Child Development. 1999;69:1171–1197. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1998.tb06166.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Bos KJ, Fox NA, Zeanah CH, Nelson CA. Effects of early psychosocial deprivation on the development of memory and executive function. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience. 2009;3:16. doi: 10.3389/neuro.08.016.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Boyce WT, Ellis BJ. Biological sensitivity to context: I. An evolutionary-developmental theory of the origins and functions of stress reactivity. Development and Psychopathology. 2005;17:271–301. doi: 10.1017/S0954579405050145. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Boyce WT, Quas JA, Abbey A, Smider NA, Essex MJ, Kupfer DJ. Autonomic reactivity and psychopathology in middle childhood. British Journal of Psychiatry. 2001;179:144–150. doi: 10.1192/bjp.179.2.144. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Bradley RH, Corwyn RF, Burchinal M, McAdoo HP, Coll CG. The home environments of children in the United States part II: Relations with behavioral development through age thirteen. Child Development. 2001;72:1868–1886. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.t01-1-00383. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Bradley RH, Corwyn RF, McAdoo HP, Coll CG. The home environments of children in the United States part I: Variations by age, ethnicity, and poverty status. Child Development. 2001;72(6):1844–1867. doi: 10.1111/cdev.2001.72.issue-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Carlson SM, Meltzoff AN. Bilingual experience and executive functioning in young children. Developmental Science. 2008;11:282–298. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00675.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Carthy T, Horesh N, Apter A, Edge MD, Gross JJ. Emotional reactivity and cognitive regulation in anxious children. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2010;48:384–393. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2009.12.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Caspi A, Houts RM, Belsky DW, Goldman-Mellor SJ, Harrington H, Israel S, … Moffitt TE. The p factor: One general psychopathology factor in the structure of psychiatric disorders? Clinical Psychological Science. 2014;2:119–137. doi: 10.1177/2167702613497473. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Chase-Lansdale PL, Cherlin AJ, Kiernan KE. The long-term effects of parental divorce on the mental health of young adults: A developmental perspective. Child Development. 1995;66:1614–1634. doi: 10.2307/1131900. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Cicchetti D. Developmental psychopathology: Reactions, reflections, projections. Developmental Review. 1993;13:471–502. doi: 10.1006/drev.1993.1021. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  28. Cicchetti D. Contextualism and developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology. 1996;10:137–141. doi: 10.1017/S0954579498001540. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Cicchetti D, Lynch M. Toward an ecological/transactional model of community violence and child maltreatment: Consequences for children’s development. Psychiatry. 1993;56:96–118. doi: 10.1080/00332747.1993.11024624. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Cicchetti D, Rogosch FA. Equifinality and multifinality in developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology. 1996;8:597–600. doi: 10.1017/S0954579400007318. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  31. Cicchetti D, Toth SL. The development of depression in children and adolescents. American Psychologist. 1998;53:221–241. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.53.2.221. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Cicchetti D, Toth SL. The past achievements and future promises of developmental psychopathology: The coming of age of a discipline. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2009;50:16–25. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.2009.50.issue-1-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Clearfield MW, Niman LC. SES affects infant cognitive flexibility. Infant Behavior and Development. 2012;35:29–35. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2011.09.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Collishaw S, Pickles A, Messer J, Rutter M, Shearer C, Maughan B. Resilience to adult psychopathology following childhood maltreatment: Evidence from a community sample. Child Abuse & Neglect. 2007;31:211–229. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2007.02.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Conway M, Mendelson M, Giannopoulos C, Csank PAR, Holm SL. Childhood and adult sexual abuse, rumination on sadness, and dysphoria. Child Abuse and Neglect. 2005;2004(28):393–410. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2003.05.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Costello EJ, Compton SN, Keeler G, Angold A. Relationships between poverty and psychopathology: A natural experiment. JAMA: Journal of the American Medical Association. 2003;290:2023–2029. doi: 10.1001/jama.290.15.2023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Crane C, Heron J, Gunnell D, Lewis G, Evans J, Williams JMG. Childhood traumatic events and adolescent overgeneral autobiographical memory: Findings in a UK cohort. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry. 2014;45:330–338. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2014.02.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Crews FT, Boettiger CA. Impulsivity, frontal lobes and risk for addiction. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior. 2009;93:237–247. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2009.04.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Deater-Deckard K, Chen N, Wang Z, Bell MA. Socioeconomic risk moderates the link between household chaos and maternal executive function. Journal of Family Psychology. 2012;26:391–399. doi: 10.1037/a0028331. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Delgado MR, Olsson A, Phelps EA. Extending animal models of fear conditioning to humans. Biological Psychology. 2006;73:39–48. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2006.01.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. DePrince AP, Weinzierl KM, Combs MD. Executive function performance and trauma exposure in a community sample of children. Child Abuse & Neglect. 2009;33:353–361. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2008.08.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Dillon DG, Holmes AJ, Birk JL, Brooks N, Lyons-Ruth K, Pizzagalli DA. Childhood adversity is associated with left basal ganglia dysfunction during reward anticipation in adulthood. Biological Psychiatry. 2009;66:206–213. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.02.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Dodge KA, Bates JE, Pettit GS. Mechanisms in the cycle of violence. Science. 1990;250:1678–1683. doi: 10.1126/science.2270481. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Dodge KA, Pettit GS, Bates JE, Valente E. Social information-processing patterns partially mediate the effect of early physical abuse on later conduct problems. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1995;104:632–643. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.104.4.632. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Dong M, Anda RF, Felitti VJ, Dube SR, Williamson DF, Thompson TJ, Giles WH. The interrelatedness of multiple forms of childhood abuse, neglect, and household dysfunction. Child Abuse and Neglect. 2004;28:771–784. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2004.01.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Dube SR, Felitti VJ, Dong M, Chapman DP, Giles WH, Anda RF. Childhood abuse, neglect, and household dysfunction and the risk of illicit drug use: The adverse childhood experiences study. Pediatrics. 2003;111:564–572. doi: 10.1542/peds.111.3.564. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Dubowitz H, Papas MA, Black MM, Starr RH., Jr Child neglect: Outcomes in high-risk urban preschoolers. Pediatrics. 2002;109:1100–1107. doi: 10.1542/peds.109.6.1100. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Edwards VJ, Holden GW, Felitti VJ, Anda RF. Relationship between multiple forms of childhood maltreatment and adult mental health in community respondents: Results from the adverse childhood experiences study. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2003;160:1453–1460. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.8.1453. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Ellis BJ, Essex MJ, Boyce WT. Biological sensitivity to context: II. Empirical explorations of an evolutionary-developmental theory. Development and Psychopathology. 2005;17:303–328. doi: 10.1017/S0954579405050157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Evans GW, Gonnella C, Mareynyszyn LA, Gentile L, Salpekar N. The role of chaos in poverty and children’s socioemotional adjustment. Psychological Science. 2005;16:560–565. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.01575.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Evans GW, Li D, Whipple SS. Cumulative risk and child development. Psychological Bulletin. 2013;139:1342–1396. doi: 10.1037/a0031808. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Evans GW, Wachs T. Chaos and its influence on children’s development: An ecological perspective. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 2009. [Google Scholar]
  53. Farah MJ, Betancourt L, Shera DM, Savage JH, Giannetta JM, Brodsky NL, … Hurt H. Environmental stimulation, parental nurturance and cognitive development in humans. Developmental Science. 2008;11:793–801. doi: 10.1111/desc.2008.11.issue-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Farah MJ, Shera DM, Savage JH, Betancourt L, Gianetta JM, Brodsky NL, Hurt H. Childhood poverty: Specific associations with neurocognitive development. Brain Research. 2006;1110:166–174. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.06.072. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Felitti VJ, Anda RF, Nordenberg D, Williamson DF, Spitz AM, Edwards V, Marks JS. Relationship of childhood abuse and household dysfunction to many of the leading causes of death in adults: The Adverse Childhood Experiences (ACE) Study. American Journal of Preventive Medicine. 1998;14:245–258. doi: 10.1016/S0749-3797(98)00017-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Fergusson DM, Lynskey MT. Adolescent resiliency to family adversity. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 1996;37:281–292. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.1996.37.issue-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Fernald A, Marchman VA, Weisleder A. SES differences in language processing skill and vocabulary are evident at 18 months. Developmental Science. 2013;16(2):234–248. doi: 10.1111/desc.12019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Finkelhor D, Ormrod R, Turner H. Polyvictimization and trauma in a national longitudinal cohort. Development and Psychopathology. 2007;19:149–166. doi: 10.1017/S0954579407070083. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Flagel SB, Clark JJ, Robinson TE, Mayo L, Czuj A, Willuhn E, Akil H. A selective role for dopamine in stimulus-reward learning. Nature. 2011;469:53–57. doi: 10.1038/nature09588. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Flouri E. Early family environments may moderate prediction of low educational attainment in adulthood: The cases of childhood hyperactivity and authoritarian parenting. Educational Psychology. 2007;27:737–751. doi: 10.1080/01443410701309282. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  61. Fox SE, Levitt P, Nelson CA. How the timing and quality of early experiences influence the development of brain architecture. Child Development. 2010;81(1):28–40. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01380.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Fristad MA, Jedel R, Weller RA, Weller EB. Psychosocial functioning in children after the death of a parent. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1993;150:511–513. doi: 10.1176/ajp.150.3.511. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Garrett P, Ng’andu N, Ferron J. Poverty experiences of young children and the quality of their home environments. Child Development. 1994;65:331–345. doi: 10.2307/1131387. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Gee DG, Gabard-Durnam LJ, Flannery J, Goff B, Humphreys KL, Telzer EH, Tottenham N. Early developmental emergence of human amygdala-prefrontal connectivity after maternal deprivation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2013;110:15638–15643. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1307893110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Gee DG, Humphreys KL, Flannery J, Goff B, Telzer EH, Shapiro M, Tottenham N. A developmental shift from positive to negative connectivity in human amygdala-prefrontal circuitry. Journal of Neuroscience. 2013;33:4584–4593. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3446-12.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Glaser JP, van Os J, Portegijs PJM, Myin-Germeys I. Childhood trauma and emotional reactivity to daily life stress in adult frequent attenders of general practitioners. Journal of Psychosomatic Research. 2006;61:229–236. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2006.04.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Goeleven E, De Raedt R, Baert S, Koster EHW. Deficient inhibition of emotional information in depression. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2006;93:149–157. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2006.03.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Goff B, Gee DG, Telzer EH, Humphreys KL, Gabard-Durnam LJ, Flannery DJ, Tottenham N. Reduced nucleus accumbens reactivity and adolescent depression following early-life stress. Neuroscience. 2013;249:129–138. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2012.12.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Goff B, Tottenham N. Early-life adversity and adolescent depression: Mechanisms involving the ventral striatum. CNS Spectrums. 2014;20:337–345. doi: 10.1017/S1092852914000674. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Gonzales NA, Cauce AM, Friedman RJ, Mason CA. Family, peer, and neighborhood influences on academic achievement among African-American adolescents: One-year prospective effects. American Journal of Community Psychology. 1996;24:365–387. doi: 10.1007/BF02512027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Gottlieb G. Experiential canalization of behavioral development: Results. Developmental Psychology. 1991a;27:35–39. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.27.1.35. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  72. Gottlieb G. Experiential canalization of behavioral development: Theory. Developmental Psychology. 1991b;27:4–13. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.27.1.4. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  73. Green JG, McLaughlin KA, Berglund P, Gruber MJ, Sampson NA, Zaslavsky AM, Kessler RC. Childhood adversities and adult psychopathology in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication (NCS-R) I: Associations with first onset of DSM-IV disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2010;62:113–123. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.186. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Gunnar MR, Frenn K, Wewerka S, Van Ryzin MJ. Moderate versus severe early life stress: Associations with stress reactivity and regulation in ?10–?12 year-old children. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2009;34:62–75. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.08.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Hamby SL, Grych J. The web of violence: exploring connections among different forms of interpersonal violence and abuse. Dordrecht, the Netherlands: Springer; 2013. [Google Scholar]
  76. Hammond SI, Muller U, Carpendale JIM, Bibok MB, Liebermann-Finestone DP. The effects of parental scaffolding on preschoolers’ executive function. Developmental Psychology. 2012;48:271–281. doi: 10.1037/a0025519. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Hankin BL, Badanes LS, Abela JR, Watamura SE. Hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis dysregulation in dysphoric children and adolescents: Cortisol reactivity to psychosocial stress from preschool through middle adolescence. Biological Psychiatry. 2010;68:484–490. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.04.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Hanson JL, Chung MK, Avants BB, Rudolph KD, Shirtcliff EA, Gee JC, Pollak SD. Structural variations in prefrontal cortex mediate the relationship between early childhood stress and spatial working memory. Journal of Neuroscience. 2012;32:7917–7925. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0307-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Hart B, Risley TR. Meaningful differences in the everyday experiences of young American children. Baltimore, MD: Brookes; 1995. [Google Scholar]
  80. Heim C, Binder EB. Current research trends in early life stress and depression: Review of human studies on sensitive periods, gene–environment interactions, and epigenetics. Experimental Neurology. 2012;233:102–111. doi: 10.1016/j.expneurol.2011.10.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Heleniak C, Jenness J, Van der Stoep A, McCauley E, McLaughlin KA. Childhood maltreatment exposure and disruptions in emotion regulation: A transdiagnostic pathway to adolescent internalizing and externalizing psychopathology. Cognitive Therapy and Research. doi: 10.1007/s10608-015-9735-z. in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Heleniak C, Riese H, Ormel J, McLaughlin KA. Autonomic nervous system reactivity as a mechanism linking child trauma to adolescent psychopathology. in press. Manuscript under review. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Hensch T. Critical period plasticity in local cortical circuits. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2005;6:877–888. doi: 10.1038/nrn1787. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Herringa RJ, Birn RM, Ruttle PL, Stodola DE, Davidson RJ, Essex MJ. Childhood maltreatment is associated with altered fear circuitry and increased internalizing symptoms by late adolescence. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2013;110:19119–19124. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1310766110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Hildyard KL, Wolfe DA. Child neglect: Developmental issues and outcomes. Child Abuse and Neglect. 2002;26:679–695. doi: 10.1016/s0145-2134(02)00341-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Hobson CW, Scott S, Rubia K. Investigation of cool and hot executive function in ODD/CD independently of ADHD. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2011;52:1035–1043. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.2011.52.issue-10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Hoff E. The specificity of environmental influence: Socioeconomic status affects early vocabulary development via maternal speech. Child Development. 2003;74:1368–1378. doi: 10.1111/cdev.2003.74.issue-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Humphreys KL, Zeanah CH. Deviations from the expectable environment in early childhood and emerging psychopathology. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2015;40:154–170. doi: 10.1038/npp.2014.165. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Jamieson JP, Mendes WB, Blackstock E, Schmader T. Turning the knots in your stomach into bows: Reappraising arousal improves performance on the GRE. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 2010;46:208–212. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2009.08.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Jamieson JP, Nock MK, Mendes WB. Mind over matter: Reappraising arousal improves cardiovascular and cognitive responses to stress. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 2012;141:417–422. doi: 10.1037/a0025719. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Johansen JP, Cain CK, Ostroff LE, LeDoux JE. Molecular mechanisms of fear learning and memory. Cell. 2011;147:509–524. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2011.10.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Joorman J. Differential effects of rumination and dysphoria on the inhibition of irrelevant emotional material: Evidence from a negative priming task. Cognitive Therapy and Research. 2006;30:149–160. doi: 10.1007/s10608-006-9035-8. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  93. Joorman J, Gotlib IH. Emotion regulation in depression: Relation to cognitive inhibition. Cognition & Emotion. 2010;24:281–298. doi: 10.1080/02699930903407948. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Kessler RC, Davis CG, Kendler KS. Childhood adversity and adult psychiatric disorder in the US National Comorbidity Survey. Psychological Medicine. 1997;27(5):1101–1119. doi: 10.1017/S0033291797005588. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Kessler RC, McLaughlin KA, Green JG, Gruber MJ, Sampson NA, Zaslavsky AM, Williams DR. Childhood adversities and adult psychopathology in the WHO World Mental Health Surveys. British Journal of Psychiatry. 2010;197:378–385. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.110.080499. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Keyes KM, Eaton NR, Krueger RF, McLaughlin KA, Wall MM, Grant BF, Hasin DS. Childhood maltreatment and the structure of common psychiatric disorders. The British Journal of Psychiatry. 2012;200:107–115. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.111.093062. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Kishiyama MM, Boyce WT, Jimenez AM, Perry LM, Knight RT. Socioeconomic disparities affect prefrontal function in children. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2009;21:1106–1115. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Kuhl PK. Early language acquisition: Cracking the speech code. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2004;5:831–843. doi: 10.1038/nrn1533. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Lambert HK, King KM, Monahan KC, McLaughlin KA. Differential associations of dimensions of environmental adversity with emotion regulation and cognitive control in adolescence. 2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Landry SH, Miller-Loncar CL, Smith KE, Swank PR. The role of early parenting in children’s development of executive processes. Developmental Neuropsychology. 2002;21:15–41. doi: 10.1207/S15326942DN2101_2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Linver MR, Brooks-Gunn J, Kohen DE. Family processes as pathways from income to young children’s development. Developmental Psychology. 2002;38:719–734. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.38.5.719. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Luthar SS. Vulnerability and resilience: A study of high-risk adolescents. Child Development. 1991;62:600–616. doi: 10.2307/1131134. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Luthar SS, Cicchetti D, Becker B. The construct of resilience: A critical evaluation and guidelines for future work. Child Development. 2000;71:543–562. doi: 10.1111/cdev.2000.71.issue-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Lynch M, Cicchetti D. An ecological-transactional analysis of children and contexts: The longitudinal interplay among child maltreatment, community violence, and children’s symptomatology. Development and Psychopathology. 1998;10:235–257. doi: 10.1017/S095457949800159X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Martinussen R, Hayden J, Hogg-Johnson S, Tannock R. A meta-analysis of working memory impairments in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2005;44:377–384. doi: 10.1097/01.chi.0000153228.72591.73. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Masten A. Ordinary magic: Resilience processes in development. American Psychologist. 2001;56:227–238. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.56.3.227. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Masten A, Best KM, Garmezy N. Resilience and development: Contributions from the study of children who overcome adversity. Development and Psychopathology. 1990;2:425–444. doi: 10.1017/S0954579400005812. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  108. McCrory EJ, De Brito SA, Kelly PA, Bird G, Sebastian CL, Mechelli A, … Viding E. Amygdala activation in maltreated children during pre-attentive emotional processing. The British Journal of Psychiatry. 2013;202:269–276. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.112.116624. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. McCrory EJ, De Brito SA, Sebastian CL, Mechelli A, Bird G, Kelly PA, Viding E. Heightened neural reactivity to threat in child victims of family violence. Current Biology. 2011;21:R947–948. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.10.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. McCrory E, De Brito SA, Viding E. Research review: The neurobiology and genetics of maltreatment and adversity. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2010;51:1079–1095. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02271.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. McEwen BS. Protective and damaging effects of stress mediators. New England Journal of Medicine. 1998;338:171–179. doi: 10.1056/NEJM199801153380307. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. McEwen BS. Allostasis and allostatic load Implications for Neuropsychopharmacology. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2000;22:108–124. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(99)00129-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. McLaughlin KA, Conron KJ, Koenen KC, Gilman SE. Childhood adversity, adult stressful life events, and risk of past-year psychiatric disorder: A test of the stress sensitization hypothesis in a population-based sample of adults. Psychological Medicine. 2010;40:1647–1658. doi: 10.1017/S0033291709992121. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. McLaughlin KA, Fox NA, Zeanah CH, Nelson CA. Adverse rearing environments and neural development in children: The development of frontal EEG asymmetry. Biological Psychiatry. 2011;70:1008–1015. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.08.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. McLaughlin KA, Green JG, Gruber MJ, Sampson NA, Zaslavsky A, Kessler RC. Childhood adversities and first onset of psychiatric disorders in a national sample of US adolescents. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2012;69:1151–1160. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.2277. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. McLaughlin KA, Hatzenbuehler ML. Mechanisms linking stressful life events and mental health problems in a prospective, community-based sample of adolescents. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2009;44:153–160. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2008.06.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. McLaughlin KA, Hatzenbuehler ML, Hilt LM. Emotion dysregulation as a mechanism linking peer victimization to internalizing symptoms in adolescents. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2009;77:894–904. doi: 10.1037/a0015760. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  118. McLaughlin KA, Kubzansky LD, Dunn EC, Waldinger RJ, Vaillant GE, Koenen KC. Childhood social environment, emotional reactivity to stress, and mood and anxiety disorders across the life course. Depression and Anxiety. 2010;27:1087–1094. doi: 10.1002/da.v27.12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  119. McLaughlin KA, Nolen-Hoeksema S. Rumination as a transdiagnostic factor in deprssion and anxiety. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2011;49:186–193. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2010.12.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. McLaughlin KA, Peverill M, Gold AL, Alves S, Sheridan MA. Child maltreatment and neural systems underlying emotion regulation. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2015;54:753–762. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2015.06.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  121. McLaughlin KA, Sheridan MA, Alves S, Mendes WB. Child maltreatment and autonomic nervous system reactivity: Identifying dysregulated stress reactivity patterns using the biopsychosocial model of challenge and threat. Psychosomatic Medicine. 2014;76:538–546. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0000000000000098. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  122. McLaughlin KA, Sheridan MA, Lambert HK. Childhood adversity and neural development: deprivation and threat as distinct dimensions of early experience. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2014;47:578–591. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.10.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. McLaughlin KA, Sheridan MA, Tibu F, Fox NA, Zeanah CH, Nelson CA. Causal effects of the early caregiving environment on development of stress response systems in children. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2015;112:5637–5642. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1423363112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  124. McLaughlin KA, Sheridan MA, Winter W, Fox NA, Zeanah CH, Nelson CA. Widespread reductions in cortical thickness following severe early-life deprivation: A neurodevelopmental pathway to ADHD. Biological Psychiatry. 2014;76:629–638. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.08.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  125. McLaughlin KA, Zeanah CH, Fox NA, Nelson CA. Attachment security as a mechanism linking foster care placement to improved mental health outcomes in previously institutionalized children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2012;53:46–55. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.2011.53.issue-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  126. Mehta MA, Gore-Langton E, Golembo N, Colvert E, Williams SCR, Sonuga-Barke E. Hyporesponsive reward anticipation in the basal ganglia following severe institutional deprivation early in life. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2010;22:2316–2325. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21394. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  127. Mendes WB, Blascovich J, Major B, Seery M. Challenge and threat responses during downward and upward social comparisons. European Journal of Social Psychology. 2001;31:477–497. doi: 10.1002/(ISSN)1099-0992. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  128. Mendes WB, Major B, McCoy S, Blascovich J. How attributional ambiguity shapes physiological and emotional responses to social rejection and acceptance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 2008;94:278–291. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.94.2.278. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  129. Mezzacappa E, Kindlon D, Earls F. Child abuse and performance task assessments of executive functions in boys. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2001;42:1041–1048. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.2001.42.issue-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  130. Michl LC, McLaughlin KA, Shepherd K, Nolen-Hoeksema S. Rumination as a mechanism linking stressful life events to symptoms of depression and anxiety: Longitudinal evidence in early adolescents and adults. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2013;122:339–352. doi: 10.1037/a0031994. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  131. Miyake A, Friedman NP. The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions: Four general conclusions. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2012;21:8–14. doi: 10.1177/0963721411429458. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  132. Miyake A, Friedman NP, Rettinger DA, Shah P, Hegarty M. How are visuospatial working memory, executive functioning, and spatial abilities related? A latent-variable analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General. 2001;130:621–640. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.130.4.621. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  133. Moffitt TE, Arseneault L, Belsky J, Dickson N, Hancox RJ, Harrington H, Caspi A. A gradient of childhood self-control predicts health, wealth, and public safety. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2011;108:2693–2698. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1010076108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  134. Monroe SM. Modern approaches to conceptualizing and measuring human life stress. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology. 2008;4:33–52. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.4.022007.141207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  135. Moulson MC, Fox NA, Zeanah CH, Nelson CA. Early adverse experiences and the neurobiology of facial emotion processing. Developmental Psychology. 2009;45:17–30. doi: 10.1037/a0014035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  136. Mueller SC, Maheu FS, Dozier M, Peloso E, Mandell D, Leibenluft E, Ernst M. Early-life stress is associated with impairment in cognitive control in adolescence: An fMRI study. Neuropsychologia. 2010;48:3037–3044. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.06.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  137. Mullen PE, Martin JL, Anderson J, Romans SE, Herbison GP. Childhood sexual abuse and mental health in adult life. The British Journal of Psychiatry. 1993;163:721–732. doi: 10.1192/bjp.163.6.721. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  138. Myin-Germeys I, Peeters F, Havermans R, Nicolson NA, DeVries MW, Delespaul P, Van Os J. Emotional reactivity to daily life stress in psychosis and affective disorder: An experience sampling study. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica. 2003;107:124–131. doi: 10.1034/j.1600-0447.2003.02025.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  139. Nelson CA, Zeanah CH, Fox NA, Marshall PJ, Smyke AT, Guthrie D. Cognitive recovery in socially deprived young children: The bucharest early intervention project. Science. 2007;318:1937–1940. doi: 10.1126/science.1143921. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  140. Noble KG, McCandliss BD, Farah MJ. Socioeconomic gradients predict individual differences in neurocognitive abilities. Developmental Science. 2007;10:464–480. doi: 10.1111/desc.2007.10.issue-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  141. Noble KG, Norman MF, Farah MJ. Neurocognitive correlates of socioeconomic status in kindergarten children. Developmental Science. 2005;8:74–87. doi: 10.1111/desc.2005.8.issue-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  142. Nolen-Hoeksema S. The role of rumination in depressive disorders and mixed anxiety/depressive symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2000;109:504–511. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.109.3.504. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  143. Nolen-Hoeksema S, Stice E, Wade E, Bohon C. Reciprocal relations between rumination and bulimic, substance abuse, and depressive symptoms in female adolescents. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2007;116:198–207. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.116.1.198. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  144. Nolen-Hoeksema S, Watkins E. A heuristic for developing transdiagnostic models of psychopathology: Explaining multifinality and divergent trajectories. Perspectives on Psychological Science. 2011;6:589–609. doi: 10.1177/1745691611419672. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  145. Nolen-Hoeksema S, Wisco BE, Lyubomirsky S. Rethinking rumination. Perspectives on Psychological Science. 2008;3:400–424. doi: 10.1111/ppsc.2008.3.issue-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  146. O’Connor TG, Rutter M, Beckett C, Keaveney L, Kreppner JM. The effects of global severe privation on cognitive competence: Extension and longitudinal follow-up. English and Romanian Adoptees Study Team. Child Development. 2000;71:376–390. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00151. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  147. O’Doherty JP. Reward representations and reward-related learning in the human brain: Insights from neuroimaging. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2004;14:769–776. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2004.10.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  148. Patrick ME, Blair C, Maggs JL. Executive function, approach sensitivity, and emotional decision making as influences on risk behaviors in young adults. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 2008;30:449–462. doi: 10.1080/13803390701523109. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  149. Pollak SD, Cicchetti D, Hornung K, Reed A. Recognizing emotion in faces: Developmental effects of child abuse and neglect. Developmental Psychology. 2000;36:679–688. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.36.5.679. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  150. Pollak SD, Cicchetti D, Klorman R, Brumaghim JT. Cognitive brain event-related potentials and emotion processing in maltreated children. Child Development. 1997;68:773–787. doi: 10.2307/1132032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  151. Pollak SD, Klorman R, Thatcher JE, Cicchetti D. P3b reflects maltreated children’s reactions to facial displays of emotion. Psychophysiology. 2001;38:267–274. doi: 10.1111/psyp.2001.38.issue-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  152. Pollak SD, Sinha P. Effects of early experience on children’s recognition of facial displays of emotion. Developmental Psychology. 2002;38:784–791. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.38.5.784. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  153. Pollak SD, Tolley-Schell SA. Selective attention to facial emotion in physically abused children. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2003;112:323–338. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.112.3.323. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  154. Pollak SD, Vardi S, Putzer Bechner AM, Curtin JJ. Physically abused children’s regulation of attention in response to hostility. Child Development. 2005;76:968–977. doi: 10.1111/cdev.2005.76.issue-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  155. Rao U, Hammen C, Ortiz LR, Chen LA, Poland RE. Effects of early and recent adverse experiences on adrenal response to psychosocial stress in depressed adolescents. Biological Psychiatry. 2008;64:521–526. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.05.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  156. Raver CC, Blair C, Willoughby M The Family life Project Key Investigators. Poverty as a predictor of 4-year-olds’ executive function: New perspectives on models of differential susceptibility. Developmental Psychology. 2013;49:292–304. doi: 10.1037/a0028343. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  157. Rougier NP, Noelle DC, Braver TS, Cohen JD, O’Reilly RC. Prefrontal cortex and flexible cognitive control: Rules without symbols. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2005;102:7338–7343. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0502455102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  158. Sameroff AJ, Gutman LM, Peck SC. Adaptation among youth facing multiple risks. In: Luthar SS, editor. Resilience and vulnerability: Adaptation in the context of childhood adversities. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press; 2003. pp. 364–391. [Google Scholar]
  159. Sarin S, Nolen-Hoeksema S. The dangers of dwelling: An examination of the relationship between rumination and consumptive coping in survivors of childhood sexual abuse. Cognition & Emotion. 2010;24:71–85. doi: 10.1080/02699930802563668. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  160. Sebastian CL, McCrory EJ, Cecil CAM, Lockwood CM, De Brito SA, Fontaine NMG, Viding E. Neural responses to affective and cognitive theory of mind in children with conduct problems and varying levels of callous-unemotional traits. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2012;68:814–822. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.2070. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  161. Sergeant JA, Geurts H, Oosterlaan J. How specific is a deficit of executive functioning for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder? Behavioural Brain Research. 2002;130:3–28. doi: 10.1016/S0166-4328(01)00430-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  162. Shackman JE, Shackman AJ, Pollak SD. Physical abuse amplifies attention to threat and increases anxiety in children. Emotion. 2007;7:838–852. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.7.4.838. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  163. Sheridan MA, Fox NA, Zeanah CH, McLaughlin KA, Nelson CA. Variation in neural development as a result of exposure to institutionalization early in childhood. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2012;109:12927–12932. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1200041109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  164. Sheridan MA, McLaughlin KA. Dimensions of early experience and neural development: Deprivation and threat. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2014;18:580–585. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2014.09.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  165. Sheridan MA, McLaughlin KA, Winter W, Fox NA, Zeanah CH, Nelson CA. Early caregiving environment causally impacts reward response and neural structure, mediating risk for depression in adolescence. 2016 Under review. [Google Scholar]
  166. Sheridan MA, Sarsour K, Jutte D, D’Esposito M, Boyce WT. The impact of social disparity on prefrontal function in childhood. PloS One. 2012;7(4):e35744. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0035744. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  167. Shonkoff JP, Garner AS. The lifelong effects of early childhood adversity and toxic stress. Pediatrics. 2012;129:232–246. doi: 10.1542/peds.2011-2663. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  168. Siegle GJ, Thompson W, Carter CS, Steinhauer SR, Thase ME. Increased amygdala and decreased dorsolateral prefrontal BOLD responses in unipolar depression: Related and independent features. Biological Psychiatry. 2007;61:198–209. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.05.048. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  169. Silk JS, Steinberg L, Morris AS. Adolescents’ emotion regulation in daily life: Links to depressive symptoms and problem behaviors. Child Development. 2003;74:1869–1880. doi: 10.1046/j.1467-8624.2003.00643.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  170. Slopen N, McLaughlin KA, Fox NA, Zeanah CH, Nelson CA. Alterations in neural processing and psychopathology in children raised in institutions. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2012;69:1022–1030. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2012.444. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  171. Smyke AT, Koga S, Johnson DE, Fox NA, Marshall PJ, Nelson CA … the BEIP Core Group. The caregiving context in institution-reared and family-reared infants and toddlers in Romania. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2007;48:210–218. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01694.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  172. Smyke AT, Zeanah CH, Fox NA, Nelson CA, Guthrie D. Placement in foster care enhances quality of attachment among young institutionalized children. Child Development. 2010;81:212–223. doi: 10.1111/cdev.2010.81.issue-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  173. Spratt EG, Friedenberg SL, Swenson CC, Larosa A, De Bellis MD, Macias MM, Brady KT. The effects of early neglect on cognitive, language, and behavioral functioning in childhood. Psychology. 2012;3:175–182. doi: 10.4236/psych.2012.32026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  174. Sroufe LA. Considering normal and abnormal together: The essence of developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology. 1990;2:335–347. doi: 10.1017/S0954579400005769. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  175. Sroufe LA. Psychopathology as an outcome of development. Development and Psychopathology. 1997;9:251–268. doi: 10.1017/S0954579497002046. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  176. Sroufe LA. The concept of development in developmental psychopathology. Child Development Perspectives. 2009;3:178–183. doi: 10.1111/cdep.2009.3.issue-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  177. Sroufe LA, Egeland B, Kreutzer T. The fate of early experience following developmental change: Longitudinal approaches to individual adaptation in childhood. Child Development. 1990;61:1363–1373. doi: 10.2307/1130748. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  178. Stein MB, Simmons AN, Feinstein JS, Paulus MP. Increased amygdala and insula activation during emotion processing in anxiety-prone subjects. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2007;164:318–327. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.164.2.318. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  179. Steinberg L. Age of opportunity: Lessons from the new science of adolescence. New York, NY: Houghton Miffiin Harcourt; 2014. [Google Scholar]
  180. Steinberg L, Elmen JD, Mounts NS. Authoritative parenting, psychosocial maturity, and academic success among adolescents. Child Development. 1989;60:1424–1436. doi: 10.2307/1130932. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  181. Steinberg L, Lamborn SD, Dornbusch SM, Darling N. Impact of parenting practices on adolescent achievement: Authoritative parenting, school involvement, and encouragement to succeed. Child Development. 1992;63:1266–1281. doi: 10.2307/1131532. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  182. Steinberg L, Mounts NS, Lamborn SD, Dornbusch SM. Authoritative parenting and adolescent adjustment across varied ecological niches. Journal of Research on Adolescence. 1991;1:19–36. [Google Scholar]
  183. Strathearn L. Maternal neglect: Oxytocin, dopamine, and the neurobiology of attachment. Journal of Neuroendocrinology. 2011;23:1054–1065. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2826.2011.02228.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  184. Suslow T, Konrad C, Kugel H, Rustadt D, Zwitserlood P, Schoning S, Dannlowski U. Automatic mood-congruent amygdala responses to masked facial expressions in major depression. Biological Psychiatry. 2010;67:155–160. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.07.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  185. Thomas KM, Drevets WC, Dahl RE, Ryan ND, Birmaher B, Eccard CH, Casey BJ. Amygdala response to fearful faces in anxious and depressed children. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2001;58:1057–1063. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.58.11.1057. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  186. Tibu F, Sheridan MA, McLaughlin KA, Fox NA, Zeanah CH, Nelson CA. Disruptions of working memory and inhibition mediate the association between exposure to institutionalization and symptoms of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Psychological Medicine. 2016;46:529–541. doi: 10.1017/S0033291715002020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  187. Tottenham N. Human amygdala development in the absence of species-expected caregiving. Developmental Psychobiology. 2012;54:598–611. doi: 10.1002/dev.20531. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  188. Tottenham N, Hare T, Millner A, Gilhooly T, Zevin JD, Casey BJ. Elevated amygdala response to faces following early deprivation. Developmental Science. 2011;14:190–204. doi: 10.1111/desc.2011.14.issue-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  189. Trickett PK, Noll JG, Putnam FW. The impact of sexual abuse on female development: Lessons from a multigenerational, longitudinal research study. Development and Psychopathology. 2011;23:453–476. doi: 10.1017/S0954579411000174. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  190. Uher R, McGuffin P. The moderation by the serotonin transporter gene of environmental adversity in the etiology of depression: 2009 update. Molecular Psychiatry. 2010;15:18–22. doi: 10.1038/mp.2009.123. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  191. Wager TD, Smith EE. Neuroimaging studies of working memory: A meta-analysis. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience. 2003;3:255–274. doi: 10.3758/CABN.3.4.255. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  192. Weisleder A, Fernald A. Talking to children matters: Early language experience strengthens processing and builds vocabulary. Psychological Science. 2013;24:2143–2152. doi: 10.1177/0956797613488145. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  193. Weiss B, Dodge KA, Bates JE, Pettit GS. Some consequences of early harsh discipline: Child aggression and a maladaptive social information processing style. Child Development. 1992;63:1321–1335. doi: 10.2307/1131558. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  194. Werner H, Kaplan B. Symbol formation: An organismic-developmental approach to language and the expression of thought. New York, NY: Wiley; 1963. [Google Scholar]
  195. Willcutt EG, Doyle AE, Nigg JT, Faraone SV, Pennington BF. Validity of the executive function theory of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A meta-analytic review. Biological Psychiatry. 2005;57:1336–1346. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.02.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  196. Williams JMG, Barnhofer T, Crane C, Hermans D, Raes F, Watkins E, Dalgleish T. Autobiographical memory specificity and emotional disorder. Psychological Bulletin. 2007;133:122–148. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.133.1.122. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  197. Windsor J, Benigno JP, Wing CA, Carroll PJ, Koga SF, Nelson CA, Zeanah CH. Effect of foster care on young children’s language learning. Child Development. 2011;82(4):1040–1046. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01604.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  198. Wolfe DA, Sas L, Wekerle C. Factors associated with the development of posttraumatic stress disorder among child victims of sexual abuse. Child Abuse & Neglect. 1994;18:37–50. doi: 10.1016/0145-2134(94)90094-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  199. Yechiam E, Kanz JE, Bechara A, Stout JC, Busemeyer JR, Altmaier EM, Paulsen JS. Neurocognitive deficits related to poor decision making in people behind bars. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 2008;15:44–51. doi: 10.3758/PBR.15.1.44. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  200. Zeanah CH, Egger HL, Smyke AT, Nelson CA, Fox NA, Marshall PJ, Guthrie D. Institutional rearing and psychiatric disorders in Romanian preschool children. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2009;166:777–785. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.08091438. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  201. Zeanah CH, Nelson CA, Fox NA, Smyke AT, Marshall PJ, Parker SW, Koga S. Designing research to study the effects of institutionalization on brain and behavioral development: The bucharest early intervention project. Development and Psychopathology. 2003;15:885–907. doi: 10.1017/S0954579403000452. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  202. Zeanah CH, Smyke AT, Koga SF, Carlson E The Bucharest Early Intervention Project Core Group. Attachment in institutionalized and community children in Romania. Child Development. 2005;76:1015–1028. doi: 10.1111/cdev.2005.76.issue-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES