Abstract
Little is known about the individual factors that predict outcomes in Internet-administered psychological treatments. We hypothesized that greater cognitive flexibility (i.e. the ability to simultaneously consider several concepts and tasks and switch effortlessly between them in response to changes in environmental contingencies) would provide a better foundation for learning and employing the cognitive restructuring techniques taught and exercised in therapy, leading to greater treatment gains. Participants in three trials featuring Internet-administered psychological treatments for depression (n = 36), social anxiety disorder (n = 115) and tinnitus (n = 53) completed the 64-card Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) prior to treatment. We found no significant associations between perseverative errors on the WCST and treatment gains in any group. We also found low accuracy in the classification of treatment responders. We conclude that lower cognitive flexibility, as captured by perseverative errors on the WCST, should not impede successful outcomes in Internet-delivered psychological treatments.
Keywords: Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, Perseveration, Prediction, Psychotherapy, Internet
Introduction
Mounting evidence suggests that Internet-delivered psychological treatment programs based on cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) (Andersson, 2009; Andersson, 2014) are effective treatments of mood disorders, anxiety disorders, and somatic disorders like tinnitus (Andersson et al., 2013). More recently, similar treatment programs based on psychodynamic principles have also been created and found to be effective (Johansson et al., 2012). Although several studies have investigated predictors of treatment response in Internet interventions (Nordgreen et al., 2012; Hedman et al., 2012), few studies (Andersson, Carlbring & Grimlund, 2008) have investigated the impact of individual differences in executive functions on treatment gains.
Cognitive flexibility (CF), an executive function, refers to the ability to simultaneously consider several concepts and tasks, and switch effortlessly between them in response to changes in environmental contingencies (Miyake et al., 2000). The conceptualization of CF is similar to the processes involved in the cognitive restructuring techniques used in CBT. In cognitive restructuring, the patient is taught to identify negative automatic thoughts, find ways to reality-test these thoughts, and then develop novel, more functional ways of interpreting the situation (Johnco, Wuthrich & Rapee, 2014). This conceptual overlap between CF and cognitive restructuring techniques suggests that pre-treatment CF abilities may predict the successful acquisition and use of CF strategies in CBT, leading to greater treatment gains. Lower CF abilities have often been associated with mental disorders, especially depression (Snyder, Miyake & Hankin, 2015). Emerging findings have also implicated abnormal cognitive processes in tinnitus (Andersson & McKenna, 2006), and cognitive restructuring is a key part of CBT treatment of this condition (Hesser et al., 2012). Despite these findings associating CF and mental distress, there is limited research on whether CF predicts psychotherapy outcomes for mental disorders, and the few findings that are available have been mixed (Mohlman & Gorman, 2005; Moritz et al., 2005; D’Alcante et al., 2012). One previous study found an association between cognitive flexibility and cognitive restructuring skill acquisition (Johnco, Wuthrich & Rapee, 2013), while another failed to find such an association (Johnco, Wuthrich & Rapee, 2014).
No previous study has investigated whether CF predicts outcomes in Internet-delivered CBT, which despite sharing many similarities with traditional CBT, has some inherent characteristics that we reasoned should put a greater demand on CF abilities. In most Internet interventions, psychoeducation, cognitive and behavioral exercises and other therapeutic components cannot be customized by a therapist according to the patient’s unique situation to the same degree as in traditional therapy. Few Internet interventions feature live therapist contact, meaning that it is not possible for the therapist and the patient to work together on a cognitive restructuring task. Hence, successfully completing Internet interventions likely requires a greater ability to not only comprehend the delivered therapeutic material, but also to identify emotional, cognitive and behavioral patterns entirely independently or with very little therapist assistance. Although cognitive restructuring has traditionally been considered a therapeutic technique specific to CBT, CF may also be important in psychodynamic therapy, where the goal is to explore, identify and change unconscious patterns of cognitions and emotions (Johansson et al., 2012). Little research has investigated neurocognitive predictors of treatment response to psychodynamic therapy, and there has been no study on CF and Internet-administered psychodynamic therapy.
The 64-card Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) (Greve, 2001) is an established neuropsychological test of set-shifting ability, i.e. CF in the face of changing reinforcement schedules (Dehaene & Changeux, 1991). We reasoned that greater CF prior to commencing therapy (as measured by a low number of perseverative error on the WCST) should provide a better foundation for learning, improving and utilizing strategies that rely on CF taught in therapy, regardless of type of therapy, leading to greater symptom reduction. If such an association were to be found, it would justify efforts to personalize clinical treatments based on the patient’s neuropsychological abilities to achieve better outcomes. Studying the predictive power of CF in the context of psychotherapy also provides indirect insight into the mechanisms of therapeutic change. Here we report the results of a multi-sample study designed to investigate the association between CF and symptom reduction in Internet-delivered psychological treatment.
Materials and Methods
Data for the current study was collected as part of three randomized trials: therapist-guided CBT for social anxiety disorder (SAD) (Boettcher et al., 2014), psychodynamic treatment for major depressive disorder (MDD) (Johansson et al., 2012) and CBT or Acceptance and Commitment Therapy (ACT) for tinnitus (Hesser et al., 2012), all Internet-delivered. The primary results of these studies have been reported previously; for full methods, see the respective publication. The intervention studies received ethical approval from the regional ethical review boards in either Linköping (2010/386-31 and 2008/234-08) or Umeå (2012/132-31Ö), and all participants provided written, informed consent.
Samples
Because the aim of the current study was not to evaluate treatment efficacy, analyses were conducted per protocol: participants classified as treatment dropouts (including those with zero completed treatment modules) and/or had incomplete post-treatment data were not included in analyses. One additional participant in the SAD group with a WCST total completion time of 5170 s (final sample mean = 407.72 [SD = 151.51]) was excluded due to presumed technical issues with the online WCST administration. Final sample sizes for the SAD group was n = 115 (all received treatment), n = 83 for the tinnitus group (n = 53 received treatment: n = 28 ACT, n = 25 CBT), and n = 73 for the MDD group (n = 36 received treatment). Pre- and post-treatment symptom scores on the primary outcome measure in each study, along with WCST metrics, age and sex for each subject, were compiled (see Table 1). The total sample mean age was 41.65 (SD = 14.66) years, but there was an overall difference in age (F[2,268] = 35.18, p < .001), driven by SAD < MDD (p < .001) and SAD < tinnitus (p < .001) pair-wise differences (Bonferroni-corrected p < .05). Groups were not matched on sex (Fisher’s exact test p = .0014), but because there was no sex-difference in any WCST score (all p > .157), this was not considered a confounding variable. On raw WCST metrics, groups differed only on Total completion time; however, there were no differences between groups in age-adjusted WCST metrics (all F[2,268] < 1.21, p > .3).
Table 1. Sample characteristics.
Variable | A. Social anxiety disorder | B. Tinnitus | C. Depression | Statistics |
---|---|---|---|---|
Mean age (SD) | 34.09 (10.53) | 49.13 (14.94) | 45.04 (14.44) | F[2,268] = 35.18, p < .001, A < (B, C) |
% Females | 60.9% | 47.0% | 75.3% | FET p = .0014 |
Perseverative errors† | 13.35 (6.08) | 14.39 (6.05) | 13.92 (6.93) | F[2,268] = 0.663, p = .516 |
Total errors† | 20.14 (8.31) | 22 (9.04) | 22.22 (10.15) | F[2,268] = 1.566, p = .211 |
Categories completed† | 2.44 (1.42) | 2.46 (1.44) | 2.33 (1.47) | F[2,268] = 0.189, p = .828 |
Total correct† | 43.86 (8.31) | 42 (9.04) | 41.78 (10.15) | F[2,268] = 1.566, p = .211 |
Trials to complete first category† | 19.17 (12.55) | 20.12 (13.78) | 22.11 (14.45) | F[2,268] = 1.074, p = .343 |
Completion time† | 367.59 (114.45) | 453.84 (152.5) | 418.51 (183.62) | F[2,268] = 8.15, p < .001, A < B* |
Notes:
Raw scores, not adjusted for age.
No longer significant when using age-adjusted completion time.
FET, Fisher’s exact test.
Procedures
In the MDD and tinnitus samples, WCST and pre-treatment (screening) data was collected prior to any treatment commencing, and post-treatment data soon afterwards. In the SAD sample, WCST data and pre-treatment symptom scores were collected after two weeks of initial attention bias modification, and post-treatment symptoms scores after a nine-week CBT program. The two forms of attention bias modification used produced similar results (Boettcher et al., 2014), and were hence considered equal in the current study. To account for the possibility that CF was associated with treatment gains only in certain investigated treatment types, analyses were conducted both using the full sample and group-wise.
Measures
Online Wisconsin Card Sorting Test
An online, in-house developed version of the standard, single-deck version of the WCST (Greve, 2001) featuring 64 response cards, each administered together with four stimulus cards, was completed by all participants prior to commencing treatment (see Fig. 1 for screenshot). Previous research (Axelrod, Paolo & Abraham, 1997) has shown that in clinical settings it is inappropriate to extrapolate norms from the full WCST version to the single-deck version used in the current study. In the WCST, participants are tasked with sorting the response card by matching it to the stimulus card. They are not told about the matching principle (number, color or shape), but are forced to deduce this based upon the feedback received after the last sorting attempt. Number of Perseverative errors (PE), i.e. the continued used of a sorting strategy that was labelled incorrect during the last trial, was used as CF metric. Clinical WCST norm data are age-adjusted, but because the validity of applying foreign, pen-and-paper WCST norms to our online version has not been independently validated, we opted instead to take age into account by regressing out the effect of age on number of PE and using this age-adjusted metric for statistical analyses. Since the current study did not feature a representative, healthy comparison group, percentage PE of total errors was also calculated for each subject and regression analyses repeated using these self-normalized scores.
We assessed the psychometric validity of online WCST by performing a principal component analysis (PCA) to estimate the latent structure of our results and comparing with past findings. A composite two-factor structure has been proposed, derived from a meta-analysis of ten factor analyses in four studies featuring neurological, senior and/or healthy samples (Greve, Ingram & Bianchini, 1998). The proposed composite solution includes Categories completed and Total correct scores loading oppositely to Perseverative errors and Total errors on factor one, with zero loadings on factor two for all but Total correct. Due to unavailable WCST metrics, we were unable to replicate the exact analysis. This notwithstanding, performing a principal component analysis with forced two-component extraction resulted in a structure generally consistent with the proposed composite two-factor solution with regards to component direction and strength. This was despite substantial differences in sample characteristics, suggesting adequate psychometric validity (see Table S1 for more details).
Symptom measures
The primary outcome measures in the studies were the Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale—Self-Report version (Fresco et al., 2001), the Beck Depression Inventory II (Beck, Steer & Brown, 1996), and the Tinnitus Handicap Inventory (Newman, Jacobson & Spitzer, 1996). Raw pre-post score differences, along with percentage symptom reduction (PSR) (D’Alcante et al., 2012) in participants who received active treatment (total n = 204) was calculated. The latter was calculated to provide comparable cross-sample scores. PSR differed between groups [F[2,201] = 15.03, p < .001), such that PSRMDD > PSRTinnitus > PSRSAD (all pair-wise contrast p < 0.027, Bonferroni-corrected).
Statistical analyses
We used two methods to test our hypothesis that cognitive flexibility is associated with treatment gains. First, age-adjusted PE was entered as a predictor of raw pre-treatment symptom scores in standard linear regression models, group-wise. Then, number of PE was used to predict PSR in the full sample and group-wise, and pre-post raw score difference group-wise. These analyses were repeated featuring percentage PE of total errors.
Second, performance of the PE metric in classifying treatment responders vs. low-responders was investigated using receiver-operating characteristics (ROC). Symptom reductions of 25%, 50% and 75% were used as separate thresholds for treatment response. Analyses were conducted both group-wise and featuring the full sample. Bootstrapped 95% confidence intervals for area under the curve (AUC) for each classification curve were used to test whether classification accuracy differed from chance. All analyses were conducted in the R (3.2) statistical environment, using the pROC (Robin et al., 2011) and psych (Revelle, 2015) packages.
Results
Linear associations
There were no significant associations between pre-treatment symptom scores and PE in any group. There were also no significant associations between PE and pre-post difference in symptom scores in any group, or any significant associations between PE and PSR in either the full sample or each group separately (see Table 2). In the SAD group, the positive association between PSR and PE approached significance (p = .065). However, inspection of the scatter plot revealed two PSR outliers more than three standard deviations from the group mean. If these were removed, the correlation was no longer approaching significance (see Fig. 2 for scatterplots). Re-running all analyses using percentage PE of total errors did not change results.
Table 2. Univariate association between perseverative errors and treatment gains.
Sample | n | Outcome | Parameter estimate | Parameter 95% CI | p |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Pre-treatment scores | |||||
Social anxiety disorder | 115 | LSAS-SR | −0.145 | −0.804—0.513 | 0.663 |
Tinnitus | 83 | THI | 0.104 | −0.437—0.645 | 0.703 |
Depression sample | 73 | BDI-II | 0.051 | −0.167—0.270 | 0.640 |
Pre-post score difference | |||||
Social anxiety disorder | 115 | LSAS-SR | −0.284 | −0.800—0.232 | 0.278 |
Tinnitus | 53 | THI | 0.145 | −0.602—0.892 | 0.699 |
Depression sample | 36 | BDI-II | 0.162 | −0.224—0.549 | 0.399 |
Pre-post Percentage Symptom Reduction (PSR) | |||||
Full sample | 204 | PSR | −0.233 | −0.876 —0.410 | 0.476 |
Social anxiety disorder | 115 | PSR | −0.790† | −1.629—0.049† | 0.065† |
Tinnitus | 53 | PSR | 0.148 | −1.081—1.376 | 0.811 |
Depression sample | 36 | PSR | 0.495 | −0.868—1.858 | 0.466 |
Note:
No longer approaching significance (p > .075) after removal of two PSR outliers. Perseverative errors are age-adjusted.
Classification accuracy
Out of the twelve ROC curves investigated (full sample + group-wise × three thresholds for treatment response), there was only one instance when the AUC confidence interval did not cover 0.5. In this case, in the SAD group using the 75% PSR cut-off, only n = 2 scored above the PSR cut-off, leading to an irregular curve shape and a confidence interval ranging 0.51–0.81 (see Fig. 3 for ROC curves).
Discussion
Contrary to our hypothesis, we found no evidence suggesting an association between our measure of pre-treatment CF and post-treatment improvement. This indicates that comparably low CF (measured by number of perseverative errors on the WCST) should not be an obstacle in achieving symptom reduction after Internet-delivered psychological treatment, and that pre-treatment measurements of CF will not predict treatment outcomes. The lack of predictive power is congruent with there being no linear correlation between pre-treatment symptom measures and number of PE.
In the context of traditional psychotherapy, findings on associations between CF and cognitive restructuring skill acquisition, considered important for improvement, have been mixed (Johnco, Wuthrich & Rapee, 2013; Johnco, Wuthrich & Rapee, 2014). We reasoned that because Internet-delivered therapy by design includes less contact with a therapist (if included at all), it requires the patient to have greater CF abilities to independently identify cognitive, behavioral and emotional patterns, and to successfully apply cognitive restructuring techniques to improve their condition. While our study design cannot directly test this assumption, our negative finding, consistent with some prior research on traditional psychotherapy, suggests that even in psychotherapeutic formats that place a greater demand on the patient’s own ability to comprehend and successfully execute cognitive restructuring exercises, there is no association between CF (as measured by the WCST) and treatment gains.
While lower CF is often reported in psychiatric samples, comparably little is known about the precise relationship between CF and psychiatric symptoms. It is noteworthy that even direct training of executive functions (including CF) appears to have little or no generalized effect beyond improved performance on the specific task trained (Snyder, Miyake & Hankin, 2015). Recently, variants of the WCST that feature emotional content (Deveney & Deldin, 2006; Aker & Landrø, 2014) have been developed and validated. Future studies should investigate whether these have an improved ability to differentiate clinical from non-clinical samples and predict psychotherapy outcomes. Future studies featuring larger samples should measure a wider range of executive functions, such as response inhibition and set-updating, and preferably in an affective context. This will likely be required in order to accurately model the complex neurocognitive mechanisms implicated in the manifestation and treatment of mental and psychosomatic disorders, and as a result, predict therapy outcomes with accuracy.
The strengths of our study include a relatively large, well-powered and diverse sample drawn from three different studies on effective treatments of two common psychiatric disorders and one psychosomatic disorder. To control for differences in sample and treatment characteristics, we performed analyses using both the entire sample and the individual groups. Although originally designed to measure the cognitive effects of frontal lobe damage, the WCST is considered a valid and established measure of PE. We age-adjusted PE scores, and re-running analyses using self-normalized number of PE (i.e. percentage PE of total errors) did not change findings. Our study design also has some limitations. Primarily, only one measure of CF, number of perseverative errors on the WCST, was used for analyses. While we acknowledge that a composite measure calculated using results from several CF-indexing neuropsychological tests would have been preferable, past studies have reported associations between this particular measure (number of PE on the WCST) and cognitive restructuring abilities (Johnco, Wuthrich & Rapee, 2013). Second, the in-house developed, online 64-card WCST version used in the current study has not been independently validated against other measures. However, principal component analysis revealed a latent structure similar to past reports (Greve, Ingram & Bianchini, 1998) and nothing in the scoring pattern suggests abnormal functioning of our version. Third, the lack of a representative healthy comparison group limits our ability to conclude whether pre-treatment CF in our clinical samples was lower than population norms. Overall WCST performance in our sample appears to be lower than previous reports of healthy Western samples (Kohli & Kaur, 2006), yet the lack of linear associations between symptom scores and number of PE suggests that reduced CF is not an integral component of these conditions. Finally, we did not include measures of cognitive restructuring skill acquisition or utilization, which could be potential mediators in the relationship between pre-treatment CF and treatment gains.
Conclusions
Contrary to our hypothesis, we found no generic or treatment-specific predicative power of cognitive flexibility (perseverative errors on the WCST) on patient improvement following Internet-delivered psychological treatment for social anxiety disorder, depression or tinnitus. Hence, comparably lower cognitive flexibility, as measured by the perseverative errors on the WCST, should not impede successful outcomes in Internet-delivered psychological treatments.
Supplemental Information
Funding Statement
The authors received no funding for this work.
Additional Information and Declarations
Competing Interests
Gerhard Andersson is an Academic Editor for PeerJ.
Author Contributions
Philip Lindner conceived and designed the experiments, analyzed the data, wrote the paper, prepared figures and/or tables, reviewed drafts of the paper.
Per Carlbring conceived and designed the experiments, reviewed drafts of the paper.
Erik Flodman performed the experiments, reviewed drafts of the paper.
Amanda Hebert performed the experiments, reviewed drafts of the paper.
Stephanie Poysti performed the experiments, reviewed drafts of the paper.
Filip Hagkvist reviewed drafts of the paper, made significant contributions to interpretation of findings and suggested methods.
Robert Johansson conceived and designed the experiments, performed the experiments, reviewed drafts of the paper.
Vendela Zetterqvist Westin conceived and designed the experiments, performed the experiments, reviewed drafts of the paper.
Thomas Berger conceived and designed the experiments, contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools, reviewed drafts of the paper.
Gerhard Andersson conceived and designed the experiments, reviewed drafts of the paper.
Human Ethics
The following information was supplied relating to ethical approvals (i.e., approving body and any reference numbers):
Social anxiety disorder sample: Regional Ethics Board of Umeå, Sweden (2012/132-31Ö); Major depression sample: Regional Ethics Board of Linköping, Sweden (2010/386-31); Tinnitus sample: Regional Ethics Board of Linköping, Sweden (2008/234-08).
Data Deposition
The following information was supplied regarding data availability:
The raw data can only be made available on request due to restrictions of the ethical review board which did not include permission to make the data publicly available. Please contact Philip Lindner with your requests: philip.lindner@psychology.su.se.
References
- Aker & Landrø (2014).Aker M, Landrø NI. Executive control of emotional processing: a set-shifting task. The Clinical Neuropsychologist. 2014;28(8):1311–1320. doi: 10.1080/13854046.2014.984762. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andersson (2009).Andersson G. Using the Internet to provide cognitive behaviour therapy. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2009;47(3):175–180. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2009.01.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andersson et al. (2013).Andersson G, Carlbring P, Ljótsson B, Hedman E. Guided internet-based CBT for common mental disorders. Journal of Contemporary Psychotherapy. 2013;43(4):223–233. doi: 10.1007/s10879-013-9237-9. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Andersson (2014).Andersson G. The Internet and CBT: A Clinical Guide. Boca Raton: CRC Press; 2014. [Google Scholar]
- Andersson, Carlbring & Grimlund (2008).Andersson G, Carlbring P, Grimlund A. Predicting treatment outcome in internet versus face to face treatment of panic disorder. Computers in Human Behavior. 2008;24(5):1790–1801. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2008.02.003. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Andersson & McKenna (2006).Andersson G, McKenna L. The role of cognition in tinnitus. Acta Oto-Laryngologica. 2006;126(Supplement 556):39–43. doi: 10.1080/03655230600895226. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Axelrod, Paolo & Abraham (1997).Axelrod BN, Paolo AM, Abraham E. Do normative data from the full WCST extend to the abbreviated WCST? Assessment. 1997;4(1):41–46. doi: 10.1177/107319119700400105. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Beck, Steer & Brown (1996).Beck AT, Steer RA, Brown GK. Beck Depression Inventory-II. San Antonio: Psychological Corporation; 1996. [Google Scholar]
- Boettcher et al. (2014).Boettcher J, Hasselrot J, Sund E, Andersson G, Carlbring P. Combining attention training with internet-based cognitive-behavioural self-help for social anxiety: a randomised controlled trial. Cognitive Behaviour Therapy. 2014;43(1):34–48. doi: 10.1080/16506073.2013.809141. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- D’Alcante et al. (2012).D’Alcante CC, Diniz JB, Fossaluza V, Batistuzzo MC, Lopes AC, Shavitt RG, Deckersbach T, Malloy-Diniz L, Miguel EC, Hoexter MQ. Neuropsychological predictors of response to randomized treatment in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry. 2012;39(2):310–317. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2012.07.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dehaene & Changeux (1991).Dehaene S, Changeux JP. The Wisconsin Card Sorting Test: theoretical analysis and modeling in a neuronal network. Cerebral Cortex. 1991;1(1):62–79. doi: 10.1093/cercor/1.1.62. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deveney & Deldin (2006).Deveney CM, Deldin PJ. A preliminary investigation of cognitive flexibility for emotional information in major depressive disorder and non-psychiatric controls. Emotion. 2006;6(3):429–437. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.6.3.429. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fresco et al. (2001).Fresco DM, Coles ME, Heimberg RG, Liebowitz MR, Hami S, Stein MB, Goetz D. The Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale: a comparison of the psychometric properties of self-report and clinician-administered formats. Psychological Medicine. 2001;31(6):1025–1035. doi: 10.1017/S0033291701004056. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Greve (2001).Greve KW. The WCST-64: a standardized short-form of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. The Clinical Neuropsychologist. 2001;15(2):228–234. doi: 10.1076/clin.15.2.228.1901. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Greve, Ingram & Bianchini (1998).Greve KW, Ingram F, Bianchini KJ. Latent structure of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test in a clinical sample. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology. 1998;13(7):597–609. doi: 10.1093/arclin/13.7.597. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hedman et al. (2012).Hedman E, Andersson E, Ljótsson B, Andersson G, Schalling M, Lindefors N, Rück C. Clinical and genetic outcome determinants of Internet- and group-based cognitive behavior therapy for social anxiety disorder. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica. 2012;126(2):126–136. doi: 10.1111/acps.2012.126.issue-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hesser et al. (2012).Hesser H, Gustafsson T, Lundén C, Henrikson O, Fattahi K, Johnsson E, Westin VZ, Carlbring P, Mäki-Torkko E, Kaldo V, Andersson G. A randomized controlled trial of internet-delivered cognitive behavior therapy and acceptance and commitment therapy in the treatment of tinnitus. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2012;80(4):649–661. doi: 10.1037/a0027021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johansson et al. (2012).Johansson R, Ekbladh S, Hebert A, Lindström M, Möller S, Petitt E, Poysti S, Larsson MH, Rousseau A, Carlbring P, Cuijpers P, Andersson G. Psychodynamic guided self-help for adult depression through the internet: a randomised controlled trial. PLoS ONE. 2012;7(5):e1934. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0038021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnco, Wuthrich & Rapee (2013).Johnco C, Wuthrich VM, Rapee RM. The role of cognitive flexibility in cognitive restructuring skill acquisition among older adults. Journal of Anxiety Disorders. 2013;27(6):576–584. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2012.10.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnco, Wuthrich & Rapee (2014).Johnco C, Wuthrich VM, Rapee RM. The influence of cognitive flexibility on treatment outcome and cognitive restructuring skill acquisition during cognitive behavioural treatment for anxiety and depression in older adults: results of a pilot study. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2014;57C(1):55–64. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2014.04.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kohli & Kaur (2006).Kohli A, Kaur M. Wisconsin Card Sorting Test: normative data and experience. Indian Journal of Psychiatry. 2006;48(3):181. doi: 10.4103/0019-5545.31582. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Miyake et al. (2000).Miyake A, Friedman NP, Emerson MJ, Witzki AH, Howerter A, Wager TD. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “Frontal Lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology. 2000;41(1):49–100. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1999.0734. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mohlman & Gorman (2005).Mohlman J, Gorman JM. The role of executive functioning in CBT: a pilot study with anxious older adults. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2005;43(4):447–465. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2004.03.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moritz et al. (2005).Moritz S, Kloss M, Jacobsen D, Fricke S, Cuttler C, Brassen S, Hand I. Neurocognitive impairment does not predict treatment outcome in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2005;43(6):811–819. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2004.06.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Newman, Jacobson & Spitzer (1996).Newman CW, Jacobson GP, Spitzer JB. Development of the Tinnitus Handicap Inventory. Archives of Otolaryngology–Head & Neck Surgery. 1996;122(2):143–148. doi: 10.1001/archotol.1996.01890140029007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nordgreen et al. (2012).Nordgreen T, Havik OE, Ost LG, Furmark T, Carlbring P, Andersson G. Outcome predictors in guided and unguided self-help for social anxiety disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2012;50(1):13–21. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2011.10.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Revelle (2015).Revelle W. psych: procedures for psychological, psychometric, and personality research. 2015 Available at http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=psych .
- Robin et al. (2011).Robin X, Turck N, Hainard A, Tiberti N, Lisacek F, Sanchez J-C, Müller M. pROC: an open-source package for R and S+ to analyze and compare ROC curves. BMC Bioinformatics. 2011;12:77. doi: 10.1186/1471-2105-12-77. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Snyder, Miyake & Hankin (2015).Snyder HR, Miyake A, Hankin BL. Advancing understanding of executive function impairments and psychopathology: bridging the gap between clinical and cognitive approaches. Frontiers in Psychology. 2015;6:328. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00328. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.