Abstract
The extent to which epistasis affects the genetic architecture of complex traits is difficult to quantify, and identifying variants in natural populations with epistatic interactions is challenging. Previous studies in Drosophila implicated extensive epistasis between variants in genes that affect neural connectivity and contribute to natural variation in olfactory response to benzaldehyde. In this study, we implemented a powerful screen to quantify the extent of epistasis as well as identify candidate interacting variants using 203 inbred wild‐derived lines with sequenced genomes of the Drosophila melanogaster Genetic Reference Panel (DGRP). We crossed the DGRP lines to P[GT1]‐element insertion mutants in Sema‐5c and neuralized (neur), two neurodevelopmental loci which affect olfactory behavior, and to their coisogenic wild‐type control. We observed significant variation in olfactory responses to benzaldehyde among F1 genotypes and for the DGRP line by mutant genotype interactions for both loci, showing extensive nonadditive genetic variation. We performed genome‐wide association analyses to identify the candidate modifier loci. None of these polymorphisms were in or near the focal genes; therefore, epistasis is the cause of the nonadditive genetic variance. Candidate genes could be placed in interaction networks. Several candidate modifiers are associated with neural development. Analyses of mutants of candidate epistatic partners with neur (merry‐go‐round (mgr), prospero (pros), CG10098, Alhambra (Alh) and CG12535) and Sema‐5c (CG42540 and bruchpilot (brp)) showed aberrant olfactory responses compared with coisogenic controls. Thus, integrating genome‐wide analyses of natural variants with mutations at defined genomic locations in a common coisogenic background can unmask specific epistatic modifiers of behavioral phenotypes.
Keywords: Chemosensation, Drosophila melanogaster Genetic Reference Panel, genetic architecture, genome‐wide association study, quantitative traits
The potential for adaptive evolution depends on genetic variation within a population. How such variation is maintained remains an enigma (Charlesworth 2015; Félix & Barkoulas 2015; Mackay 2010). Waddington (1942) proposed that organisms harbor a reservoir of hidden (cryptic) genetic variation, which can be mobilized as a consequence of environmental changes or genetic perturbations (Gibson & Dworkin 2004; Waddington 1942). Furthermore, it has been proposed that such modifiers can buffer the genome against newly arising mutations through suppressing epistasis (Mackay 2014; Swarup et al. 2012; Yamamoto et al. 2009). Identifying epistasis is challenging when neither the genetic background nor environmental conditions can be controlled. In addition, the magnitude of the effects of epistasis on quantitative trait phenotypes depends on the allele frequencies of the interacting genes (Mackay 2014). Thus, changes in allele frequencies between interacting partners that give rise to nonlinear or even antagonistic phenotypic effects can account for irreproducible results in genome‐wide association (GWA) studies in human populations, and suppressing epistasis can contribute to the ‘missing heritability’ of quantitative traits (Manolio et al. 2009).
Epistasis can be more readily detected in genetic model organisms in which crosses between mutations can be made and for which allele frequencies are balanced in quantitative trait locus (QTL) mapping populations (Mackay 2014). Indeed, significant epistatic interactions have been found in QTL‐mapping studies on sporulation efficiency (Deutschbauer & Davis 2005; Gerke et al. 2009) and gene expression (Brem et al. 2005) in yeast; thermal preference in Caenorhabditis elegans (Gaertner et al. 2012); bristle number (Dilda & Mackay 2002), wing shape (Chandler et al. 2014; Gilchrist & Partridge 2001), longevity (Magwire et al. 2010), enzyme activity (Clark & Wang 1997), metabolic rate and flight velocity (Montooth et al. 2003) and locomotor behaviors (van Swinderen & Greenspan 2005; Yamamoto et al. 2008) in Drosophila melanogaster; body weight and adiposity traits (Cheverud et al. 2001; Jarvis & Cheverud 2011; Leamy et al. 2011; Stylianou et al. 2006), litter size (Peripato et al. 2004) and production of serum‐like growth factor‐1 (Hanlon et al. 2006) in mice; growth rate in chickens (Carlborg et al. 2006; Pettersson et al. 2011); growth rate (Kroymann & Mitchell‐Olds 2005; Wentzel et al. 2007) and metabolites (Rowe et al. 2008) in Arabidopsis thaliana; and, differences in whole plant and inflorescence architecture between maize and teosinte (Doebley et al. 1995). Furthermore, studies on phenotypic variation in D. melanogaster have inferred pervasive epistasis for recovery time from a chill‐induced coma, and starvation stress resistance, as well as startle, olfactory and aggressive behavior (Huang et al. 2012; Shorter et al. 2015; Swarup et al. 2013). Despite evidence that epistasis is an important feature of the genetic architecture of complex traits, genome‐wide identification of specific genes that engage in epistatic interactions with defined target genes remains a challenge.
Here, we describe an experimental design that enables us to identify epistatic modifiers of defined target alleles using the Drosophila melanogaster Genetic Reference Panel (DGRP; Mackay et al. 2012; Huang et al. 2014). The DGRP consists of 205 inbred wild‐derived D. melanogaster lines with fully sequenced genomes, in which all 4853802 single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) and 1296080 non‐SNP variants (insertions, deletions and copy number variants) as well as 16 polymorphic inversions have been identified (Huang et al. 2014). We previously inferred that epistasis is an important component of the genetic architecture of olfactory behavior, both for natural variation in the DGRP (Swarup et al. 2013) as well as for new mutations (Fedorowicz et al. 1998; Sambandan et al. 2006). Two of these mutants with large effects on olfactory behavior had P[GT1]‐insertions at the neurogenic Sema‐5c and neuralized (neur) loci and were characterized in greater detail (Rollmann et al. 2007,2008).
Here, we asked whether and to what extent the insertional mutations at neur and Sema‐5c genetically interact with variants affecting olfactory behavior in the DGRP by performing quantitative complementation tests (Long et al. 1996; Mackay 2014). We crossed 203 DGRP lines to the neur and Sema‐5c mutant alleles as well as to Canton S(B), the coisogenic line in which they were induced (Norga et al. 2003). We evaluated the extent to which the mutant alleles had the same effect in the DGRP lines, and found that nonadditive genetic variance accounted for over 70% of the total genetic variance for each mutation. We then performed GWA analysis and showed that none of the top interacting variants were in or near the focal genes; therefore, the nonadditive genetic variance is attributable to epistasis and not dominance. We identified polymorphisms in 24 and 31 candidate genes that contribute to variation in epistatic interactions with Sema‐5c and neur, respectively. We show that these genes can be used to construct interaction networks in which additional putative epistatic partners can be computationally recruited. We also show that several candidate epistatic partners of the target genes also affect the behavioral phenotype. Thus, generating heterozygotes between defined transposon insertion mutants and natural variants in controlled genetic backgrounds is an effective strategy for genome‐wide identification of candidate epistatic modifiers.
Material and methods
Fly stocks
All flies were reared on cornmeal–molasses–agar medium at 25°C, 60–75% relative humidity and a 12‐h light/dark cycle. Males of 203 DGRP lines (Huang et al. 2014) were crossed to females of homozygous Sema‐5cBG2386 and neurBG2391 P[GT1]‐element insertion lines in the Canton S(B) coisogenic background (Bellen et al. 2004; Lukacsovich et al. 2001) as well as to Canton S(B) coisogenic females. To analyze effects of candidate epistatic modifier genes on olfactory behavior, we obtained homozygous mutant lines of nine genes from the Exelixis collection (Thibault et al. 2004) and the corresponding coisogenic control line. We obtained a UAS‐RNAi line for bruchpilot (brp) along with the isogenic progenitor w1118 control from the Vienna Drosophila Resource Center and crossed these to an ubiquitin‐GAL4 driver line (w1118; Ubi‐Gal4) backcrossed in the Canton S(B) background.
Behavioral assay
Benzaldehyde was obtained from Sigma‐Aldrich, Inc. (St. Louis, MO, USA). Olfactory behavior to 1% (v/v) benzaldehyde was measured between 1300 and 1600 h for five replicate groups of fifty 4‐ to 7‐day‐old flies, males and females separately, for the homozygous Sema‐5cBG2386 and neurBG2391 P[GT1]‐element insertion lines and Canton S(B) coisogenic control, and for three replicate sets of fifty 4‐ to 7‐day‐old females per replicate of the F1 progenies of DGRP lines crossed with Sema‐5cBG2386, neurBG2391 and Canton S(B), using a high throughput modification of the classical dipstick assay (Anholt et al. 1996), exactly as described previously (Swarup et al. 2011). In this assay, response indices are computed after flies have been allowed to distribute between two interconnected tubes, either near or away from the odorant source, where the response index (RI) = number of flies in the collection tube/total number of flies. RI = 1 indicates a maximal aversive response to the odorant, RI = 0.5 indicates indifference to the odorant, and RI = 0 indicates that all flies remain near the odor source. Replicates were run on different days to randomize environmental variation. For functional validation of candidate epistatic modifier genes, we quantified olfactory responses for at least five replicates of females from Exelixis mutants and 10 replicates of ubiquitin‐GAL4 x UAS‐brpRNAi females.
Statistical analyses and genome‐wide associations
To analyze variation in olfactory behavior among the DGRP lines, we ran ANOVA models of form: Y = μ + L + G + L × G + E, where L (random) denotes DGRP lines; G (fixed) denotes either the Sema‐5c and Canton S(B) genotypes or the neur and Canton S(B) genotypes; L × G (random) is the line by genotype interaction term (i.e. denotes nonadditive effects) and E is the within‐line variance. To evaluate the effects of mutations or RNAi knockdown of candidate genes on olfactory behavior, we performed Dunnett's tests compared with the appropriate controls.
We performed GWA analyses using DGRP freeze 2.0 sequences using the pipeline from the DGRP website (http://dgrp2.gnets.ncsu.edu/). Briefly, this analysis evaluates the strength of association of alternative DGRP alleles with quantitative trait phenotypes for each segregating variant, after accounting for any effects of Wolbachia infection, karyotype of common polymorphic inversions and polygenic relatedness. We performed GWA analyses separately for DGRP/Sema‐5c, DGRP/neur and the difference between DGRP/Canton S(B) and DGRP/Sema‐5c and DGRP/Canton S(B) and DGRP/neur F1 females. The latter two analyses specifically test for variants with nonadditive effects because the variation in the difference between the control and mutant phenotypes is equivalent to the genotype by line interaction term. All analyses were performed using genotype means. Gene interaction networks were constructed using GeneMANIA (Warde‐Farley et al. 2010).
Results
Nonadditive variance for olfactory behavior
Previous studies identified P‐element insertion mutants that affect olfactory avoidance behavior in response to benzaldehyde (Anholt et al. 1996; Sambandan et al. 2006) and showed extensive epistatic interactions among them (Fedorowicz et al. 1998; Sambandan et al. 2006). Two of these P‐element mutants at the Sema‐5c and neur loci had large effects on olfactory behavior. The Sema‐5c mutant also affected startle response (Rollmann et al. 2007; Yamamoto et al. 2008) and the neur mutant had pleiotropic effects on startle behavior and aggression (Rollmann et al. 2008; Yamamoto et al. 2008). Both mutations showed structural alterations in the mushroom bodies of the brain (Rollmann et al. 2007; Rollmann et al. 2008).
We first confirmed the effects of the homozygous Sema‐5cBG2386 and neurBG2391 P[GT1]‐element insertion lines on olfactory behavior compared with the coisogenic parental Canton S(B) strain. The Canton S(B) control showed strong avoidance behavior to 1% (v/v) benzaldehyde, as expected (Sambandan et al. 2006), while behavioral responses were greatly reduced in the Sema‐5cBG2386 and neurBG2391 mutants, confirming the impairment of the behavioral phenotype described previously. We estimated the homozygous (a) and heterozygous (d) effects of the mutations in the Canton S(B) genetic background, where a is one half of the difference between the mean olfactory behavior of the homozygous Canton S(B) and mutant genotypes and d is the difference between the mean olfactory behavior of the heterozygotes and the average of the two homozygotes (Falconer & Mackay 1996). The effect of the Sema‐5c mutation is additive in females (a = 0.1336; d = 0.0208) and nearly recessive in males (a = 0.1108; d = 0.0888) and the effect of the neur mutation is semi‐dominant in both females (a = 0.1176; d = −0.0815) and males (a = 0.0827; d = −0.0675) (Fig. 1a).
Figure 1.
Genome‐wide screen for nonadditive modifiers of the Sema‐5C and neur loci in the DGRP. (a) Behavioral responses to benzaldehyde of female (black bars) and male (gray bars) control Canton S(B) flies and coisogenic Sema‐5c and neur mutants. Error bars represent standard errors of the mean. Statistically significant differences (P < 0.05) for each sex are determined by Tukey's test and indicated by letters above the bars, where different letters denote significantly different line means. (b) Schematic representation of the crossing scheme for genome‐wide identification of candidate nonadditive modifiers. (c) Response indices for behavioral responses to 1% (v/v) benzaldehyde of F1 hybrids from 203 DGRP lines crossed to Canton S(B), indicated by red symbols, and F1 hybrids from 203 DGRP lines crossed to the Sema‐5cBG2386 mutant, indicated by the blue symbols. Differences between the control and mutant F1s are indicated by black lines that connect the corresponding red and blue symbols. (d) Response indices for behavioral responses to 1% (v/v) benzaldehyde of F1 hybrids from 203 DGRP lines crossed to Canton S (B), indicated by red symbols, and F1 hybrids from 203 DGRP lines crossed to the neurBG2391 mutant, indicated by the blue symbols. Differences between the control and mutant F1s are indicated by black lines that connect the corresponding red and blue symbols.
To identify naturally occurring modifiers of the Sema‐5cBG2386 and neurBG2391 alleles with nonadditive effects, we crossed each mutant and the control to each of the DGRP lines and tested the F1 offspring for olfactory behavior (Fig. 1b). To simplify the experimental design and facilitate identification of modifiers on the X chromosome, we restricted our screen to F1 females. We observed significant phenotypic variation across the DGRP lines for the Canton S(B), Sema‐5cBG2386 and neurBG2391 crosses (Fig. 1c,d; Tables 1 and S1, Supporting Information) with average response index scores generally indicative of avoidance behavior (RI > 0.5).
Table 1.
Analyses of variance of DGRP lines crossed to Canton S(B) and coisogenic P‐element insertional mutations (neur BG2391 and Sema‐5C BG2389)
F1 genotype | Source | df | SS × 103 | MS × 103 | F | P‐Value | σ 2 × 104 | SE × 104 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
DGRP/Canton S(B) | Line | 202 | 807.192 | 3.996 | 2.06 | <0.0001 | 5.827 | 1.149 |
Residual | 524 | 1014.988 | 1.937 | 19.234 | 1.179 | |||
Total | 726 | 1822.18 | 25.061 | |||||
DGRP/neur BG2391 | Line | 202 | 1568.732 | 7.766 | 1.9 | <0.0001 | 10.196 | 2.35 |
Residual | 525 | 2140.425 | 4.077 | 41.098 | 2.55 | |||
Total | 727 | 3709.157 | 51.294 | |||||
DGRP/ Sema‐5C BG2389 | Line | 202 | 1954.522 | 9.676 | 2.81 | <0.0001 | 17.19 | 2.84 |
Residual | 512 | 1760.256 | 3.438 | 34.405 | 2.15 | |||
Total | 714 | 3714.778 | 51.595 | |||||
DGRP/Canton S(B) & DGRP/neur BG2391 | Genotype | 1 | 237.965 | 237.965 | 47.35 | <0.0001 | Fixed | Fixed |
Line | 203 | 1344.442 | 6.623 | 1.30 | 0.0305 | 2.205 | 1.188 | |
Genotype × Line | 201 | 1022.676 | 5.088 | 1.69 | <0.0001 | 5.791 | 1.512 | |
Residual | 1049 | 3155.591 | 3.008 | 30.185 | 1.321 | |||
Total | 1454 | 5760.674 | 38.181 | |||||
DGRP/Canton S(B) & DGRP/ Sema‐5C BG2389 | Genotype | 1 | 91.405 | 91.405 | 12.98 | 0.0004 | Fixed | Fixed |
Line | 203 | 2116.264 | 10.425 | 1.46 | 0.0035 | 3.097 | 1.396 | |
Genotype × Line | 201 | 1431.433 | 7.122 | 1.89 | <0.0001 | 8.614 | 1.681 | |
Residual | 1037 | 3900.884 | 3.762 | 26.789 | 1.176 | |||
Total | 1442 | 7539.986 | 38.500 |
df, degrees of freedom; MS, type III mean squares; F, F statistic; σ 2, variance component; SE, standard error of variance component.
We performed analyses of variance to partition the variation in olfactory behavior into sources attributable to the main effect of Genotype (Canton S(B) and the mutation), DGRP Line and the Genotype by Line interaction term (Table 1). We observed significant effects of Genotype in both analyses, but this term only explained 4.13% and 1.21% of the total sums of squares for neur and Sema‐5c, respectively. We observed significant genetic variation in olfactory behavior in both comparisons, with the total genetic variance accounting for 20.9% and 30.4% of the phenotypic variance for neur and Sema‐5c, respectively. With additive gene action, we expect that the differences in phenotypic values between the Canton S(B) × DGRP crosses and the mutant × DGRP crosses would be constant; i.e. there would be no variation in the difference between the DGRP/Canton S(B) and DGRP/mutation genotypes, as assessed by the significance of the Genotype by Line interaction terms. However, this is not what we observed. There is extensive variation in the difference between these phenotypic values, which is evidence of nonadditivity (Fig. 1c,d; Tables 1 and S1). Remarkably, 72.4% and 73.5% of the total genetic variance in olfactory behavior is attributable to naturally occurring variation with nonadditive effects on neur and Sema‐5c, respectively (Table 1).
GWA analysis for nonadditive modifiers
The nonadditive genetic variance in olfactory behavior in the two mutant backgrounds could be due to alleles in neur or Sema‐5c causing variation in the degree of dominance at these loci (allelic failure to complement) or by unlinked modifiers of the focal loci (epistatic failure to complement). As the DGRP lines are sequenced, we can distinguish between these two possibilities by performing GWA analyses. The limited number of DGRP lines and large number of polymorphic markers (∼2.5 million) prevents GWA from reaching significance at a strict Bonferroni‐corrected threshold unless effects are large. Therefore, we used a nominal significance threshold of P < 10−5 to report associations, based on the appearance of deviations from the linear expectation of quantile–quantile plots in this P‐value range (Fig. S1). When we performed GWA analyses across the DGRP lines crossed to Sema‐5cBG2386 and neurBG2391, we identified 53 and 80 candidate genes, respectively, that harbor polymorphisms associated with variation in olfactory response to benzaldehyde (Tables S2 and S3). These candidate genes were different for Sema‐5cBG2386 and neurBG2391, with only dunce (dnc) in common between the two analyses.
Next, we correlated phenotypic differences between the control and mutant DGRP crosses with genome‐wide DNA sequence variation to identify candidate genes representing nonadditive modifiers of the Sema‐5cBG2386 and neurBG2391 alleles (Fig. 2). Associations observed across mutant sensitized DGRP backgrounds are confounded by main effects and possible epistatic interactions, whereas analyses of the differences between the phenotypic effects of the DGRP × mutant hybrids and their DGRP × Canton S (B) reflect only epistasis. We identified 39 polymorphisms, including 36 biallelic SNPs and three indels, in 24 genes that modify the effects of the Sema‐5cBG2386 allele at P < 10−5 (Fig. 2a; Table S4). These genes include brp, involved with maintenance of the presynaptic active zone for neurotransmitter release (Wagh et al. 2006). We also identified SNPs in chemosensory receptors, notably an intronic SNP in Gr39a and a synonymous SNP in Ir56c; a nonsynonymous SNP in Tetraspanin 39D and several predicted genes of unknown function. We identified 78 polymorphisms, including 66 biallelic SNPs, 3 multiple nucleotide polymorphisms (MNPs), and 9 indels in 31 genes that modify the effects of the neurBG2391 allele (Fig. 2b; Table S5). The associated genes include dunce, which encodes a cyclic AMP phosphodiesterase (Byers et al. 1981); Dop1R2, which encodes a dopamine receptor; rickets (rk), which encodes a neuropeptide receptor for the molting hormone bursicon (Loveall & Deitcher 2010), two long noncoding RNAs, two microRNAs (mir‐968 and mir‐1002), and several predicted genes of unknown function.
Figure 2.
Manhattan plots for genome‐wide associations for differences in olfactory behavior for F1 hybrids from 203 DGRP lines crossed to Canton S(B) and Sema‐5cBG2386 (a) or Canton S(B) and the neurBG2391 mutant (b). The X‐axes represent the color‐coded major chromosome arms of the Drosophila genome and the Y‐axes the probability for association. The horizontal line marks the P < 10−5 nominal reporting threshold.
Seven candidate genes with polymorphisms that were associated with phenotypic variation in olfactory behavior among the DGRP × Sema‐5c hybrids were also significantly associated when we performed the GWA on the difference between the DGRP × Canton S (B) control and DGRP × Sema‐5c, namely numb, Aats‐val, Kdm4B,Toll‐6, CG42540, CG42684 and RhoGAP19D. In addition, 21 candidate genes with polymorphisms that were associated with phenotypic variation in olfactory behavior among the DGRP × neur hybrids were also significantly associated when we performed the GWA on the difference between the DGRP × Canton S (B) control and DGRP × neur, including RluA‐2, CR44216, CG10019, CG42669, CG13800, mod(mdg4), CG6074, DNApol‐alpha73, CG17991, DopR2, Ref1, Alh, CG10098, CR42738, CG14610, CG2656, pros, mgr, mRpL40, Sirt4 and dnc.
Notably, none of the top (P < 10−5) variants associated with nonadditive modifiers of Sema‐5c and neur were located in or near these genes. Therefore, it appears that the nonadditive variance we observe is primarily because of epistatic modifiers; variants contributing to any nonadditive variance from variation in the degree of dominance cannot be detected at the reporting significance threshold.
Genetic networks of modifier loci
We used the GeneMANIA program (www.genemania.org; Warde‐Farley et al. 2010) to explore to what extent candidate epistatic modifier genes for the Sema‐5cBG2386 and neurBG2391 alleles identified by our GWA analyses form interactive networks, using as input all candidate modifier genes with nominally significant associations at P < 10−5 (Table S4 and S5). This program visualizes physical interactions among gene products, genetic interactions, and coexpression among candidate genes. These analyses show that candidate genes that epistatically modify the effects of Sema‐5cBG2386 (Fig. 3) and neurBG2391 (Fig. 4) in our GWA analyses participate in known genetic and physical interaction networks as well as gene coexpression networks with the focal genes and each other. These networks also recruit additional genes that interact physically or genetically or are coexpressed with these candidate genes, or Sema‐5c or neur. The interaction network for Sema‐5c contains 91 coregulated pairs, 23 pairs of physically interacting gene products and 13 known genetic interactions (Fig. 3), whereas the interaction network for neur contains 59 coregulated pairs, 19 pairs of physically interacting gene products and 10 known genetic interactions (Fig. 4).
Figure 3.
Genetic interaction network for Sema‐5c derived from GWA analyses of the differences between Canton S(B) and DGRP crosses and corresponding Sema‐5cBG2386 and DGRP crosses. The focal gene is indicated on a red background. Candidate epistatic partners identified by our GWA analysis are indicated on a black background. Computationally recruited interacting partners are indicated on a gray background. Purple edges represent coexpressed pairs, red edges physically interacting pairs, and green edges known genetic interactions.
Figure 4.
Genetic interaction network for neur derived from GWA analyses of the differences between Canton S(B) and DGRP crosses and corresponding neurBG2391 and DGRP crosses. The focal gene is indicated on a red background. Candidate epistatic partners identified by our GWA analysis are indicated on a black background. Computationally recruited interacting partners are indicated on a gray background. Purple edges represent coexpressed pairs, red edges physically interacting pairs and green edges known genetic interactions.
To further explore the functional significance of candidate epistatic partners, we asked whether mutations in candidate genes that were identified in our GWA analyses, showed coregulated expression with the target gene and were homozygous viable also altered olfactory behavior. We tested olfactory responses of females of coisogenic Exelixis mutants and their control at 0.3% (v/v) benzaldehyde, which was a maximally discriminating concentration for this genetic background. The selected genes, which were connected in the GeneMANIA networks and have coregulated expression with Sema‐5c, included CG42684, CG34408, CG42540 and brp (Fig. 5a) and selected genes for behavioral testing for neur included mgr, pros, CG10098, Alh, CG12535 and dnc (Fig. 5b). pros and mgr are located in close proximity and opposite orientation and the associated SNP (3R_7233525) is located in the upstream regions of both genes. The polymorphic marker at 3R_2931095 is a deletion in Alh associated with variation in olfactory behavior in DGRP × neurBG2391 F1 females. A second associated SNP in Alh is upstream of this gene and of CG10098.
Figure 5.
Reduced networks of candidate epistatic partners for Sema‐5c (a) and neur (b). The focal genes are indicated on a red background. Candidate epistatic partners are indicated on a black background. Interconnecting genes in the network that were not identified by our GWA analyses are indicated on a gray background. Red borders indicate genes that were functionally validated with RNAi or mutants. Blue edges indicate coexpression and the green edge indicates a known genetic interaction. The dotted green edges indicate previously unknown epistatic interactions postulated from our GWA results.
A mutant of CG42540, a candidate epistatic modifier of Sema‐5c, showed aberrant olfactory behavior compared with the coisogenic control (Fig. 5a; Table 2). The presynaptic gene product encoded by brp was of special interest, but the brp Exelixis mutant was not homozygous viable. However, knockdown of brp expression with RNAi resulted in a statistically significant effect on olfactory behavior compared with the control (Fig. 5a; Table 2). Mutants of five of the six tested genes (mgr, pros, CG10098, Alh, dnc and CG12535) implicated as modifiers of neur showed aberrant olfactory behavior compared with the control (Fig. 5b; Table 2). Thus, a total of eight out of the ten candidate epistatic partner genes tested were themselves functionally implicated in modulating the behavioral phenotype.
Table 2.
Functional effects of candidate epistatic partners on olfactory behavior
Focal gene | Candidate epistatic partner | P‐value | n |
---|---|---|---|
neur | Alh | 0.0010* | 5 |
neur | CG10098 | 0.0065* | 5 |
neur | CG12535 | 0.0104* | 5 |
neur | dnc | 0.0678 | 3 |
neur | mgr | <.0001* | 2 |
neur | pros | <.0001* | 5 |
Sema‐5C | brp | 0.0005* | 10 |
Sema‐5C | CG34408 | 0.9880 | 5 |
Sema‐5C | CG42540 | 0.0499* | 5 |
Sema‐5C | CG42684 | 1.0000 | 5 |
P‐values for olfactory responses of mutants vs. control by Dunnett's test.
Discussion
There is growing evidence that epistatic interactions contribute to the genetic architecture of complex traits (Carlborg et al. 2006; Chandler et al. 2014; Chari & Dworkin 2013; Clark & Wang 1997; Doebley et al. 1995; Gaertner et al. 2012; Gibson & Dworkin 2004; Hanlon et al. 2006; Huang et al. 2012; Jarvis & Cheverud 2011; Kroymann & Mitchell‐Olds 2005; Leamy et al. 2011; Mackay 2014; Ober et al. 2015; Pettersson et al. 2011; Rowe et al. 2008; Swarup et al. 2013). Pervasive epistasis adds an additional layer of complexity to the genotype‐phenotype relationship. Thus, epistatic interactions can confound interpretations of causality in human GWA studies, and suppressing epistasis may be a contributing factor to the ‘missing heritability’ conundrum (Manolio et al. 2009; Zuk et al. 2012).
Epistatic modifiers are challenging to detect in human populations because of ‘statistical noise’ contributed by heterogeneous genetic backgrounds and uncontrolled environmental effects. Here, we document an experimental design in D. melanogaster, where statistical noise can be minimized through strict control of genetic background and growth conditions as well as replicate measurements on large numbers of individuals of the same genotype, to detect individual modifiers of defined neurogenic loci that affect a behavioral fitness phenotype, the response to odorants. However, we are aware that epistasis between naturally segregating variants and a large effect mutant locus may present a different scenario from epistasis between naturally segregating variants.
We were able to identify specific candidate epistatic modifiers on the phenotypic effects of defined target loci by combining analyses of natural variants in fully sequenced inbred lines with coisogenic transposon mutations at defined loci. This approach can, in principle, be expanded and applied to a broad range of phenotypes to document the contributions of nonadditive effects on the genetic architecture of complex traits, not only at a statistical, but also at a molecular level.
Gene networks can be constructed based on physical interactions, genetic interactions or coexpression. Regarding the neur and Sema‐5c loci we identified genetic interactions between genes that belong to coregulated expression modules and showed that, although we did not functionally confirm epistasis directly, 80% of candidate epistatic modifiers of neur and Sema‐5c tested themselves affect olfactory behavior. This is similar to the validation rate of candidate genes identified by previous GWA studies on the DGRP (Arya et al. 2015; Dembeck et al. 2015; Harbison et al. 2013; Jordan et al. 2012; Swarup et al. 2013; Weber et al. 2012). Furthermore, previous transposon insertion based screens for olfactory behavior have shown that only 4–6% of transposon insertion lines affect olfactory behavior (Anholt et al. 1996; Sambandan et al. 2006) compared with an 80% success rate in our experiment. One caveat, however, is that the Exelixis transposon mutants used to functionally test pros and mgr might affect the expression of both genes, limiting unambiguous assignment of the causal epistatic partner(s) for neur. This is also the case for the mutants corresponding to Alh and CG10098.
We note that the number of genes tested represents only a subset of all putative epistatic modifier loci identified in our genome‐wide screens. The gene action of neur or Sema‐5c on the phenotype is likely to represent the sum of multiple nonlinear gene–gene interactions, including both antagonistic suppressor and enhancer effects (Fig. S2). In a previous study (Sambandan et al. 2006), we did not observe direct epistatic interactions between Sema‐5c and neur and there is no overlap between their interacting partners (Table S4 and S5). However, we cannot exclude higher order epistatic interactions between the two networks that involve computationally recruited genes. In addition, we note that the majority of epistatic effects are such that the minor allele in the mutant × DGRP F1 background is associated with a decrease in olfactory behavior; i.e. it is the major allele that suppresses (canalizes) the effect of the mutation. The interconnectivity of the transcriptome and the prevalence of epistasis support the hypothesis that networks of epistatic interactions may provide a protective buffering capacity that confers homeostasis to the transcriptome in the face of genetic perturbations or environmental fluctuations (Swarup et al. 2012; Yamamoto et al. 2009). This concept is likely not specific to flies, but applicable across phyla, including humans.
Supporting information
Figure S1: Quantile–quantile (Q–Q) plots for GWA analyses. (a) Q–Q plot for GWA analysis of olfactory responses of F1 progeny from Sema‐5cBG2386 and DGRP crosses. (b) Q–Q plot for GWA analyses of the differences of olfactory responses of F1 progeny between Canton S(B) and DGRP crosses and corresponding Sema‐5cBG2386 and DGRP crosses. (c) Q–Q plot for GWA analysis of olfactory responses of F1 progeny from neurBG2391 and DGRP crosses. (d) Q–Q plot for GWA analyses of the differences of olfactory responses of F1 progeny between Canton S(B) and DGRP crosses and corresponding neurBG2391 and DGRP crosses.
Figure S2: Epistatic reaction norms for olfactory responses between mutants and control for major and minor alleles of candidate epistatic modifier SNPs. The response indices of the major or minor alleles for a particular SNP are calculated by averaging the response index scores from all lines with the major or minor alleles, respectively. Crosses between DGRP lines and Canton S(B) are indicated in blue, crosses between DGRP lines and neur mutants are indicated in red, and crosses between DGRP lines and Sema‐5c mutants are indicated in green. P‐values for all epistatic interactions illustrated are < 10−5.
Table S1: Phenotypic values for olfactory behavioral responses to 1% (v/v) benzaldehyde.
Table S2: Top polymorphisms (P < 10−5) identified by GWA for olfactory behavior of progeny from 203 DGRP lines crossed to Sema‐5cBG2386.
Table S3: Top polymorphisms (P < 10−5) identified by GWA for olfactory behavior of progeny from 203 DGRP lines crossed to neurBG2391.
Table S4: Top polymorphisms (P < 10−5) identified by GWA for differences in olfactory behavior between progeny from 203 DGRP lines crossed to Canton S(B) and Sema‐5cBG2386.
Table S5: Top polymorphisms (P < 10−5) identified by GWA for differences in olfactory behavior between progeny from 203 DGRP lines crossed to Canton S(B) and neurBG2391.
Acknowledgments
We thank Dr. Akihiko Yamamoto for maintaining fly stocks. X.H. was supported by a visiting scholar award from the China Scholarship Council (No. 201208440319) and S.Z. was in part supported by an Institutional National Research Service Award from the National Institute of Environmental Health Sciences, T32 ES7046‐35. This work was supported by NIH grants R01 GM059469 and R01 GM045146 to R.R.H.A. and T.F.C.M.
References
- Anholt, R.R.H. , Lyman, R.F. & Mackay, T.F.C. (1996) Effects of single P‐element insertions on olfactory behavior in Drosophila melanogaster . Genetics 143, 293–301. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arya, G.H. , Magwire, M.M. , Huang, W. , Serrano‐Negron, Y.L. , Mackay, T.F.C. & Anholt, R.R.H. (2015) The genetic basis for variation in olfactory behavior in Drosophila melanogaster . Chem Senses 40, 233–243. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bellen, H.J. , Levis, R.W. , Liao, G. , He, Y. , Carlson, J.W. , Tsang, G. , Evans‐Holm, M. , Hiesinger, P.R. , Schulze, K.L. , Rubin, G.M. , Hoskins, R.A. & Spradling, A.C. (2004) The BDGP gene disruption project: single transposon insertions associated with 40% of Drosophila genes. Genetics 167, 761–781. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brem, R.B. , Storey, J.D. , Whittle, J. & Kruglyak, L. (2005) Genetic interactions between polymorphisms that affect gene expression in yeast. Nature 436, 701–703. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Byers, D. , Davis, R.L. & Kiger, J.A. Jr. (1981) Defect in cyclic AMP phosphodiesterase due to the dunce mutation of learning in Drosophila melanogaster . Nature 289, 79–81. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carlborg, O. , Jacobsson, L. , Ahgren, P. , Siegel, P. & Andersson, L. (2006) Epistasis and the release of genetic variation during long‐term selection. Nat Genet 38, 418–420. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Charlesworth, B. (2015) Causes of natural variation in fitness: evidence from studies of Drosophila populations. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 112, 1662–1669. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chandler, C.H. , Chari, S. , Tack, D. & Dworkin, I. (2014) Causes and consequences of genetic background effects illuminated by integrative genomic analysis. Genetics 196, 1321–1336. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chari, S. & Dworkin, I. (2013) The conditional nature of genetic interactions: the consequences of wild‐type backgrounds on mutational interactions in a genome‐wide modifier screen. PLoS Genet 9, e1003661. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cheverud, J.M. , Vaughn, T.T. , Pletscher, L.S. , Peripato, A.C. , Adams, E.S. , Erikson, C. & Cheverud, J.M. (2001) Genetic architecture of adiposity in the cross of LG/J and SM/J inbred mice. Mamm Genome 12, 3–12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clark, A.G. & Wang, L. (1997) Epistasis in measured genotypes: Drosophila P‐element insertions. Genetics 147, 157–163. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dembeck, L.M. , Huang, W. , Magwire, M.M. , Lawrence, F. , Lyman, R.F. & Mackay, T.F.C. (2015) Genetic architecture of abdominal pigmentation in Drosophila melanogaster . PLoS Genet 11, e1005163. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deutschbauer, A.M. & Davis, R.W. (2005) Quantitative trait loci mapped to single nucleotide resolution in yeast. Nat Genet 37, 1333–1340. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dilda, C.L. & Mackay, T.F.C. (2002) The genetic architecture of Drosophila sensory bristle number. Genetics 162, 1655–1674. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Doebley, J. , Stec, A. & Gustus, C. (1995) Teosinte branched1 and the origin of maize: evidence for epistasis and the evolution of dominance. Genetics 141, 333–346. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Falconer, D.S. & Mackay, T.F.C. (1996) Introduction to Quantitative Genetics, 4th edn. Addison Wesley Longman, Essex. [Google Scholar]
- Fedorowicz, G.M. , Fry, J.D. , Anholt, R.R.H. & Mackay, T.F.C. (1998) Epistatic interactions between smell‐impaired loci in Drosophila melanogaster . Genetics 148, 1885–1891. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Félix, M.A. & Barkoulas, M. (2015) Pervasive robustness in biological systems. Nat Rev Genet 16, 483–496. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gaertner, B.E. , Parmenter, M.D. , Rockman, M.V. , Kruglyak, L. & Phillips, P.C. (2012) More than the sum of its parts: a complex epistatic network underlies natural variation in thermal preference behavior in Caenorhabditis elegans . Genetics 192, 1533–1542. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gerke, J. , Lorenz, K. & Cohen, B. (2009) Genetic interactions between transcription factors cause natural variation in yeast. Science 323, 498–501. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gibson, G. & Dworkin, I. (2004) Uncovering cryptic genetic variation. Nat Rev Genet 5, 681–690. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gilchrist, A.S. & Partridge, L. (2001) The contrasting genetic architecture of wing size and shape in Drosophila melanogaster . Heredity 86, 144–152. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hanlon, P. , Lorenz, W.A. , Shao, Z. , Harper, J.M. , Galecki, A.T. , Miller, R.A. & Burke, D.T. (2006) Three‐locus and four‐locus QTL interactions influence mouse insulin‐like growth Factor‐I. Physiol Genome 26, 46–54. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Harbison, S.T. , McCoy, L.J. & Mackay, T.F.C. (2013) Genome‐wide association study of sleep in Drosophila melanogaster . BMC Genomics 14, 281. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huang, W. , Richards, S. , Carbone, M.A. et al. (2012) Epistasis dominates the genetic architecture of Drosophila quantitative traits. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 109, 15553–15559. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huang, W. , Massouras, A. , Inoue, Y. et al. (2014) Natural variation in genome architecture among 205 Drosophila melanogaster Genetic Reference Panel lines. Genome Res 24, 1193–1208. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jarvis, J.P. & Cheverud, J.M. (2011) Mapping the epistatic network underlying murine reproductive fatpad variation. Genetics 187, 597–610. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jordan, K.W. , Craver, K.L. , Magwire, M.M. , Cubilla, C.E. , Mackay, T.F.C. & Anholt, R.R.H. (2012) Genome‐wide association for sensitivity to chronic oxidative stress in Drosophila melanogaster . PLoS ONE 7, e38722. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kroymann, J. & Mitchell‐Olds, T. (2005) Epistasis and balanced polymorphism influencing complex trait variation. Nature 435, 95–98. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leamy, L.J. , Gordon, R.R. & Pomp, D. (2011) Sex‐, diet‐, and cancer‐dependent epistatic effects on complex traits in mice. Front Genet 2, 71. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Long, A.D. , Mullaney, S.L. , Mackay, T.F.C. & Langley, C.H. (1996) Genetic interactions between naturally occurring alleles at quantitative trait loci and mutant alleles at candidate loci affecting bristle number in Drosophila melanogaster . Genetics 144, 1497–1510. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Loveall, B.J. & Deitcher, D.L. (2010) The essential role of bursicon during Drosophila development. BMC Dev Biol 10, 92. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lukacsovich, T. , Asztalos, Z. , Awano, W. , Baba, K. , Kondo, S. , Niwa, S. & Yamamoto, D. (2001) Dual‐tagging gene trap of novel genes in Drosophila melanogaster . Genetics 157, 727–742. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mackay, T.F.C. (2010) Mutations and quantitative genetic variation: lessons from Drosophila. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 365, 1229–1239. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mackay, T.F.C. , Richards, S. , Stone, E.A. et al. (2012) The Drosophila melanogaster Genetic Reference Panel. Nature 482, 173–178. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mackay, T.F.C. (2014) Epistasis and quantitative traits: using model organisms to study gene‐gene interactions. Nat Rev Genet 15, 22–33. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Magwire, M.M. , Yamamoto, A. , Carbone, M.A. , Roshina, N.V. , Symonenko, A.V. , Pasyukova, E.G. , Morozova, T.V. & Mackay, T.F.C. (2010) Quantitative and molecular genetic analyses of mutations increasing Drosophila life span. PLoS Genet 6, e1001037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Manolio, T.A. , Collins, F.S. , Cox, N.J. et al. (2009) Finding the missing heritability of complex diseases. Nature 461, 747–753. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Montooth, K.L. , Marden, J.H. & Clark, A.G. (2003) Mapping determinants of variation in energy metabolism, respiration and flight in Drosophila. Genetics 165, 623–635. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Norga, K.K. , Gurganus, M.C. , Dilda, C.L. , Yamamoto, A. , Lyman, R.F. , Patel, P.H. , Rubin, G.M. , Hoskins, R.A. , Mackay, T.F.C. & Bellen, H.J. (2003) Quantitative analysis of bristle number in Drosophila mutants identifies genes involved in neural development. Curr Biol 13, 1388–1396. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ober, U. , Huang, W. , Magwire, M. , Schlather, M. , Simianer, H. & Mackay, T.F.C. (2015) Accounting for genetic architecture improves sequence based genomic prediction for a Drosophila fitness trait. PLoS ONE 10, e0126880. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peripato, A.C. , De Brito, R.A. , Matioli, S.R. , Pletscher, L.S. , Vaughn, T.T. & Cheverud, J.M. (2004) Epistasis affecting litter size in mice. J Evol Biol 17, 593–602. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pettersson, M. , Besnier, F. , Siegel, P.B. & Carlborg, O. (2011) Replication and explorations of high‐order epistasis using a large advanced intercross line pedigree. PLoS Genet 7, e1002180. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rollmann, S.M. , Yamamoto, A. , Goossens, T. , Zwarts, L. , Callaerts‐Végh, Z. , Callaerts, P. , Norga, K. , Mackay, T.F.C. & Anholt, R.R.H. (2007) The early developmental gene Semaphorin 5c contributes to olfactory behavior in adult Drosophila. Genetics 176, 947–956. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rollmann, S.M. , Zwarts, L. , Edwards, A.C. , Yamamoto, A. , Callaerts, P. , Norga, K. , Mackay, T.F.C. & Anholt, R.R.H. (2008) Pleiotropic effects of Drosophila neuralized on complex behaviors and brain structure. Genetics 179, 1327–1336. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rowe, H.C. , Hansen, B.G. , Halkier, B.A. & Kliebenstein, D.J. (2008) Biochemical networks and epistasis shape the Arabidopsis thaliana metabolome. Plant Cell 20, 1199–1216. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sambandan, D. , Yamamoto, A. , Fanara, J.J. , Mackay, T.F.C. & Anholt, R.R.H. (2006) Dynamic genetic interactions determine odor‐guided behavior in Drosophila melanogaster . Genetics 174, 1349–1363. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shorter, J. , Couch, C. , Huang, W. , Carbone, M.A. , Peiffer, J. , Anholt, R.R.H. & Mackay, T.F.C. (2015) Genetic architecture of natural variation in Drosophila melanogaster aggressive behavior. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 112, E3555–E3563. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stylianou, I.M. , Korstanje, R. , Li, R. , Sheehan, S. , Paigen, B. & Churchill, G.A. (2006) Quantitative trait locus analysis for obesity reveals multiple networks of interacting loci. Mamm Genome 17, 22–36. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Swarup, S. , Islam Williams, T. & Anholt, R.R.H. (2011) Functional dissection of Odorant binding protein genes in Drosophila melanogaster . Genes Brain Behav 10, 648–657. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Swarup, S. , Harbison, S.T. , Hahn, L.E. , Morozova, T.V. , Yamamoto, A. , Mackay, T.F.C. & Anholt, R.R.H. (2012) Extensive epistasis for olfactory behaviour, sleep and waking activity in Drosophila melanogaster . Genet Res 94, 9–20. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Swarup, S. , Huang, W. , Mackay, T.F.C. & Anholt, R.R.H. (2013) Analysis of natural variation reveals neurogenetic networks for Drosophila olfactory behavior. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 110, 1017–1022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thibault, S.T. , Singer, M.A. , Miyazaki, W.Y. et al. (2004) A complementary transposon tool kit for Drosophila melanogaster using P and piggyBac . Nat Genet 36, 283–287. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- van Swinderen, B. & Greenspan, R.J. (2005) Flexibility in a gene network affecting a simple behavior in Drosophila melanogaster . Genetics 169, 2151–2163. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Waddington, C.H. (1942) Canalization of development and the inheritance of acquired characters. Nature 150, 563–565. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wagh, D.A. , Rasse, T.M. , Asan, E. , Hofbauer, A. , Schwenkert, I. , Dürrbeck, H. , Buchner, S. , Dabauvalle, M.C. , Schmidt, M. , Qin, G. , Wichmann, C. , Kittel, R. , Sigrist, S.J. & Buchner, E. (2006) Bruchpilot, a protein with homology to ELKS/CAST, is required for structural integrity and function of synaptic active zones in Drosophila. Neuron 49, 833–844. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Warde‐Farley, D. , Donaldson, S.L. , Comes, O. , Zuberi, K. , Badrawi, R. , Chao, P. , Franz, M. , Grouios, C. , Kazi, F. , Lopes, C.T. , Maitland, A. , Mostafavi, S. , Montojo, J. , Shao, Q. , Wright, G. , Bader, G.D. & Morris, Q. (2010) The GeneMANIA prediction server: biological network integration for gene prioritization and predicting gene function. Nucleic Acids Res 38 (Suppl), W214–W220. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weber, A.L. , Khan, G.F. , Magwire, M.M. , Tabor, C.L. , Mackay, T.F.C. & Anholt, R.R.H. (2012) Genome‐wide association analysis of oxidative stress resistance in Drosophila melanogaster . PLoS ONE 7, e34745. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wentzel, A.M. , Rowe, H.C. , Hansen, B.G. , Ticconi, C. , Halkier, B.A. & Kliebenstein, D.J. (2007) Linking metabolic QTLs with network and cis‐eQTLs controlling biosynthetic pathways. PLoS Genet 3, 1687–1701. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yamamoto, A. , Zwarts, L. , Norga, K. , Callaerts, P. , Mackay, T.F.C. & Anholt, R.R.H. (2008) Neurogenetic networks for startle‐induced locomotion in Drosophila. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 105, 12393–12398. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yamamoto, A. , Anholt, R.R.H. & Mackay, T.F.C. (2009) Epistatic interactions attenuate mutations affecting startle behaviour in Drosophila melanogaster . Genet Res 91, 373–382. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zuk, O. , Hechter, E. , Sunyaev, S.R. & Lander, E.S. (2012) The mystery of missing heritability: genetic interactions create phantom heritability. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 109, 1193–1198. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Figure S1: Quantile–quantile (Q–Q) plots for GWA analyses. (a) Q–Q plot for GWA analysis of olfactory responses of F1 progeny from Sema‐5cBG2386 and DGRP crosses. (b) Q–Q plot for GWA analyses of the differences of olfactory responses of F1 progeny between Canton S(B) and DGRP crosses and corresponding Sema‐5cBG2386 and DGRP crosses. (c) Q–Q plot for GWA analysis of olfactory responses of F1 progeny from neurBG2391 and DGRP crosses. (d) Q–Q plot for GWA analyses of the differences of olfactory responses of F1 progeny between Canton S(B) and DGRP crosses and corresponding neurBG2391 and DGRP crosses.
Figure S2: Epistatic reaction norms for olfactory responses between mutants and control for major and minor alleles of candidate epistatic modifier SNPs. The response indices of the major or minor alleles for a particular SNP are calculated by averaging the response index scores from all lines with the major or minor alleles, respectively. Crosses between DGRP lines and Canton S(B) are indicated in blue, crosses between DGRP lines and neur mutants are indicated in red, and crosses between DGRP lines and Sema‐5c mutants are indicated in green. P‐values for all epistatic interactions illustrated are < 10−5.
Table S1: Phenotypic values for olfactory behavioral responses to 1% (v/v) benzaldehyde.
Table S2: Top polymorphisms (P < 10−5) identified by GWA for olfactory behavior of progeny from 203 DGRP lines crossed to Sema‐5cBG2386.
Table S3: Top polymorphisms (P < 10−5) identified by GWA for olfactory behavior of progeny from 203 DGRP lines crossed to neurBG2391.
Table S4: Top polymorphisms (P < 10−5) identified by GWA for differences in olfactory behavior between progeny from 203 DGRP lines crossed to Canton S(B) and Sema‐5cBG2386.
Table S5: Top polymorphisms (P < 10−5) identified by GWA for differences in olfactory behavior between progeny from 203 DGRP lines crossed to Canton S(B) and neurBG2391.