Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2017 May 1.
Published in final edited form as: Child Care Health Dev. 2016 Apr 5;42(3):343–350. doi: 10.1111/cch.12336

Obesity Leads to Declines in Motor Skills across Childhood

Jessica Cheng 1, Patricia East 1, Estela Blanco 2, Eastern Kang Sim 1, Marcela Castillo 2, Betsy Lozoff 3, Sheila Gahagan 1
PMCID: PMC4841726  NIHMSID: NIHMS762279  PMID: 27059409

Abstract

Background

Poor motor skills have been consistently linked with a higher body weight in childhood, but the causal direction of this association is not fully understood. This study investigated the temporal ordering between children’s motor skills and weight status at 5 and 10 years.

Methods

Participants were 668 children (54% male) who were studied from infancy as part of an iron-deficiency anemia preventive trial and follow-up study in Santiago, Chile. All were healthy, full term, and weighing 3 kg or more at birth. Cross-lagged panel modeling was conducted to understand the temporal precedence between children’s weight status and motor proficiency. Analyses also examined differences in gross and fine motor skills among healthy weight, overweight, and obese children.

Results

A higher BMI at 5 years contributed to declines in motor proficiency from 5 to 10 years. There was no support for the reverse; that is, poor motor skills at 5 years did not predict increases in relative weight from 5 to 10 years. Obesity at 5 years also predicted declines in motor proficiency. When compared to normal weight children, obese children had significantly poorer total and gross motor skills at both 5 and 10 years. Overweight children had poorer total and gross motor skills at 10 years only. The differences in total and gross motor skills among normal-weight, overweight, and obese children appear to increase with age. There were small differences in fine motor skill between obese and non-obese children at 5 years only.

Conclusions

Obesity preceded declines in motor skills and not the reverse. Study findings suggest that early childhood obesity intervention efforts might help prevent declines in motor proficiency which, in turn, may positively impact children’s physical activity and overall fitness levels.

Keywords: body mass index, motor skills, obesity, overweight status

Introduction

Poor motor skills have been linked with higher body weight in childhood in a number of studies (D’Hondt, DeForche, Vaeyens, Vandorpe et al., 2011; Lopes, Stodden, & Rodrigues, 2013; Wrotniak, Epstein, Dorn, Jones, & Kondilis, 2006). Overweight and obese children show poorer gross motor skill performance as well as worse fine motor precision and manual dexterity than their healthy-weight peers (D’Hondt, Deforche, DeBourdeaudhuij, & Lenoir, 2009; Gentier, D’Hondt, Shult, Deforche, Augustijn, Hoorne, Verlaecke, DeBourdeaudhuij, & Lenoir, 2013). To date, however, the temporal precedence, or causal direction, of this association is unclear. That is, it is currently not known whether high body mass contributes to poor motor skills, or whether poor motor proficiency leads to higher body mass. Several longitudinal studies have been conducted in which only weight or motor skill have been used to predict subsequent changes in either weight or motor skill. For example, D’Hondt and colleagues (2013) reported that an unhealthy body mass index (BMI) at 6 to 10 years predicted declines in gross motor coordination performance 2 years later (D’Hondt, Deforche, Gentier, DeBourdeaudhuij, Vaeyens, Philippaerts, & Lenoir, 2013). However, the reverse was not tested: that is, whether initial poor motor skills contribute to a subsequent unhealthy weight. Similarly, other studies have found that low levels of fitness and motor competence are strongly associated with the risk of subsequently becoming overweight or obese (Hruby, Chomitz, Arsenault et al., 2012; Kim, Must, Fitzmaurice et al., 2005; Rodrigues, Stodden, & Lopes, 2016). However, these studies did not also test the possibility that being overweight or obese predicts subsequent declines in motor competence. Thus, from the available studies, the temporal ordering between children’s motor skills and their body weight remains unclear. Understanding the causal ordering of the association between children’s motor skill and their weight would be helpful for preventive purposes. In addition, examining this issue in early development might serve to identify the origins of the motor skill-adiposity link. That is, examining the within- and across-time associations between motor skill and adiposity starting at this stage in development might yield insights into uncovering the initiating factor.

The Current Study

The current study analyzed BMI and fine and gross motor skill data for 668 children who were studied at 5 and 10 years of age. Cross-lagged panel models (Selig & Little, 2012) were used to identify the temporal precedence in the relationship between children’s motor skills and their weight status. Specifically, we analyzed how children’s 5-year total motor skills (both gross and fine) predict changes in weight status from 5 to 10 years, while simultaneously examining how 5-year weight status predicts changes in motor skills from 5 to 10 years. We used three indices of weight status: BMI z-scores, whether obese or not, and whether overweight or not (omitting children who were obese at either time point). This approach allowed us to consider whether being overweight but not obese hinders children’s motor skill development, as well as vice versa: that is, whether poor motor proficiency is a risk for subsequently becoming overweight for those who have not developed obesity. For these across-age analyses, total motor skills were examined, or those that simultaneously consider both gross and fine motor skills. This is important to note because several studies in this field have examined age-related changes in gross motor skills only (e.g., D’Hondt et al., 2011, 2013).

In addition, this study examined whether healthy weight, overweight, and obese children differ in their fine and gross motor skills at 5 and 10 years of age. Many previous studies that examined children’s motor skill by weight status have compared only healthy weight and obese children (Gentier et al., 2013). Previous research also shows that weight class differences in gross motor coordination appear more pronounced in older (10–12 years) than younger (5–7 years) children (D’Hondt et al., 2011) and become more marked over time (D’Hondt et al., 2013; Lopes et al., 2013). The current study aims to clarify whether fine motor skills differ by children’s weight status, will examine overweight children separately from obese children, and will examine whether differences in children’s motor skill among healthy weight, overweight and obese children become more marked over time.

Methods

Participants

Participants were 668 children (54% male) who were studied as part of an iron-deficiency anemia preventive trial and follow-up study in Santiago, Chile (Lozoff, DeAndraca, Castillo, Walter, Smith & Pino, 2003). The study originally involved 1,657 infants who were recruited from community clinics in Santiago between 1991 and 1996. All infants were healthy, full-term (birth weight ≥ 3.0 kg), and had no perinatal complications or acute or chronic illnesses. At infancy, children’s families were working-class, mothers had an average of 9 years education (equivalent to 9th-grade education), and 85% of fathers were present in the home. BMI and motor skills were measured at 5 and 10 years. 668 children had complete BMI and motor proficiency data at both ages; these individuals form the sample for this study’s analyses. The age periods of 5 and 10 years encompass important milestones in children’s motor, cognitive, and socioemotional development, including the refinement of perceptual and motor skills (Collins, 1984; Williams & Stith, 1980). The infant study and the 5- and 10-year follow-ups were approved by the institutional review boards of the three participating universities. Signed informed consent was obtained from parents at all study time points, and assent was obtained from children at 10 years.

Measures

Motor skills

Children’s motor skills were measured by the Bruininks-Oseretsky Test of Motor Proficiency Short Form (BOTMP-SF; Bruininks, 1978). The BOTMP-SF is a well-established, valid, and reliable test of motor proficiency (Spironello, Hay, Missiuna, Faught, & Cairney, 2010). The BOTMP-SF includes 14 subtests, six of which assess gross motor skills (GM; e.g., running speed, balance, bilateral coordination), six that assess fine motor skills (FM; e.g., visual-motor control), and two that assess simultaneous gross and fine motor skills (e.g., grabs ball with both hands). A total standard score, adjusted for child age, is used to interpret motor skill proficiency. Thus, the total score reflects proficiency of gross and fine motor skills equally. The test was administered by five trained psychologists at the Institute of Nutrition and Food Technology (INTA), University of Chile. Inter-rater agreement of ≥ 80% was established prior to the start of the study.

Body mass index

At 5 and 10 years, a research nurse at INTA, University of Chile measured children’s height and weight. Weight was measured to 0.1 kg using a SECA scale. Height was measured to 0.1 cm using a Holtain stadiometer. BMI (kg/m2) was calculated and converted to z-scores using CDC recommended methodology (Kuczmarski, Ogden, Guo, et al., 2002). BMI z-scores incorporate age and gender differences and thereby allow for comparisons across age and gender.

Weight status

Children’s weight status was defined using CDC standards, with obesity defined as ≥ 95th percentile and overweight as ≥ 85th for age and gender (Kuczmarski et al., 2002).

Covariates

Based on correlational results (described below), analyses controlled for child gender, the level of mothers’ education when the child was 10 years old, and the nurturing quality of the home environment at age 5 (measured using the Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment; possible score range: 0 – 45; Bradley, Mundfrom, Whiteside, Casey, & Barrett, 1994). Iron deficiency and iron-deficiency anemia during infancy (scored as present or absent based on venous blood samples at 6, 12, or 18 months) were also considered as covariates based on research that shows that iron-deficient anemic infants are uncharacteristically lethargic, less physically active, and have low energy (Lozoff, 2007). However, neither iron-deficiency anemia nor iron deficiency in infancy were significantly associated with any of the model variables and were not considered further. Children’s ages at the 5- and 10-year assessments (coded in months) were also not related to any of the study variable and, were not considered further.

Statistical analysis

To examine initial relations among the study variables and to identify potential covariates, we conducted Pearson correlations. Next, we conducted analyses of covariance to determine whether healthy weight, overweight, and obese children differ in their total, gross, and fine motor skills at ages 5 and 10. These analyses controlled for child gender, 5-year HOME scores, and 10-year maternal education (discussed in Results).

We then conducted cross-lagged panel analysis within a structural equation modeling framework (Selig & Little, 2012) to estimate the within- and across-time relations between children’s weight and motor proficiency (using Mplus version 6.12; Muthén & Muthén 1998–2011). Three models were analyzed. We examined the relations between children’s motor proficiency and their: 1) BMI, 2) obesity status (yes, no), and 3) overweight status (yes, no). As shown in Figure 1, all models included autoregressive paths, within-time paths, and across-time paths. Here, the autoregressive paths function as a control for the cross-lagged path. Thus, a significant cross-lagged path when autoregressive paths are included can be interpreted as variable1 at Time 1 is associated with change in variable2 from Time 1 to Time 2. Four model fit indices were used to examine how well the models fit the data: the chi-square, which is not significant when model fit is good (Bentler, 1990); the comparative fit index (CFI), with values above .90 indicating a reasonably good fit (Hu & Bentler, 1999); the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA), with values less than .08 indicating adequate fit (Kline, 2011), and the standardized root mean square residual (SRMR), which is a measure of the mean absolute value of the covariance residuals, with values less than .08 considered favorable (Bentler, 1990).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Mean total standardized motor skills per weight group at age 5 years (left) and 10 years (right).

Results

At age 5, 20.4% of children were overweight and 21.7% were obese (combined 42.1%; Table 1). At age 10, 22.9% were overweight and 18.1% were obese (combined 41.0%). There were no gender differences in these weight classifications. Table 1 also shows mean motor proficiency scores and BMI z-scores at 5 and 10 years. Both BMI z-scores (t [667] = 4.15, p < .001) and motor proficiency scores (t [667] = 18.74, p < .001) decreased across age.

Table 1.

Descriptive characteristics of the sample (N = 668).

5 years 10 years
% Male 54.0% 54.0%
Maternal education (yrs) 9.8 (2.57) 9.8 (2.60)
HOME score* 35.0 (6.40) 36.6 (7.00)
Motor proficiency*± 52.4 (8.18) 45.0 (10.23)
BMI z score* 0.84 (1.00) 0.73 (0.95)
% Overweight* 20.4% 22.9%
% Obese* 21.7% 18.1%
% Healthy weight 57.9% 59%

Note. Values given are percentages or means (SD).

*

Significant across-time change (p < .001).

±

Summed standardized Bruininks-Oseretsky score reflecting total motor proficiency.

Pearson correlations (Table 2) indicated that BMI z-scores and motor proficiency scores were significantly negatively correlated within-time at both age points. Five-year BMI z-scores and motor proficiency were also strongly and significantly correlated with their respective values at 10 years. Gender was correlated with motor proficiency at both ages, such that boys had higher total motor scores than girls. In addition, children’s nurturing home environment at 5 years was positively correlated with their 10-year motor proficiency, and 10-year maternal education was positively correlated with children’s 10-year BMI. Given these relations, gender was included as a covariate on 5- and 10-year motor proficiency scores in the cross-lagged modeling analyses; the 5-year HOME score was included as a covariate on 10-year motor proficiency; and mothers’ education at 10 years was included as a covariate on children’s 10-year BMI z-score.

Table 2.

Correlations among model variables and covariates (N = 668)

5yr Motor
proficiency
5yr BMI
z-score
5 yr
Obesity
10 yr Motor
proficiency
10yr BMI
z-score
10yr Obesity
5y Motor proficiency -
5y BMI z-score −.09* -
5 yr Obesitya −.16*** .62*** -
10 yr Motor proficiency .40*** −.19*** −.16*** -
10 yr BMI z-score −.09* .78*** .47*** −.19*** -
10y Obesitya −.12** .48*** .61*** −.15*** .44*** -
Child genderb .13*** −.03 .07 .26*** .05 .07
5y Mothers’ education .00 .04 −.01 .02 .07 .02
5y HOME .07 .05 .03 .10** .06 .01
10y Mothers’ education −.03 .01 −.02 .06 .08* .01
10y HOME .06 .07 .06 .06 .05 .03
*

p < .05.

**

p < .01.

***

p < .001.

a

0 = no, 1 = yes.

b

0 = female, 1 = male.

Weight group differences

The mean total, gross, and fine motor skill scores for healthy weight, overweight, and obese children at ages 5 and 10 are shown in Table 3. Results of the ANCOVA and post-hoc contrasts indicate that, at 5 years, obese children had poorer total and gross motor skills than both their healthy-weight and overweight peers. At age 10, both obese and overweight children differed from healthy-weight children in total and gross motor proficiency. Also at age 10, obese children had lower gross motor proficiency scores than overweight children. Figure 1 graphically illustrates the total mean skill values per weight group at both ages. The differences in total and gross motor skills among normal-weight, overweight, and obese children appear to increase with age.

Table 3.

Total, gross, and fine motor skills at 5 and 10 years, stratified by healthy weight (HW), overweight (OW), and obesity (OB) status

HW OW OB F [2,666] Contrast
5 year (N = 668) 387 136 145

  Total motor proficiency standardized score 53.0 (8.1) 52.8 (7.5) 50.3 (8.6) 6.62** HW>OB; OW>OB
  Gross motor skills 13.7 (3.8) 13.3 (3.9) 11.9 (4.3) 11.08*** HW>OB; OW>OB
  Fine motor skills 13.3 (2.9) 13.4 (2.8) 13.1 (3.1) < 1

10 year (N = 668) 394 153 121

  Total motor proficiency standardized score 45.8 (10.1) 43.5 (10.1) 40.9 (9.5) 24.09*** HW>OW; HW>OB
  Gross motor skills 25.4 (3.9) 23.8 (4.0) 21.9 (4.1) 47.06*** HW>OW; HW >OB;OW>OB
   Fine motor skills 26.9 (3.6) 26.5 (3.8) 26.5 (3.6) 1.49

Note. Values are means (SD). F values controlled for child gender, 5-year HOME scores, and 10-year maternal education.

**

p < .01.

***

p < .001.

There were no differences among the groups in fine motor skills at either age. However, post-hoc analyses of the individual fine motor subtests showed one significant difference: obese children performed worse on a fine motor precision task at 5 years (drawing lines) than both overweight and healthy-weight children (F [2,666] = 3.00, p < .05, controlling for child gender). There were no group differences on any of the fine motor subtests at age 10.

Modeling results

The model fit indices for the crossed-lagged panel model analyzing children’s motor skills and BMI indicated good model fit (Figure 2). At both time points (and consistent with the correlations), children’s motor skills and BMI were significantly negatively associated. The cross-lagged paths indicated that a higher BMI at 5 years was associated with significant declines in motor skills from 5 to 10 years (β = −.16, p < .001). However, the reverse was not true; that is, 5-year motor proficiency was not related to changes in relative weight from 5 to 10 years (β = −.02, ns).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Cross-lagged panel model results for children’s total motor proficiency score and BMI at 5 and 10 years. N = 668. Standardized coefficients are shown. Dashed paths are not statistically significant. The model controlled for child gender, 5-year maternal education, and 10- year home environment, not shown. Model fit indices were as follows: χ2 (4) = 5.24, ns; CFI: .998; RMSEA: .022; SRMR: .014.

*p < .05. **p <. 01. ***p < .001.

Figure 3 shows the cross-lagged panel model of children’s motor skills and their obesity status (yes, no). Model fit indices again indicated that model fit was good. These results were similar to those using BMI z scores, such that an obese status at age 5 was associated with significant declines in motor proficiency from age 5 to 10 (β = −.12, p < .01), but 5-year motor skills were unrelated to change in obesity status from 5 to 10 years (β = −.03, ns).

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Cross-lagged panel model results for children’s total motor proficiency score and obesity status (yes, no; coded 1, 0) at 5 and 10 years. N = 668. Standardized coefficients are shown. Dashed paths are not statistically significant. The model controlled for child gender, 5- year maternal education, and 10-year home environment, not shown. Model fit indices were as follows: χ2 (3) = 4.64, ns; CFI: .996; RMSEA: .029; SRMR: .017. **p <. 01. ***p < .001.

Figure 4 shows the cross-lagged model analyzing overweight status (yes, no; and omits those who were obese at either time point, or 182 children). The model fit indices indicated that model fit was good. In this model, an overweight status at age 5 was marginally associated with declines in motor proficiency from age 5 to 10 (β = −.07, p < .09), while 5-year motor skills were unrelated to a change in overweight status from 5 to 10 years (β = −.06, ns).

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Cross-lagged panel model results for children’s total motor proficiency score and overweight status (yes, no; coded 1, 0; excludes obese children) at 5 and 10 years. N = 486. Standardized coefficients are shown. Dashed paths are not statistically significant. The model controlled for child gender, 5-year maternal education, and 10-year home environment, not shown. Model fit indices were as follows: χ2 (3) = 5.50, ns; CFI: .985; RMSEA: .041; SRMR: .019. +p < .09. *p < .05. ***p < .001.

Discussion

We found that a high BMI and, separately, being obese at age 5 predicted declines in children’s motor skills from 5 to 10 years. The reverse was not found: that is, poor motor proficiency at age 5 did not predict obesity or increases in BMI. These findings indicate that a heavier weight status is a precursor and not a consequence of poor motor skills. Results also show that being overweight at age 5 was not sufficient to produce significant declines in motor skills from 5 to 10 years. However, marginally significant declines in motor skill were associated with overweight status at age 5, which might hold future significance, particularly if these children become obese at a later age. The overall pattern of results suggests that excess adiposity might inhibit obese children from developing and exercising their motor skills, which contributes to declines in motor proficiency relative to healthy-weight children (Castetbon & Andreyeva, 2012). These findings are clinically important because poor motor skill development is associated with children’s physical inactivity and poor fitness levels (Burgi, Meyer, Granacher, Schindler et al., 2011; Wrotniak et al., 2006). Relative declines in children’s motor proficiencies might also be associated with self-perceptions of poorer physical competence, which again might serve as a catalyst for inactivity and consequent weight gain (Spessato, Gabbard, Robinson, & Valentini, 2012).

We also observed significant differences in total and gross motor proficiency depending on children’s weight status at both 5 and 10 years. When compared to normal weight children, obese children had significantly poorer total and gross motor skills at both ages. Overweight children exhibited poorer total and gross motor skills than normal-weight children at 10 years only. In addition, overweight children had better total motor proficiency than obese children at 5 years, but by age 10, were equivalent to obese children in total motor proficiency. Thus, overweight children appear to be performing more like obese children in terms of motor skills at age 10. The differences in total and gross motor skills among healthy-weight, overweight, and obese children appeared to increase with age. This is consistent with previous research showing that weight class differences in gross motor coordination become more pronounced at older ages (D’Hondt et al., 2011, 2013). These findings suggest the need to address children’s weight issues at an early age, prior to the onset of large disparities in motor skills between healthy-weight and unhealthy-weight children.

Some prior studies have shown fine motor skill impairments in obese children (D’Hondt et al., 2009; Gentier et al., 2013). In our study, there were no group differences in children’s overall fine motor skills at either time point, but obese children performed worse than overweight and healthy-weight children on a line drawing task at 5 years. This result suggests that fine motor impairments may be evident in obese children, although confined to a specific fine motor ability and not persistent. Further investigation to corroborate this finding is warranted.

Limitations and strengths

The current study was unable to compute cross-lagged analyses for gross and fine motor skills separately because these subtest scores of the BOTMP-SF are not standardized and thus could not be compared across age; only the BOTMP-SF total motor proficiency scores could be analyzed across age. Nevertheless, the ANCOVA results provide valuable information indicating that the weight groups differ primarily in gross motor skill at both age 5 and 10. We also could not compare mean level motor score differences by weight group across age because weight group status changed for 32% of participants between 5 and 10 years. In addition, to address the possible influence of children’s socio-economic background on the relations between motor skills and weight status, we controlled for mothers’ years of education as a proxy for socio-economic status (Krieger, Williams, & Moss, 1997). Thus, the results found adjusted for children’s socioeconomic background. It is possible, however, that unmeasured features in children’s environments could have influenced the relations found. For example, children from more enriched environments could have more opportunity for motor skill development than children from poorer environments (Estabrooks, Lee, & Gyurcsik, 2003), which could influence children’s weight status. Socioeconomic factors and their association with children’s weight and motor skill development should be considered more specifically in research of this kind. Finally, because this study involved Chilean children, it is not known whether the current findings would hold in a population with different obesity rates, as well as different cultural norms for physical activity. The degree to which study results may differ for children of other cultures is not known.

Strengths of the study are that the cross-lagged models were tested using a large sample and included several relevant statistical controls. Thus, the relations that emerged did so while controlling for key child and family characteristics known to be correlated with both child weight (Wang, 2001) and motor skill (Okely & Booth, 2004). In addition, the cross-lagged models simultaneously tested both possible across-time relations between children’s weight and motor skill, thereby providing an opportunity to discern temporal ordering between these measures and a likely causal direction of effects. Because 5-year measures of each construct were included in the model, we were able to assess the effects of 5-year motor skill and weight status on subsequent changes in the opposing measure. In addition, our analyses considered obese children separate from overweight children, and we included an analysis of differences in fine motor skills vis-à-vis children’s weight status group.

Conclusion

The purpose of this study was to examine the temporal ordering of children’s relative weight and motor skills, a question with considerable practical significance. The results indicate that a high relative weight contributes to subsequent declines in motor skills and not the reverse. These findings suggest that early childhood obesity intervention efforts might help prevent subsequent declines in children’s motor proficiency, which may produce secondary benefits of positively impacting children’s physical activity and fitness levels (Rodrigues et al., 2016; Wrotniak et al., 2006). The results also suggest that intervention efforts that target overweight and obese children might be best timed prior to kindergarten or first-grade or before significant declines in motor skill occur.

Key messages.

  • A higher BMI and, separately, being obese at 5 years were associated with significant declines in motor proficiency from 5 to 10 years.

  • Motor skills at 5 years were unrelated to changes in either BMI or weight status from 5 to 10 years.

  • The differences in total and gross motor skills among healthy-weight, overweight, and obese children appear to increase with age.

  • Early childhood obesity intervention efforts might help minimize subsequent declines in children’s motor skills and serve to increase physical activity and overall fitness.

Acknowledgments

Sources of Support

Grant R01-HL-088530 from the National Institute of Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute (PI: S.G);

Grant R01-HD33487 from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (PIs: Lozoff & Gahagan)

References

  1. Bentler P. Comparative fit indices in structural models. Psychological Bulletin. 1990;107:238–246. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.107.2.238. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Bradley RH, Mundfrom DJ, Whiteside L, Casey PH, Barrett K. A factor analytic study of the infant-toddler and early childhood versions of the HOME Inventory administered to white, Black, and Hispanic American parents of children born preterm. Child Development. 1994;65:880–888. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1994.tb00790.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Bruininks R. Bruininks-Oseretsky Test of Motor Proficiency. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service; 1978. [Google Scholar]
  4. Burgi F, Meyer U, Granacher U, Schindler C, Marques-Vidal P, Kriemler S, Puder JJ. Relationship of physical activity with motor skills, aerobic fitness and body fat in preschool children: A cross-sectional and longitudinal study. International Journal of Obesity. 2011;35:937–944. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2011.54. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Castetbon K, Andreyeva T. Obesity and motor skills among 4 to 6-year-old children in the United States: Nationally-representative surveys. BMC Pediatrics. 2012;12:28–36. doi: 10.1186/1471-2431-12-28. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Collins WA, editor. Development during middle childhood: The years from six to twelve. Washington, DC: National Academies Press; 1984. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. D’Hondt E, Deforche B, De Bourdeaudhuij I, Lenoir M. Relationship between motor skill and body mass index in 5-to 10-year-old children. Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly. 2009;26:21–37. doi: 10.1123/apaq.26.1.21. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. D'Hondt E, Deforche B, Gentier I, De Bourdeaudhuij I, Vaeyens R, Philippaerts R, Lenoir M. A longitudinal analysis of gross motor coordination in overweight and obese children versus normal-weight peers. International Journal of Obesity. 2013;37:61–67. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2012.55. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. D'Hondt E, Deforche B, Vaeyens R, Vandorpe V, Vandendriessche J, Pion J, Philippaerts R, De Bourdeaudhuij I, Lenoir M. Gross motor coordination in relation to weight status and age in 5-to 12-year-old boys and girls: A cross-sectional study. International Journal of Pediatric Obesity. 2011;6:e556–e564. doi: 10.3109/17477166.2010.500388. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Estabrooks PA, Lee RE, Gyurcsik NC. Resources for physical activity participation: Does availability and accessibility differ by neighborhood socioeconomic status? Annals of Behavioral Medicine. 2003;25:100–104. doi: 10.1207/S15324796ABM2502_05. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Gentier I, D’Hondt E, Shultz S, Deforche B, Augustijn M, Hoorne S, Verlaecke K, De Bourdeaudjuij I, Lenoir M. Fine and gross motor skills differ between healthy-weight and obese children. Research on Developmental Disabilities. 2013;34:4043–4051. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2013.08.040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Hruby A, Chomitz VR, Arsenault LN, Must A, Economos CD, McGowan RJ, Sacheck JM. Predicting maintenance or achievement of healthy weight in children: The impact of changes in physical fitness. Obesity. 2012;20:1720–1717. doi: 10.1038/oby.2012.13. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Hu LT, Bentler PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling. 1999;6:1–55. [Google Scholar]
  14. Kim J, Must A, Fitzmaurice GM, Gillman MW, Chomitz V, Kramer E, McGowan R, Peterson KE. Relationship of physical fitness to prevalence and incidence of overweight among school children. Obesity Research. 2005;13:1246–1254. doi: 10.1038/oby.2005.148. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Kline RB. Principles and practice of structural equation modeling. 3rd. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 2011. [Google Scholar]
  16. Krieger N, Williams DR, Moss N. Measuring social class in U.S. public health research: Concepts, methodologies, and guidelines. Annual Review of Public Health. 1997;18:341–378. doi: 10.1146/annurev.publhealth.18.1.341. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Kuczmarski RJ, Ogden CL, Guo SS, Grunner-Stawn LM, Flegal KM, Mei Z, Wei R, Curtin LR, Roche AF, Johnson CL. 2000 CDC growth charts for the United States: Methods and development. Vital Health Statistics, Series 11. 2002;246:1–190. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Lopes VP, Stodden DF, Rodrigues LP. Weight status is associated with cross-sectional trajectories of motor co-ordination across childhood. Child: Care, Health and Development. 2013;40:891–899. doi: 10.1111/cch.12127. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Lozoff B. Iron deficiency and child development. Food and Nutrition Bulletin. 2007;28:S560–S571. doi: 10.1177/15648265070284S409. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Lozoff B, DeAndraca I, Castillo M, Walter T, Smith J, Pino P. Behavioral and developmental effects of preventing iron-deficiency anemia in healthy full-term infants. Pediatrics. 2003;112:846–854. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Muthén LK, Muthén BO. Mplus user’s guide. 5th. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén; 1998–2007. [Google Scholar]
  22. Okely A, Booth M. Master of fundamental movement skills among children in New South Wales: Prevalence and sociodemographic distribution. Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport. 2004;7:358–372. doi: 10.1016/s1440-2440(04)80031-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Rodrigues LP, Stodden DF, Lopes VP. Developmental pathways of change in fitness and motor competence are related to overweight and obesity status at the end of primary school. Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport. 2016;19:87–92. doi: 10.1016/j.jsams.2015.01.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Selig JP, Little TD. Autoregressive and cross-lagged panel analysis for longitudinal data. In: Laursen B, Little TD, Card NA, editors. Handbook of Developmental Research Methods. New York: Guilford Press; 2012. pp. 265–278. [Google Scholar]
  25. Spessato BC, Gabbard C, Robinson L, Valentini NC. Body mass index, perceived and actual physical competence: the relationship among young children. Child: Care, Health and Development. 2012;39:845–850. doi: 10.1111/cch.12014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Spironello C, Hay J, Missiuna C, Faught BE, Cairney J. Concurrent and construct validation of the short form of the Bruininks-Oseretsky Test of Motor Proficiency and the Movement-ABC when administered under field conditions: implications for screening. Child: Care, Health and Development. 2010;36:499–507. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2214.2009.01066.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Wang Y. Cross-national comparison of childhood obesity: The epidemic and the relationship between obesity and socioeconomic status. International Journal of Epidemiology. 2001;30:1129–1136. doi: 10.1093/ije/30.5.1129. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Williams J, Stith MM. Middle childhood: Behavior and development. Macmillan Publishing Company; 1980. [Google Scholar]
  29. Wrotniak BH, Epstein LH, Dorn JM, Jones KE, Kondilis VA. The relationship between motor proficiency and physical activity in children. Pediatrics. 2006;118:e1758–e1765. doi: 10.1542/peds.2006-0742. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES