Abstract
Elaborate, sexually dimorphic traits are widely thought to evolve under sexual selection through female preference, male–male competition, or both. The orangethroat darter (Etheostoma spectabile) is a sexually dichromatic fish in which females exhibit no preferences for male size or coloration. We tested whether these traits affect individual reproductive success in E. spectabile when multiple males are allowed to freely compete for a female. The quality and quantity of male coloration were associated with greater success in maintaining access to the female and in spawning as the primary male (first male to participate). On the other hand, sneaking behavior showed little correlation with coloration. Male breeding coloration in E. spectabile may therefore demonstrate how intrasexual competition can be a predominant factor underlying the evolution of male ornaments.
Keywords: Darter, male coloration, male competition, sexual selection, visual signaling
Introduction
Sexual selection theory, as originated by Darwin (1871), proposes that conspicuous male traits can evolve if they improve mating success through attractiveness to females, usefulness in competition with other males, or both. The notion that intrasexual contests can select for large body size or weaponry is fairly uncontroversial. On the other hand, male ornaments such as bright colors have traditionally been categorized as targets of female choice, and the effect of male–male competition on their evolution is less understood (Andersson 1994). Such ornaments though are involved in male–male competition across a wide range of taxa, as attested by numerous studies (e.g., Ligon et al. 1990; Mateos and Carranza 1997; Benson and Basolo 2006; Bajer et al. 2011; Crothers et al. 2011; Baird et al. 2013).
Conspicuous male traits may be favored through intrasexual selection if they enable males to remotely evaluate rivals and avoid the cost of fighting unnecessary battles with mismatched opponents (Maynard Smith and Parker 1976; Rohwer 1982; Maynard Smith and Brown 1986). In this capacity, male ornaments should communicate relevant attributes such as size, condition, or fighting ability (Parker 1974; Zahavi 1977, 1981). A well‐studied example is melanin‐ or carotenoid‐based badges of status, which are kept “honest” by physiological limitations and/or social mechanisms (Johnstone and Norris 1993; Jawor and Breitwisch 2003; Whiting et al. 2003). In rare cases, male–male competition has been found to drive color evolution independent of female choice (Grether 1996). These data suggest that male–male competition can select for the same types of ornaments as female choice and that the latter should not simply be presumed as the underlying cause of male ornamentation. Indeed, male–male competition has been proposed to be more effective than female preferences in promoting the elaboration of male ornaments: as variation in an honest signal of male quality need not rely on genetic variation and can be maintained in the face of ongoing selection, the signal should thus remain useful over time and persist in the population (Berglund et al. 1996).
Sexual dichromatism is prevalent in the darters (Percidae: Etheostomatinae), a species‐rich clade of North American freshwater fishes. The spectacular and diverse male coloration found in many darter species has long been considered to result from sexual selection (Reeves 1907; Mendelson 2003) and has sparked interest in the role that male coloration may have played in facilitating darter speciation (Martin and Mendelson 2013; Williams et al. 2013). Surprisingly, little empirical data exist on the mechanism by which sexual selection operates on male coloration in darters, or on whether coloration affects male reproductive success at all. In fact, Pyron (1995) and Fuller (2003) did not find the colorfulness of male darters to be predictive of male reproductive success.
Here, we investigate whether male coloration in the orangethroat darter (Etheostoma spectabile) (Agassiz, 1854) is under sexual selection via male–male competition. During the breeding season, male E. spectabile closely follow females while attempting to drive away rival males. When the female is ready to spawn, she buries herself shallowly in gravel and waits for a male to arrive. Females may spawn multiple times in short succession, and neither sex exhibits parental care (Winn 1958a; Pyron 1995; Zhou et al. 2015). Competition between males for females can be intense, and spawning between a single female and multiple males is common (Pyron 1995). Male E. spectabile in breeding condition express vivid bluish and reddish colors (Page 1983). However, female E. spectabile show no preference for larger or more colorful males in dichotomous trials (Pyron 1995). Thus, male coloration may instead play a signaling role in male–male competition.
Materials and Methods
Adult E. spectabile were collected from Cottonwood Creek (Cumberland Co., IL) via kick‐seine in April and May 2011, during the breeding season. Males were housed in groups of four in 76‐L aquariums (bottom dimensions 76 × 30 cm) where behavioral observations took place; the aquariums contained gravel substrate and a sponge filter, which was removed for experimental observations. The males varied in standard length (mean 44.1 ± 0.46 mm SE; range 37–55 mm) and coloration. Females were separately housed in group tanks. The fish were maintained at a temperature of 20°C and a 14:10 light:dark cycle and were fed frozen bloodworms (chironomid larvae) daily.
For individual identification, each male within a tank was marked with subcutaneously injected yellow Visible Implant Elastomer (VIE; Northwest Marine Technology, Shaw Island, WA, USA) in one of the four locations (left side of first dorsal fin, right side of first dorsal fin, left side of second dorsal fin, and right side of second dorsal fin). VIE is widely used in ichthyological research for individual identification and has not been known to affect behavior (Croft et al. 2004, 2005; Leblond and Reebs 2006; Weston and Johnson 2008; Phillips and Fries 2009). The males were allowed to recover from VIE injection overnight before behavioral trials began.
Experimental observations
Behavioral trials began with the introduction of a female to an established set of four males, who were allowed to freely compete for spawning opportunities. Following a 5‐min acclimation period, fish behaviors were observed for 1 h. If the female failed to spawn within 10 min, then the trial was discarded and the set of males was tested with another female on a subsequent day. Fish were excluded from further testing once spawning occurred; thus, each of the 16 completed trials involved a different set of males and female (total n = 64 males, 16 females). All trials took place between one and three days postcapture.
Three behaviors were recorded for each male: (1) the number of nosedigs attended by the male, defined by his being within one body length of the performing female (if more than one male attended, each was scored as an equal fraction of 1), (2) the number of spawning events in which the male was the first to participate, hereafter termed the “primary” male, and (3) the number of spawning events in which the male participated as a sneaker, that is, subsequent to the primary male.
Quantification of male coloration
After experimental trials, we measured male coloration following the methods described in Zhou et al. (2014). Briefly, fish were anesthetized in 0.03% tricaine methanesulfonate (MS‐222), which maximizes color expression in darters (Gumm and Mendelson 2011). The males were then photographed under standard fluorescent lighting against a white background with a Nikon Coolpix 8700 (Nikon, Melville, NY, USA) digital camera. A ColorChecker chart (X‐Rite, Grand Rapids, MI, USA) was included in the photograph to allow correction for variation in ambient lighting, using the inCamera plug‐in (version 4.0.1; PictoColor Software, Burnsville, MN, USA) for Photoshop CS4 (Adobe Systems, San Jose, CA, USA).
Ten male color traits distributed across the body and fins were measured (Fig. 1). Five traits were categorized as “blue”: cheek (CK), first dorsal fin blue (D1B), second dorsal fin blue (D2B), anal fin blue (AB), and lateral bar (LB); and five as “red”: branchiostegal rays (BR), first dorsal fin red (D1R), second dorsal fin red (D2R), caudal peduncle spot (CPS), and abdomen (BD). RGB values were measured from each trait using the Photoshop eyedropper tool set to average from a 3 × 3 pixel square; each trait was sampled three times and the average values used. The RGB color space describes colors as an additive mixture of red, green, and blue (range 0–255). We converted the RGB values to a luminance channel R+G+B and two color channels (R − G)/(R + G) and (G − B)/(G + B), hereafter referred to as the red–green difference (R − G) and the green–blue difference (G − B) channels. A positive R − G value indicates a stronger red component in the color, whereas a negative R − G value indicates a stronger green component; correspondingly, a positive G − B value indicates a stronger green component in the color, whereas a negative G − B value indicates a stronger blue component.
Figure 1.
Example male Etheostoma spectabile showing color traits measured (uppercase labels, abbreviations given in text) and fish regions used for blue/red area measurement (a – first dorsal fin, b – second dorsal fin, c – anal fin, and d – caudal region).
We also quantified the proportional area of blue and red coloration on four regions of the fish: first dorsal fin (D1B area and D1R area), second dorsal fin (D2B area and D2R area), anal fin (AB area), and caudal region (CB area and CR area). Caudal region was delimited by a straight line drawn between the origins of the second dorsal and anal fins, and included the entire caudal fin (Fig. 1). The photographs were processed to isolate areas of blue and red using the Threshold Colour plug‐in (version 1.10, G. Landini) in ImageJ (version 1.43u, Wayne Rasband, National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, MD, USA). Processing took place in the CIE Lab color space, which describes colors in terms of L* (lightness), a* (red/green), and b* (blue/yellow). Blue areas were isolated by setting L* to exclude colors above 200/255 and b* to exclude colors above 130/255. Red areas were isolated by setting L* to exclude colors above 200/255 and a* to exclude colors below 125/255. The total area of each fish region was obtained via tracing with the ImageJ polygonal selection tool.
Data analysis
All statistical analyses were performed in SAS (version 9.3; SAS Institute, Cary, NC, USA). Our goal was to determine which male traits (size, luminance, color, blue/red area) were most tightly correlated with male mating success. To do this, we first used principal component analyses (PCA) to synthesize variation in luminance, color, and blue/red area across all traits (results of each PCA are given in Fig. 2; see Zhou et al. 2014 for a similar approach). Principal components that accounted for >10% of the total variation were included in subsequent analyses.
Figure 2.
Eigenvector loadings, ordered by size, on variables included in principal component analyses, showing (A) luminance PC1, (B) color PC1, (C) color PC2, (D) color PC3, (E) blue/red area PC1, (F) blue/red area PC2, and (G) blue/red area PC3. Trait abbreviations are as given in text. Numbers in parentheses indicate the proportion of total variation accounted for by the principal component.
Correlates of male competitive ability and reproductive success were examined using logistic regression (GLIMMIX procedure). We analyzed the proportion of nosedigs attended, the proportion of spawning events attended as the primary, and the proportion of spawning events attended as a sneaker by each male of the total for his trial. For each analysis, the explanatory variables tested were the PC scores for luminance PC1, color PC1–PC3, and blue/red area PC1–PC3; these variables were all adjusted to be relative to their trial means. Experimental trial (n = 16) was set as a random effect, and we also included residuals as a random effect to scale for over‐ or underdispersion.
As several of the principal components analyzed were correlated with standard length (Table 1), we also performed the three analyses above after size‐adjusting the raw PC scores by regressing each principal component against standard length (REG procedure) and taking the residuals; standard length was then included in the model as a separate effect. Again, these variables were adjusted to be relative to the trial means.
Table 1.
Correlations between male coloration and standard length
Principal component | β | df | F | P |
---|---|---|---|---|
Luminance PC1 | −0.34 | 1,62 | 21.78 | <0.01 |
Color PC1 | −0.08 | 1,62 | 1.31 | 0.26 |
Color PC2 | 0.23 | 1,62 | 19.09 | <0.01 |
Color PC3 | −0.22 | 1,62 | 24.57 | <0.01 |
Blue/red area PC1 | 0.33 | 1,62 | 56.42 | <0.01 |
Blue/red area PC2 | −0.01 | 1,62 | 0.05 | 0.82 |
Blue/red area PC3 | −0.03 | 1,62 | 1.23 | 0.27 |
Results
Average male size varied among trials (range 39.5–49.9 mm), but this did not alter the pattern of behavior. Invariably, at least one male attempted to guard the female, and male–male competition was intense. Males confronted one another by flaring their fins, chasing, and biting. Nosedig attendance was positively correlated with spawning as the primary male (β = 0.91, F 1,47 = 333.42, P < 0.01) and negatively correlated with spawning as a sneaker (β = −0.27, F 1,44 = 3.74, P = 0.06). Therefore, competitively superior males were better able to arrive first at spawning events while competitively inferior males were forced to act more as sneakers.
Multiple aspects of male color and blue/red area, but not luminance, were associated with competitive and reproductive success (Table 2, Fig. 3). The first and second components of color mostly described variation in the G − B and R − G channels, respectively, with blue and red traits generally bearing opposite signs (Fig. 2A,B). Both components correlated positively with nosedig attendance and PC1 maintained this correlation with spawning as the primary male (Table 2, Fig. 3A,B). Thus, more successful males tended to be greener (lower R − G and higher G − B values) and redder (higher R − G and lower G − B values). The effects of PC1 and PC2 became nonsignificant after size adjustment (Table 2). PC3 had a larger effect than PC1 or PC2 and was consistently negatively correlated with nosedig attendance and spawning as the primary, regardless of size adjustment (Table 2, Fig. 3C). It described a more complex pattern of color variation (Fig. 2C). The red trait loadings suggest that successful males had redder dorsal fins, CPS, and abdomen (higher R − G and lower G − B values), and less orange branchiostegal rays (lower R − G and G − B values). The blue trait loadings suggest that successful males tended to have bluer first dorsal fins and LBs (lower R − G and G − B values), less blue second dorsal and anal fins (higher R − G and G − B values), and less green cheeks (higher R − G and lower G − B values).
Table 2.
Effects of male coloration and body size on competition and spawning
Effect | β | df | F | P | After size adjustment | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
β | df | F | P | |||||
Nosedig attendance | ||||||||
Luminance PC1 | −0.01 | 1,41 | 0.01 | 0.91 | −0.10 | 1,40 | 0.46 | 0.50 |
Color PC1 | 0.46 | 1,41 | 41.34 | <0.01 | 0.37 | 1,40 | 1.11 | 0.30 |
Color PC2 | 0.58 | 1,41 | 43.28 | <0.01 | 0.13 | 1,40 | 0.43 | 0.52 |
Color PC3 | −0.89 | 1,41 | 117.95 | <0.01 | −0.78 | 1,40 | 5.02 | 0.03 |
Blue/red area PC1 | 0.84 | 1,41 | 44.29 | <0.01 | 0.63 | 1,40 | 2.05 | 0.16 |
Blue/red area PC2 | 0.07 | 1,41 | 1.25 | 0.27 | 0.02 | 1,40 | 0 | 0.94 |
Blue/red area PC3 | −1.23 | 1,41 | 62.95 | <0.01 | −0.80 | 1,40 | 2.24 | 0.14 |
Standard length | 1,40 | 13.43 | <0.01 | |||||
Spawning as primary | ||||||||
Luminance PC1 | 0.03 | 1,41 | 0.02 | 0.90 | 0.03 | 1,40 | 2.00 | 0.16 |
Color PC1 | 0.32 | 1,41 | 5.06 | 0.03 | 0.13 | 1,40 | 0.55 | 0.46 |
Color PC2 | 0.24 | 1,41 | 1.69 | 0.20 | −0.01 | 1,40 | 0.02 | 0.88 |
Color PC3 | −0.58 | 1,41 | 12.49 | <0.01 | −0.52 | 1,40 | 8.09 | <0.01 |
Blue/red area PC1 | 1.39 | 1,41 | 30.68 | <0.01 | 1.20 | 1,40 | 4.76 | 0.04 |
Blue/red area PC2 | 0.03 | 1,41 | 0.06 | 0.81 | 0.03 | 1,40 | 0.35 | 0.56 |
Blue/red area PC3 | −1.09 | 1,41 | 15.93 | <0.01 | −0.93 | 1,40 | 4.29 | 0.04 |
Standard length | 1,40 | 11.10 | <0.01 | |||||
Spawning as sneaker | ||||||||
Luminance PC1 | −0.18 | 1,38 | 0.52 | 0.48 | 0.14 | 1,37 | 0.16 | 0.70 |
Color PC1 | 0.48 | 1,38 | 7.75 | 0.01 | 0.27 | 1,37 | 0.43 | 0.51 |
Color PC2 | 0.25 | 1,38 | 2.45 | 0.13 | 0.24 | 1,37 | 0.03 | 0.86 |
Color PC3 | 0.01 | 1,38 | 0 | 0.95 | 0.00 | 1,37 | 0.03 | 0.86 |
Blue/red area PC1 | −0.63 | 1,38 | 6.14 | 0.02 | −0.21 | 1,37 | 0.02 | 0.90 |
Blue/red area PC2 | −0.39 | 1,38 | 5.82 | 0.02 | −0.35 | 1,37 | 1.83 | 0.18 |
Blue/red area PC3 | 0.23 | 1,38 | 0.70 | 0.41 | 0.17 | 1,37 | 0.03 | 0.85 |
Standard length | 1,37 | 3.79 | 0.06 |
Figure 3.
Relationships between nosedig attendance, a proxy for male competitive ability, and (A–E) components of male coloration, relative to the tank means. Each point represents an individual male.
The first and third principal components of blue/red area were positively and negatively correlated, respectively, with nosedig attendance and spawning as the primary male (Fig. 3D,E). These effects remained significant after adjusting for size (Table 2). PC1 loaded positively onto all traits (Fig. 2E), indicating that males with proportionally greater amounts of blue and red were more successful. PC3 loaded most positively onto caudal region blue area and most negatively onto anal fin blue area (Fig. 2G). Therefore, more successful males tended to have greater amounts of blue on the anal fin and smaller amounts of blue on the body.
Sneaking behavior was also correlated with some aspects of color and blue/red area, although these effects were not significant after adjusting for size (Table 2). Males that spawned more as a sneaker tended to be greener (lower R − G and higher G − B values) and had proportionally lesser amounts of color (lower scores on area PC1), red in particular (lower scores on area PC2, which loaded positively onto red traits and negatively onto blue traits).
Body size was strongly related to competitive ability and spawning behavior. Males that were relatively larger within their tank attended a greater proportion of nosedigs and consequently spawned more as the primary male (Table 2, Fig. 4A). Relatively smaller males tended to spawn more as sneakers, although the effect was not significant (Table 2, Fig. 4B).
Figure 4.
Relationships between relative body size and (A) spawning as the primary male and (B) spawning as a sneaker male. Each point represents an individual male.
Discussion
Male–male competition is likely an agent of sexual selection in E. spectabile, with both body size and male coloration influencing competitive ability. We observed that male E. spectabile competed vigorously both to gain access to the female and to deny access to other males. Female E. spectabile do not exhibit preferences for large versus small males or bright versus dull males, either in association time or in latency to spawning (Pyron 1995). In lieu of overt mate choice, female E. spectabile appear to perform nosedigs in proportion to the amount of male pursuit (Zhou et al. 2015). Nosedig attendance thus represents a proxy metric for male competitive ability in our study, as males that are better able to keep rivals at bay and spend more time near the female should also receive a commensurately greater number of nosedigs.
Males that were competitively superior correspondingly enjoyed more opportunities to spawn as the primary male. However, sneaking behavior was common, as is the case in nature due to high population density (Pyron 1995). In egg‐burying darters such as E. spectabile and the closely related Etheostoma caeruleum Storer, 1845, only a small number of eggs are released per spawning bout (Winn 1958a; Fuller 1991; pers. obs.); thus, the participation of one or more sneaker males may entail substantial losses of paternity to the primary male. If so, this could drive strong selection for traits that enable males to monopolize access to females, that is, keeping all rivals far enough away that they cannot reach the female in the few seconds before she finishes spawning. Conspicuous visual signals may be beneficial under such conditions as they enable males to more effectively warn off potential rivals (Andersson 1994). A link between competition and coloration has been experimentally demonstrated in the Pecos pupfish (Cyprinodon pecosensis) Echelle and Echelle, 1978, wherein the expression of breeding coloration increased in males kept in view of other males (Kodric‐Brown 1996), and in the three‐spined stickleback (Gasterosteus aculeatus) Linnaeus, 1758, wherein the size of the red color patch in males increased or decreased following intrasexual contests in accordance with the outcome (Candolin 1999).
To our knowledge, our data represent the first quantitative evidence that male coloration is subject to sexual selection in darters. Both the quantity (blue/red area PC scores) and quality (color PC scores) of male coloration appear to predict spawning success. Males that expressed a relatively greater amount of blue and red coloration, especially on the anal fin, were more successful competitively and reproductively. The relationship between color quality and male reproductive success was more complex: the red components of male coloration varied in a mostly consistent manner, in that males with redder fins and bodies attended more nosedigs and spawned more as the primary male. On the other hand, more successful males were bluer on the first dorsal fin and body, and less blue on the second dorsal and anal fins. Curiously, Zhou et al. (2014) found in E. spectabile and E. caeruleum that variation in red traits was more diverse than blue traits at the across‐species and across‐population levels. One possible explanation is that there are local selective forces that act consistently on red color within populations but lead to divergence between populations.
The signaling role of male coloration in E. spectabile must be considered with respect to the effect of body size, as many aspects of coloration vary consistently across males of different sizes (Zhou et al. 2014). Larger male E. spectabile were better able to guard the females and as a result spawned more as the primary male. The advantage of larger body size in intraspecific contests has been widely documented among animal taxa, including in a variety of teleost species (e.g., Rowland 1989; Quinn and Foote 1994; Moretz 2003; Thünken et al. 2011). Many species with strong male–male competition have evolved male color signals that advertise physical attributes, that is, “badges of status”, allowing males to avoid the costs of entering pointless contests with mismatched opponents (Rowher 1975; Maynard Smith and Parker 1976; Maynard Smith 1982). Across diverse species such as house sparrows (Passer domesticus) (Linnaeus, 1758) (Møller 1987), the paper wasp Polistes dominulus (Tibbetts and Dale 2004), and Iberian rock lizards (Lacerta monticola monticola) Boulenger, 1905 (Martín and López 2007), direct aggression between males increases with increasing badge similarity. Darter male colors may function similarly: confrontations between male E. spectabile begin with visual fin flaring displays that may or may not escalate to physical attacks (Zhou et al. 2015). We found that correlations between male coloration and reproductive success occurred mostly in the same directions as the correlations between coloration and body size; furthermore, we found the same general relationship between male coloration and reproductive success before and after adjusting for standard length. These results support the notion that color is the more proximate visual signal during male–male contests: for a given size of male, the more successful individuals are those that express coloration further in the direction consistent with larger size.
Red color traits may be particular targets of intrasexual selection in darters, as the ability to perceive long‐wavelength colors seems to be necessary to elicit conspecific male aggression in E. spectabile (Zhou et al. 2015). Reddish coloration in darters results from carotenoid pigments (Porter et al. 2002; Zhou et al. 2014); a large body of research supports the hypothesis that carotenoid pigments represent “honest” signals of male quality, as they must be acquired from dietary sources (Candolin 2000; Grether et al. 2001; Hill et al. 2002; Navara and Hill 2003; Griggio et al. 2007). For example, the size and intensity of carotenoid‐based epaulet ornaments in male red‐shouldered widowbirds (Euplectes ardens) (Boddaert, 1783) predicts fighting ability (Pryke et al. 2001; Pryke and Andersson 2003). Similarly, various aspects of the carotenoid‐based coloration on the frill of the frillneck lizard (Chlamydosaurus kingii) Gray, 1825 correlate with competitive ability (Hamilton et al. 2013). In E. caeruleum, the spectral characteristics of carotenoid‐based reddish coloration are correlated with parasite load (Ciccotto et al. 2014). It is therefore plausible that red coloration may be especially important when rival male E. spectabile are evaluating one another.
In the context of male–male contests, the consistent pattern of variation observed by different patches of red coloration across the body and dorsal fins of male E. spectabile may also represent a case of redundant signaling, that is, multiple signals that together allow males to more thoroughly assess the condition of rival males. Redundant signals are predicted to be more prevalent in aggregating species due to a lowered cost of making assessments (Møller and Pomiankowski 1993; Hebets and Papaj 2005). During the spawning season, E. spectabile of both sexes congregate in areas with appropriate substrate, and the high number of encounters between males may favor redundant signaling so as to avoid the costs of unneeded aggression.
Female‐guarding behavior was observed in all trials irrespective of the mean male size within the tank; therefore, sneaking does not appear to be a phenotypically distinct strategy in E. spectabile as in some other teleost species (Oliveira et al. 2005), but rather a plastic behavioral response to relative competitive inferiority. Given that sneaking is an opportunistic act, our finding that male coloration correlates weakly, if at all, with sneaking behavior is not surprising.
A persistent complication in the study of sexually selected traits is that such traits are often involved in both attracting mates and deterring rivals, resulting in an interaction of intersexual and intrasexual selective forces that may or may not be congruent (Moore 1990; Berglund et al. 1996; Kodric‐Brown 1996; Hunt et al. 2009). Competition among males for access to females appears to have played a central role in the evolution of sexually dimorphic coloration in E. spectabile, and likely also in other darter species such as E. caeruleum that share the same mating system (Winn 1958b). Given that female E. spectabile do not appear to exhibit mating preferences (Pyron 1995), male breeding coloration in this species may be solely attributable to intrasexual competition. Sexual selection processes in darters may therefore illustrate the potential of male–male contests to promote the evolution of sexual ornaments.
Conflict of Interest
None declared.
Acknowledgments
This project was approved by the University of Illinois Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (IACUC), and all fish were treated in compliance with IACUC protocol no. 10063. We would like to thank N. Flores‐Hutton, J. Kadich, E. Lee, T. Lin, A.J. Mon, F. Rodriguez, and K. Tarrant for their assistance during the project. Funding for this research was provided by the National Science Foundation (Award no. 1210743).
References
- Andersson, M. 1994. Sexual selection. Princeton Univ. Press, Princeton, NJ. [Google Scholar]
- Baird, T. A. , Baird T. D., and Shine R.. 2013. Showing red: male coloration signals same‐sex rivals in an Australian water dragon. Herpetologica 69:436–444. [Google Scholar]
- Bajer, K. , Molnár O., Török J., and Herczeg G.. 2011. Ultraviolet nuptial colour determines fight success in male European green lizards (Lacerta viridis). Biol. Lett. 7:866–868. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Benson, K. E. , and Basolo A. L.. 2006. Male–male competition and the sword in male swordtails, Xiphophorus helleri . Anim. Behav. 71:129–134. [Google Scholar]
- Berglund, A. , Bisazza A., and Pilastro A.. 1996. Armaments and ornaments: an evolutionary explanation of traits of dual utility. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. Lond. 58:385–399. [Google Scholar]
- Candolin, U. 1999. Male–male competition facilitates female choice in sticklebacks. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 266:785–789. [Google Scholar]
- Candolin, U. 2000. Changes in expression and honesty of sexual signalling over the reproductive lifetime of sticklebacks. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 267:2425–2430. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ciccotto, P. J. , Dresser D. J., and Mendelson T. C.. 2014. Association between parasite load and orange, but not blue, male nuptial colouration in Etheostoma caeruleum . J. Fish Biol. 84:1590–1598. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Croft, D. P. , Krause J., and James R.. 2004. Social networks in the guppy (Poecilia reticulata). Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 271:S516–S519. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Croft, D. P. , James R., Ward A. J. W., Botham M. S., Mawdsley D., and Krause J.. 2005. Assortative interactions and social networks in fish. Oecologia 143:211–219. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crothers, L. , Gering E., and Cummings M.. 2011. Aposematic signal variation predicts male‐male interactions in a polymorphic poison frog. Evolution 65:599–605. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Darwin, C. 1871. The descent of man and selection in relation to sex. John Murray, London. [Google Scholar]
- Fuller, R. C. 2003. Disentangling female mate choice and male competition in the rainbow darter, Etheostoma caeruleum . Copeia 2003:138–148. [Google Scholar]
- Grether, G. F. 1996. Intrasexual competition alone favors a sexually dimorphic ornament in the rubyspot damselfly Hetaerina americana . Evolution 50:1949–1957. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grether, G. F. , Hudon J., and Endler J. A.. 2001. Carotenoid scarcity, synthetic pteridine pigments and the evolution of sexual coloration in guppies (Poecilia reticulata). Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 268:1245–1253. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Griggio, M. , Serra L., Licheri D., Monti A., and Pilastro A.. 2007. Armaments and ornaments in the rock sparrow: a possible dual utility of a carotenoid‐based feather signal. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 61:423–443. [Google Scholar]
- Gumm, J. M. , and Mendelson T. C.. 2011. The evolution of multi‐component visual signals in darters (genus Etheostoma). Curr. Zool. 57:125–139. [Google Scholar]
- Hamilton, D. G. , Whiting M. J., and Pryke S. R.. 2013. Fiery frills: carotenoid‐based coloration predicts contest success in frillneck lizards. Behav. Ecol. 24:1138–1149. [Google Scholar]
- Hebets, E. A. , and Papaj D. R.. 2005. Complex signal function: developing a framework of testable hypotheses. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 57:197–214. [Google Scholar]
- Hill, G. E. , Inouye C. Y., and Montgomerie R.. 2002. Dietary carotenoids predict plumage coloration in wild house finches. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 269:1119–1124. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hunt, J. , Breuker C. J., Sadowski J. A., and Moore A. J.. 2009. Male–male competition, female mate choice and their interaction: determining total sexual selection. J. Evol. Biol. 22:13–26. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jawor, J. M. , and Breitwisch R.. 2003. Melanin ornaments, honesty, and sexual selection. Auk 120:249–265. [Google Scholar]
- Johnstone, R. A. , and Norris K.. 1993. Badges of status and the cost of aggression. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 32:127–134. [Google Scholar]
- Kodric‐Brown, A. 1996. Role of male–male competition and female choice in the development of breeding coloration in pupfish (Cyprinodon pecosensis). Behav. Ecol. 7:431–437. [Google Scholar]
- Leblond, C. , and Reebs S. G.. 2006. Individual leadership and boldness in shoals of golden shiners (Notemigonus crysoleucas). Behaviour 143:1263–1280. [Google Scholar]
- Ligon, J. D. , Thornhill R., Zuk M., and Johnson K.. 1990. Male–male competition, ornamentation and the role of testosterone in sexual selection in red jungle fowl. Anim. Behav. 40:367–373. [Google Scholar]
- Martín, J. , and López P.. 2007. Scent may signal fighting ability in male Iberian rock lizards. Biol. Lett. 3:125–127. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martin, M. D. , and Mendelson T. C.. 2013. Incomplete behavioural isolation and asymmetric female preference in darter sister species (Percidae: Etheostoma). J. Fish Biol. 83:1371–1380. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mateos, C. , and Carranza J.. 1997. The role of bright plumage in male‐male interactions in the ring‐necked pheasant. Anim. Behav. 54:1205–1214. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maynard Smith, J. 1982. Evolution and the theory of games. Cambridge Univ. Press, Cambridge, UK. [Google Scholar]
- Maynard Smith, J. , and Brown R. L. W.. 1986. Competition and body size. Theor. Popul. Biol. 30:166–179. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maynard Smith, J. , and Parker G. A.. 1976. The logic of asymmetric contests. Anim. Behav. 24:159–175. [Google Scholar]
- Mendelson, T. C. 2003. Sexual isolation evolves faster than hybrid inviability in a diverse and sexually dimorphic genus of fish (Percidae: Etheostoma). Evolution 57:317–327. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Møller, A. P. 1987. Variation in badge size in male house sparrows Passer domesticus: evidence for status signalling. Anim. Behav. 35:1637–1644. [Google Scholar]
- Møller, A. P. , and Pomiankowski A.. 1993. Why have birds got multiple sexual ornaments? Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 32:167–176. [Google Scholar]
- Moore, A. J. 1990. The evolution of sexual dimorphism by sexual selection: the separate effects of intrasexual selection and intersexual selection. Evolution 44:315–331. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moretz, J. A. 2003. Aggression and RHP in the northern swordtail fish, Xiphophorus cortezi: the relationship between size and contest dynamics in male‐male competition. Ethology 109:995–1008. [Google Scholar]
- Navara, K. J. , and Hill G. E.. 2003. Dietary carotenoid pigments and immune function in a songbird with extensive carotenoid‐based plumage coloration. Behav. Ecol. 14:909–916. [Google Scholar]
- Oliveira, R. F. , Ros A. F., and Gonçalves D. M.. 2005. Intra‐sexual variation in male reproduction in teleost fish: a comparative approach. Horm. Behav. 48:430–439. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Page, L. M. 1983. Handbook of darters. TFH Publications, Saddle Brook, NJ. [Google Scholar]
- Parker, G. A. 1974. Assessment strategy and the evolution of fighting behaviour. J. Theor. Biol. 47:223–243. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Phillips, C. T. , and Fries J. N.. 2009. An evaluation of visible implant elastomer for marking the federally listed fountain darter and the San Marcos salamander. N. Am. J. Fish. Manage. 29:529–532. [Google Scholar]
- Porter, B. A. , Fiumera A. C., and Avise J. C.. 2002. Egg mimicry and allopaternal care: two mate‐attracting tactics by which nesting striped darter (Etheostoma virgatum) males enhance reproductive success. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 51:350–359. [Google Scholar]
- Pryke, S. R. , and Andersson S.. 2003. Carotenoid‐based epaulettes reveal male competitive ability: experiments with resident and floater red‐shouldered widowbirds. Anim. Behav. 66:217–224. [Google Scholar]
- Pryke, S. R. , Lawes M. J., and Andersson S.. 2001. Agonistic carotenoid signalling in male red‐collared widowbirds: aggression related to the colour signal of both the territory owner and model intruder. Anim. Behav. 62:695–704. [Google Scholar]
- Pyron, M. 1995. Mating patterns and a test for female mate choice in Etheostoma spectabile (Pisces, Percidae). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 36:407–412. [Google Scholar]
- Quinn, T. P. , and Foote C. J.. 1994. The effects of body size and sexual dimorphism on the reproductive behaviour of sockeye salmon, Oncorhynchus nerka . Anim. Behav. 48:751–761. [Google Scholar]
- Reeves, C. D. 1907. The breeding habits of the rainbow darter (Etheostoma coeruleum Storer), a study in sexual selection. Biol. Bull. 14:35–59. [Google Scholar]
- Rohwer, S. 1982. The evolution of reliable and unreliable badges of fighting ability. Am. Zool. 22:531–546. [Google Scholar]
- Rowher, S. 1975. The social significance of avian winter plumage variability. Evolution 29:593–610. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rowland, W. J. 1989. The effects of body size, aggression and nuptial coloration on competition for territories in male threespine sticklebacks, Gasterosteus aculeatus . Anim. Behav. 37:282–289. [Google Scholar]
- Thünken, T. , Baldauf S. A., Kullmann H., Schuld J., Hesse S., and Bakker T. C.. 2011. Size‐related inbreeding preference and competitiveness in male Pelvicachromis taeniatus (Cichlidae). Behav. Ecol. 22:358–362. [Google Scholar]
- Tibbetts, E. A. , and Dale J.. 2004. A socially enforced signal of quality in a paper wasp. Nature 432:218–222. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weston, M. R. , and Johnson R. L.. 2008. Visible implant elastomer as a tool for marking etheostomine darters (Actinopterygii: Percidae). Southeast. Nat. 7:159–164. [Google Scholar]
- Whiting, M. J. , Nagy K. A., and Bateman P. W.. 2003. Evolution and maintenance of status‐signalling badges: experimental manipulations in lizards Pp. 47–82 in Fox S. F., McCoy J. K. and Baird T. A., eds. Lizard social behavior. Johns Hopkins Press, Baltimore, MD. [Google Scholar]
- Williams, T. H. , Gumm J. M., and Mendelson T. C.. 2013. Sexual selection acting on a speciation trait in darters (Percidae: Etheostoma). Behav. Ecol. 24:1407–1414. [Google Scholar]
- Winn, H. E. 1958a. Comparative reproductive behavior and ecology of fourteen species of darters (Pisces‐Percidae). Ecol. Monogr. 28:155–191. [Google Scholar]
- Winn, H. E. 1958b. Observation on the reproductive habits of darters (Pisces‐Percidae). Am. Midl. Nat. 59:190–212. [Google Scholar]
- Zahavi, A. 1977. Reliability in communication systems and the evolution of altruism Pp. 253–259 in Stonehouse B. and Perrins C. M., eds. Evolutionary ecology. Macmillan, London. [Google Scholar]
- Zahavi, A. 1981. Natural selection, sexual selection and the selection of signals Pp. l33–138 in Scudder G. G. E., Reveal J. L., eds. Evolution today: Proceedings of the Second International Congress of systematics and evolution. Carnegie Mellon University, Pittsburgh, PA. [Google Scholar]
- Zhou, M. , Johnson A. J., and Fuller R. C.. 2014. Patterns of male breeding color variation differ across species, populations, and body size in rainbow and orangethroat darters. Copeia 2014:297–308. [Google Scholar]
- Zhou, M. , Loew E. R., and Fuller R. C.. 2015. Sexually asymmetric colour‐based species discrimination in orangethroat darters. Anim. Behav. 106:171–179. [Google Scholar]