Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2017 May 3.
Published in final edited form as: Neuroscience. 2015 Nov 10;321:121–137. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.11.009

Using Mice to Model Obsessive Compulsive Disorder: From Genes to Circuits

Susanne E Ahmari 1,2
PMCID: PMC4843997  NIHMSID: NIHMS744001  PMID: 26562431

1. Introduction

Obsessive Compulsive Disorder (OCD) is a severe, chronic, and highly prevalent psychiatric disorder that affects between 1.5–3% of people worldwide, independent of ethnicity and cultural group studied1. According to the 1996 report by the World Health Organization, it was listed as the world’s 10th leading cause of illness-related disability2. In the US alone, the costs associated with treatment of OCD and work loss due to symptoms are estimated at up to 10 billion dollars annually3. Yet despite its severity, high prevalence, and clear societal cost, current OCD therapies are only partially effective. This is in part because OCD remains under-diagnosed, under-treated, and understudied due to lack of recognition by health care providers, stigma associated with symptoms by patients, and lack of understanding of the seriousness of the illness by the general public and research organizations. In order to ultimately develop improved treatments for this severe mental illness, we need further research to gain an improved understanding of the pathophysiology that underlies obsessions and compulsions.

Though studies in OCD patients can provide some insight into the disease process, studies in humans are inherently limited in their ability to dissect pathologic processes because of their non-invasive nature. The recent development of strategies for genetic and circuit-specific manipulation in rodent models finally allows us to identify the molecular, cellular, and circuit events that lead to abnormal repetitive behaviors and affect dysregulation relevant to OCD. This review will highlight recent studies in mouse model systems that have used transgenic and optogenetic tools in combination with classic pharmacology and behavioral techniques to advance our understanding of these pathologic processes.

1.1. Clinical Features of OCD

Despite recent changes to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual (DSM-5), the core clinical features of OCD remain the same4,5. OCD consists of obsessions, which are recurrent, persistent, intrusive thoughts, impulses, or images; or compulsions, which are repetitive behaviors or mental acts that patients typically engage in to reduce the severe anxiety or dread that is associated with the obsessions68. Though both obsessions and compulsions are not required to meet diagnostic criteria for OCD, both are typically present9 and tend to be linked together, such that a compulsion is performed in response to a particular obsessive thought. Notably, although OCD is no longer classified as an anxiety disorder according to DSM-5, anxiety symptoms are a prominent feature in many OCD patients10,11. Specifically, obsessions are often associated with significant distress, and compulsions are typically performed in a conscious attempt to reduce this severe distress. For example, an intrusive thought about one’s hands being contaminated would typically be accompanied by a spike in anxiety followed by a compulsive hand-washing ritual, and completion of the ritual would lead to a temporary decrease in anxiety. Although performing compulsions can provide immediate relief, it is typically fleeting. In fact, execution of these rituals is actually believed to strengthen dysfunctional neural circuits that underlie OCD, leading ultimately to increased anxiety and symptom persistence. These illness features are important to consider when developing neurobiological models of the disorder.

It is clear that OCD is a heterogeneous disorder12, but it is less clear how the illness should be divided into subgroups. This is an active area of investigation, and therefore many metrics currently exist for ‘carving the joints’. First, there is strong evidence that tic-related OCD is neurobiologically distinct, with different neurochemical features, distinct striatal pathophysiology, and higher prevalence in males13. There have also been suggestions that early-onset OCD (mean age of onset between 7.5 and 12.5 years14) may correspond to a distinct subtype with different genetic and environmental underpinnings15. Variations in level of insight into OCD symptoms may also represent biologically meaningful differences16, and are now delineated by specifiers in DSM-5. Finally, differences in specific content of obsessions and compulsions may potentially correspond to distinct neurobiological substrates17, as most clearly demonstrated for hoarding (now a separate disorder in DSM-5)18,19.

1.2. Neural Circuit Abnormalities Associated with OCD

Over the past 20 years, functional and structural neuroimaging studies have led to the discovery of aberrant neural circuits in OCD patients. A remarkable convergence of findings from neuroanatomical and functional studies collectively implicates cortico-striato-thalamo-cortical (CSTC) circuits in OCD pathophysiology2023. Though discrepancies are found in the directionality of findings, this is typically attributed to either heterogeneity of illness, or differences in stage of development or illness course. These findings are described in detail below.

1.2.1. Structural neuroimaging

Although exact findings have varied across studies, structural abnormalities in CSTC circuits involving orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), and striatum have been repeatedly demonstrated in OCD2426. The largest structural MRI study to date reported reduced medial OFC gray matter and increased gray matter in the highly-connected27,28 ventral striatum (i.e. ventral putamen, nucleus accumbens, and olfactory tubercle)29. In addition, a recent meta-analysis reported reduced volumes of left ACC and bilateral OFC, and increased thalamic volumes bilaterally, but no differences in basal ganglia volumes relative to control samples30. However, another meta-analysis demonstrated changes in basal ganglia (i.e. increased bilateral caudate gray matter volume) as well as decreased bilateral ACC volume, in OCD patients31, while a recent mega-analysis demonstrated a reduction in ACC, dorsomedial PFC, and inferior frontal gyrus volumes, with group-by-age interactions in putamen, OFC, and insula26. Finally, a study combining structural MRI and behavioral testing demonstrated that impairment on a response inhibition task (Stop-Signal Task) in both OCD patients and unaffected first-degree relatives was correlated with decreased grey matter in OFC and right inferior frontal cortex, and increased grey matter in cingulate cortex, parietal cortex, and striatum32. Thus, structural imaging studies in OCD have collectively demonstrated changes in ACC, OFC, and striatal volume, although there are inconsistencies in directionality of findings between studies, particularly in the striatum.

It is important to consider the potential reasons for these inconsistencies in order to develop pathophysiologic models of OCD. Likely key contributors to variation between structural imaging studies are relatively small samples, inclusion of heterogeneous patient populations (i.e. different ages, comorbidities, medication status, and length of illness course), and methodological differences, including exact brain regions studied. For example, older region of interest studies focused exclusively on areas that were thought a priori to be involved in OCD pathophysiology, and tended to use more subjective manual and semi-automatic methods to measure brain volumes. To overcome these limitations, more recent studies have used unbiased automated whole-brain voxel-based morphometry approaches; however, these have tended to be limited by small sample sizes, potentially rendering them prone to the generation of false positives. In an attempt to correct these deficiencies, the two meta-analyses discussed briefly above attempted to synthesize discrepant findings in region-of-interest studies30 and voxel-based morphometry studies33, respectively. Synthesizing region-of-interest studies which had highly variable divisions for particular brain regions in the original articles, Rotge et al reported reduced volumes in left ACC and bilateral OFC in OCD, and increased thalamic volumes bilaterally, but no changes in caudate volume. However, of the 14 studies examined, caudate volume was examined in only 3 studies, 2 of which were from pediatric samples. When the remaining adult study was examined, a decrease in caudate volume was observed, but the ability to generalize findings from this single study is limited. In contrast, by examining voxel-based morphometry studies in an attempt to reduce a priori bias, Radua et al found evidence for increased bilateral caudate gray matter volume, as well as decreased bilateral ACC volume in OCD patients. Additionally, they showed that more severe OCD patients were more likely to report increased caudate volumes, which could have accounted for the discrepancies with Rotge et al, which had significant heterogeneity in terms of illness length, medication status, and treatment-refractory nature of patients.

In an attempt to overcome the limitations of both of these meta-analyses, de Wit et al 26 performed a voxel-based morphometry mega-analysis, in which they obtained raw data from 780 participants and re-analyzed it using a uniform pipeline for pre-processing and analysis. This again demonstrated a reduction in ACC volume, consistent with the prior two meta-analyses. However, in contrast, this mega-analysis showed a relative preservation of both OFC and putamen with aging compared to healthy controls, suggesting possible activation-mediated neural plasticity leading to a relative volume increase as patients age. They suggest that the lack of findings in the caudate may be due to differences in age, treatment history, and medication status, and also note that data for 530 out of 780 participants in this study has not been published before; differences from past studies could therefore be due to increased power in the mega-analysis. Whether methodological differences also contribute to these differences will only become clear when the original source data are published.

1.2.2. Functional neuroimaging

Similar to findings from structural studies, OFC, ACC, and caudate (specifically the head) have likewise been implicated in OCD using PET and fMRI; functional studies also highlight anterior thalamus17,20,34. These brain regions are linked by well-described neuroanatomical connections24. Notably, OCD subjects demonstrate hyperactivity in these areas both at rest and with symptom provocation, though OFC shows the most robust activation20. In further support of the role of this regional hyperactivity in symptom generation, most studies have found that successful serotonin reuptake inhibitor (SRI) or cognitive behavioral therapy treatment was associated with reduced activity in OFC or caudate, with decreased ACC activity being less prominent35,36. Finally, recent fMRI studies of resting state connectivity have also generally supported a role for cortical-basal ganglia circuit dysfunction in OCD, demonstrating abnormal connectivity of orbitofrontal cortex3740, anterior cingulate39,41, ventral striatum37,3941, dorsal striatum40,41, putamen38,41, and anterior thalamus41. However, other regions including subthalamic nucleus38, cerebellum41,42, and temporal cortex42 have also been implicated, and directionality of findings varies across studies. Discrepancies in resting state data are less prominent than those seen in structural imaging studies, likely because fewer studies exist. While three prior studies have demonstrated increased resting state connectivity between striatum and OFC (using both seed-based37,40 and graph theory38 approaches), one recent seed-based study found the opposite result– i.e., reduced functional connectivity between OFC and ventral striatum in OCD39. One theory for this discrepancy is that Posner et al exclusively examined unmedicated patients, and that observations of increased functional connectivity in the other studies could stem from medication effects. This theory is called into question by Beucke et al’s study, which used different methodology (graph theory) to show reduced local connectivity in ventral striatum in an unmedicated patient subgroup; however, in addition to the methodological differences, Posner et al’s subjects were extremely clean (almost half of patients medication naïve; average of 94 weeks off medication in other patients; very little co-morbidity). Determining how medication status, illness course, and potential compensatory changes may relate to resting state fMRI findings in OCD is a key direction for future studies.

1.2.3. Cognitive activation studies

Based on the theory that circuit dysfunction in particular mental illnesses may only be unmasked during performance of neurocognitive tasks, there has been a recent shift towards performing OCD imaging studies during cognitive activation paradigms. Many tasks have been used, though executive functions have been particularly emphasized23. First, several studies have shown hyperactivity of dorsal ACC (dACC) in OCD patients during performance of tasks involving error monitoring and/or conflict resolution, suggesting that dACC and connected regions might function differently in OCD43; these findings correlate well with baseline functional studies. In addition, studies have used Go/NoGo tasks to assess inhibitory control in OCD, both of which report decreased activation of OFC44,45. Similarly, greater frontostriatal activation has been demonstrated in unmedicated OCD patients during engagement of control and conflict resolution on the Simon task46. Finally, decreased activation of the lateral OFC, as well as the lateral PFC and parietal cortex, has been demonstrated in both OCD patients and their unaffected first-degree relatives in a reversal learning task47. Overall, these findings support the idea that cortical-basal ganglia circuits are dysfunctional in OCD, and may contribute to symptom generation.

1.2.4. Working Model of OCD Pathophysiology

By synthesizing the studies reviewed above, several models of OCD pathophysiology have been proposed22,48. Though models differ in details, they consistently share the idea that obsessions and compulsions somehow result from malfunctioning neural circuits that include OFC, ACC, caudate, anterior thalamus, and/or ventral striatum. However, as discussed in detail above, it is important to consider the fact that some structural and resting state fMRI studies have shown differential effects, leading to challenges in developing a fully integrated model of circuit-based OCD pathophysiology. Potential factors include methodological differences between studies, inclusion of heterogeneous patient populations, and exact brain regions studied. Although further study is warranted to tease out the key factors that account for these differences, in aggregate, these findings can be synthesized by an integrative model which prioritizes functional, rather than structural, neuroimaging findings. PET and fMRI studies have consistently demonstrated hyperactivity in OFC, striatum, and thalamus at baseline and with symptom provocation; this meshes well with the increased OFC-striatal connectivity seen in most resting state fMRI studies in OCD. Though structural studies have been less consistent, this could be explained both by the methodological differences described in detail above, and by the fact that either decreases or increases in volume of individual brain structures could potentially lead to similar functional outcomes by disrupting normal inter-regional communication. Finally, the specific brain regions affected may depend on the particular OCD subtype. For example, based on functional imaging studies, it has been proposed that different OCD symptom dimensions (e.g., symmetry/ordering vs. washing/cleaning) may have different underlying neural substrates within CSTC circuits12. Thus, different OCD subtypes could have distinct core neurobiologic deficits leading to differences in both neuroimaging findings and neurocognitive task performance. However, as discussed above (see Section 1.1), the most accurate way to segregate OCD into different subtypes is still a matter of active investigation and debate. For example, symptom dimensions frequently change over time in individuals with OCD49, suggesting either that the functional status of the underlying neural substrates is also fluctuating, or that it may not be possible to broadly delineate subtypes using symptom dimensions.

In line with models of CSTC dysfunction in OCD, evidence from recent human studies suggests that OCD patients have dysfunction in core neural processes mapped onto CSTC circuits, such as response inhibition e.g., response inhibition; 50,51,52, sensorimotor gating53, and fear-learning54, as will be discussed further in Section 3. In addition, a group of studies that examined the balance between goal-directed versus habitual behavior in OCD patients is particularly interesting. Although the ways in which goal-directed and habitual performance cooperate and/or interfere with each other in healthy subjects is still an area of active investigation (see Balleine & O’Doherty for comprehensive review55), there is growing evidence that patients with OCD are biased to perform habits, sometimes at the expense of performance goal-directed actions, which may be impaired at baseline56. Interestingly, this bias towards increased habit formation in OCD not only applies to appetitive habits, it also extends to avoidant habits that may be more relevant to the clinical symptoms seen in patients57,58. Though it is challenging in general to make direct links between dysfunctional neural processes and symptoms in patients (highlighted by Gillan et al’s finding that avoidance habits did not correlate with the YBOCS compulsion subscale57), the possibility that impaired regulation of the goal-directed behavior/habit balance contributes to symptom generation is intriguing.

Another leading pathophysiologic model that is not mutually exclusive is based on the fact that different populations of striatal spiny projection neurons differentially regulate the direct and indirect basal ganglia pathways. Given the known functions of the direct pathway (i.e. striatum→globus pallidus interna→substantia nigra) and indirect pathway (i.e. striatum→globus pallidus externa→subthalamic nucleus) in modulating thalamic input to cortex and in generating motor patterns, this has led to the hypothesis that OCD symptoms result from excess activity in direct versus indirect OFC-subcortical pathways59. This imbalance could lead to OCD symptoms in a variety of ways. For example, increased direct pathway activity could lead to decreased inhibition of thalamus, which in turn would decrease filtering of intrusive thoughts and images to cortex, thus triggering compulsions. Alternatively, it is possible that OCD symptoms stem from increased firing of excitatory neurons in cortical areas including mOFC, ACC, or supplementary motor area, ultimately generating intrusive thoughts and images that override other sensorimotor input 60. In turn, this could trigger ritualistic compulsions driven by striatum through persistent activation of the direct pathway. However, it is important to note that many different models could be proposed to synthesize findings from neuroimaging and neurocognitive studies in OCD, and that no single proposal can provide an explanatory model for all findings in the literature–in part because of heterogeneity of patients, as well as heterogeneity of tasks used, measurement parameters, and specific brain regions studied. This is highlighted by two recent animal studies discussed further below, which indicate it is possible that hyperactivity in medial OFC regions connected to limbic structures may lead to OCD-relevant symptoms61, while hyperactivity in lateral OFC regions important for inhibitory control may lead to symptom reduction62. As described below, studies in rodents can be used to directly tease out these differences and test these models.

1.2.5. Other candidate regions

Though current models suggest that dysfunctional CSTC circuits are important in generation and/or maintenance of OCD symptoms, evidence for involvement of other structures is beginning to accumulate43. For example, while CSTC models do not currently provide a clear explanation for increased anxiety observed in OCD, exaggerated responses in amygdala observed after presentation of OCD-specific stimuli could be responsible63. Furthermore, although dACC has been classically linked to conflict monitoring/obsessions in OCD, there is evidence that it also plays a role in expression of fear responses64. dACC hyperactivation could therefore explain increased anxiety observed in OCD patients. Finally, recent studies have demonstrated that OCD patients have impaired extinction recall in a fear-conditioning paradigm, with accompanying alterations in cerebellum, posterior cingulate, and putamen activity during extinction recall, and reduced hippocampus and caudate activation during fear extinction54. Integration of other brain structures may therefore be necessary to generate a satisfying explanatory model of OCD pathophysiology and symptom generation.

2. Testing Proposed Etiologies in Animal Model Systems

2.1. Transgenic Models of OCD

A role for genetics in OCD is supported by evidence from both twin and family studies65, and increased heritability in pediatric-onset OCD suggests an even greater genetic contribution66. In many other complex psychiatric disorders, investigation of etiology using transgenic technology has been aided by the identification of families with highly penetrant gene abnormalities. For example, microdeletions in chromosomal region 22q11.2 are associated with a 20–30 fold increased risk for schizophrenia67, and mouse models of the 22q11.2 deletion syndrome have led to significant insights into the potential role of this region in generating cognitive deficits in schizophrenia68. Similarly, a family was recently identified in which Tourette’s Syndrome was linked with a rare but highly penetrant mutation in histidine decarboxylase, the key enzyme required for biosynthesis of histidine69,70. Parallel studies were then performed in histidine decarboxylase knockout mice to attempt to identify pathophysiologic mechanisms of Tourette’s syndrome, and dopaminergic abnormalities in the striatum were observed71. In contrast to these examples, the field of OCD research has been hindered by the lack of identification of families carrying rare genetic variants of large effect. In addition, genome-wide association studies (GWAS) in OCD have failed to identify common variants that reach genome-wide significance, though this is still an active area of investigation since it is most likely that common variants provide the most important genetic contribution in OCD72,73,74. This has limited the investigation of OCD pathophysiology using transgenic technology. Despite this fact, several transgenic mouse models have serendipitously yielded significant insights into pathophysiologic mechanisms underlying abnormal repetitive behaviors, and are now providing potential links back to the human disorder via targeted genetic screens in OCD patients. In this section, we will present findings from these models, as well as discuss potential future directions.

2.1.1. Hoxb8 knockout mice

One of the first transgenic models suggested to have relevance for OCD was the Hoxb8 knockout (KO). These knockout mice were initially generated to pursue the investigation of the role of Hox genes in neurodevelopmental patterning. After constitutive knockout of Hoxb8, a striking behavioral pattern emerged. Mice began to over-groom themselves, and persisted in this behavior even after developing severe facial and body lesions75. Because Hoxb8 is also located in the periphery and Hoxb8 KOs demonstrated defects in nociception (which could theoretically drive excessive grooming behavior due to lack of peripheral sensory feedback), a follow-up study was performed to localize the cells responsible for the phenotype76. In this study, Chen et al observed that the only cells in the brain labeled by the Hoxb8 lineage were bone-marrow derived microglia76. Strikingly, when they performed bone marrow transplants in HoxB8 KOs whose brains had been irradiated to remove all resident knockout microglia, they observed a complete reversal of the abnormal grooming behavior accompanied by repopulation of the brain with microglia from the healthy transplant donor mouse. This intervention did not rescue the nociceptive deficits, suggesting that the grooming abnormalities were not linked to peripheral nervous system defects. Though future studies will be needed to further specify the locus of these effects in the brain, together, these two studies generally highlighted the potential importance of perseverative grooming as a mouse behavior relevant to OCD, and raised the intriguing possibility that microglial pathology could be relevant to this disorder. Though further work needs to be performed, this finding is of particular clinical interest because of the potential link between pediatric-onset OCD and autoimmune dysregulation, which has been proposed as a factor in PANDAS (Pediatric Autoimmune Neuropsychiatric Disorders Associated with Streptococcal Infection)77,78. Additional human genetic studies will also be necessary to clarify the link between Hoxb8 gene disruption and the human OCD phenotype.

2.1.2. Slc1A1 knockout mice

One of the more consistently replicated genetic findings in OCD is an association with the neuronal glutamate transporter SLC1A1 (protein: EAAT3 or EAAC1)7985, although a recent meta-analysis showed only a modest association of 2/9 SNPs with OCD86, and SLC1A1 has not emerged as a probable locus from recent GWAS studies72,73. Findings cluster in the 3′ region, with most evidence for association with the rs301430C allele. In cell models and brain tissue, this allele is associated with increased SLC1A1 expression, suggesting that overexpression contributes to OCD susceptibility80. Coding variants are very rare (3/1400 subjects screened) and do not clearly segregate with OCD87,88. Thus, noncoding polymorphisms most likely account for the association of SLC1A1 with OCD.

Though SLC1A1 knockout mice do not demonstrate clear OCD-relevant phenotypes, they have not yet been screened in targeted behavioral tests89. In addition, and perhaps more importantly, it is likely that brain-wide deletion is less relevant to OCD pathophysiology than targeted alteration of expression. Given that the SLC1A1 non-coding variants most clearly associated with OCD appear to be associated with increased gene expression, over-expression of the gene may be more relevant to OCD pathophysiology. Ongoing studies are therefore investigating whether tissue-specific manipulations of SLC1A1 are more relevant to the human clinical phenotype. Examining the outcome of targeted expression changes in specific neural circuits will allow us to directly address the molecular, cellular, and behavioral impact of this OCD candidate gene.

2.1.3. SAPAP3 knockout mice

More recently, it was serendipitously determined that SAPAP3, a post-synaptic density protein concentrated in cortico-striatal synapses, may be relevant to particular OCD subtypes90. Targeted knockout of this gene led to the development of perseverative grooming behavior resulting in facial lesions, generating a phenotype that was strikingly similar to that observed in the Hoxb8 KO model. However, in addition to the abnormally elevated grooming, SAPAP3 KOs also displayed increased anxiety when tested in the elevated zero maze. Interestingly, both of these behaviors were reversed after administration of fluoxetine at 5mg/kg for 6 days, which likely represents a subchronic administration paradigm when compared to human treatment regimens. Lentiviral rescue of SAPAP3 via broad injections in the striatum normalized both the grooming and the anxiety-like behaviors. Furthermore, in vitro electrophysiologic investigation began to determine the pathophysiologic mechanisms underlying these abnormal behaviors by demonstrating that SAPAP3 KOs had decreased fEPSP amplitudes compared to controls (dominated by AMPAR transmission in the recording conditions used), and increased NMDAR fEPSPs; this is consistent with their observation of altered NMDA receptor subunit composition reflecting a more immature ‘juvenile’ subtype (i.e. increased levels of NR2B and decreased levels of NR2A subunits). An additional study isolated these disturbances to cortico-striatal, rather than thalamo-striatal, synapses91. Though genetic investigation in a relatively small group of patients based on these results found no association of SAPAP3 SNPs with OCD, it did find associations in a subset of OCD patients with grooming disorders (e.g. pathological skin-picking, trichotillomania, nail-biting)92. It is important to note that despite these findings, SAPAP3 has not neared genome wide significance in OCD GWAS studies to date. However, the DLGAP1 gene (alternative name for another SAPAP family member, SAPAP1) appears to be emerging as a leading candidate from GWAS studies (though it has not reached unambiguous genome-wide significance), potentially providing further evidence of a role for this gene family in OCD pathophysiology93. Independent of the human studies, the findings from SAPAP3 KO mice clearly demonstrate a link between molecular changes at cortico-striatal synapses and repetitive pathological behaviors.

2.1.4. Slitrk5 knockout mice

In a similar unexpected finding, Shmelkov et al (2010)94 determined that knockout of the Slitrk5 post-synaptic protein led to abnormal behaviors potentially relevant to OCD. Slitrk5 is a transmembrane protein that is enriched at cortico-striatal synapses, and consists of an extracellular domain that shares homology with the Slit/Robo family of axon guidance molecules, and an intercellular domain that shares homology with the trkB receptor. Though the function of these proteins is still under investigation, they have been implicated in neuronal survival, outgrowth of neuronal processes, and synapse formation95. Interestingly, although an a priori link was previously established between Slitrk1 and Tourette’s Syndrome based on genetic studies, knockout of the Slitrk1 gene did not lead to abnormal repetitive behaviors; rather, it led to increased anxiety measured using the elevated plus maze, and increased immobility time on the forced swim and tail suspension tests96. In contrast, targeted inactivation of Slitrk5, which had a comparatively unknown function and no previous link to psychiatric disorders, led to perseverative grooming resulting in lesions and increased anxiety. Both of these behaviors were reversed by treatment with chronic, but not acute, fluoxetine, which is consistent with the pharmacologic response profile seen in OCD patients97,98. Notably, Slitrk5 KOs also showed selective overactivation of OFC, as measured by baseline expression of the immediate early gene, FosB, providing another parallel to human fMRI and PET studies demonstrating baseline OFC hyperactivation. In addition, they show decreased expression of striatal glutamate receptors, with decreased levels of GluR1, GluR2, NR2A, and NR2B. Combined with the findings from SAPAP3 KO mice discussed above, this suggests a model whereby disruption of striatal post-synaptic density proteins leads to abnormal trafficking and insertion of glutamate receptors in striatal synapses, and abnormal cortico-striatal communication. Whether these disruptions in synaptic function are specific to particular projections or neuronal subtypes remains to be determined. In an attempt to link these findings back to humans, preliminary evidence indicates the presence of rare Slitrk5 genetic variants in OCD patients; however, these findings must still be validated (F. Lee, personal communication).

2.1.5. Specificity of repetitive behaviors for OCD?

Notably, many transgenic strains billed as animal models of OCD have similar behavioral profiles as those seen in animal models of highly comorbid disorders, such as Tourette’s syndrome and autism. For example, the two transgenic OCD animal models most studied to date (SAPAP3 and Slitrk5 KO mice) both demonstrate increased perseverative grooming leading to facial lesions and anxiety, and Hoxb8 KOs similarly demonstrate perseverative, self-destructive grooming. These phenotypes are most strikingly shared with mouse models of autism99 [e.g. Shank3 KOs100 and BTBR101). It is therefore an increasingly important topic for the field to identify shared versus specific neural circuit abnormalities and behavioral phenotypes that can distinguish animal models of distinct but comorbid complex psychiatric disorders.

2.1.6. Future Directions

The transgenic models described above have been incredibly useful for generating new insights into potential pathophysiologic mechanisms underlying OCD-relevant behaviors. By generating new transgenic models based on more definitive GWAS studies, or combining multiple genes of small effect in a single animal model, the field may be able to better recapitulate the neurodevelopmental progression of OCD in preclinical models.

2.2. Strategies for Testing Function of OCD-related Circuits in Rodent Models

As described above, convergent evidence from human functional and structural imaging studies implicates cortico-striatal-thalamic circuit dysfunction in OCD. However, because causality cannot be directly tested in human studies, it is not possible to determine whether this dysfunction leads to OCD symptoms, is a compensatory response that results from attempts to fight symptoms, or is simply an epiphenomenon. New technologic approaches can now be used to tease these possibilities apart. In this section, we will review advanced circuit dissection techniques and recent studies from the OCD animal literature that exploit novel approaches to determine whether CSTC circuits are necessary and/or sufficient to either generate or alleviate OCD-like symptoms in mice.

2.2.1. Transgenic Circuit Dissection

One of the earliest direct investigations testing the potential role of cortico-striatal circuits in OCD-related behaviors was performed by Campbell et al102. Hypothesizing that cortico-striatal circuit activation would lead to OCD-like behaviors, they creatively used transgenic technology to drive expression of the active subunit of cholera toxin under the control of the dopamine 1 receptor (D1) promoter (D1-CT mice). This led to excessive stimulation of a random subset of D1-expressing neurons, which were concentrated in layers 2 and 3 of the somatosensory cortex (which stimulate layer V striatal-projecting cortical neurons in motor cortex), the amygdala intercalated nucleus (which ultimately regulates amygdala output to ventral striatum), and layer 2 of the piriform cortex (which stimulates ventral striatum/nucleus accumbens). These animals displayed an interesting and complex phenotype. Whereas normal mice tend to engage in many different behaviors for short periods of time in their natural environment and show frequent behavioral switching, D1-CT mice demonstrated behavioral perseveration independent of the behavior they were engaged in, and were less likely to switch to a new behavior. This general tendency towards perseveration independent of the specific behavior involved may have particular relevance for OCD compared to other disorders demonstrating abnormal repetitive behaviors, because compulsions observed in OCD tend to be composed of multiple behaviors performed in sequence, as opposed to more stereotyped repetitive behaviors such as are typically observed in autism (repetitive hand-flapping, rocking) or Tourette’s syndrome (repetitive stereotyped motor or vocal tics). In addition, there is evidence that compulsive rituals in OCD tend to be more flexible over time in comparison to stereotyped repetitive behaviors seen in other disorders49; which compulsion is performed in response to an intrusive thought can change depending on context, environmental circumstances, or life stage, suggesting that the abnormal motor programs are not as rigid as those observed in other disorders. Because the cholera toxin was expressed in a random distributed subset of neurons and the systems level effects on circuit activation are therefore complex, the specific neuronal subpopulations responsible for these intriguing behavioral findings remain to be determined. Also, since the D1-CT transgene stimulates cortical layer 2/3 neurons, rather than directly stimulating cortico-striatal afferents, the construct validity for OCD is debatable. As will be seen below, new technologic advances now permit refined manipulation of specific circuits in order to identify which brain subregions and cell types are involved in the generation of abnormal repetitive behaviors.

2.2.2. Optogenetic Dissection

Though many different theories exist regarding the etiology of OCD symptoms, two major models of OCD pathophysiology have recently been investigated using optogenetic technology. First, as discussed briefly above, it is has been suggested that OCD symptoms result from deficits in inhibitory control. This concept is based on a group of studies demonstrating either behavioral deficits in OCD patients during inhibitory control tasks such as the Stop Signal Task (SST) and Go/No-Go, or abnormal activation of cortical and striatal regions during normal performance on these tasks32,51,103105. Although findings in the human literature are not always consistent across studies (see 106), this body of work has led to the proposal that compulsive behaviors in OCD emerge during failures of normal inhibitory processes. By extension, this theory suggests that improvements in inhibitory control may decrease the frequency of performance of compulsions.

To investigate this theory in an animal model system, Burguiere et al62 trained SAPAP3 KOs and wild-type mice in a conditioned grooming task. In response to an unconditioned stimulus (drop of water on the forehead), both SAPAP3 KOs and wild-type controls demonstrated increased grooming. If a conditioned stimulus (tone) was played before the water drop was delivered, both WT and SAPAP3 KOs acquired conditioned responses and groomed in response to the tone early in the training paradigm. However, later in training, when WT mice began to inhibit their response to the tone, instead delaying grooming until the water drop was presented, SAPAP3 KO mice continued to groom both in response to the tone and in response to the water drop. These findings were interpreted as evidence of impaired inhibitory control in the SAPAP3 KO mice. To next determine if inhibitory control could be enhanced in the SAPAP3 KOs via manipulation of cortico-striatal circuit function, the authors expressed the excitatory light-activated ion channel, channelrhodopsin (ChR2), in the lateral orbitofrontal cortex (lOFC) of both SAPAP3 KOs and wild-type controls. Activation of ChR2 (10Hz 473nm blue light) in either lOFC cell bodies or lOFC terminals projecting to the centromedial striatum in SAPAP3 KOs led to a shift in grooming responses away from the tone (conditioned stimulus) towards the water drop (unconditioned stimulus), thus rendering the behavioral profile more like a healthy control. This was accompanied by normalization of the striatal firing rate, which was increased at baseline compared to controls. Further experiments indicated that the impaired behavior and excessive striatal firing resulted from decreased functioning of fast-spiking interneurons in SAPAP3 KO striatum, and that 10Hz stimulation of lOFC to centromedial striatum projections compensated for the impaired interneuron function. This intriguing paper highlighted the potential importance of lOFC activation in overcoming response inhibition deficits, which may provide mechanistic insight into observations of lOFC hypofunction in OCD patients during neurocognitive tasks.

A second (but not mutually exclusive) theory suggests that OCD does not result from primary deficits in inhibitory control, but rather stems from a stronger drive to perform abnormal compulsive behaviors in response to either a greatly increased frequency of intrusive thoughts or increased likelihood that intrusive thoughts will persist. In this scenario, even if cognitive control processes are functioning normally, they may not be sufficient to inhibit the performance of compulsions. In the context of this theory, the etiology of the increased drive is still unknown, but could result from increased baseline activation of the direct vs. indirect pathway 59, abnormal reward processing leading to increased performance of goal-directed behaviors107, or enhanced motivation to perform avoidance-related behaviors108.

A second set of optogenetic experiments investigating OCD-relevant behaviors using mouse models may lend support to this theory. To simulate the orbitofrontal cortex and striatal hyperactivity observed in OCD patients in human functional imaging studies, Ahmari et al61 injected ChR2 into glutamatergic neurons of medial OFC (mOFC). By selectively stimulating striatal terminals on projections from mOFC to ventromedial striatum (VMS), we were able to activate only mOFC-VMS synapses, and determine the impact of stimulation on OCD-relevant behaviors. While acute optogenetic stimulation of mOFC-VMS projections had no impact on OCD-like behaviors, instead leading to an increase in locomotion, repeated stimulation for only five minutes a day over the course of 5–7 days led to a gradual and progressive increase in grooming behavior. Notably, this increase in grooming was not time-locked to the stimulation, suggesting that neural plasticity changes were required for the development of the abnormal perseverative grooming. Though an alternative interpretation would be that the brain regions responsible for the abnormal behaviors are downstream of the OFC-VMS synapse, the plasticity theory was supported by our finding that repeated stimulation of OFC-VMS projections led to an increase in evoked firing rate at mOFC-VMS synapses as measured by in vivo electrophysiology. Further supporting the potential relevance of these findings to OCD, we observed that chronic (but not acute) treatment with high-dose fluoxetine (18mg/kg) normalized both the repetitive grooming behavior and the increase in evoked firing rate induced by repeated optogenetic stimulation. Because serotonin reuptake inhibitors such as fluoxetine are effective in a subset of OCD patients when used chronically at high doses, this further supports the potential relevance of these circuits both in the pathology and the treatment of OCD. Furthermore, these findings support the idea that increased pathologic drive through OFC-striatal circuits can lead to increased selection of pathological actions. Ongoing work is examining the relationship of these processes to reward learning, and further dissecting the molecular and cellular mechanisms underlying the progressive evolution of abnormal repetitive behaviors.

2.2.3. Chemogenetic Dissection

Another recently developed set of technologies, which fall broadly under the category of chemogenetics, are also currently being used in animal models to probe the neural circuits linked to OCD. These tools allow for cell-type specific expression of mutant G-protein coupled receptors (GPCRs) that can be activated by administration of small synthetic molecules; the mutant GPCRs are known as DREADDs (Designer Receptors Exclusively Activated by Designer Drugs). The designer drugs can be administered either systemically via drinking water or intraperitoneal injection, or locally via intracerebral infusion, thus maximizing the flexibility of the system. Since these artificially synthesized drugs have been designed to be otherwise inert, after administration they only activate receptors that are localized on the cell populations of interest. Whereas optogenetic technology allows for rapid and specific activation and inhibition, DREADD technology yields more sustained activation and inhibition that may be more consistent with pathophysiologic processes. Though this technology has not yet formally been applied to the investigation of OCD-related behaviors, it is currently being used to investigate the neural circuits underlying behavioral functions that may be relevant to OCD, as further described in Section 3.

3. Leveraging Rodent Behavioral Assays Relevant to OCD

As highlighted in the above discussion, significant progress has been made in identification of genes and circuits whose dysfunction leads to abnormal repetitive behaviors that may have relevance to OCD and related disorders. In order to capitalize on these advances, it is important to combine these transgenic, optogenetic, and chemogenetic methods with the rich history of rodent behavioral models examining OCD-relevant tasks. While some of these behavioral models have been developed with an eye towards face validity– i.e. tasks which lead to the development of perseverative behaviors that ‘look like’ compulsive rituals in OCD– others directly translate tasks which show abnormalities in human OCD patients into rodent models. In the sections below, we will first give a brief overview of non-transgenic behaviorally-based animal models that demonstrate face validity for perseverative behaviors, and finish by discussing promising behavioral tasks identified in human OCD studies that are currently being dissected using rodent model systems.

3.1. Spontaneous Perseverative Behaviors

The study of OCD using animal models is grounded in a longstanding literature based on observations of animals engaged in spontaneous, stereotyped, seemingly purposeless or self-destructive behaviors. These include barbering (repetitive hair biting and pulling), acral paw-lick (repetitive paw-licking leading to lesions in dogs), and zoo-related stereotypies (e.g. pacing in stereotyped patterns around cages) (see 109111 for review). In contrast to these examples, which tend to be observed in captivity or in stressful conditions, deer mice, which have been suggested as an ethological model of OCD, demonstrate repetitive running, jumping, and flipping behaviors at baseline112. Though the exact relationship between these spontaneous repetitive behaviors and OCD is unclear, and the role of stress in their generation may be confounding, these models can be used to dissect circuits underlying stereotyped behaviors via either targeted lesions or drug injections through stereotactically-placed cannulae. For example, striatal in vivo microdialysis in deer mice showed that increased glutamate directly preceded stereotyped behaviors, lending support to the hypothesis that dysfunction in glutamatergic signaling may lead to OCD symptomatology113. This example demonstrates the potential utility of models showing spontaneous repetitive behaviors for both generating and testing hypotheses about pathophysiology.

3.2. Interactions with Physical Objects

Early investigations of non-spontaneous OCD-related behaviors generally observed animal behavior during interactions with artificial objects, and determined the extent of the repetitive or stereotyped nature of these actions. Examples include nestlet shredding114, screen gnawing115, digging116, and marble burying114, which has gained particular prominence as a tool for genetic screening in recent years. Of note, it is difficult to determine the relevance of findings from these assays to human OCD, because many of these tests are species specific (e.g. screen gnawing and nestlet shredding). In addition, the pharmacologic response profile of digging-related tasks, particularly marble burying, may be more consistent with that of generalized anxiety (i.e. responsive to benzodiazepines) than of OCD117; however, there is also evidence that marble burying is not correlated with anxiety measures in the open field and light-dark test, and may be more appropriately considered as a proxy measure of repetitive digging118. All told, it is therefore likely that more complex behavioral paradigms may have greater translational relevance for the study of OCD neural circuitry.

3.3. Pharmacologically-induced Perseverative Behaviors

Classic pharmacologic approaches have also been used to study abnormal repetitive behaviors. Though much of this literature focuses on stereotyped behaviors generated by amphetamine administration, two pharmacologic stimulation models have been investigated which may have greater relevance to the abnormal repetitive behaviors observed in OCD.

3.3.1. Quinpirole-induced checking

In the late 1990’s, Szechtman et al119 discovered that chronic administration of the D2/D3 agonist quinpirole led to the development of compulsive checking behavior in rats. Specifically, in an open field environment, rats revisited objects excessively, and performed specific behavioral sequences when revisiting these preferred locations. These behaviors were partially attenuated by clomipramine, the tricyclic antidepressant with the most selectivity for serotonin reuptake inhibition and one of the first-line treatments for OCD. Interestingly, this finding of partial (but not full) attenuation may actually suggest pharmacologic relevance of this model to the majority of OCD patients, who demonstrate partial treatment responses to serotonin reuptake inhibition. Further work has explored the utility of this model for the investigation of the mechanism of action for deep brain stimulation (DBS) treatment in OCD, and has provided preclinical evidence supporting a potential role for high frequency DBS of both STN120 and nucleus accumbens121. Furthermore, recent work has extended these findings by demonstrating that quinipirole also increases checking-like behavior in an operant-based observing response task with uncertain reinforcement122. Thus, the quinpirole-induced checking model may yield insights into pathophysiologic mechanisms relevant to OCD, and provide some utility as a preclinical screen for pharmacologic treatments.

3.3.2. Serotonin-1B Receptor (5-HT1B) Agonist Administration

Although the role of the 5-HT1B receptor in aggressive and impulsive behaviors has been more clearly delineated123128, some evidence from pharmacologic challenge studies129 also suggests that abnormalities in 5-HT1B receptor function (sometimes referred to as 5-HT1D-beta in human literature) play a role in OCD. In addition, though weak, some genetic association studies provide tentative aggregate support for a role in OCD130; however there was a lack of an association in a recent meta-analysis74. Studies based on these findings investigated the potential relationship between 5-HT1B stimulation and the development of abnormal repetitive behaviors. Shanahan et al found that injection of a 5-HT1B agonist leads both to perseverative locomotion and prepulse inhibition deficits (also observed in OCD patients), both of which are reversed with chronic, but not acute, fluoxetine treatment131. A follow-up study localized the responsible receptors to the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) 132, providing parallels to the circuit abnormalities seen in OCD patients, and a more recent study indicated that 5-HT1B agonist treatment also led to impaired performance on the delayed alternation task133, paralleling impaired performance in OCD patients (see Section 3.4.2). A strength of this model is that it exhibits strong predictive validity for the chronic time course and high serotonin reuptake inhibitor dose necessary for effective OCD treatment in the subset of patients who are treatment responders.

3.4. Tasks Demonstrating Abnormal Performance in OCD Patients

With expansion in the number of complex rodent behavioral tasks, and increased availability of animal paradigms that are designed to directly mimic human tasks, we are now better able to directly assess neurocognitive functions relevant to OCD using animal models. Many recent human studies have uncovered either altered performance or abnormal brain activation patterns in OCD patients during behavioral tasks that can be translated into rodents. Though a detailed review of the animal literature related to circuit dissection of each of these paradigms is beyond the scope of this review, we will highlight the findings from the human literature, as well as important next steps in animal models given these results.

3.4.1. Reversal Learning

Reversal learning is an important measure of cognitive flexibility, which is thought to be impaired in OCD patients. This is manifested symptomatically in both the tendency to repeatedly perform compulsive ritualized behaviors, and the difficulty of engaging in alternative behaviors in response to obsessive thoughts. Abnormal reversal learning performance (either impaired learning or slower reaction times) and altered neural activity during reversal learning combine to form one of the more well-replicated neurocognitive findings in OCD patients47,134136. Improved reversal learning has been demonstrated following treatment with a serotonin reuptake inhibitor in rodents137139, while serotonin depletion leads to impairments in human subjects140142, suggesting that involved neural mechanisms may be relevant to OCD pathophysiology. Dissection of neural circuits underlying reversal learning in rats has made substantial progress, and has focused on the lateral OFC and ventral striatum as key involved regions. Whether similar brain regions are involved in reversal learning in mice remains to be seen, though Gourley and colleagues have demonstrated involvement of mOFC in a mouse instrumental reversal paradigm143. Future work will link findings from mice and rats, and leverage new technologies to determine how online manipulation of activity in specific brain regions impacts this cognitive process.

3.4.2. Delayed Alternation

Another translatable behavioral task that has been proposed to measure behavioral reversal in the context of a distinct aspect of set-shifting is the delayed alternation task144,145. Though this task shares some similarities with reversal learning tasks described above (i.e. it is thought to measure the ability to learn a rule and then subsequently inhibit and/or reverse it to achieve reward145; it has been shown to be dependent on OFC function146), and has a distinct structure which may require different learning mechanisms that have different underlying neural substrates. Specifically, it requires immediate shifting of strategy after a correct response, as opposed to learning and maintaining a particular set and then adopting a new strategy when the rule is changed. Deficits on this task have been observed in OCD patients with consistency147150, and more recently, have been isolated to perseverative errors following correct responses147. Translating the delayed alternation task to mice may therefore yield insight into potential molecular and circuit mechanisms relevant to OCD, as highlighted by Woehrle et al133, who have shown that 5-HT1B receptor activation leads to impaired delayed alternation task performance, and that this deficit is prevented by chronic fluoxetine pre-treatment.

3.4.3. Habit Learning

As discussed briefly above, exciting new developments in the OCD clinical literature have highlighted the potential importance of abnormal habit learning in the pathophysiology of OCD. Recent studies demonstrated that OCD patients shift towards habitual rather than goal-directed behavior on an instrumental learning task. Although participants with OCD were able to flexibly adjust their behavior during the training phase of the task in order to optimize the likelihood of receiving rewards in response to stimuli, they showed overreliance on habits and impaired flexible goal-directed responding during the subsequent ‘slips of action’ and outcome devaluation task56. Further support for impairments in goal-directed behavior was provided in a subsequent study examining counterfactual decision-making in OCD patients, which is the ability to prospectively compare the expected results of action-outcome scenarios, and use these findings to guide economic choices. Compared to healthy controls, OCD patients had disrupted goal-directed forward modeling, relying instead on current expected value to make their economic decisions; this is consistent with a model of disrupted goal-directed cognitive behavior in OCD151. Furthermore, two recent studies in separate cohorts of patients have determined that this overreliance on habit formation also extends to habits formed in avoidance of a feared stimulus, which may be more relevant to the symptoms observed in OCD patients57,108. In these studies, subjects were overtrained on a shock-avoidance task. Though a devaluation sensitivity task showed that both OCD patients and controls could inhibit unnecessary behavioral responses before overtraining, OCD patients exhibited greater avoidance habits after overtraining, despite appropriate ratings of shock expectancy. Subsequent fMRI demonstrated hyperactivation of the caudate in OCD patients with excessive habits, as well as hyperactivation of mOFC during the acquisition of avoidance behavior.

These human studies suggest that the switch from goal-directed behaviors to habits is an important area for future investigation in preclinical OCD research. Optogenetic and chemogenetic approaches are now being applied to understand the neural circuit mechanisms underlying this switch. Towards this goal, two recent studies investigated the development of habitual behavior using a combination of optogenetics and in vivo electrophysiology in awake behaving animals. In the first study, rats were overtrained on a maze task to develop habitual behavior. When infralimbic cortex activity was subsequently disrupted using halorhodopsin, online blockade of habitual behavior occurred within an average of 2–3 trials152. This led to the suggestion that the manifestation of habits is under cortical control, and that modulation of the involved circuits could potentially serve as a targeted treatment for disorders with excessive habit formation. In the second study, they observed that the shift from goal-directed to habitual behavior was correlated with activation of neural ensembles in the infralimbic cortex and sensorimotor striatum, and that the upper layers of the infralimbic cortex closely tracked habit states late in overtraining. Subsequent optogenetic disruption of infralimbic cortex activity using halorhodopsin during overtraining again prevented habit formation153.

In a more recent study, a novel instrumental lever-pressing task was developed in which individual mice switched between goal-directed (Random Ratio schedule) and habitual actions (Random Interval schedule), allowing within-subject comparisons. By performing simultaneous in vivo recordings in OFC, dorsomedial striatum, and dorsolateral striatum during this paradigm, they demonstrated that the same neurons have different activity patterns depending on whether an individual lever press is goal-directed or habitual, suggesting the absence of distinct neuronal ensembles that control goal-directed vs habitual actions. They also showed that OFC and dorsomedial striatum become more engaged when lever presses are goal-directed, whereas dorsolateral striatum becomes less active. Reinforcing the importance of the OFC for goal-directed behavior, they found that OFC inhibition using DREADDS led to a disruption of goal-directed lever presses, while selective optogenetic activation of OFC led to increased lever pressing for goal-directed actions (Random Ratio schedule) only in the devalued condition, and did not simply increase the number of lever-presses overall. Notably, optogenetic stimulation did not change habitual actions (Random Interval schedule). The authors suggest that these results reveal an important role for OFC in re-evaluating outcomes to guide appropriate performance of instrumental actions, which may have implications for the generation of behaviors that are compulsively performed even when the outcome is devalued107. The relationship of such behaviors to the compulsions observed in OCD remains to be determined.

3.4.4. Sensorimotor Gating

Deficits in sensorimotor gating as measured by prepulse inhibition (PPI), a neural process that has been linked to cortico-stratial-pallido-pontine circuits154, have been observed in many psychiatric disorders. Though PPI deficits are classically associated with schizophrenia, they have also been observed in OCD53,155,156. The reflexive PPI response is typically calculated by measuring the amplitude of the eyeblink in response to a startling auditory stimulus, and then determining how much the response is inhibited by presentation of a lower decibel ‘prepulse’ before the startling stimulus. Our recent publication suggests that PPI deficits may be more prominent in patients who have a history of past or current tic disorder53, which may yield clues regarding differences in the neural circuitry underlying different OCD subtypes–specifically, tic-related vs non-tic-related OCD. This finding may also offer a circuit-level explanation for the extensive comorbidity between schizophrenia, Tourette’s Syndrome, and OCD, all of which demonstrate PPI deficits. Though the neural circuitry of PPI has not yet been explored using optogenetic approaches, further dissection of this important translational endophenotype may help to further address these questions.

3.4.5. Response Inhibition

As discussed in Section 2.2 above, a prominent theory in the OCD human literature suggests that performance of compulsive behaviors may be related to deficits in response inhibition. According to this theory, when presented with a trigger (for example, a stove), someone who suffers from OCD would have difficulty inhibiting their conditioned response to the trigger (for example, checking repeatedly to make sure the stove is turned off in order to prevent the house from burning down). Though this theory contrasts with the idea that OCD may be more related to excessive drive to perform actions (i.e. behavior that is goal-directed but is focused on maladaptive/misdirected actions), these concepts are not mutually exclusive–i.e. subthreshold impairments in response inhibition could couple with an increased propensity towards goal-directed actions, leading to the performance of compulsive behaviors. In keeping with this concept, several studies have now demonstrated impaired response inhibition in adults with OCD and/or first-degree relatives using a standard Stop Signal Task32,52,105,157, which assesses the ability to inhibit a prepotent motor response. Similarly, there is evidence for abnormal brain activation in OCD patients during response inhibition measured with other tasks, including the Stroop36,158,159 and the Simon tasks160. Though in sum, a recent meta-analysis suggests that the effect sizes observed across studies of response inhibition in OCD are not clinically significant106,161, another recent study suggests differently51. Thus, sufficient positive data have been reported that more detailed investigation of the neural correlates underlying response inhibition is warranted. Significant inroads have already been made using refined lesion and pharmacologic blockade studies in rats (for example, see162164); future studies will more precisely dissect the neural circuits and specific cell types involved using advanced technologies in mouse model systems.

3.4.6. Fear Learning

Though OCD is no longer classified as an anxiety disorder according to DSM-5, it is widely accepted that anxiety is an important component of the disorder. Though it is still unclear whether anxiety plays an etiologic role in the generation of OCD or instead is a consequence of symptoms, from a clinical perspective, ritualized behaviors are typically performed with the conscious intent of reducing anxiety or dread associated with obsessive thoughts. Notably, this is an effective, albeit temporary, strategy for short-term resolution of anxiety, although it ultimately leads to maintenance or even strengthening of anxiety due to continuous reinforcement of compulsive behaviors.

To further explore the relationship between OCD and anxiety, researchers have recently begun to examine the construct of fear learning in OCD patients. Milad et al (2013)54 demonstrated that although OCD patients have normal fear learning when compared to healthy controls, they have impaired fear extinction recall when responses are examined 1 day later. This has significant implications for whether there are particular OCD subpopulations that would have difficulty maintaining gains that they obtained while engaged in exposure therapy due to extinction recall impairment.

Another aspect of fear learning which is just beginning to be explored in OCD is the formation of habits in avoidance. As discussed above, there is increasing evidence from the human literature that people with OCD are more prone to forming habits than healthy controls. Recent work by Gillan et al108 has demonstrated that this does not occur only for appetitive habits, but also for habits in avoidance. In fact, it has been suggested that avoidance habits may have even more direct relevance to OCD than appetitive habits, since compulsions in OCD patients tend to be performed specifically to avoid or diminish the impact of anxiety-provoking stimuli. Paradigms to explore this construct in rodents are now being developed165 to facilitate dissection of the involved circuits.

3.4.7. Translation from rat to mouse

Though the rich behavioral paradigms described above have classically been developed and used in rats, these complex paradigms are now increasingly being translated into murine models to take advantage of core facilities offering widely-available genetically-based tools for assaying cell-specific functions–e.g. cell- and tissue-specific Cre transgenic lines (Gensat; Allen Brain Atlas; JAX Cre resource); and cell-specific optogenetic and chemogenetic viral vectors (Penn Vector Core; UNC Viral Vector Core). The development of CRISPR technology is beginning to make these cell-specific interventions more feasible in rats, and will ultimately pave the way towards using similar approaches in primate species such as marmosets. Thus, future studies may be able to identify the cells and circuits underlying higher order cognitive functions relevant to OCD in primate species with cortical development similar to humans.

4. Conclusions

The Future: Application of Findings from Animal Models to Develop Targeted Treatments

The examples described above clearly demonstrate the utility of animal models for investigation of pathologic abnormal repetitive behaviors. However, critical evaluation of transgenic, pharmacologic, optogenetic, and chemogenetic models is crucial to determine their relevance to OCD. As described in Section 3, using translatable probes of neural circuits that are reliably abnormal in OCD patients is one method for validation, to help ensure that dissection of molecular and cellular abnormalities will ultimately yield information relevant for treatment development166.

Since the development of improved treatments is one of the major goals of preclinical neuropsychiatric research, it is important to consider how the different approaches described above may be useful for development of different types of treatment. For example, transgenic animal models may be particularly useful for drug development where high-throughput screens are required, because of the ability to rapidly generate large cohorts with relatively stable behavioral abnormalities. In contrast, optogenetic and chemogenetic techniques may be more important for the development and refinement of circuit-based treatment approaches, including deep brain stimulation (DBS) and transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS). Finally, as cumulative evidence begins to highlight abnormalities of particular neurocognitive functions in OCD, such as sensorimotor gating, reversal learning, response inhibition, goal-directed versus habitual behavior, and fear-extinction, we will be able to apply findings from the rich literature investigating the basic neurobiology of these core neural processes to gain improved understanding of circuit dysfunction in OCD, which may be helpful for development of psychotherapeutic approaches. Detailed investigation of these behavioral constructs, which have relevance to both OCD and other psychiatric disorders, may ultimately lead to the development of treatments for symptoms that cross disease boundaries.

Highlights.

  • OCD is a severe, chronic, and prevalent disorder that affects 1.5–3% of people

  • Animal models can be used to dissect pathologic changes underlying OCD

  • Methodologic advances allow precise manipulation of genes and circuits in mice

  • Combining these tools with behavior can advance our understanding of OCD pathology

Footnotes

Disclosure/Conflicts of Interest

No specific funding was received for this work. Dr. Ahmari has no conflicts of interest to disclose.

Financial Disclosures for the previous 12 months

The following grants are reported over the past 12 months. Dr. Ahmari: NIMH K08MH087718, NIMH R21 MH096200, NARSAD Young Investigator Award, Burroughs Wellcome CAMS Award, MQ Fellows Award, and NIMH R01MH104255.

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

References

  • 1.Koran LM. Quality of life in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychiatric Clinics of North America. 2000;23:509–517. doi: 10.1016/s0193-953x(05)70177-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Murray CJ, LA . The Global Burden of Disease: a comprehensive assessment of mortality and disability from diseases, injuries and risk factors in 1990 and projected to 2020. I. Harvard School of Public Health; Global Burden of Disease and Injury Series. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Eaton WW, et al. The burden of mental disorders. Epidemiologic reviews. 2008;30:1–14. doi: 10.1093/epirev/mxn011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Goodman WK, Grice DE, Lapidus KA, Coffey BJ. Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder. Psychiatr Clin North Am. 2014;37:257–267. doi: 10.1016/j.psc.2014.06.004. S0193-953X(14)00058-6 [pii] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Leckman JF, et al. Obsessive-compulsive disorder: a review of the diagnostic criteria and possible subtypes and dimensional specifiers for DSM-V. Depress Anxiety. 2010;27:507–527. doi: 10.1002/da.20669. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Monzani B, Rijsdijk F, Harris J, Mataix-Cols D. The structure of genetic and environmental risk factors for dimensional representations of DSM-5 obsessive-compulsive spectrum disorders. JAMA psychiatry. 2014;71:182–189. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.3524. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Van Ameringen M, Patterson B, Simpson W. DSM-5 obsessive-compulsive and related disorders: clinical implications of new criteria. Depression and anxiety. 2014;31:487–493. doi: 10.1002/da.22259. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Stein DJ, Craske MA, Friedman MJ, Phillips KA. Anxiety disorders, obsessive-compulsive and related disorders, trauma- and stressor-related disorders, and dissociative disorders in DSM-5. The American journal of psychiatry. 2014;171:611–613. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2014.14010003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Williams MT, et al. Myth of the pure obsessional type in obsessive--compulsive disorder. Depression and anxiety. 2011;28:495–500. doi: 10.1002/da.20820. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Diniz JB, et al. Outlining new frontiers for the comprehension of obsessive-compulsive disorder: a review of its relationship with fear and anxiety. Revista brasileira de psiquiatria. 2012;34(Suppl 1):S81–91. doi: 10.1590/s1516-44462012000500007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Bienvenu OJ, et al. Is obsessive-compulsive disorder an anxiety disorder, and what, if any, are spectrum conditions? A family study perspective. Psychological medicine. 2012;42:1–13. doi: 10.1017/S0033291711000742. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Mataix-Cols D, Rosario-Campos MC, Leckman JF. A multidimensional model of obsessive-compulsive disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2005;162:228–238. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.2.228. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Baxter LR, Jr, et al. Am J Psychiatry. 1988;145:1560–1563. doi: 10.1176/ajp.145.12.1560. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Geller DA. Obsessive-compulsive and spectrum disorders in children and adolescents. The Psychiatric clinics of North America. 2006;29:353–370. doi: 10.1016/j.psc.2006.02.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Eichstedt JA, Arnold SL. Childhood-onset obsessive-compulsive disorder: a tic-related subtype of OCD? Clin Psychol Rev. 2001;21:137–157. doi: 10.1016/s0272-7358(99)00044-6. S0272-7358(99)00044-6 [pii] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Leckman JF, et al. Obsessive-compulsive disorder: a review of the diagnostic criteria and possible subtypes and dimensional specifiers for DSM-V. Depression and Anxiety. 2010;27:507–527. doi: 10.1002/da.20669. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Mataix-Cols D, et al. Distinct neural correlates of washing, checking, and hoarding symptom dimensions in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2004;61:564–576. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.61.6.56461/6/564. pii. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Mataix-Cols D, et al. Hoarding disorder: a new diagnosis for DSM-V? Depress Anxiety. 2010;27:556–572. doi: 10.1002/da.20693. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Saxena S. Is compulsive hoarding a genetically and neurobiologically discrete syndrome? Implications for diagnostic classification. Am J Psychiatry. 2007;164:380–384. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.164.3.380. 164/3/380 [pii] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Rauch SL, Savage CR, Alpert NM, Fischman AJ, Jenike MA. The functional neuroanatomy of anxiety: a study of three disorders using positron emission tomography and symptom provocation. Biol Psychiatry. 1997;42:446–452. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(97)00145-5. S0006-3223(97)00145-5 [pii] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Rotge JY, et al. Anatomical alterations and symptom-related functional activity in obsessive-compulsive disorder are correlated in the lateral orbitofrontal cortex. Biol Psychiatry. 2010;67:e37–38. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.10.007. S0006-3223(09)01207-4 [pii] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Saxena S, Bota RG, Brody AL. Brain-behavior relationships in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Semin Clin Neuropsychiatry. 2001;6:82–101. doi: 10.1053/scnp.2001.21833. S1084361201000107 [pii] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Maia TV, Cooney RE, Peterson BS. The neural bases of obsessive-compulsive disorder in children and adults. Dev Psychopathol. 2008;20:1251–1283. doi: 10.1017/S0954579408000606S0954579408000606. pii. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Pittenger C, Bloch MH, Williams K. Glutamate abnormalities in obsessive compulsive disorder: neurobiology, pathophysiology, and treatment. Pharmacol Ther. 2011;132:314–332. doi: 10.1016/j.pharmthera.2011.09.006. S0163-7258(11)00184-7 [pii] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Rodman AM, et al. Neuroimaging contributions to novel surgical treatments for intractable obsessive-compulsive disorder. Expert review of neurotherapeutics. 2012;12:219–227. doi: 10.1586/ern.11.189. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.de Wit SJ, et al. Multicenter voxel-based morphometry mega-analysis of structural brain scans in obsessive-compulsive disorder. The American journal of psychiatry. 2014;171:340–349. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2013.13040574. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Di Martino A, et al. Functional connectivity of human striatum: a resting state FMRI study. Cerebral cortex. 2008;18:2735–2747. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Haber SN. The primate basal ganglia: parallel and integrative networks. Journal of chemical neuroanatomy. 2003;26:317–330. doi: 10.1016/j.jchemneu.2003.10.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Pujol J, et al. Mapping structural brain alterations in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Archives of general psychiatry. 2004;61:720–730. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.61.7.720. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Rotge JY, et al. Meta-analysis of brain volume changes in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biological psychiatry. 2009;65:75–83. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.06.019S0006-3223(08)00787-7. pii. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Radua J, Mataix-Cols D. Voxel-wise meta-analysis of grey matter changes in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Br J Psychiatry. 2009;195:393–402. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.108.055046195/5/393. pii. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Menzies L, et al. Neurocognitive endophenotypes of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Brain : a journal of neurology. 2007;130:3223–3236. doi: 10.1093/brain/awm205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Radua J, van den Heuvel OA, Surguladze S, Mataix-Cols D. Meta-analytical comparison of voxel-based morphometry studies in obsessive-compulsive disorder vs other anxiety disorders. Archives of general psychiatry. 2010;67:701–711. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.70. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Rotge JY, et al. Provocation of obsessive-compulsive symptoms: a quantitative voxel-based meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2008;33:405–412. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Rauch SL, et al. Predictors of fluvoxamine response in contamination-related obsessive compulsive disorder: a PET symptom provocation study. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2002;27:782–791. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(02)00351-2. S0893133X02003512 [pii] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Nakao T, et al. Brain activation of patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder during neuropsychological and symptom provocation tasks before and after symptom improvement: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Biological psychiatry. 2005;57:901–910. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.12.039. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Harrison BJ, et al. Brain corticostriatal systems and the major clinical symptom dimensions of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biological psychiatry. 2013;73:321–328. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.10.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Beucke JC, et al. Abnormally high degree connectivity of the orbitofrontal cortex in obsessive-compulsive disorder. JAMA psychiatry. 2013;70:619–629. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Posner J, et al. Reduced functional connectivity within the limbic cortico-striato-thalamo-cortical loop in unmedicated adults with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Human brain mapping. 2014;35:2852–2860. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22371. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Harrison BJ, et al. Altered corticostriatal functional connectivity in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Archives of general psychiatry. 2009;66:1189–1200. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.152. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Anticevic A, et al. Global resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging analysis identifies frontal cortex, striatal, and cerebellar dysconnectivity in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2014;75:595–605. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.10.021S0006-3223(13)00978-5. pii. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Hou JM, et al. Resting-state functional connectivity abnormalities in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder and their healthy first-degree relatives. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2014;39:304–311. doi: 10.1503/jpn.130220. pii. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Milad MR, Rauch SL. Obsessive-compulsive disorder: beyond segregated cortico-striatal pathways. Trends Cogn Sci. 2012;16:43–51. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.11.003. S1364-6613(11)00236-1 [pii] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Page LA, et al. A functional magnetic resonance imaging study of inhibitory control in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychiatry research. 2009;174:202–209. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2009.05.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Roth RM, et al. Event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging of response inhibition in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2007;62:901–909. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.12.007. S0006-3223(06)01555-1 [pii] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Marsh R, Maia TV, Peterson BS. Functional disturbances within frontostriatal circuits across multiple childhood psychopathologies. Am J Psychiatry. 2009;166:664–674. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.08091354. appi.ajp.2009.08091354[pii] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Chamberlain SR, et al. Orbitofrontal dysfunction in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder and their unaffected relatives. Science. 2008;321:421–422. doi: 10.1126/science.1154433. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Ting JT, Feng G. Neurobiology of obsessive-compulsive disorder: insights into neural circuitry dysfunction through mouse genetics. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2011;21:842–848. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2011.04.010. S0959-4388(11)00068-7 [pii] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Bloch MH, Landeros-Weisenberger A, Rosario MC, Pittenger C, Leckman JF. Meta-analysis of the symptom structure of obsessive-compulsive disorder. The American journal of psychiatry. 2008;165:1532–1542. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.08020320. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.van Velzen LS, Vriend C, de Wit SJ, van den Heuvel OA. Response inhibition and interference control in obsessive-compulsive spectrum disorders. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2014;8:419. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00419. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.van Velzen LS, Vriend C, de Wit SJ, van den Heuvel OA. Response inhibition and interference control in obsessive-compulsive spectrum disorders. Frontiers in human neuroscience. 2014;8:419. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00419. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Chamberlain SR, et al. Impaired cognitive flexibility and motor inhibition in unaffected first-degree relatives of patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2007;164:335–338. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.164.2.335. 164/2/335 [pii] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Ahmari SE, Risbrough VB, Geyer MA, Simpson HB. Impaired sensorimotor gating in unmedicated adults with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2012;37:1216–1223. doi: 10.1038/npp.2011.308. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Milad MR, et al. Deficits in conditioned fear extinction in obsessive-compulsive disorder and neurobiological changes in the fear circuit. JAMA psychiatry. 2013;70:608–618. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.914. quiz 554. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Balleine BW, O’Doherty JP. Human and rodent homologies in action control: corticostriatal determinants of goal-directed and habitual action. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35:48–69. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.131npp2009131. pii. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Gillan CM, et al. Disruption in the balance between goal-directed behavior and habit learning in obsessive-compulsive disorder. The American journal of psychiatry. 2011;168:718–726. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.10071062. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Gillan CM, et al. Enhanced avoidance habits in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biological psychiatry. 2014;75:631–638. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.02.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Gillan CM, et al. Functional Neuroimaging of Avoidance Habits in Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2014 doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2014.14040525. appiajp201414040525. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Saxena S, Brody AL, Schwartz JM, Baxter LR. Neuroimaging and frontal-subcortical circuitry in obsessive-compulsive disorder. The British journal of psychiatry. Supplement. 1998:26–37. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Russo M, et al. Obsessive-compulsive disorder: a “sensory-motor” problem? International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology. 2014;92:74–78. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2014.02.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Ahmari SE, et al. Repeated cortico-striatal stimulation generates persistent OCD-like behavior. Science. 2013;340:1234–1239. doi: 10.1126/science.1234733. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Burguiere E, Monteiro P, Feng G, Graybiel AM. Optogenetic stimulation of lateral orbitofronto-striatal pathway suppresses compulsive behaviors. Science. 2013;340:1243–1246. doi: 10.1126/science.1232380. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Simon D, Adler N, Kaufmann C, Kathmann N. Amygdala hyperactivation during symptom provocation in obsessive-compulsive disorder and its modulation by distraction. Neuroimage Clin. 2014;4:549–557. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2014.03.011S2213-1582(14)00041-2. pii. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Milad MR, et al. A role for the human dorsal anterior cingulate cortex in fear expression. Biol Psychiatry. 2007;62:1191–1194. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.04.032. S0006-3223(07)00401-5 [pii] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Pauls DL, Abramovitch A, Rauch SL, Geller DA. Obsessive-compulsive disorder: an integrative genetic and neurobiological perspective. Nature reviews. Neuroscience. 2014;15:410–424. doi: 10.1038/nrn3746. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Pauls DL. The genetics of obsessive compulsive disorder: a review of the evidence. American journal of medical genetics. 2008;148C:133–139. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.c.30168. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Bassett AS, et al. The schizophrenia phenotype in 22q11 deletion syndrome. The American journal of psychiatry. 2003;160:1580–1586. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.9.1580. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Sigurdsson T, Stark KL, Karayiorgou M, Gogos JA, Gordon JA. Impaired hippocampal-prefrontal synchrony in a genetic mouse model of schizophrenia. Nature. 2010;464:763–767. doi: 10.1038/nature08855. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Fernandez TV, et al. Rare copy number variants in tourette syndrome disrupt genes in histaminergic pathways and overlap with autism. Biological psychiatry. 2012;71:392–402. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.09.034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Ercan-Sencicek AG, et al. L-histidine decarboxylase and Tourette’s syndrome. The New England journal of medicine. 2010;362:1901–1908. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa0907006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Castellan Baldan L, et al. Histidine decarboxylase deficiency causes tourette syndrome: parallel findings in humans and mice. Neuron. 2014;81:77–90. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.10.052. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Mattheisen M, et al. Genome-wide association study in obsessive-compulsive disorder: results from the OCGAS. Mol Psychiatry. 2014 doi: 10.1038/mp.2014.43mp201443. pii. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Stewart SE, et al. Genome-wide association study of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Mol Psychiatry. 2013;18:788–798. doi: 10.1038/mp.2012.85mp201285. pii. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Taylor S. Molecular genetics of obsessive-compulsive disorder: a comprehensive meta-analysis of genetic association studies. Mol Psychiatry. 2013;18:799–805. doi: 10.1038/mp.2012.76. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Greer JM, Capecchi MR. Hoxb8 is required for normal grooming behavior in mice. Neuron. 2002;33:23–34. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(01)00564-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Chen SK, et al. Hematopoietic origin of pathological grooming in Hoxb8 mutant mice. Cell. 2010;141:775–785. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2010.03.055. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Bernstein GA, Victor AM, Pipal AJ, Williams KA. Comparison of clinical characteristics of pediatric autoimmune neuropsychiatric disorders associated with streptococcal infections and childhood obsessive-compulsive disorder. Journal of child and adolescent psychopharmacology. 2010;20:333–340. doi: 10.1089/cap.2010.0034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Snider LA, Swedo SE. PANDAS: current status and directions for research. Mol Psychiatry. 2004;9:900–907. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001542. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Wu K, Hanna GL, Rosenberg DR, Arnold PD. The role of glutamate signaling in the pathogenesis and treatment of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2012;100:726–735. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2011.10.007. S0091-3057(11)00329-7 [pii] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Arnold PD, Sicard T, Burroughs E, Richter MA, Kennedy JL. Glutamate transporter gene SLC1A1 associated with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2006;63:769–776. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.63.7.769. 63/7/769 [pii] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Dickel DE, et al. Association testing of the positional and functional candidate gene SLC1A1/EAAC1 in early-onset obsessive-compulsive disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2006;63:778–785. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.63.7.778. 63/7/778 [pii] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Shugart YY, et al. A family-based association study of the glutamate transporter gene SLC1A1 in obsessive-compulsive disorder in 378 families. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2009;150B:886–892. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30914. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Stewart SE, et al. Association of the SLC1A1 glutamate transporter gene and obsessive-compulsive disorder. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2007;144B:1027–1033. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30533. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Wendland JR, et al. A haplotype containing quantitative trait loci for SLC1A1 gene expression and its association with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2009;66:408–416. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.6. 66/4/408 [pii] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Samuels J, et al. Comprehensive family-based association study of the glutamate transporter gene SLC1A1 in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2011;156B:472–477. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.31184. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Stewart SE, et al. Meta-analysis of association between obsessive-compulsive disorder and the 3′ region of neuronal glutamate transporter gene SLC1A1. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2013;162B:367–379. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.32137. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Veenstra-VanderWeele JXT, Ruggiero AM, Anderson LR, Jones ST, Himle JA, Kennedy JL, Richter MA, Hanna GL, Arnold PD. Functional studies and rare variant screening of SLC1A1/EAAC1/EAAT3 in males with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychiatric Genetics. doi: 10.1097/YPG.0b013e328353fb63. In Press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Veenstra-VanderWeele J, et al. Genomic organization of the SLC1A1/EAAC1 gene and mutation screening in early-onset obsessive-compulsive disorder. Mol Psychiatry. 2001;6:160–167. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4000806. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Aoyama K, et al. Neuronal glutathione deficiency and age-dependent neurodegeneration in the EAAC1 deficient mouse. Nat Neurosci. 2006;9:119–126. doi: 10.1038/nn1609. nn1609 [pii] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Welch JM, et al. Cortico-striatal synaptic defects and OCD-like behaviours in Sapap3-mutant mice. Nature. 2007;448:894–900. doi: 10.1038/nature06104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Wan Y, et al. Circuit-selective striatal synaptic dysfunction in the Sapap3 knockout mouse model of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biological psychiatry. 2014;75:623–630. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.01.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Bienvenu OJ, et al. Sapap3 and pathological grooming in humans: Results from the OCD collaborative genetics study. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2009;150B:710–720. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30897. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Mattheisen M, et al. Genome-wide association study in obsessive-compulsive disorder: results from the OCGAS. Mol Psychiatry. 2015;20:337–344. doi: 10.1038/mp.2014.43. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Shmelkov SV, et al. Slitrk5 deficiency impairs corticostriatal circuitry and leads to obsessive-compulsive-like behaviors in mice. Nature medicine. 2010;16:598–602. doi: 10.1038/nm.2125. 591p following 602. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Proenca CC, Gao KP, Shmelkov SV, Rafii S, Lee FS. Slitrks as emerging candidate genes involved in neuropsychiatric disorders. Trends in neurosciences. 2011;34:143–153. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2011.01.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Katayama K, et al. Slitrk1-deficient mice display elevated anxiety-like behavior and noradrenergic abnormalities. Mol Psychiatry. 2010;15:177–184. doi: 10.1038/mp.2008.97. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Greist JH, et al. WCA recommendations for the long-term treatment of obsessive-compulsive disorder in adults. CNS spectrums. 2003;8:7–16. doi: 10.1017/s1092852900006908. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Insel TR. New pharmacologic approaches to obsessive compulsive disorder. The Journal of clinical psychiatry. 1990;51(Suppl):47–51. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4684-4697-5_44. discussion 56–48. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Lewis S. Autism: grooming mice to model autism. Nature reviews. Neuroscience. 2011;12:248–249. doi: 10.1038/nrn3033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Peca J, et al. Shank3 mutant mice display autistic-like behaviours and striatal dysfunction. Nature. 2011;472:437–442. doi: 10.1038/nature09965. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 101.McFarlane HG, et al. Autism-like behavioral phenotypes in BTBR T+tf/J mice. Genes, brain, and behavior. 2008;7:152–163. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2007.00330.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 102.Campbell KM, et al. OCD-Like behaviors caused by a neuropotentiating transgene targeted to cortical and limbic D1+ neurons. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 1999;19:5044–5053. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-12-05044.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 103.Bari A, Robbins TW. Inhibition and impulsivity: behavioral and neural basis of response control. Progress in neurobiology. 2013;108:44–79. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2013.06.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 104.Morein-Zamir S, et al. Punishment promotes response control deficits in obsessive-compulsive disorder: evidence from a motivational go/no-go task. Psychological medicine. 2013;43:391–400. doi: 10.1017/S0033291712001018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 105.Chamberlain SR, Fineberg NA, Blackwell AD, Robbins TW, Sahakian BJ. Motor inhibition and cognitive flexibility in obsessive-compulsive disorder and trichotillomania. The American journal of psychiatry. 2006;163:1282–1284. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.163.7.1282. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 106.Abramovitch A, Abramowitz JS. Improbability of response inhibition as a causal etiological factor of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychiatry research. 2014;217:253–254. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2014.01.050. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 107.Gremel CM, Costa RM. Orbitofrontal and striatal circuits dynamically encode the shift between goal-directed and habitual actions. Nature communications. 2013;4:2264. doi: 10.1038/ncomms3264. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 108.Gillan CM, et al. Functional neuroimaging of avoidance habits in obsessive-compulsive disorder. The American journal of psychiatry. 2015;172:284–293. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2014.14040525. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 109.Rapoport JL. Recent advances in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology. 1991;5:1–10. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 110.Stein DJ, Shoulberg N, Helton K, Hollander E. The neuroethological approach to obsessive-compulsive disorder. Comprehensive psychiatry. 1992;33:274–281. doi: 10.1016/0010-440x(92)90053-s. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 111.Joel D. Current animal models of obsessive compulsive disorder: a critical review. Progress in neuro-psychopharmacology & biological psychiatry. 2006;30:374–388. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2005.11.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 112.Powell SB, Newman HA, Pendergast JF, Lewis MH. A rodent model of spontaneous stereotypy: initial characterization of developmental, environmental, and neurobiological factors. Physiology & behavior. 1999;66:355–363. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(98)00303-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 113.Presti MF, Watson CJ, Kennedy RT, Yang M, Lewis MH. Behavior-related alterations of striatal neurochemistry in a mouse model of stereotyped movement disorder. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2004;77:501–507. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2003.12.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 114.Angoa-Perez M, Kane MJ, Briggs DI, Francescutti DM, Kuhn DM. Marble burying and nestlet shredding as tests of repetitive, compulsive-like behaviors in mice. Journal of visualized experiments : JoVE. 2013:50978. doi: 10.3791/50978. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 115.Chou-Green JM, Holscher TD, Dallman MF, Akana SF. Compulsive behavior in the 5-HT2C receptor knockout mouse. Physiology & behavior. 2003;78:641–649. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(03)00047-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 116.Deacon RM. Digging and marble burying in mice: simple methods for in vivo identification of biological impacts. Nature protocols. 2006;1:122–124. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2006.20. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 117.Hayashi E, Kuratani K, Kinoshita M, Hara H. Pharmacologically distinctive behaviors other than burying marbles during the marble burying test in mice. Pharmacology. 2010;86:293–296. doi: 10.1159/000321190. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 118.Thomas A, et al. Marble burying reflects a repetitive and perseverative behavior more than novelty-induced anxiety. Psychopharmacology. 2009;204:361–373. doi: 10.1007/s00213-009-1466-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 119.Szechtman H, Sulis W, Eilam D. Quinpirole induces compulsive checking behavior in rats: a potential animal model of obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) Behavioral neuroscience. 1998;112:1475–1485. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.112.6.1475. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 120.Winter C, et al. High frequency stimulation and temporary inactivation of the subthalamic nucleus reduce quinpirole-induced compulsive checking behavior in rats. Experimental neurology. 2008;210:217–228. doi: 10.1016/j.expneurol.2007.10.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 121.Mundt A, et al. High-frequency stimulation of the nucleus accumbens core and shell reduces quinpirole-induced compulsive checking in rats. The European journal of neuroscience. 2009;29:2401–2412. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.06777.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 122.Eagle DM, et al. The dopamine D2/D3 receptor agonist quinpirole increases checking-like behaviour in an operant observing response task with uncertain reinforcement: a novel possible model of OCD. Behavioural brain research. 2014;264:207–229. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2013.12.040. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 123.Olivier B, van Oorschot R. 5-HT1B receptors and aggression: a review. European journal of pharmacology. 2005;526:207–217. doi: 10.1016/j.ejphar.2005.09.066. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 124.Clark MS, Neumaier JF. The 5-HT1B receptor: behavioral implications. Psychopharmacology bulletin. 2001;35:170–185. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 125.Gingrich JA, Hen R. Dissecting the role of the serotonin system in neuropsychiatric disorders using knockout mice. Psychopharmacology. 2001;155:1–10. doi: 10.1007/s002130000573. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 126.Saudou F, et al. Enhanced aggressive behavior in mice lacking 5-HT1B receptor. Science. 1994;265:1875–1878. doi: 10.1126/science.8091214. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 127.Brunner D, Hen R. Insights into the neurobiology of impulsive behavior from serotonin receptor knockout mice. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 1997;836:81–105. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1997.tb52356.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 128.Bouwknecht JA, et al. Absence of 5-HT(1B) receptors is associated with impaired impulse control in male 5-HT(1B) knockout mice. Biological psychiatry. 2001;49:557–568. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(00)01018-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 129.Barr LC, Goodman WK, Price LH, McDougle CJ, Charney DS. The serotonin hypothesis of obsessive compulsive disorder: implications of pharmacologic challenge studies. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. 1992;53(Suppl):17–28. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 130.Camarena B, Aguilar A, Loyzaga C, Nicolini H. A family-based association study of the 5-HT-1Dbeta receptor gene in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol. 2004;7:49–53. doi: 10.1017/S1461145703003869. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 131.Shanahan NA, et al. Chronic reductions in serotonin transporter function prevent 5-HT1B-induced behavioral effects in mice. Biological psychiatry. 2009;65:401–408. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.09.026S0006-3223(08)01164-5. pii. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 132.Shanahan NA, Velez LP, Masten VL, Dulawa SC. Essential role for orbitofrontal serotonin 1B receptors in obsessive-compulsive disorder-like behavior and serotonin reuptake inhibitor response in mice. Biological psychiatry. 2011;70:1039–1048. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.07.032S0006-3223(11)00779-7. pii. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 133.Woehrle NS, Klenotich SJ, Jamnia N, Ho EV, Dulawa SC. Effects of chronic fluoxetine treatment on serotonin 1B receptor-induced deficits in delayed alternation. Psychopharmacology. 2013;227:545–551. doi: 10.1007/s00213-013-2985-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 134.Remijnse PL, et al. Reduced orbitofrontal-striatal activity on a reversal learning task in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Archives of general psychiatry. 2006;63:1225–1236. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.63.11.1225. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 135.Remijnse PL, et al. Differential frontal-striatal and paralimbic activity during reversal learning in major depressive disorder and obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychological medicine. 2009;39:1503–1518. doi: 10.1017/S0033291708005072. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 136.Valerius G, Lumpp A, Kuelz AK, Freyer T, Voderholzer U. Reversal learning as a neuropsychological indicator for the neuropathology of obsessive compulsive disorder? A behavioral study. The Journal of neuropsychiatry and clinical neurosciences. 2008;20:210–218. doi: 10.1176/appi.neuropsych.20.2.210. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 137.Brown HD, Amodeo DA, Sweeney JA, Ragozzino ME. The selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor, escitalopram, enhances inhibition of prepotent responding and spatial reversal learning. Journal of psychopharmacology. 2012;26:1443–1455. doi: 10.1177/0269881111430749. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 138.Furr A, Lapiz-Bluhm MD, Morilak DA. 5-HT2A receptors in the orbitofrontal cortex facilitate reversal learning and contribute to the beneficial cognitive effects of chronic citalopram treatment in rats. The international journal of neuropsychopharmacology/official scientific journal of the Collegium Internationale Neuropsychopharmacologicum. 2012;15:1295–1305. doi: 10.1017/S1461145711001441. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 139.Bari A, et al. Serotonin modulates sensitivity to reward and negative feedback in a probabilistic reversal learning task in rats. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35:1290–1301. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.233. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 140.Clarke HF, Dalley JW, Crofts HS, Robbins TW, Roberts AC. Cognitive inflexibility after prefrontal serotonin depletion. Science. 2004;304:878–880. doi: 10.1126/science.1094987. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 141.Murphy FC, Smith KA, Cowen PJ, Robbins TW, Sahakian BJ. The effects of tryptophan depletion on cognitive and affective processing in healthy volunteers. Psychopharmacology. 2002;163:42–53. doi: 10.1007/s00213-002-1128-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 142.Clarke HF, et al. Prefrontal serotonin depletion affects reversal learning but not attentional set shifting. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 2005;25:532–538. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3690-04.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 143.Gourley SL, Lee AS, Howell JL, Pittenger C, Taylor JR. Dissociable regulation of instrumental action within mouse prefrontal cortex. The European journal of neuroscience. 2010;32:1726–1734. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07438.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 144.Freedman M, Oscar-Berman M. Selective delayed response deficits in Parkinson’s and Alzheimer’s disease. Archives of neurology. 1986;43:886–890. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1986.00520090026011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 145.Chamberlain SR, Blackwell AD, Fineberg NA, Robbins TW, Sahakian BJ. The neuropsychology of obsessive compulsive disorder: the importance of failures in cognitive and behavioural inhibition as candidate endophenotypic markers. Neuroscience and biobehavioral reviews. 2005;29:399–419. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.11.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 146.Freedman M, Black S, Ebert P, Binns M. Orbitofrontal function, object alternation and perseveration. Cerebral cortex. 1998;8:18–27. doi: 10.1093/cercor/8.1.18. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 147.Moritz S, et al. Perseveration and not strategic deficits underlie delayed alternation impairment in obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) Psychiatry research. 2009;170:66–69. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2008.09.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 148.Abbruzzese M, Bellodi L, Ferri S, Scarone S. Frontal lobe dysfunction in schizophrenia and obsessive-compulsive disorder: a neuropsychological study. Brain and cognition. 1995;27:202–212. doi: 10.1006/brcg.1995.1017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 149.Abbruzzese M, Ferri S, Scarone S. The selective breakdown of frontal functions in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder and in patients with schizophrenia: a double dissociation experimental finding. Neuropsychologia. 1997;35:907–912. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(96)00095-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 150.Gross-Isseroff R, et al. Alternation learning in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biological psychiatry. 1996;39:733–738. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(95)00179-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 151.Gillan CM, et al. Counterfactual processing of economic action-outcome alternatives in obsessive-compulsive disorder: further evidence of impaired goal-directed behavior. Biological psychiatry. 2014;75:639–646. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.01.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 152.Smith KS, Virkud A, Deisseroth K, Graybiel AM. Reversible online control of habitual behavior by optogenetic perturbation of medial prefrontal cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2012;109:18932–18937. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1216264109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 153.Smith KS, Graybiel AM. A dual operator view of habitual behavior reflecting cortical and striatal dynamics. Neuron. 2013;79:361–374. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.05.038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 154.Swerdlow NR, Geyer MA, Braff DL. Neural circuit regulation of prepulse inhibition of startle in the rat: current knowledge and future challenges. Psychopharmacology. 2001;156:194–215. doi: 10.1007/s002130100799. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 155.Hoenig K, Hochrein A, Quednow BB, Maier W, Wagner M. Impaired prepulse inhibition of acoustic startle in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biological psychiatry. 2005;57:1153–1158. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.01.040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 156.Swerdlow NR, Benbow CH, Zisook S, Geyer MA, Braff DL. A preliminary assessment of sensorimotor gating in patients with obsessive compulsive disorder. Biological psychiatry. 1993;33:298–301. doi: 10.1016/0006-3223(93)90300-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 157.Boisseau CL, et al. Behavioral and cognitive impulsivity in obsessive-compulsive disorder and eating disorders. Psychiatry Res. 2012;200:1062–1066. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2012.06.010S0165-1781(12)00321-6. pii. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 158.Hartston HJ, Swerdlow NR. Visuospatial priming and stroop performance in patients with obsessive compulsive disorder. Neuropsychology. 1999;13:447–457. doi: 10.1037//0894-4105.13.3.447. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 159.Bannon S, Gonsalvez CJ, Croft RJ, Boyce PM. Response inhibition deficits in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychiatry research. 2002;110:165–174. doi: 10.1016/s0165-1781(02)00104-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 160.Marsh R, et al. Altered activation in fronto-striatal circuits during sequential processing of conflict in unmedicated adults with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biological psychiatry. 2014;75:615–622. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.02.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 161.Abramovitch A, Abramowitz JS, Mittelman A. The neuropsychology of adult obsessive-compulsive disorder: a meta-analysis. Clinical psychology review. 2013;33:1163–1171. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2013.09.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 162.Eagle DM, et al. Contrasting roles for dopamine D1 and D2 receptor subtypes in the dorsomedial striatum but not the nucleus accumbens core during behavioral inhibition in the stop-signal task in rats. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 2011;31:7349–7356. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6182-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 163.Bari A, Eagle DM, Mar AC, Robinson ES, Robbins TW. Dissociable effects of noradrenaline, dopamine, and serotonin uptake blockade on stop task performance in rats. Psychopharmacology. 2009;205:273–283. doi: 10.1007/s00213-009-1537-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 164.Eagle DM, et al. Serotonin depletion impairs waiting but not stop-signal reaction time in rats: implications for theories of the role of 5-HT in behavioral inhibition. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2009;34:1311–1321. doi: 10.1038/npp.2008.202. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 165.Bravo-Rivera C, Roman-Ortiz C, Brignoni-Perez E, Sotres-Bayon F, Quirk GJ. Neural structures mediating expression and extinction of platform-mediated avoidance. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 2014;34:9736–9742. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0191-14.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 166.Ahmari SE, Eich T, Cebenoyan D, Smith EE, Blair Simpson H. Assessing neurocognitive function in psychiatric disorders: A roadmap for enhancing consensus. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2014 doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2014.06.011. S1074-7427(14)00125-7 [pii] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES