Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2017 Sep 1.
Published in final edited form as: J Crim Justice. 2016 Sep 1;46:64–81. doi: 10.1016/j.jcrimjus.2016.03.001

Adult-onset offenders: Is a tailored theory warranted?

Amber L Beckley a,b,*, Avshalom Caspi a,c,d, Honalee Harrington a, Renate M Houts a, Tara Renae Mcgee e, Nick Morgan f, Felix Schroeder a, Sandhya Ramrakha g, Richie Poulton g, Terrie E Moffitt a,c,d
PMCID: PMC4845670  NIHMSID: NIHMS773782  PMID: 27134318

Abstract

Purpose

To describe official adult-onset offenders, investigate their antisocial histories and test hypotheses about their origins.

Methods

We defined adult-onset offenders among 931 Dunedin Study members followed to age 38, using criminal-court conviction records.

Results

Official adult-onset offenders were 14% of men, and 32% of convicted men, but accounted for only 15% of convictions. As anticipated by developmental theories emphasizing early-life influences on crime, adult-onset offenders’ histories of antisocial behavior spanned back to childhood. Relative to juvenile-offenders, during adolescence they had fewer delinquent peers and were more socially inhibited, which may have protected them from conviction. As anticipated by theories emphasizing the importance of situational influences on offending, adult-onset offenders, relative to non-offenders, during adulthood more often had schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, and alcohol-dependence, had weaker social bonds, anticipated fewer informal sanctions, and self-reported more offenses. Contrary to some expectations, adult-onset offenders did not have high IQ or high socioeconomic-status families protecting them from juvenile conviction.

Conclusions

A tailored theory for adult-onset offenders is unwarranted because few people begin crime de novo as adults. Official adult-onset offenders fall on a continuum of crime and its correlates, between official non-offenders and official juvenile-onset offenders. Existing theories can accommodate adult-onset offenders.

Keywords: adult-onset offending, theory, life-course criminology, longitudinal, Dunedin


It seems counterintuitive that someone who successfully navigated the volatile adolescent period crime-free would suddenly start engaging in crime as an adult. Yet, according to official data, adult-onset offending exists. Adult-onset offenders, as reported by most studies, represent a substantial portion of ever-convicted individuals (although the size of this adult-onset group is uncertain because of methodological heterogeneity among studies, see Table 1). According to projections of lifetime conviction risk, at least one-quarter of first-time convictions will occur after 30 years of age, well into adulthood (Skardhamar, 2014). Ample cautionary evidence, however, shows that individuals’ age of onset of criminal behavior is overestimated by official data (Elander, Rutter, Simonoff, & Pickles, 2000; Farrington, 1989; Farrington, Jolliffe, Loeber, & Homish, 2007; Kazemian & Farrington, 2005; McGee & Farrington, 2010; Moffitt, Caspi, Rutter, & Silva, 2001; Sohoni, Paternoster, McGloin, & Bachman, 2014; Theobald & Farrington, 2011). As a result, an initial official crime record during adulthood cannot necessarily be interpreted as evidence that the offender began criminal activity as an adult.

Table 1.

Evidence of adult-onset offending, sources from 1998 to 2014. Updated table of Eggleston & Laub (2002)1

Study name/description Data source Analytic sample Followed
to age:
Type of crime data Adult offenders Juvenile-onset adult
offenders
Adult-onset adult
offenders

n % of
sample
n % of adult
offenders
n % of adult
offenders
Prospective studies appearing in Eggleston & Laub (2002)

St. Louis Municipal
  Psychiatric Clinic Study
Robins (1966) 441 males and
females in St.
Louse, antisocial
referrals and
nondelinquent
controls
43 Nontraffic arrests 233 53% 218 94% 15 6%
Glueck Study Glueck & Glueck (1968) 880 males in
Boston, one half
delinquent
31 Arrests for
nontraffic offenses
328 37% 266 81% 62 19%
Cambridge-Somerville
  Study
McCord (1978) 506 males in
Massachusetts
mid to
late 40s
Serious convictions 91 18% 50 55% 41 45%
Marion County Youth
  Study
Polk et al. (1981) 1,227 males in the
10th grade in 1964
in Marion County,
OR
30 Police and court
records for minor
and serious
offending
90 7.3% 35 29% 55 61%
Cambridge Study in
  Delinquent Development
Farrington (1983) 395 males in
London
25 Nonminor
convictions
107 27% 55 51% 52 49%
Langan & Farrington (1983) 395 males in
London
25 Burglary or violence
convictions
55 14% 19 35% 36 65%
Swedish Project
  Metropolitan
Janson (1983) 7,710 males in
Stockholm, Sweden
26 Crimes known to
police, including
nonminor traffic
1,639 21% 601 37% 1,038 63%
Kratzer & Hodgins (1999) 13,852 males and
females in
Stockholm, Sweden
30 All criminal
convictions,
including nonminor
traffic
1,945 14% 800 41% 1,145 59%
Racine Cohort Studies Shannon (1988) 633 males and
females born in
1942 in Wisconsin
32 Nontraffic police
contacts
242 38% 118 49% 124 51%
Shannon (1988) 1,297 males and
females born in
1949 in Wisconsin
25 Nontraffic police
contacts
472 36% 305 65% 167 35%
Shannon (1998) 1,357 males and
females born in
1955 in Wisconsin
32 Nontraffic police
contacts
458 34% 236 51% 222 29%
1945 Philadelphia Birth
  Cohort Follow-up Study
Wolfgang et al. (1987) 975 males in
Philadelphia born in
1945
30 Police contacts for
nontraffic offenses
290 30% 176 61% 114 39%
Individual Development
  and Environment
Magnusson (1988) 1,389 males and
females in Orebro,
Sweden
30 Nonminor arrests 248 18% 99 14% 149 86%
Montreal Study LeBlanc and Frechette (1989) 1,602 males in
Montreal
25 Convictions for
indictable crimes
172 11% 25 14% 149 86%
LeBlanc and Frechette (1989) 470 male wards of
the court in
Montreal
25 Convictions for
indictable crimes
339 72% 288 85% 51 15%
LeBlanc and Frechette (1989) 196 male wards of
the court in
Montreal
25 Self-report
offending
177 90% 150 85% 27 15%
Kauai Study Werner & Smith (1992) 505 males and
females in Kauai, HI
32 Nontraffic police
records and court
convictions
31 6% 21 68% 10 32%
1958 Philadelphia Birth
  Cohort
Tracy & Kempf-Leonard (1996) 27,160 males and
females in
Philadelphia born in
1958
26 Police contacts for
nontraffic offenses
3,617 13% 2,041 56% 1,576 44%
Prospective studies published from 1999 forward, not included in Eggleston & Laub (2002)

Racine data Eggleston & Laub (2002) 732 males and
females in Racine,
WI born 1942 or
1949
25 and
32
Police contact for
nontraffic offenses
179 24% 96 54% 83 46%
Youth Court Survey and
  Adult Criminal Court
  Survey
Carrington, Matarazzo, & deSouza (2005) 323,694 Canadian
males and females
born 1979–1980
22 Court referrals and
convictions
37,426 12% 12,044 32% 25,382 68%
Philadelphia portion of
  National Collaborative
  Perinatal Project
Gomez-Smith & Piquero (2005) 987 African
American males and
females born 1959–1962
36–39 Convictions and
police contacts
154 16% 76 49% 78 51%
Department of Child and
  Adolescent Psychiatry of
  the Central Institute of
  Mental Health,
  Mannheim
Lay et al. (2005) 321 German males
and females born in
1970
25 Official convictions,
self-reports, and
interviews with
parents
72 22% 27 38% 45 63%
Jyväskylä Longitudinal
  Study of Personality and
  Social Development
Pulkkinen, Lyyra, & Kokko (2009) 196 Finnish males
born in 1959
47 Official convictions,
police-registered
crime, and self-
reports
89 45% 57 64% 32 36%
Cambridge Study in
  Delinquent Development
McGee & Farrington (2010) 404 British males
born in 1953 for
which a criminal
records search was
conducted at 48
years of age
50 Official convictions 1672 41% 129 77% 38 23%
Sohoni et al. (2014) 411 British males
born in 1953
50 Official convictions,
adulthood at 25
years of age
1672 43% 138 83% 29 17%
National Longitudinal
  Study of Adolescent
  Health (Add Health),
  Waves 1–3
Mata & van Dulmen (2012) 5,579 males and
females 13–18 years
of age at Wave 1,
with complete
antisocial behavior
data
18–25 Self-reported
antisocial behavior
9–13%
(est)2
Rochester Youth
  Development Study
Sohoni et al. (2014) 638 male Rochester
public school
students born ca.
1973–1975
31–33 Arrest records,
adulthood at 25
years of age
3852 60% 335 87% 50 13%
Stockholm Birth Cohort Nilsson et al. (2013) 13,715 Swedish
males and females
born in 1953
48 Police-registered
crime
1,759 13% 812 46% 947 54%
Retrospective/offender only studies published from 1999 forward, not included in Eggleston & Laub (2002)

Colorado bond
commissioner processing
DeLisi (2006) 500 male and
female frequent
offenders with
intake in Colorado
from 1995–2000
Mean
age of 40
Criminal records
and self-reports
500 100% 192 38% 308 62%
Baltimore City Detention
Center
Simpson, Yahner, & Dugan (2008) 351 adult females
incarcerated in
Baltimore
Mean
age of 35
Self-reports 342 100% 156 46% 186 54%
Southwestern prison Gunnison & McCartan (2010) 131 adult females
incarcerated in a
Southwestern
prison
25 to 44 Self-reports 131 100% 55 42% 76 58%
Three-year statewide
classification study
Harris (2011) 3,598 males and
females sentenced
to felony probation
in a large south
central US state
during October
1993
48 Criminal records 3,598 100% 481 13% 3,117 87%
Life history interviews Carr & Hanks (2012) 30 females
incarcerated in a
local jail
26 to 55 Official convictions
and self-reports
30 100% 22 73% 8 27%
Swedish Project
  Metropolitan
Andersson et al. (2012) 518 females with a
criminal record in
Stockholm, Sweden
30 Crimes known to
police, including
nonminor traffic
10% (est)2
1983–84 Queensland
Longitudinal Data Cohort
Thompson et al. (2014a) 40,523 male and
female offenders
25 Youth and adult
court finalizations,
youth police
cautions
40,5232 100% 19,310 48% 21,213 52%

Notes:

1

- Sources reporting only results from group-based trajectory models are excluded.

2

– This figure is estimated based on group-based trajectory modeling.

3

– This figure includes juvenile-only offenders; the percentage of adult-onset offenders is thus across the entire offender group. In these cases the proportion of adult-onset offenders among adult-offenders is likely greater than the figure reported in the final column.

There are both practical and theoretical reasons for investigating the official age of onset of crime. Practically, adult-onset offenders represent a sizable proportion of official offenders and warrant an appropriate response from the criminal justice system, ranging from targeted interventions to increasing the age limit for processing within the juvenile justice system. Adult-onset offenders also pose challenges to life-course developmental theories, which have generally not anticipated the existence of the adult-onset offender (DeLisi & Piquero, 2011). Examination of the adult-onset offender may lead to important theoretical insights about the origins of criminal behavior (Piquero, Oster, Mazerolle, Brame, & Dean, 1999; Thornberry & Krohn, 2011).

In this study, we investigated adult-onset offending. We used data from the Dunedin Longitudinal Study which has followed a 1972–73 birth cohort for four decades in New Zealand. Based on past research (see Table 1), we anticipated finding official adult-onset offenders in the Dunedin cohort. We additionally sought to find the presence of an official “social-adulthood-onset offender” – someone who is first convicted at or after 25 years of age – based on the idea that modern-day adolescence is prolonged (Arnett, 2000). First, we tested whether official adult-onset offenders had an unofficial history of criminal behavior, as has been found in past studies (see, for example, McGee & Farrington, 2010; Sohoni et al., 2014). Going beyond these studies, we examined the unofficial history of criminal behavior among adult-onset offenders back to early childhood using multiple reporting sources (self, parents, teachers, and police). Second, we tested whether the types of crime for which official adult-onset offenders were convicted could illuminate potential causes of official adult-onset offending. Third, because most official-adult onset offenders had a history of antisocial behavior we were able to add a fresh conceptualization of how theories originally designed to predict de novo adult-onset offending could be extended to explain a first official conviction during adulthood. We examined 10 specific hypotheses derived from theories on adult-onset offending that could explain why some people appear to be adult-onset offenders, or are first detected during adulthood. By testing these 10 hypotheses in a single, contemporary cohort we were able to go beyond previous studies, unifying the literature on adult-onset offending and drawing a comprehensive picture of the typical adult-onset offender. We conclude by discussing policy responses to adult-onset offenders and the theoretical implications of adult-onset offending for life-course criminology.

Previous evidence on adult-onset offenders

In Table 1, we present evidence of adult-onset offending found in previous prospective and retrospective studies on adult-onset offenders. The 35 analyses used 25 unique datasets, 19 of which were from the United States. Of the 35 analyses, 4 used the Cambridge Study of Delinquent Development (CSDD). Studies of contemporary cohorts beyond the CSDD and from outside of the USA would help to address generalizability of descriptive data about adult-onset offending. The percentage of adult-onset offenders varied widely across the studies, from 6% to 87%, likely due to methodological heterogeneity. For example, the analytic samples ranged from 30 to over 300,000, and used different sexes, ages, and definitions of offending (14 analyses used official criminal conviction). A few studies (noted in Table 1) included juvenile-only offenders in the denominator, which likely meant that the percent of adult-onset offenders among adult offenders, reported in the final column, was underestimated. Together the studies provide important and robust evidence that adult-onset offenders should be found in studies of criminal behavior, regardless of the period or location from which the data come, the age to which the subjects are followed, or the measure of criminal behavior used. However, due to wide variation in methods, it is difficult to synthesize across these studies to extract a picture of the adult-onset offender. Moreover, previous studies testing explanations of adult-onset offending tended to focus on only one or two explanations, possibly due to data constraints. It thus remains unclear whether existing theories of crime can achieve a coherent picture of the adult-onset offender. To achieve a more coherent profile, it is important to test all hypotheses that have been put forward simultaneously in the same sample.

Age of onset in official and self-reported data

Criminal justice system data often overestimate the age of onset of criminal offending. Members of the Dunedin cohort, in the present study, self-reported an abundance of violence, theft, and substance offenses during adolescence (Moffitt et al., 2001). Officially, however, only 15 percent of the cohort had, by 22 years of age, ever been convicted of a crime (Moffitt et al., 2001). Among the self-reported adolescent offenders in the CSDD, only about half were officially recorded as offenders (Farrington et al., 2007). Additionally, CSDD boys’ official age of onset was, on average, five years later than their self-reported age of onset of crime (Theobald & Farrington, 2014). Among men from the Rochester Youth Development Study who had never been arrested by 32 years of age, over three-quarters had self-reported some type of offense by 18 years of age, and around half had self-reported a violent or serious offense by 18 years of age (Sohoni et al., 2014). This pattern of early onset and later conviction is unsurprising. Most people, it has been argued, engage in criminal behavior during adolescence (Moffitt, 1993), yet a minority of people acquire a criminal record during adolescence. Criminal conviction, an indication that the individual is legally responsible for a crime, requires both apprehension by police and successful prosecution. Successful detection and prosecution of all criminal behavior is, however, impossible (Mosher, Hart, & Miethe, 2011). Official data, thus, appear inadequate for capturing the age of onset of criminal behavior.

Official data may also overestimate the age at which criminal behavior begins because the criminal justice system is constrained by a lower age bound. Children below a certain age, usually ranging between 10 and 15 years, cannot be held liable and convicted for criminal behavior. Prospective identification of offenders is controversial, yet it is instructive to consider whether the typical adult-onset offender was also antisocial as a child or just lagging behind juvenile-onset offenders in their antisocial development. Some studies suggest that the typical official adult-onset offender’s childhood antisocial behavior looks similar to that of the typical official juvenile-onset offender (Pulkkinen, Lyyra, & Kokko, 2009; Zara & Farrington, 2013). Thus, relying on official data may obscure important information regarding early childhood antisocial behaviors and, ergo, the development of antisocial behavior over the life-course of adult-onset offenders.

We tested the hypothesis that many official adult-onset offenders engage in antisocial behavior from early life. Many past studies incorporating unofficial sources have only included self-report data during adolescence (with the notable exception of the CSDD), preventing insights on the development of antisocial behavior among adult-onset offenders. We extend beyond past research by analyzing reports of unprosecuted antisocial behavior from parents, teachers, the police, and self-reports from childhood onwards.

Adult-onset offenders’ offense specialization and extent of offending

Offenders are known to commit a variety of types of crime, but tend towards offenses with utilitarian motivations, such as theft and fraud, as they age (Farrington, 2014; Laub & Sampson, 2003; Massoglia, 2006; Piquero et al., 1999; Stattin, Magnusson, & Reichel, 1989). Adult-onset offenders, in particular, may be more specialized than juvenile-onset offenders because of their limited criminal experience and established routines with regular antisocial opportunities (Catalano et al., 2005; Farrington, 2014). Adult-onset offenders may also gravitate towards sexoffending as they age (Lussier, Tzoumakis, Cale, & Amirault, 2010). To our knowledge, only one study has explicitly compared all conviction types between adult-onset and juvenile-onset offenders (McGee & Farrington, 2010). This study found that compared to juvenile-onset offenders, adult-onset offenders, committed proportionally more fraud, theft from work, vandalism, and sex crimes (McGee & Farrington, 2010). However, adult-onset offenders, appear to maintain an overall lower level of offending than juvenile-onset high-chronic offenders, even during the same adult age-period (Andersson, Levander, Svensson, & Levander, 2012; Broidy et al., 2015; Chung, Hill, Hawkins, Gilchrist, & Nagin, 2002; van der Geest, Blokland, & Bijleveld, 2009). Adult-onset offenders’ crime specialization may indicate causes of their criminal activity and could also have implications for justice-system policy (Piquero et al., 1999). We tested whether certain types of criminal convictions were relatively more likely among official adult-onset offenders, compared to official juvenile-onset offenders, and we compared the frequency of convictions between official onset-groups.

Explanations for official adult-onset offending

Eggleston and Laub (2002) summarized the then-current state of criminological theory and research on adult-onset offenders. Many theories of crime over the life course denied the existence of the adult-onset offender. Yet, adult-onset offenders seemed to appear in many studies (see Table 1) and it was argued that the adult-onset offender warranted systematic study. With the theoretical foundation from which to study the adult-onset offender under-developed, researchers sought to apply established theories to the adult-onset offender and new theories, which could better incorporate adult-onset offending, emerged. This lead us to identify two sets of theories of the adult-onset offender (Farrington, 2006; Sohoni et al., 2014).

The first set of theories emphasized early-life influences on offending at any time in the life-course. These theories implied that true adult-onset antisocial behavior was highly unlikely because antisocial behavior was thought to develop during childhood and adolescence under the influence of both early-emerging individual characteristics (such as low intelligence and low self-control) and family influences (including low socioeconomic status). Examples of theories in this set are Gottfredson & Hirschi’s (1990) general theory of crime and Moffitt’s (1993) dual taxonomy. According to these theories the rare adult-onset offender was likely to have followed a non-traditional path of development. For example, Moffitt hypothesized that young males who abstained from crime while at the peak age of crime participation must have personal characteristics, such as social timidity or inhibition that reduced their opportunities to take part in the normative law-breaking activities of delinquent peer groups. This implies that any offender who first initiates crime as an adult will have, as a juvenile, been socially inhibited and will have lacked delinquent peers.

The second set of theories emphasized situational influences on offending during adulthood. These theories implied that adult-onset antisocial behavior could begin in earnest during adulthood due to changes in the social environment. The major life-course theories that fell under this paradigm were Farrington’s (2006, 2011) integrative cognitive antisocial potential, Catalano & Hawkins’ (1996) social development model (see also Catalano et al., 2005), LeBlanc’s integrated multilayered control theory (1997), Sampson & Laub’s (1993) age-graded theory of informal social control, Thornberry (1987)/Thornberry & Krohn’s (2011) interactional theory, and Wikström’s situational action theory (2004, 2005). Like the aforementioned developmental theories, these theories did not explicitly describe the process of adult-onset offending. However, the situational influences they emphasize also allow for offending to begin after the peak age of crime, during adulthood. For example, Sampson and Laub’s age-graded theory of informal social control would predict that adult-onset offenders result from a lack of social bonds during adulthood. These theories can also be applied to explain why anyone might be detected and convicted at any age; for example, conditions that tend to onset in adulthood such as alcoholism or mental illness might result in offending that is more publicly visible and attracts the attention of police. Since the applicability of developmental and situational theories to adult-onset offending has already been explicated (see, for example, Farrington, 2006; Sohoni et al., 2014) we are left with how to reconcile these theories in light of evidence that many official adult-onset offenders have a history of undetected antisocial behavior. Developmental and situational theories may be best put to use to explain how an individual may persist in and first be detected for antisocial behavior during adulthood.

We have developed hypotheses based on these two sets of theories of the adult-onset offender and past evidence on adult-onset offending. Hypotheses under the first set of theories must explain why or how official adult-onset offenders avoided an early official criminal record, despite engaging in criminal behavior as adolescents. These hypotheses distinguish official adult-onset offenders from official juvenile-onset offenders (that is, from offenders who were first convicted during adolescence).

Hypothesis 1: Official adult-onset offenders, compared to juvenile-onset offenders, report fewer offenses during adolescence

Blumstein and Cohen (1987) argued that the more offenses one commits, the greater the likelihood of being captured. Official adult-onset offenders may avoid detection and prosecution by committing a relatively small number of offenses. Official adult-onset offenders from the CSDD, compared to official juvenile-onset offenders, had self-reported fewer offenses as boys (Kazemian & Farrington, 2005; McGee & Farrington, 2010; Zara & Farrington, 2010), which appeared to reduce their likelihood of acquiring a juvenile record (Farrington et al., 2007).

Hypothesis 2: Official adult-onset offenders, compared to juvenile-onset offenders, come from families with higher socioeconomic status

Critical criminological theory argues that with higher socioeconomic class comes the privilege of avoiding a criminal record. Perhaps the best example of this privilege is shown in Chambliss’ (1973) classic “The Saints and the Roughnecks”, in which the high socioeconomic status Saints frequently offended but were never arrested. Official adult-onset offenders may enjoy the protection of their high socioeconomic status families during adolescence. This benefit may fade with the transition to adulthood. Some evidence indicates that adult-onset offenders, compared to juvenile-onset offenders, may be less likely to come from a low-income family (Zara & Farrington, 2010).

Hypothesis 3: Official adult-onset offenders, compared to juvenile-onset offenders, are more intelligent

Low intelligence is a well-known risk factor for juvenile-onset offending (Farrington, 2011). Official adult-onset offenders may be more intelligent than juvenile-onset offenders and, consequently, more successful at evading detection and prosecution. Some research has supported the idea that later-age official onset of offending is tied to higher intelligence (Bellair, McNulty, & Piquero, 2014; Zara & Farrington, 2010).

Hypothesis 4: Official adult-onset offenders, compared to juvenile-onset offenders, have fewer delinquent peers during adolescence

Attachment to delinquent peers is known to increase the individual risk of delinquency (Laub & Sampson, 2011), and may also increase the likelihood of serious group offending and the risk of apprehension and conviction (Erickson, 1973; Tillyer & Tillyer, 2014). For adolescents who are already delinquent, research has shown, joining with delinquent peers further exacerbates criminal behavior (Thornberry & Krohn, 1997; Vitaro, Tremblay, & Bukowski, 2000). Official adult-onset offenders may have relatively few delinquent peers and, thereby, avoid detection and apprehension as juveniles. Evidence from the CSDD supports the hypothesis that adult-onset offenders have fewer delinquent friends (Zara & Farrington, 2009, 2010).

Hypothesis 5: Official adult-onset offenders, compared to juvenile-onset offenders, are more likely to be socially inhibited

Socially inhibited people are timid and withdrawn. Adolescents tend to offend in the company of others (Farrington, 2011), an activity that social inhibition could curb. Socially inhibited adolescents may be excluded from their peer groups, including delinquent peer groups, and, thereby, be insulated from group crime (Moffitt, 1993, p. 689; Owens & Slocum, 2015; Theobald & Farrington, 2014, p. 3338). Such insulation could reduce the risk of apprehension and detection. Official adult-onset offenders may be socially inhibited, which excludes them from high-risk group offending. Research has shown that social inhibition may be related to adult-onset offending (Zara & Farrington, 2009).

Hypotheses under the second set of theories, though often initially meant to explain de novo adult offending, must explain why adult-onset offenders first get caught, prosecuted, and convicted during adulthood. We test an additional set of 5 hypotheses that seek to explain why people without a juvenile criminal record would acquire a record during adulthood. These hypotheses distinguish official adult-onset offenders from official non-offenders (that is, from people who are non-criminal, or who continue to avoid apprehension or conviction).

Hypothesis 6: Official adult-onset offenders, compared to official non-offenders, report more offenses during adulthood

As in hypothesis one (above), a high level of offending is likely to increase the risk of detection and capture (Blumstein & Cohen, 1987). Official adult-onset offenders, compared to official non-offenders, may be offending at relatively high rates. Additionally, a low-rate adolescent offender who avoided a criminal record as a juvenile but continued offending as an adult, may find his or her luck run out. This hypothesis implies adult-onset offenders will self-report more offenses as adults compared to non-offenders.

Hypothesis 7: Official adult-onset offenders, compared to official non-offenders, are more likely to have adult-onset schizophrenia or bipolar disorder

Schizophrenia and bipolar disorder can cause people to become disconnected from reality and have abnormal thoughts. These mental health problems often begin during the transition from late adolescence to early adulthood, and have been connected to greater risks for crime (Arseneault, Moffitt, Caspi, Taylor, & Silva, 2000; Fazel, Långström, Hjern, Grann, & Lichtenstein, 2009; Fazel, Lichtenstein, Grann, Goodwin, & Långström, 2010). Individuals with schizophrenia and manic symptoms of bipolar disorder show disorganized behavior and often attract public attention. Official adult-onset offenders may have adult-onset schizophrenia or bipolar disorder, which increases their risk of apprehension and conviction for crime. There is some evidence of official adult-onset offending being connected to these types of mental health disorders (Elander et al., 2000; Farrington, 1989; Zara & Farrington, 2010, 2013).

Hypothesis 8: Official adult-onset offenders, compared to official non-offenders, are more likely to be dependent on alcohol or other substances

Alcohol dependence tends to peak between the ages of 18 and 20 years (National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism, 2008), and hard drug use and dependence tend to peak about two years later (Wagner & Anthony, 2002). Alcohol dependence in adulthood is related to more criminal convictions (Meier et al., 2013) and drug dependence, in particular, may be a reason for offending (Mumola & Karberg, 2006). People with drug and alcohol problems may be unsuccessful at transitioning to stable adult roles with strong informal social controls (Thornberry, 2005), which could extend criminal behavior into adulthood. Additionally, drug and alcohol dependence may lead to erratic public behavior, drawing public attention and increasing the risk of apprehension and conviction for crime. Official adult-onset offenders may be dependent on alcohol or substances during adulthood, which increases their risk of apprehension for crime. Some research has supported the connection between late-onset crime and substance dependence (Elander et al., 2000; Farrington, 1989; Pulkkinen et al., 2009; Zara & Farrington, 2010).

Hypothesis 9: Official adult-onset offenders, compared to official non-offenders, have weaker intimate-partner attachment bonds

Intimate relationships, as argued by the age-graded theory of informal social control, are an important mechanism in discouraging crime (Laub & Sampson, 2003; Sampson & Laub, 1993). Good quality intimate relationships may also discourage crime (Giordano, Schroeder, & Cernkovich, 2007). Serious intimate relationships usually begin during adulthood and may curb adolescent antisocial behavior. Official adult-onset offenders may have weak adult intimate-partner attachment bonds, which increases their risk of apprehension and conviction for crime. Some evidence has indicated that ending a marital relationship contributes to adult-onset crime (Kivivuori & Linderborg, 2010). Other research has shown that adult-onset offenders were generally less likely to be in a romantic relationship (Mata & van Dulmen, 2012).

Hypothesis 10: Official adult-onset offenders, compared to official non-offenders, have a lower expectation of informal sanctions from actors and institutions

The expectation of informal sanctions from actors and institutions such as friends, family, partners, and employers, may deter criminal behavior (Laub & Sampson, 2011). As people become more free and independent with the transition to adulthood, they may perceive that such actors and institutions will have weakening reactions to criminal behavior; this may be especially true among people who become cut-off from education and social services (Krohn, Gibson, & Thornberry, 2013; Osgood, Foster, & Courtney, 2010; Thornberry, 2005). Official adult-onset offenders may perceive weak informal social control during adulthood, which increases their risk for apprehension and conviction for crime. Research has shown that late-onset crime is related to loss of or mild informal sanctions (Kivivuori & Linderborg, 2010; Mata & van Dulmen, 2012; Sampson & Laub, 1993; Zara & Farrington, 2010).

Data

Data source

We analyzed adult-onset criminal offending among participants of the Dunedin Multidisciplinary Health and Development Study, a longitudinal investigation of health and behavior in a representative birth cohort (Poulton, Moffitt, & Silva, 2015). Study participants (N=1,037; 91% of eligible births; 52% male) were all of the individuals born between April 1972 and March 1973 in Dunedin, New Zealand (NZ), who were eligible for the longitudinal study based on residence in the province of Otago, and who participated in the first assessment at age 3. The cohort represents the full range of socioeconomic status on NZ’s South Island and matches the NZ National Health and Nutrition Survey on adult health indicators (e.g., BMI, smoking, GP visits). Study participants were primarily white; fewer than 7% self-identified as having partial non-white-European ancestry, matching NZ’s South Island. Assessments were carried out in phases at birth and ages 3, 5, 7, 9, 11, 13, 15, 18, 21, 26, 32, and, most recently, 38 years, when 95% of the 1,007 Study participants still alive took part. At each assessment, each Study participant (including outmigrants) is brought to the University of Otago research unit for a full day of interviews and examinations. Assessments also include data from parents, teachers, and informants chosen by the Study participant as someone who knew them well. Data also include linkage to administrative record data sets. To be included in the present report, Study participants had to have either been convicted of a crime in NZ, or survived to phase 38 data collection and lived in NZ as an adult. Our analytic sample of 931 Study members excludes 106 people from the original Study whom we could not definitively consider to be non-offenders through adulthood because of death (n = 24), outmigration (n = 42), long-term missing to the Study (n = 31), or refusal to allow the phase 38 records search (n = 9). As such, the 931 Study participants included in this report either appeared in the conviction records or survived to the phase 38 records search and lived in NZ without a conviction.

Variables

The Dunedin Study contains extensive information about the Study participants relevant to examining adult-onset offending. Table 2 provides information about the variables that we examined, including descriptive statistics by sex.

Table 2.

Variables used in the analysis of adult-onset offending in the Dunedin cohort and their frequency distributions, by sex

Variable Description Males
(n=484)
Females
(n=447)


%/Mean (SD) %/Mean (SD)
Official criminal conviction
Criminal conviction Dichotomous indicator of a conviction for crime. 42.2% 14.8%
Age at first conviction Age at which first criminal conviction occurred, among convicted Study
participants.
19.46 (4.36) 20.85 (5.96)
Unprosecuted, pre-adult antisocial behavior
Evidence of antisocial behavior during childhood
Parent & teacher reports
of childhood antisocial
behavior
Scale of parent and teacher reports of participant antisocial behaviors averaged
across ages 5, 7, 9 and 11. The composite scale ranged from 0 to 8, with one point
allocated for endorsement for each of the following items about the participant’s
behavior: destroys property, fights, disliked by other children, irritable,
disobedient, tells lies, steals, bullies others.
1.72 (1.39) 1.22 (1.03)
Evidence of antisocial behavior during adolescence
Conduct disorder, age 11–
18
Dichotomous indicator of conduct disorder. Conduct disorder was measured
according to the symptom criteria of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of
Mental Disorders (DSM-IV), which identify adolescents displaying a persistent
pattern of behavior that violates the rights of others, including physical harm. A
diagnosis of conduct disorder (using a 12-month reporting period for symptoms)
was made at each of four ages: ages 11, 13, 15, and 18. Study participants were
recorded as having conduct disorder if five or more conduct disorder symptoms
were reported at any of the four waves.
29.0% 14.9%
Any self-reported
offenses:
  Age 13 Dichotomous indicator of any one or more of thirteen types of self-reported
offenses that were consistently measured at ages 13, 15, and 18. The types of
offenses were: running away overnight (runaway), carrying a hidden weapon
(hidden weapon), purposefully destroying or damaging property (vandalism),
purposefully setting fire to a building (arson), breaking into a building to steal
something (breaking & entering), theft, taking something from a store without
paying for it (shoplifting), theft from a vehicle, taking a car without permission and
without intent to keep it (joy-riding), stealing or attempting to steal a car or
motorcycle (vehicle-theft), robbery, possessing marijuana, possessing harder
drugs.
37.3% 20.6%
  Age 15 47.4% 35.4%
  Age 18 59.4% 48.6%
  Age 13–18 75.3% 61.1%
Evidence of police contact as an adolescent up to age 18
Parent-reported police
contact, age 13–15
Dichotomous indicator of parent-reported contact with the police. The parent
reported whether the child had been in trouble with the police between the ages
of 13 and 15 at the age 15 interview.
10.4% 5.7%
Police-recorded arrest
before age 18
Dichotomous indicator of police recorded arrest, obtained by hand search of
Youth-Aid Constable records held by the Dunedin Police.
19.8% 10.1%
Variables used to test explanations of first conviction during adulthood
Hypotheses of how those first convicted as an adult evaded adolescent prosecution
Extent of self-reported
offenses:
  Age 13 Continuous measure of one or more of 13 types of self-reported offenses that
were consistently measured at ages 13, 15, and 18: runaway, hidden weapon,
vandalism, arson, breaking & entering, theft, shoplifting, theft from a vehicle, joy-
riding, vehicle-theft, robbery, possessing marijuana, and possessing harder drugs.
0.75 (1.44) 0.34 (0.79)
  Age 15 1.25 (2.15) 0.88 (1.59)
  Age 18 1.61 (2.23) 0.86 (1.35)
Family socioeconomic
status (SES), age 1–15
The socioeconomic status of Study members’ parents was measured with the
Elley-Irving scale (Elley & Irving, 1976) which assigned occupations into 1 of 6 SES
groups (from 1 = unskilled laborer to 6 = professional). The higher of either
parents' occupation was averaged spanning the period from Study members’ birth
to age 15 (1972–1987).
3.73 (1.13) 3.79 (1.11)
Childhood IQ, age 7–11 Assessed using the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children Revised (WISC-R)
(Wechsler, 1974). IQ scores for ages 7, 9 and 11 were averaged and standardized.
100.47 (15.04) 99.63 (14.06)
Delinquent Peers, age 13
& 15
Measure of delinquency in company of peers at ages 13 and 15. Mean number of
parent's affirmative responses to 10 questions about whether the Study
participant: steals in the company of others, belongs to a gang, is loyal to
delinquent friends, truants from school in the company of others, has "bad
companions", uses drugs in company of others, is part of a group that rejects
school activities, drinks alcohol in company of others, admires/associates with
rougher peers, and admires people who operate outside the law. The scale ranged
from 0 to 10.
0.90 (1.51) 0.83 (1.30)
Social potency at age 18 Social potency was assessed via the Multidimensional Personality Questionnaire
(MPQ) at the age 18 interview. People with low social potency are likely to be
timid and socially withdrawn; they prefer not to be active in a group with others.
The original scale ranged from 0 to 100, with higher scores indicating greater social
potency.
39.45 (24.17) 34.73 (23.24)
Hypotheses of why those first convicted as an adult began getting caught during adulthood
Extent of self-reported
offenses:
  Age 21 Continuous measure of one or more of 48 types of self-reported offenses that
were consistently measured at ages 21, 26, 32, and 36. Four main types of
offenses were assessed: property offenses, rule offenses, drug related offenses,
and violent offenses. Property offenses included 20 items such as vandalism,
breaking and entering, motor vehicle theft, embezzlement from work, shoplifting,
several other kinds of thefts, and several kinds of frauds. Rule offenses included 13
items such as careless and reckless driving, public drunkenness, obstructing the
work of the police, soliciting or selling sex, giving false information on a tax form,
loan application or job application, and disobeying the courts. Drug-related
offenses included 4 items about using and selling various types of illicit drugs.
Violent offenses included 6 items about simple assault, aggravated assault, gang
fighting, robbery, arson, and forced sex. Hitting a child was also assessed with 2
questions about hitting or otherwise hurting a child out of anger, with follow-up
questions ruling out situations of physical discipline. The scale ranged from 0 to 26,
with higher numbers indicating greater involvement in crime.
4.22 (4.01) 1.85 (1.98)
  Age 26 3.06 (3.03) 1.44 (1.63)
  Age 32 1.61 (2.21) 0.72 (1.25)
  Age 36
Schizophrenia or bipolar
disorder, age 21–38
Dichotomous indicator of a DSM-IV diagnosis of schizophrenia or bipolar disorder,
assessed at ages of 21, 26, 32, and 38.
4.5% 4.5%
Alcohol or substance
dependence:
  Alcohol dependence,
  age 21–38
Dichotomous indicator of Study participants’ persistent dependence on alcohol or
drugs assessed between ages 21 and 38. Respondents were asked questions which
tapped into DSM criteria for dependence on alcohol and drugs. Respondents were
noted as having a persistent history of alcohol or drug dependence if they had two
or more waves with a diagnosis of dependence for each type.
17.0% 6.3%
  Drug dependence, 12.8%
  age 21–38
12.8% 4.9%
Months living with
spouse or partner, age
21–38
Number of months reported living with spouse or de facto partner between 21
and 38 years of age. Calculated from
114.08
(57.78)
128.29
(59.76)
Very happy with
relationship:
  Age 21 Dichotomous indicator of whether participant was happy with their relationship.
Asked only of those in any relationship during the past year. Study participants
were asked about their overall happiness with their partner at ages 26 and 32.
Respondents replied that they were either “unhappy”, “somewhat happy” or “very
happy.” The majority of respondents indicated that they were very happy with
their relationship and the measure was dichotomized into “very happy” and other.
72.2% 76.9%
  Age 26 76.2% 80.1%
  Age 32
Informal sanctions, ages
21 and 26, from:
  Friends Scale of perceived informal consequences. Study participants were asked “Would
you lose the respect and good opinion of your close friends if they found out that
you…?”, “Would you lose the respect and good opinion of your parents and
relatives if they found out that you…?”, “Would it harm your chance to attract or
keep your ideal partner if people knew that you…?”, “Would it harm your future
job prospects if people knew that you…?”. Crimes queried were shoplifting, drug
use, car theft, partner violence, assault, burglary, drunk driving, and using a stolen
bank card. Responses were coded 2=yes, 1=maybe, 0=no. These questions were
asked at ages 21 and 26.
8.49 (3.47) 10.15 (3.19)
  Parents 12.53 (2.57) 13.24 (2.29)
  Partner 10.36 (3.14) 10.54 (3.32)
  Employers 12.83 (2.05) 13.30 (1.70)
  All 44.19 (8.82) 47.24 (8.54)

Adult-onset conviction

We defined adult-onset offending as an initial criminal conviction at or after 20 years of age, the age of legal majority in NZ from 1970 to the time of the last records search. The age of majority has often been used as the cutoff point in criminological studies of adult-onset offending. Research in developmental psychology, however, has suggested that contemporary cohorts have a protracted adolescence and gradually transition to adulthood during their mid-twenties (Arnett, 2000). Arnett’s concept of “social adulthood” has been incorporated in criminological theory (Thornberry, 2005), and at least one study on adult-onset offending has used Arnett’s concept and operationalized adulthood as beginning at 25 years of age (Sohoni et al., 2014). Consequently, we also analyzed a group of offenders who met the definition of social-adulthood-onset, defined as an initial criminal conviction at or after 25 years of age.

We used the participants’ first official criminal conviction as a measure of the age of onset of official criminal offending, a standard used in many past studies of adult-onset offending (see Table 1). Official criminal conviction records have the advantage of being unambiguous with regard to both the occurrence of a crime and the age of conviction. We obtained information about criminal convictions by searching the central computer system of the New Zealand Police, which provides details of all New Zealand convictions and sentences and Australian convictions communicated to the New Zealand Police. We conducted searches following the completion of each assessment at ages 21, 26, 32, and 38 (search completed in 2013). Official records of criminal conviction were available from 14 years of age onwards, the age from which criminal conviction was permissible. We tabulated criminal convictions from both youth and adult courts by grouping charges according to general types of crime (see Appendix 1).

Evidence of antisocial behavior before adulthood

To test whether official criminal conviction represented the ‘true’ onset of antisocial behavior we examined various measures of the Study participants’ unprosecuted, pre-adult antisocial behavior and police contact. We analyzed (a) reports of participants’ childhood antisocial behavior made by teachers and parents; (b) diagnoses of conduct disorder made in adolescence; (c) self-reports of juvenile delinquency; (d) parent-reports of police contact during adolescence; and (e) Study participants’ contact with police prior to age 17 as recorded on the “333 form” completed by officers after each arrest and held by the Dunedin Police. The 333 form was used by NZ police, while the Study members were growing up, to register police diversion from formal prosecution to an informal process managed by a youth constable.

Proposed causes of adult-onset conviction

We examined variables that tapped into ten hypotheses about adult-onset offending. The first five hypotheses explained why adolescent offenders may have been able to evade prosecution prior to a first conviction in adulthood. These hypotheses included fewer self-reported offenses, higher socioeconomic status, higher intelligence, fewer delinquent peers, and lower scores on a personality trait called “social potency.” The remaining five hypotheses explained why people may have offended and been apprehended for crime for the first time during adulthood. These hypotheses included more self-reported offenses; schizophrenia or bipolar disorder; substance dependence (alcohol or drugs); weak intimate-partner attachment; and self-perceived low risk of informal sanctions of crime. We analyzed a number of variables, detailed in Table 2, to test these hypotheses.

Analytical approach

Our analyses of the adult-onset offender aimed to answer four main questions: 1) Are there official adult-onset offenders in the Dunedin cohort? 2) Does adult-onset conviction indicate adult-onset antisocial activity? 3) Do adult-onset offenders tend to be convicted for different types of crimes compared to juvenile-onset offenders? 4) Which theories can explain adult-onset conviction? We answered these questions through bivariate hypothesis-testing analyses, comparing the official adult-onset offender group to the official non-offender group and/or official juvenile-onset offender group on various aspects. Bivariate analysis was an appropriate modeling choice as each test tied into a specific hypothesis about how the official adult-onset offender group compared to the non-offender or official juvenile-onset offender group. The analyses could be straightforward because none of our theory-derived hypotheses specified that a given construct alters the probability of crime in the absence of another factor, or while in interaction with another factor.

Results

Are there adult-onset offenders in the Dunedin birth cohort?

Table 3 shows descriptive information on the participants of the Dunedin Study grouped by age at first conviction for males and females, separately. We found a substantial male official adult-onset offending group. Of the male Study participants, 14% were first convicted during adulthood, at or after 20 years of age, as of the latest search of criminal justice system records in 2013, when Study participants were approximately 40 years of age. Official adult-onset men represented about one-third of convicted men in the Study. Male official adult-onset offenders were, on average, first convicted around 24 years of age. The oldest age at first conviction was 37 years; second and later convictions occurred through 40 years of age. Male official adult-onset offenders had, on average, 4 lifetime convictions each, and their convictions accounted for 15% of the men’s total convictions.

Table 3.

Description of Study participants as a function of sex and conviction status

Men Women


Never
convicted1
Juvenile-
onset2
Adult-
onset3
Social-
adulthood-
onset subset4
Never
convicted1
Juvenile-
onset2
Adult-
onset3
Social-adult
hood-onset
subset4








Participant-level descriptive statistics
Number of Study participants 280 138 66 20 381 38 28 12
Percentage of Study participants 57.9% 28.5% 13.6% 4.1% 85.2% 8.5% 6.3% 2.7%
Percentage of offenders N/A 67.7% 32.4% 9.8% N/A 57.6% 42.4% 18.2%
Mean age at first conviction
(std dev)
N/A 17.14 (1.32) 24.29 (4.55) 30.15 (3.80) N/A 17.03 (1.17) 26.04 (5.93) 31.92 (4.36)
Median age at first conviction N/A 17 22.50 30 N/A 17 22.50 32
Maximum age at first conviction N/A 19 37 37 N/A 19 38 38
Mean lifetime convictions per
participant (std dev)
N/A 11.92 (20.33) 4.24 (6.97) 2.35 (2.64) N/A 8.42 (13.69) 2.93 (2.98) 3.42 (2.61)
Conviction-level descriptive statistics
Number of convictions N/A 1,643 279 47 N/A 320 82 41
Percentage of Study
participants’ total convictions
N/A 85.5% 14.5% 2.5% N/A 79.6% 20.4% 10.2%

Notes:

1

-No conviction by age 40 years.

2

-First conviction age 14 to 19 years.

3

-First conviction age 20 years and above. The adult-onset group also includes the social-adulthood subset.

4

-First conviction age 25 years and above.

Of the male Study participants, only 4% were in the official social-adulthood-onset offender subset, first convicted at or after 25 years of age. Official social-adulthood-onset offenders represented one-tenth of convicted men. Male official social-adulthood-onset offenders were, on average, first convicted around 30 years of age. Male official social-adulthood-onset offenders had, on average, only 2 lifetime convictions each and the convictions of this subset of official adult-onset offenders accounted for only 3% of the cohort men’s total convictions.

Of the male Study participants, 29% were official juvenile-onset offenders, convicted before 20 years of age, the age of legal majority in NZ. Official juvenile-onset offenders represented two-thirds of convicted men in the Study. Male official juvenile-onset offenders were first convicted, on average, around 17 years of age, with second and later convictions occurring through 39 years of age. The official juvenile-onset men had, on average, 12 lifetime convictions each, and their convictions accounted for 85% of the men’s total convictions.

The majority of male Study participants, 58%, had not been convicted of a crime (“never convicted”).

Of the female Study members, 6% were first convicted in adulthood. Official adult-onset women represented nearly half of the convicted women in the Study. Female official adult-onset offenders were, on average, first convicted at 26 years of age. The oldest age at first conviction was 38 years; second and later convictions occurred through 40 years of age. Female official adult-onset offenders had, on average, 3 lifetime convictions each and their convictions accounted for 20% of the women’s total convictions.

Of the female Study participants, only 3% were in the official social-adulthood-onset offender subset. Official social-adulthood-onset offenders represented about one-fifth of convicted women. Female official social-adulthood-onset offenders were, on average, first convicted around 32 years of age. Female official social-adulthood-onset offenders had, on average, 3 lifetime convictions each and the convictions of this subset of adult-onset offenders accounted for 10% of the women’s total convictions.

Of the female Study members, 9% were official juvenile-onset offenders. Official juvenileonset offenders represented over half of the convicted women in the Study. Female official juvenile-onset offenders were, on average, first convicted at 17 years of age. Female official adult-onset offenders had, on average, 8 lifetime convictions each and their convictions accounted for 80% of the women’s total convictions.

The vast majority of female Study participants (85%) had not been convicted of a crime. These initial descriptive analyses (Figure 1) indicated that our main analyses should include the full official adult-onset offending group. The official social-adulthood subset was too small to study with adequate statistical power; analyses of the official social-adulthood-onset offender subset (available from the corresponding author) showed that they did not significantly differ from the full official adult-onset offender group on the remaining covariates. Likewise, these initial descriptive analyses showed that our analyses should focus on men, as comparisons among women offenders would lack adequate statistical power. Nonetheless, women appear to be somewhat different from men with regard to official adult-onset offending and we briefly return to women in the discussion. Complete analyses on women are available from the corresponding author.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Prevalence of age-of-first-conviction group by sex

Does adult-onset conviction really indicate adult-onset antisocial activity?

Table 4 presents descriptive statistics and mean or proportion difference tests of unprosecuted antisocial behavior and police contact among cohort males. These data were gathered as a part of the Dunedin Study during the cohort’s childhood and adolescence. Although the official adult-onset men were first convicted during adulthood, the data suggested that most had begun their involvement in antisocial activities as children or adolescents. When all prospectively-recorded reports of juvenile antisocial behavior were compiled (last row of Table 4), 85% of the official adult-onset men had, as a juvenile, displayed evidence of notable antisocial activities. In fact, as a group, the official adult-onset men were more similar in their antisocial behavior to the official juvenile-onset men than to the official never-convicted men. On average, the official adult-onset men, compared to the official never-convicted men, had significantly more parent- and teacher-reported childhood antisocial behavior (t = 3.91, p = <.001). During adolescence, the official adult-onset men were, on average, significantly more likely than the official never-convicted men to meet diagnostic criteria for conduct disorder (t = 3.75, p = <.001) and to have self-reported crime (with the exception of self-reported crime at 15 years of age). The official adult-onset men, compared to the official never-convicted men, were also more likely to have had contact with the police during adolescence; fully 24% of the official adult-onset men already had a formal police record of arrest or police contact as a juvenile, although these arrests had not led to a criminal conviction.

Table 4.

Do male adult-onset offenders, defined on the basis of first conviction, offend prior to adulthood? Unprosecuted antisocial behavior and police contact from childhood through late adolescence as a function of age-of-first-conviction group

Age-of-first-conviction group Test statistics


Never Convicted1 Juvenile-onset2 Adult-onset3 Adult-onset vs
Never convicted
Adult-onset vs
Juvenile-onset
Mean (SD) Mean (SD) Mean (SD) t p4 t p4


Evidence of ASB during childhood
  Parent and teacher reports of anti-social
  behavior, age 5–11 years (z-score)5
−0.25 (.85) .40 (1.15) .21 (0.95) 3.91 <0.001 −1.16 0.124
% % % z p4 z p4


Evidence of ASB during adolescence
  Conduct disorder diagnosis, age 11–18 14.1% 56.5% 33.8% 3.75 <0.001 −3.02 0.001
  Any self-reported offending at age 13 29.5% 49.0% 46.5% 2.16 0.015 −0.27 0.392
  Any self-reported offending at age 15 42.6% 56.0% 49.2% 0.93 0.180 −0.88 0.189
  Any self-reported offending at age 18 46.7% 78.4% 74.2% 3.89 < 0.001 −0.64 0.260
  Any self-reported offending, age 13–18 66.1% 88.4% 85.9% 3.12 <0.001 −0.50 0.310
Evidence of police contact up to age 18 years
  Parent-reported police contact, age 13–15 3.4% 21.9% 16.7% 3.92 <0.001 −0.83 0.204
  Police-recorded arrest before age 18 9.6% 38.4% 24.2% 3.23 <0.001 −2.00 0.023
Any adolescent ASB or police contact 65.4% 92.0% 84.9% 3.08 0.001 −1.58 0.057

Notes:

1

-No conviction by age 40 years, n=280.

2

-First conviction age 14 to 19 years, n=138.

3

-First conviction age 20 years and above, n=66.

4

-p-values are one-tailed.

5

-sex-standardized z-score.

Bold text indicates statistically significant difference; one-tailed p < .05.

ASB – antisocial behavior

Do official adult-onset men tend to be convicted of different types of crimes compared to official juvenile-onset men?

Turning to the types of crime for which Study members were convicted, the risk ratios (Table 5) indicated that some types of crime were relatively more likely among official adult-onset men compared to official juvenile-onset men. Panel A of Table 5 shows the distribution and average rate per person of the different types of non-status offense convictions up to 40 years of age for the official juvenile-onset and the official adult-onset groups; status offenses were not possible among official adult-onset men as these men were above the age of majority at their first conviction. Panel A of Table 5 also shows the relative risk of a specific type of conviction among the official adult-onset men compared to the official juvenile-onset men, given that a conviction has occurred.

Table 5.

Are male adult-onset offenders convicted of different types of crimes compared to male juvenile-onset offenders? Comparison between juvenile- and adult-onset men on types of non-status offense crime

Panel A: Convictions between ages 14 and 40 years as a function of age at first conviction.
Conviction type Age-of-first-conviction group Adult-onset vs
juvenile-onset risk
ratio
(95% CI)

Juvenile-onset Adult-onset

% Rate Per
person1
% Rate Per
person2
Property & fraud crimes 41.2% 5.31 42.7% 1.80 1.03 (0.89–1.17)
Driving under the influence &
criminal driving violations
21.4% 2.76 31.2% 1.32 1.46 (1.20–1.72)
Violent & weapon crimes 13.8% 1.78 7.5% 0.32 0.54 (0.35–0.82)
Drug-related crimes 9.6% 1.24 3.9% 0.17 0.41 (0.23–0.73)
Criminal justice system violations 9.3% 1.19 7.5% 0.32 0.81 (0.52–1.22)
Crimes against women 2.3% 0.30 2.9% 0.12 1.24 (0.58–2.45)
Odd crimes 2.4% 0.31 4.3% 0.18 1.81 (0.96–3.14)
Total 100% 12.90 100% 4.23
Panel B: Conviction period held constant across groups. Convictions between ages 20 and 40 years as a
function of age at first conviction.
Conviction type Age-of-first-conviction group Adult-onset vs
juvenile-onset risk
ratio
(95% CI)

Juvenile-onset Adult-onset

% Rate Per
person3
% Rate Per
person2
Property & fraud crimes 32.5% 3.56 42.7% 1.80 1.31 (1.11–1.55)
Driving under the influence &
criminal driving violations
18.5% 2.02 31.2% 1.32 1.69 (1.36–2.11)
Violent & weapon crimes 16.3% 1.79 7.5% 0.32 0.46 (0.30–0.71)
Drug-related crimes 13.6% 1.49 3.9% 0.17 0.29 (0.16–0.53)
Criminal justice system violations 12.3% 1.35 7.5% 0.32 0.61 (0.39–0.95)
Crimes against women 3.9% 0.42 2.9% 0.12 0.74 (0.35–1.58)
Odd crimes 2.9% 0.32 4.3% 0.18 1.48 (0.76–2.89)
Total 100% 10.96 100% 4.23

Notes:

1

-Number of observations: 124 men and 1599 convictions. Of the 138 juvenile-onset men, 14 (10%) were only ever convicted of a status offense.

2

-Number of observations: 66 men and 279 convictions.

3

-Number of observations: 85 men and 932 convictions. Of the 138 juvenile-onset men, 85 (62%) had a criminal convictions at age 20 or later.

CI-confidence interval.

Bold indicates a statistically significant different risk of conviction.

94 of the person-years (2% of the 3800 person-years in Panel A, 3% of the 3020 person-years in Panel B) were spent incarcerated.

Incarceration time did not substantively alter the results presented above and we subsequently report the more parsimonious rate of offending by person, rather than by person-year.

Among official adult-onset offenders, 30% of convictions were for driving under the influence of drugs or alcohol and other criminal driving violations; the comparable figure among official juvenile-onset offenders was 20%. A conviction for driving under the influence or a criminal driving violation was a 50% more likely type of conviction among official adult-onset men compared to official juvenile-onset men. In contrast, violent or weapon and drug crime convictions were half as likely among the official adult-onset men compared to the official juvenile-onset men. Conviction for an “odd” crime (such as offensive public behavior, peeping Tom, or an unregistered dog) was a slightly more likely type of conviction among official adult-onset men, compared to official juvenile-onset men, but not significantly so.

It is possible that the risk ratios shown in Panel A could have arisen because the official juvenile-onset group’s crime records covered more years and included the volatile adolescent period (14 to 19 years of age). Research has also shown that there may also be specific age curves for specific types of crime (Massoglia, 2006; Steffensmeier, Allan, Harer, & Streifel, 1989). To correct for this, Table 5 Panel B presents types of convictions occurring from 20 to 40 years of age for both official-onset groups, thereby holding constant across the two groups the number of years to offend and the age period of offending. Conviction-type patterns from 20 years of age onwards were similar to those for lifetime convictions. The additional finding emerged that conviction for property and fraud crime was a more likely type of conviction among the official adult-onset men than the official juvenile-onset men.

Figure 2 provides a summary of our results taking into account the exposure period (age 20–40 years). Matching the two groups on exposure produced a fairly even distribution of the type of conviction across official juvenile-onset men. In contrast, conviction types for official adult-onset men were largely concentrated as property crime and fraud, and driving-under-the-influence and other criminal driving violations. The prevalence of certain types of convictions, thus, did seem to vary between official adult-onset men and official juvenile-onset men during the same age-period. In summary, the average official adult-onset man was likely to have fewer convictions than the average official juvenile-onset man (as indicated by a lower rate per person among official adult-onset men), but the convictions were more likely to be property crime/fraud or driving crime convictions.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Convictions at 20 years of age and older as a function of age-of-first-conviction group in males. The adult-onset group had proportionally more convictions for property/fraud crimes and driving under the influence/criminal driving. The adult-onset group had proportionally fewer convictions for violent/weapon crimes and drug-related crimes

As Figure 3 shows, although official adult-onset offenders had far fewer convictions than juvenile-onset offenders, the annual percentage of convictions was similarly distributed across age between official adult-onset and official juvenile-onset men from 20 to 40 years of age. Most convictions occurred during the early 20s. From the mid-20s onwards, compared to the early 20s, fewer convictions occurred with each passing year.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Annual percentage of adult convictions, cumulative distribution. The adult-onset men committed fewer crimes, but they were proportionally committed at the same age as those committed by juvenile-onset men. For both conviction groups, most convictions after age 20 years occurred during the early 20s. However, convictions steadily accumulated through age 40

Can existing theories explain adult-onset conviction?

We next tested the ten hypotheses about official adult-onset offending by comparing mean or proportion differences between official adult-onset men and official juvenile-onset men or official never-convicted men. Table 6 presents each of the hypotheses, the variable(s) that we used to test each hypothesis, whether a given variable supported the hypothesis, the data for the mean or proportion difference test, the test statistic, and the corresponding one-tailed p-value.

Table 6.

Testing ten explanations from the literature for first conviction during adulthood

Variable Variable
supports
hypothesis
Age-of-first-conviction group Test statistics


Never
convicted
Juvenile-onset Adult-onset Adult-onset vs
never convicted
Adult-onset vs
juvenile-onset


t/z1 p t/z1 p
Panel A. People first convicted in adulthood evaded adolescent prosecution because…
H1: Fewer offenses during adolescence reduced the likelihood of apprehension
Extent of self-reported offenses, age 13 (z-score)3 No −0.19 (0.61) 0.34 (1.47) 0.11 (0.91) 2.09 0.0212 −1.11 0.1342
Extent of self-reported offenses, age 15 (z-score)3 Yes −0.21 (0.69) 0.41 (1.40) −0.01 (0.81) 1.96 0.026 −2.64 0.0052
Extent of self-reported offenses, age 18 (z-score)3 Yes −0.31 (0.63) 0.64 (1.32) 0.01 (0.91) 2.56 0.0062 −3.80 <0.0012
H2: Higher juvenile socioeconomic status protected them against formal sanctions
Family SES, ages 1–15 (z-score)3 No 0.18 (1.01) −0.29 (0.94) − 0.18 (0.93) −2.65 0.004 0.77 0.221
H3: Higher intelligence facilitated evasion of detection
Childhood IQ, ages 7–11 No 102.99 (14.88) 97.85 (14.11) 95.35 (15.50) −3.72 <0.001 −1.14 0.128
H4: Fewer delinquent peers during adolescence reduced visible delinquency and likelihood of detection
Delinquent Peers, ages 13 & 15 (z-score)3 Yes −0.24 (0.64) 0.52 (1.42) −0.06 (0.78) 1.73 0.0442 −3.67 <0.0012
H5: Social inhibition prevented visible delinquency and likelihood of detection
Social Potency, age 18 (z-score)3 Yes 0.00 (1.02) 0.12 (0.98) − 0.23 (0.92) −1.62 0.053 −2.36 0.010
Panel B. People first convicted in adulthood begin getting prosecuted because they have…
H6: More offenses during adulthood, increasing the likelihood of apprehension
Extent of self-reported offenses, age 21 (z-score)3 Yes −0.29 (0.71) 0.55 (1.29) 0.13 (0.91) 4.04 <0.001 −2.61 0.0052
Extent of self-reported offenses, age 26 (z-score)3 Yes −0.27 (0.69) 0.50 (1.29) 0.15 (1.04) 3.13 0.0012 −1.88 0.031
Extent of self-reported offenses, age 32 (z-score) 3 Yes −0.22 (0.69) 0.38 (1.41) 0.17 (0.85) 3.48 <0.0012 −1.28 0.1012
Extent of self-reported offenses, age 38 (z-score)3 Yes −0.23 (0.56) 0.39 (1.34) 0.25 (1.38) 2.66 0.0052 −0.650 0.26
H7: Adult-onset schizophrenia or bipolar disorder
Schizophrenia or bipolar disorder, age 21–38 Yes 1.8% 7.8% 9.8% 3.18 0.001 0.48 0.315
H8: Alcohol or substance dependence
Alcohol dependence, age 21–38 Yes 11.4% 25.4% 22.7% 2.41 0.008 −0.41 0.341
Drug dependence, age 21–38 Yes 5.4% 28.3% 12.1% 1.98 0.024 −2.56 0.005
H9: Weak intimate-partner attachment bonds
Months living with spouse or partner, age 21–38 No 114.95 (59.15) 118.60 (54.94) 101.30 (56.27) −1.64 0.051 −1.99 0.024
Very happy with relationship, age 21 No 66.5% 63.9% 56.3% −1.33 0.091 −0.91 0.183
Very happy with relationship, age 26 Yes 75.3% 70.2% 63.0% −1.84 0.033 −0.93 0.175
Very happy with relationship, age 32 No 76.7% 76.1% 74.1% −0.40 0.343 −0.29 0.388
H10: Lower expectations of informal sanctions from actors and institutions
Age 21 informal sanctions from:
  Friends (z-score)3 Yes 0.28 (0.94) −0.48 (0.97) −0.22 (0.92) −3.90 <0.001 1.78 0.038
  Parents (z-score)3 Yes 0.20 (0.79) −0.42 (1.23) −0.02 (1.03) −1.95 0.026 2.27 0.012
  Partner (z-score) 3 Yes 0.20 (0.83) −0.38 (1.20) −0.10 (1.00) −2.52 0.006 1.63 0.052
  Employers (z-score)3 No 0.06 (0.97) −0.08 (1.00) −0.10 (1.10) −1.20 0.116 −0.17 0.432
  All (z-score)3 Yes 0.27 (0.85) −0.48 (1.14) −0.17 (0.89) −3.65 <0.001 2.12 0.0182
Age 26 informal sanctions from:
  Friends (z-score)3 Yes 0.27 (0.86) −0.41 (1.09) −0.31 (1.00) −4.76 <0.001 0.60 0.273
  Parents (z-score)3 Yes 0.20 (0.77) −0.42 (1.32) −0.02 (0.85) −2.07 0.020 2.51 0.0072
  Partner (z-score) 3 No 0.14 (0.96) −0.29 (1.10) −0.02 (0.81) −1.29 0.099 1.91 0.0292
  Employers (z-score)3 No −0.01 (1.05) 0.08 (0.90) −0.13 (0.95) −0.89 0.188 1.53 0.064
  All (z-score)3 Yes 0.22 (0.91) −0.36 (1.13) −0.18 (0.84) 3.23 <0.001 1.22 0.1122

Notes:

1

-Test statistic is t for difference of means, z for difference in proportions.

2

-Unequal variance between groups. Degrees of freedom calculated using Satterthwaite’s (1946) equation.

3

-sex-standardized z-score.

Bold text indicates statistically significant difference between groups; one-tailed p < .05.

Panel A of Table 6 presents results of the five hypotheses on why official adult-onset men avoided detection until adulthood, despite having engaged in antisocial activities as juveniles. In Panel A, the pertinent comparison is between the official adult-onset men and the official juvenile-onset men, shown in the shaded columns. The first hypothesis predicted that the official adult-onset men had committed fewer offenses than the official juvenile-onset men during adolescence. This was theorized to reduce their likelihood of apprehension and, subsequently, conviction. This hypothesis was generally supported. The official adult-onset men had, on average, self-reported fewer offenses at 13 years of age (t = −1.11, p = .134) and significantly fewer offenses at 15 and 18 years of age compared to the official juvenile-onset men (t = −2.64, p = .005; t = −3.80, p < .001, respectively). Thus, although Table 3 shows that most of the official adult-onset men had engaged in antisocial behaviors and delinquent offending as juveniles, their average self-reported offending was less than that of the official juvenile-onset men.

The second hypothesis argued that the official adult-onset men, compared to the official juvenile-onset men, came from families with higher socioeconomic status. Presumably a higher socioeconomic status insulated official adult-onset men from a juvenile criminal conviction. This hypothesis was not supported. The official adult-onset men had a somewhat higher average family socioeconomic status as youths than the official juvenile-onset men, but not significantly so (t = 0.77, p = .221).

The third hypothesis stated that the official adult-onset men were more intelligent than official juvenile-onset men. Higher intelligence theoretically enabled official adult-onset men to evade detection as juveniles. This hypothesis was not supported. In fact, the official adult-onset men’s average level of cognitive ability was lower than that of the official juvenile-onset men, though not significantly so (t = −1.14, p = .128).

The fourth hypothesis argued that the official adult-onset men had fewer delinquent peers than the official juvenile-onset men. Having fewer delinquent peers was presumed to reduce the extent and diversity of offending, and likelihood of apprehension. This hypothesis was supported. Official adult-onset men had, on average, significantly fewer delinquent friends than official juvenile-onset men (z = −3.67, p < .001).

The fifth hypothesis stated that the official adult-onset men, compared to the official juvenile-onset men, were more socially inhibited. It was theorized that social inhibition excluded official adult-onset men from group offending, reducing the likelihood of apprehension and conviction. This personal construct of timidity was measured by low scores on a scale of “social potency.” This hypothesis was supported. The official adult-onset men’s average social potency score was significantly lower than that of the official juvenile-onset men’s average score (t = −2.36, p = .010).

Panel B of Table 6 presents results for five additional hypotheses, about why the official adult-onset men first began getting convicted during adulthood. The relevant comparison in this panel is between the official adult-onset men and the official never-convicted men, shown in the shaded columns.

The sixth hypothesis stated that the official adult-onset men committed more offenses than the official never-convicted men during adulthood. This was theorized to increase their likelihood of apprehension and, subsequently, conviction. This hypothesis was generally supported. The official adult-onset men, compared to the official never-convicted men, had, on average, self-reported more offenses at 21, 26, 32, and 38 years of age (t = 4.04, p <. 001; t = 3.13, p = .001; t = 3.48, p < .001; t = 2.66, p = .005, respectively).

The seventh hypothesis argued that the official adult-onset men were likely to have had schizophrenia or bipolar disorder. It was theorized that these mental-health illnesses could promote offenses such as assault or vagrancy, or erratic behavior that draws public attention and increases the risk of apprehension. This hypothesis was supported. Significantly more of the official adult-onset men, compared to the official never-convicted men, suffered from schizophrenia or bipolar disorder (z = 3.18, p < .001).

The eighth hypothesis predicted that official adult-onset offenders were likely to be dependent on alcohol or substances in adulthood. It was theorized that these dependency problems promote criminal behavior and also may have caused erratic, attention-drawing behavior. This hypothesis is consistent with our earlier observation that a large proportion of the convictions among the official adult-onset men were for driving under the influence. This hypothesis was supported. Significantly more of the official adult-onset men, compared to the official never-convicted men, were dependent on alcohol (z = 2.41, p = .008) and drugs (z = 1.98, p = .024).

The ninth hypothesis predicted that official adult-onset offenders had weak intimate-partner attachment bonds. Weak intimate-partner attachment bonds were theorized to be criminogenic. This hypothesis was minimally supported. The official adult-onset men had, on average, lived with a spouse or partner fewer months than the official never-convicted men between 20 and 38 years of age, but not significantly so (t = −1.64, p = .051). Official adult-onset men expressed less relationship satisfaction than official never-convicted men at all adult ages assessed, but this difference attained statistical significance only at age 26 years (age 21 z = −1.64, p = .054; age 26 z = −1.84, p = 0.033; age 32 z = −0.40, p = .343).

The tenth hypothesis argued that the official adult-onset men had low expectations of informal sanctions from actors and institutions. Low expectations of informal sanctions would theoretically promote criminal behavior. This hypothesis was generally supported. The typical official adult-onset man reported a belief that committing crimes would bring him few informal sanctions. In contrast, the typical official never-convicted man anticipated that committing crime would bring more informal sanctions. On average, the official adult-onset men, compared to the official never-convicted men, expected significantly less informal sanction from friends (age 21: t = −5.07, p < .001; age 26: t = −4.76, p < .001), parents (age 21: t = −2.55, p = .006; age 26: t = −2.07, p < .020), and a partner at age 21 (t = −2.52, p = .006), but not partner at age 26 (t = −1.29, p = .099) or employers (age 21: t = −1.20, p = 0.116; age 26: t = −0.89, p = .188). The mean official adult-onset men’s composite score of perceived informal sanctions across all four actors and institutions was also significantly lower than that of the mean official never-convicted men’s composite score (age 21: t = −3.65, p < .001; age 26: t = 3.23, p < .001), indicating lower levels of expected informal social control overall. Since expectations of informal sanctions were measured near the beginning of the adult offending period, it is unlikely that the experience of conviction altered expectations.

Finally, we tested an eleventh hypothesis (not shown in Table 6), suggested by a reviewer, although we had not found this hypothesis articulated in the literature: Perhaps adult-onset official offenders are driven to crime because they suffer unemployment as adults. They may turn to crime for economic reasons or because of a lack of legitimate social ties at work. We found that official adult-onset offenders were not significantly more likely to be unemployed compared to official non-offenders (z = 1.44, p = .076) (however, official juvenile-onset offenders were significantly more likely to be unemployed than official adult-onset offenders, z = 2.62, p = .004).

It is useful to note that for 19 of the 28 variables evaluated in Table 6, official adult-onset offenders scored in-between official non-offenders and official juvenile-onset offenders, though differences may not have always been statistically significant. Moreover, in 21 of the 28 contrasts with the official never-convicted group, the official adult-onset offender group was significantly “worse-off” in terms of the variables examined. This suggests the hypothesis that most causes of offending apply to both official juvenile- and official adult-onset offenders, regardless of the age of their first conviction.

Discussion

We used data on a contemporary representative birth cohort to study apparent adult-onset offenders and their offending. We first documented that official adult-onset offenders comprised fewer than half of all official offenders (the official social-adulthood-onset offenders comprised fewer than one-fifth of all official offenders). Additionally, official adult-onset offenders in our study were responsible for relatively few of the cohort’s total convictions (only 15%). Our results, along with past research (see, for example, Thompson et al., 2014b), indicate that as a whole, apparent adult-onset offenders are responsible for a small minority of total crime.

We next expanded upon previous evidence of pre-adult criminal behavior by analyzing behavior back through early childhood. Our results showed that most of the official adult-onset men began their antisocial activities during early childhood. In fact, on average, official adult-onset men were similar to juvenile-onset men on multiple measures of child and adolescent antisocial behavior. On two of the more sensitive measures of serious problem behavior – conduct disorder diagnosis and police-reported arrest – official adult-onset men fell in-between official never-convicted and official juvenile-onset men. Because we were able to extend our analysis back to early childhood we showed that official adult-onset offenders do not appear simply delayed in their onset of offending. Rather the results imply a continuum of the degree of pre-conviction involvement in offending, with official adult-onset men somewhere in the middle between official never-convicted men and official juvenile-onset men.

Official adult-onset men also appeared to be involved in less-serious offenses, as violent and drug-related convictions were a relatively more likely type of conviction among the official juvenile-onset men. Instead, conviction for property crime, fraud, or a driving violation was a relatively more likely type of conviction among official adult-onset men, compared to official juvenile-onset men. In absolute terms, official adult-onset men were responsible for a minority of these and all other types of crime. In fact, during adulthood, the average official adult-onset man was responsible for only one-half as many crimes as the average official juvenile-onset man. These results suggest a continuum of crime engagement and seriousness with official adult-onset men typically below official juvenile-onset men. Additionally, convictions across adulthood, between ages 20 and 40, accumulated at a similar rate among official adult-onset and official juvenile-onset men. This indicates that the official adult-onset men were not on a unique trajectory of accelerated offending as they aged.

Finally, we found at least minimal support for eight potential explanations of official adult-onset offending. In general, official adult-onset offenders appeared to avoid apprehension during adolescence, possibly because of their relatively limited criminal behavior, withdrawn nature, and lack of a delinquent peer group. During adulthood, it seemed as though the average official adult-onset offender continued to offend, unrestrained by informal social controls that typically come with age, and possibly afflicted by a mental illness or substance dependence. Perhaps because of erratic behavior, or by simply increasing the risk of apprehension with continued offending, the official adult-onset offender was eventually caught and convicted of crime. Again, the average official adult-onset man seemed to fall in-between the average official never-convicted man and the average official juvenile-onset man on measures of antisocial behavior and its corresponding risk factors.

Our analyses did not support the hypotheses that adult-onset offenders evaded detection and conviction as juveniles due to high socioeconomic status and high intelligence, which is perhaps not surprising when considering our data. First, as a group, official adult-onset offenders’ family socioeconomic status was, in fact, below average for the cohort. High socioeconomic status may offer some protection from the law, but the average official adult-onset man in our cohort was not well-off enough as a juvenile to benefit from such protection. Instead, the relatively low childhood socioeconomic status of the official adult-onset man worked as risk factor for crime. Second, the average official adult-onset male offender had apparently not evaded juvenile conviction as a result of his high intelligence. In fact, our data indicated that below-average childhood intelligence characterized the average official adult-onset offender. This result may support the idea that, among people with low intelligence, a loss of informal social control in the transition to adulthood exacerbates crime (Krohn et al., 2013). Some research has found that adult-onset offenders have lower intelligence than adolescent-limited-juvenile-onset offenders and non-offenders, but not persistent offenders (Gomez-Smith & Piquero, 2005). Our results are consistent with a continuum of risk and of crime: the average official male adult-onset offender came from somewhat lower levels in the class structure, had somewhat lower IQ, and committed a low level of crime.

There was a small group (n = 10) of official adult-onset men who appeared to be true de novo adult-onset offenders (that is, they had no reports of antisocial behavior or police contact during adolescence; see Table 4). A limitation of this study is that we were underpowered to statistically analyze this group.

With respect to policy, our results imply that resource-intensive punishment and incapacitation interventions should continue to be focused on official juvenile-onset offenders rather than official adult-onset offenders. The apparent adult-onset men in our study were responsible for a minority of crime (official and self-reported) and had lower levels of adolescent antisocial behavior, contrary to some expectations (Krohn et al., 2013). Given limited resources, efforts to curb crime may be better focused on official juvenile-onset offenders. Recent debates in popular media (“What Age Should Young Criminals Be Tried as Adults?,” 2015) and discussions hosted by the National Institute of Justice (“Discussing the Future of Justice-Involved Young Adults,” 2015) have focused on raising the age at which offenders can be tried as adults. People convicted in adult court, regardless of age of onset, are often treated punitively and incapacitated for fear that they will be a persistent danger to the public. In contrast, juvenile courts operate under a philosophy of care and rehabilitation. Our findings appear to support evidence-based recommendations for more lenient treatment of official adult-onset offenders (Thompson et al., 2014b) and the notion that such treatment may be cost-effective in light of low rates of offending (Thompson et al., 2014a). For those adult-onset offenders whose criminal behavior occurs against a backdrop of substance abuse problems and/or a mental illness, court-mandated social and/or medical intervention may be helpful. Experimental criminology should test which results in more effective crime control: raising the age at which offenders can be tried as adults or extending leniency to adults with a first-time conviction.

With respect to theory, explanations of offending seemed to apply equally to official adult-onset and official juvenile-onset offenders. Eggleston and Laub (2002) drew similar conclusions when analyzing common correlates of crime among adult offenders. The important theoretical insight from our research is that the two sets of theories of adult-onset offending, developmental theories doubting de novo adult-onset offending and situational theories allowing adult-onset offending, are not in competition with one another, but complementary to one another vis a vis the adult-onset offender. As anticipated by theories in the first set, which emphasize early-life influences in the origins of offending, official adult-onset offenders, much like official juvenile-onset offenders, had childhood-onset antisocial behavior, a below-average IQ, and low socioeconomic status families. Relative to official juvenile-offenders, during adolescence they had fewer delinquent peers and were more socially inhibited, which may have protected them from conviction. As anticipated by theories emphasizing the importance of situational influences on offending, official adult-onset offenders, relative to official non-offenders, during adulthood more often had schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, and alcohol-dependence, had weaker social bonds, anticipated fewer informal sanctions, and self-reported more offenses. Conviction for certain types of crime appears more likely among adult-onset offenders, but not to the extent that it indicates unique causes of their criminality. Rather, the pattern indicates less-serious criminality among adult-onset offenders. By testing multiple theories of adult-onset offending in the same cohort, we were able to draw a coherent picture of the typical official adult-onset offender which revealed that he is not a unique entity. The typical official adult-onset offender appears to be a “light” version of the typical official juvenile-onset offender, perhaps even representing the “low-level chronic” offender found in many trajectory studies (Piquero, 2008). Official adult-onset offending, thus, appears to have the same causes as official juvenile-onset offending.

Turning briefly to women, our results showed that official adult-onset offending may be more prevalent among convicted women than among convicted men. This finding is consistent with some studies that find 50% or more of convicted women were first convicted as an adult (Kratzer & Hodgins, 1999; Simpson, Yahner, & Dugan, 2008; Stattin & Magnusson, 1996). Additionally, the official social-adulthood-onset group was proportionally larger among women than among men. However, there were too few official female offenders in our cohort to permit more detailed analyses of these groups.

Future research on adult-onset offending should take note of the importance of performing longitudinal repeated searches of criminal convictions. In the Dunedin Study we repeatedly searched computerized police records at ages 21, 26, 32, and 38. We found that a small number of convictions (fewer than 10) in the record at younger ages did not appear in later searches. For example, certain juvenile sex crimes and a number of other juvenile convictions appeared to have been removed from the record. Fortunately, these convictions still appear in our cumulative dataset, although they have disappeared from the official record. It is possible that some families, probably those with greater resources, could bring suit to expunge a juvenile offender’s record. Studies investigating adult-onset offending may report a higher prevalence of adult-onset cases if only one mid-life record search is conducted.

There are limitations to our study. First, our description of and conclusions about adult-onset offenders are limited to one country and one time period. At the time that the Dunedin cohort was transitioning to adulthood (1991–1993), New Zealand’s unemployment rate among 15–24 year-olds climbed above 19%, a record high (World Bank, International Labour Organization, 2016). In this way, the experiences of the Dunedin cohort are similar to those of young people transitioning to adulthood in North America, the United Kingdom, and other parts of Europe after 2009 (World Bank, International Labour Organization, 2016). Nonetheless, findings about adult-onset offending may be period- and jurisdiction-specific because of local variation in the manner in which juvenile offenders are treated. As policies shift towards diversion for most juvenile offenders and conviction is reserved for only the most severe juvenile cases, the proportion of official adult-onset offenders is likely to increase as some of the diverted juveniles re-offend as adults. It is also possible that adult-onset offending would appear more diverse and become more frequent if juvenile-justice policies became more lenient. Studies that compare official adult-onset offenders in settings with lenient versus punitive juvenile justice policies would be highly informative. Second, we were clearly limited in our ability to explore adult-onset offending among women. Larger population-based samples and offender-based samples will be needed to study the prevalence and correlates of adult-onset crime among women. Finally, the Dunedin Study is primarily comprised of European-descent whites and the results may not apply to other ethnicities or ethnic minorities.

In conclusion, the age at official onset of crime appears to be inversely related to the severity of continued antisocial behavior, with official adult-onset indicating a less-severe level of antisocial behavior. It is, thus, not surprising that official adult-onset offenders appear to have a relatively weak social and justice-system impact. Life-course theories of crime already aim to explain onset of and persistence in antisocial behavior and appear quite helpful for explaining adult-onset offending. Official adult-onset offenders appear to be poor candidates for harsh sanctions from the criminal justice system and unlikely to warrant a tailored theory of offending.

Highlights.

  • Official adult-onset offenders had displayed antisocial behavior since childhood.

  • These offenders lacked delinquent peers and were socially inhibited.

  • They had more mental illness, alcoholism, weak social bonds, weak informal sanctions

  • Official adult-onset offenders lacked high IQ and high socioeconomic-status.

  • They fell on a continuum between non-offenders and juvenile-onset offenders

Acknowledgments

We thank the Dunedin Study members, their parents and teachers, Research Unit research staff, Unit administration officer Michelle McCann, and Study founder Phil Silva. For official crime data we thank the Dunedin Police and the special efforts of Joanne Cullen. The Dunedin Multidisciplinary Health and Development Research Unit is supported by the New Zealand Health Research Council and New Zealand Ministry of Business, Innovation and Employment (MBIE). ALB is supported by NICHD grant HD077482 and the FORTE Marie Curie International Fellowship Programme. This research received support from US-National Institute of Aging grant R01AG032282 and R01AG048895, UK Medical Research Council grant MR/K00381X, ESRC grant ES/M010309/1 and the Jacobs Foundation. The study protocol was approved by the institutional ethical review boards of the participating universities. Study members gave informed consent before participating.

Appendix 1

Conviction type and crimes by category and frequency for men and women of the Dunedin Study.

Conviction type Males Females


% n % n
Property & fraud crimes 40.5% 778 45.8% 184
Driving under the influence & criminal driving violations 22.3% 429 20.9% 84
Violent & weapon crimes 12.6% 242 6.5% 26
Criminal justice system violations 8.8% 169 6.7% 27
Drug-related crimes 8.6% 165 13.9% 56
Odd crimes 2.6% 50 2.7% 11
Crimes against women aged 12 and over 2.3% 45 0% 0
Status offenses 2.3% 44 3.5% 14
All 100% 1922 100% 402
Type of crime n %
Property & fraud crimes
Total property & fraud crimes 962 100.00%
Burglary 154 16.01%
Unlawfully take motor vehicle 128 13.31%
Willful damage 96 9.98%
Use document 85 8.84%
Shoplifting 80 8.32%
False pretenses < $500 bad checks 77 8.00%
Theft under $500 71 7.38%
Receive stolen property 51 5.30%
Theft out of car 42 4.37%
Theft as servant/employee 39 4.05%
Trespassing 34 3.53%
Supply false evidence 33 3.43%
Theft >$500 24 2.49%
Forgery 9 0.94%
Obtain document 6 0.62%
Breaking and entering 5 0.52%
False pretenses > $500 bad checks 4 0.42%
Theft from person 4 0.42%
Possess instruments for odometer
conversion
3 0.31%
Theft by person with special relationship 3 0.31%
Bicycle theft 2 0.21%
Credit by fraud 2 0.21%
Theft of an animal 2 0.21%
Arson 1 0.10%
Fabricating evidence 1 0.10%
Fraud 1 0.10%
Intent to defraud using document 1 0.10%
Obtain by deception 1 0.10%
Possess instruments for burglary 1 0.10%
Inaccurate distance recorder (owner) 1 0.10%
Willfully set fire to building 1 0.10%
Driving under the influence & criminal driving violations
Total driving under the influence &
criminal driving violations
513 100.00%
Excess blood alcohol driver 178 34.70%
Careless/dangerous driving 132 25.73%
Drive when disqualified 91 17.74%
Drive w/o license 23 4.48%
Unlawful interference, motor vehicle 18 3.51%
Unlicensed driver with blood alcohol 15 2.92%
Careless driving, cause injury/death 11 2.14%
Speeding work zone, dangerous speed 10 1.95%
Failure to stop for red/blue flashing lights 8 1.56%
Hit and run 8 1.56%
Drive while impaired - controlled drug 3 0.58%
Excess blood alcohol driver, causing injury 3 0.58%
No registration/no inspection sticker 3 0.58%
Speed camera/ exceed posted speed 3 0.58%
Failure to report damaged vehicle/proper 1 0.19%
Failure to wear seat belt 1 0.19%
Inconsiderate driving 1 0.19%
Learner driver unaccompanied 1 0.19%
Unauthorized drag race 1 0.19%
Unlicensed motor vehicle 1 0.19%
Wrong class of license label 1 0.19%
Violent & weapon crimes
Total violent & weapon crimes 271 100.00%
Common assault manually 69 25.46%
Possess offensive weapon, not gun 22 8.12%
Disorderly behavior likely to cause violence 21 7.75%
Assault with weapon 20 7.38%
Obscene or insulting language/behavior 19 7.01%
Assault police officer 14 5.17%
Protection order issued 14 5.17%
Assault with intent to injure 8 2.95%
Fighting in public place 8 2.95%
Inciting violence 8 2.95%
Possess firearm, no license 7 2.58%
Threaten to kill/great bodily harm (verbal) 7 2.58%
Speak threateningly 6 2.21%
Assault person, show intent to use weapon 5 1.85%
Injure/wound 4 1.48%
Possess concealed firearm 4 1.48%
Present a firearm 4 1.48%
Intimidate 3 1.11%
Robbery, aggravated manually 3 1.11%
Aggravated robbery (firearm) 2 0.74%
Aggravated robbery with cutting/stabbing
instrument
2 0.74%
Assault child 2 0.74%
Indecent act with male under age 12 2 0.74%
Indecent assault with female under age 12 2 0.74%
Injury leading to death 2 0.74%
Other threat act 2 0.74%
Threaten to kill/great bodily harm with
firearm or weapon
2 0.74%
Aggravated cruelty to animal 1 0.37%
Behave threateningly - manual 1 0.37%
Behave threateningly (other weapon) 1 0.37%
Criminal harassment 1 0.37%
Indecent act, intent to insult 1 0.37%
Intoxicated in charge of firearm 1 0.37%
Kidnapping 1 0.37%
Manslaughter 1 0.37%
Wound, reckless disregard 1 0.37%
Criminal justice system violations
Total criminal justice system violations 196 100.00%
Failure to answer bailiff summons 36 18.37%
Breach community service 26 13.27%
Resisting police 21 10.71%
Breach of periodic detention 20 10.20%
Obstruct/hinder police 17 8.67%
Failure to report for periodic detention 16 8.16%
Breach of supervision 14 7.14%
Failed to comply 12 6.12%
Contravene protection order - no firearm 11 5.61%
Breach of parole 7 3.57%
Escape custody 4 2.04%
Misleading social welfare officer 4 2.04%
Refuse to give blood sample 2 1.02%
Breach non-molestation order 1 0.51%
Breach of home detention conditions 1 0.51%
Contravene protection order - firearm 1 0.51%
Escape prison 1 0.51%
Fail to notify of change of address 1 0.51%
Left worksite (i.e. Huber) 1 0.51%
Drug-related crimes
Total drug crimes 221 100.00%
Cannabis possession 91 41.18%
Sell cannabis/cultivate 74 33.48%
Possess any drug paraphernalia 23 10.41%
Procure/possess other (hard) drugs/ use 15 6.79%
Supply hard drugs 9 4.07%
Possess prescription medicine 7 3.17%
Misuse of drugs act 1 0.45%
Produces class B drug (i.e. Meth lab) 1 0.45%
Odd crimes
Total odd crimes 61 100.00%
Offensive behavior 38 62.30%
Breach local liquor ban 3 4.92%
Unregistered dog 3 4.92%
Allow premises to be used 2 3.28%
Breach hire purchase act 2 3.28%
Peeping and peering 2 3.28%
Purchase liquor for minor 2 3.28%
Dangerous litter in public place 1 1.64%
Dog attacked person 1 1.64%
Drinking in public place 1 1.64%
Endangering transport 1 1.64%
False fire alarm 1 1.64%
Loitering near dwelling house 1 1.64%
Prepare to commit crime in public place 1 1.64%
Prostitute soliciting 1 1.64%
Unlawful assembly to disturb the peace 1 1.64%
Crimes against women
Total crimes against women 45 100.00%
Male assaults female, manually 29 64.44%
Common assault domestic (manually) 7 15.56%
Indecently assault female over 16 4 8.89%
Rape, no weapon 3 6.67%
Male assaults female (other weapon) 1 2.22%
Indecently assault female aged 12 to 16 1 2.22%
Status offenses
Total status offenses 58 100.00%
Minor in public place possess/drink liquor 17 29.31%
Minor in bar 15 25.86%
Minor in restricted area 13 22.41%
Minor purchase liquor 13 22.41%

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

References

  1. Andersson F, Levander S, Svensson R, Levander MT. Sex differences in offending trajectories in a Swedish cohort. Criminal Behaviour and Mental Health. 2012;22(2):108–121. doi: 10.1002/cbm.1822. http://doi.org/10.1002/cbm.1822. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Arnett JJ. Emerging adulthood: A theory of development from the late teens through the twenties. American Psychologist. 2000;55(5):469–480. http://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.55.5.469. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Arseneault, Moffitt TE, Caspi A, Taylor PJ, Silva PA. Mental disorders and violence in a total birth cohort: Results from the dunedin study. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2000;57(10):979–986. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.57.10.979. http://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.57.10.979. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bellair PE, McNulty TL, Piquero AR. Verbal Ability and Persistent Offending: A Race-specific Test of Moffitt’s Theory. Justice Quarterly. 2014:1–26. doi: 10.1080/07418825.2014.918166. http://doi.org/10.1080/07418825.2014.918166. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Blumstein A, Cohen J. Characterizing criminal careers. Science. 1987;237:985–991. doi: 10.1126/science.237.4818.985. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Broidy LM, Stewart AL, Thompson CM, Chrzanowski A, Allard T, Dennison SM. Life Course Offending Pathways Across Gender and Race/Ethnicity. Journal of Developmental and Life-Course Criminology. 2015;1(2):118–149. http://doi.org/10.1007/s40865-015-0008-z. [Google Scholar]
  7. Carrington PJ, Matarazzo A, deSouza P. Court careers of a Canadian birth cohort. Ontario, Canada: Canadian Center for Justice Statistics; 2005. (Research Paper No. 85-651-MIE2005006). Retrieved from http://www.publications.gc.ca/Collection/Statcan/85-561-MIE/85-561-MIE2005006.pdf. [Google Scholar]
  8. Carr NT, Hanks RS. If “60 is the New 40,” is 35 the New 15? Late Onset Crime and Delinquency. Deviant Behavior. 2012;33(5):393–411. http://doi.org/10.1080/01639625.2011.636660. [Google Scholar]
  9. Catalano RF, Hawkins JD. The social development model: A theory of antisocial behavior. In: Hawkins JD, editor. Delinquency and Crime: Current Theories. New York: Cambridge University Press; 1996. pp. 149–197. [Google Scholar]
  10. Catalano RF, Park J, Harachi TW, Haggerty KP, Abbott RD, Hawkins JD. Mediating the effects of poverty, gender, indivdiual characteristics, and external constraints on antisocial behavior: A test of the social development model and implications for developmental life-course theory. In: Farrington DP, editor. Integrated Developmental and Life-Course Theories of Offending. 1st. Vol. 14. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Publishers; 2005. pp. 93–124. [Google Scholar]
  11. Chambliss WJ. The saints and the roughnecks. Society. 1973;11(1):24–31. http://doi.org/10.1007/BF03181016. [Google Scholar]
  12. Chung I-J, Hill KG, Hawkins JD, Gilchrist LD, Nagin DS. Childhood Predictors of Offense Trajectories. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency. 2002;39(1):60–90. http://doi.org/10.1177/002242780203900103. [Google Scholar]
  13. DeLisi M. Zeroing in on early arrest onset: Results from a population of extreme career criminals. Journal of Criminal Justice. 2006;34(1):17–26. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcrimjus.2005.11.002. [Google Scholar]
  14. DeLisi M, Piquero AR. New frontiers in criminal careers research: 2000–2011: A state-of-the-art review. Journal of Criminal Justice. 2011;39(4):289–301. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcrimjus.2011.05.001. [Google Scholar]
  15. Discussing the Future of Justice-Involved Young Adults. Presented at the A Panel to Discuss Developmentally-Appropriate Responses to Justice-Involved Young Adults, Department of Justice. 2015 Sep; Retrieved from http://www.nij.gov/multimedia/Pages/video-future-of-justice-involved-young-adults-transcript.aspx.
  16. Eggleston EP, Laub JH. The onset of adult offending: A neglected dimension of the criminal career. Journal of Criminal Justice. 2002;30(6):603–622. http://doi.org/10.1016/S0047-2352(02)00193-9. [Google Scholar]
  17. Elander J, Rutter M, Simonoff E, Pickles A. Explanations for Apparent Late Onset Criminality in a High-Risk Sample of Children Followed up in Adult Life. British Journal of Criminology. 2000;40(3):497–509. http://doi.org/10.1093/bjc/40.3.497. [Google Scholar]
  18. Elley WB, Irving JC. Revised socioeconomic index for New-Zealand. New Zealand Journal of Educational Studies. 1976;11(1):1125–1136. [Google Scholar]
  19. Erickson ML. Group Violations and Official Delinquency - The Group Hazard Hypothesis. Criminology. 1973;11:127. [Google Scholar]
  20. Farrington DP. Offending from 10 to 25 years of age. In: Van Dusen KT, Mednick SA, editors. Prospective studies of crime and delinquency. Boston: Kluwar-Nijhoff Publishing; 1983. pp. 17–37. [Google Scholar]
  21. Farrington DP. Self-Reported and Official Offending from Adolescence to Adulthood. In: Klein MW, editor. Cross-National Research in Self-Reported Crime and Delinquency. Springer Netherlands: 1989. pp. 399–423. Retrieved from http://link.springer.com/chapter/10.1007/978-94-009-1001-0_18. [Google Scholar]
  22. Farrington DP. Building developmental and life-course theories of offending. In: Cullen FT, Wright JP, Blevins KR, editors. Taking Stock: The Status of Criminological Theory. Vol. 15. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction; 2006. pp. 335–364. [Google Scholar]
  23. Farrington DP. Introduction to integrated developmental and life-course theories of offending. In: Farrington DP, editor. Integrated Developmental and Life-Course Theories of Offending. 2nd. Vol. 14. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Publishers; 2011. pp. 1–14. [Google Scholar]
  24. Farrington DP. Integrated Cognitive Antisocial Potential Theory. In: Bruinsma G, Weisburd D, editors. Encyclopedia of Criminology and Criminal Justice. New York: Springer; 2014. pp. 2552–2564. Retrieved from http://link.springer.com/referenceworkentry/10.1007/978-1-4614-5690-2_500. [Google Scholar]
  25. Farrington DP, Jolliffe D, Loeber R, Homish DL. How Many Offenses are Really Committed per Juvenile Court Offender? Victims & Offenders. 2007;2(3):227–249. http://doi.org/10.1080/15564880701403934. [Google Scholar]
  26. Fazel S, Långström N, Hjern A, Grann M, Lichtenstein P. Schizophrenia, substance abuse, and violent crime. JAMA. 2009;301(19):2016–2023. doi: 10.1001/jama.2009.675. http://doi.org/10.1001/jama.2009.675. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Fazel S, Lichtenstein P, Grann M, Goodwin GM, Långström N. Bipolar disorder and violent crime: New evidence from population-based longitudinal studies and systematic review. Archives of General Psychiatry. 2010;67(9):931–938. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.97. http://doi.org/10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.97. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Giordano PC, Schroeder RD, Cernkovich SA. Emotions and Crime over the Life Course: A Neo-Meadian Perspective on Criminal Continuity and Change. American Journal of Sociology. 2007;112(6):1603–1661. http://doi.org/10.1086/512710. [Google Scholar]
  29. Glueck S, Glueck E. Delinquents and nondelinquents in perspective. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 1968. [Google Scholar]
  30. Gomez-Smith Z, Piquero AR. An examination of adult onset offending. Journal of Criminal Justice. 2005;33(6):515–525. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcrimjus.2005.08.001. [Google Scholar]
  31. Gottfredson MR, Hirschi T. A general theory of crime. xvi. Stanford University Press: Stanford University Press; 1990. [Google Scholar]
  32. Gunnison E, McCartan LM. Persistent versus late onset among female offenders: A test of state dependent and population heterogeneity interpretations. Western Criminology Review. 2010;11(3):45–62. [Google Scholar]
  33. Harris PM. The First-Time Adult-Onset Offender Findings From a Community Corrections Cohort. International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology. 2011;55(6):949–981. doi: 10.1177/0306624X10372110. http://doi.org/10.1177/0306624X10372110. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Janson C-G. Delinquency among metropolitan boys: A progress report. In: Van Dusen KT, Mednick SA, editors. Prospective studies of crime and delinquency. Boston: Kluwar-Nijhoff Publishing; 1983. pp. 147–180. [Google Scholar]
  35. Kazemian L, Farrington DP. Comparing the Validity of Prospective, Retrospective, and Official Onset for Different Offending Categories: Journal of Quantitative Criminology. 2005;21(2):127–147. http://doi.org/10.1007/s10940-005-2489-0. [Google Scholar]
  36. Kivivuori J, Linderborg H. Negative life events and self-control as correlates of self-assessed re-offending probability among Finnish prisoners. European Journal of Criminology. 2010;7(2):123–139. http://doi.org/10.1177/1477370809354139. [Google Scholar]
  37. Kratzer L, Hodgins S. A typology of offenders: a test of Moffitt’s theory among males and females from childhood to age 30. Criminal Behaviour and Mental Health. 1999;9(1):57–73. http://doi.org/10.1002/cbm.291. [Google Scholar]
  38. Krohn MD, Gibson CL, Thornberry TP. Under the Protective Bud the Bloom Awaits: A Review of Theory and Research on Adult-Onset and Late-Blooming Offenders. In: Gibson CL, Krohn MD, editors. Handbook of Life-Course Criminology. New York: Springer; 2013. pp. 183–200. Retrieved from http://link.springer.com/chapter/10.1007/978-1-4614-5113-6_11. [Google Scholar]
  39. Langan PA, Farrington DP. Two-Track or One-Track Justice? Some Evidence from an English Longitudinal Survey. The Journal of Criminal Law and Criminology (1973-) 1983;74(2):519–546. http://doi.org/10.2307/1143086. [Google Scholar]
  40. Laub JH, Sampson RJ. Shared Beginnings, Divergent Lives: Delinquent Boys to Age 70. Harvard University Press; 2003. [Google Scholar]
  41. Laub JH, Sampson RJ. A general age-graded theory of crime: Lessons learned and the future of life-course criminology. In: Farrington DP, editor. Integrated Developmental and Life-Course Theories of Offending. 2nd. Vol. 14. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Publishers; 2011. pp. 165–182. [Google Scholar]
  42. Lay B, Ihle W, Esser G, Schmidt MH. Juvenile-episodic, Continued or Adult-onset Delinquency? Risk Conditions Analysed in a Cohort of Children Followed up to the Age of 25 Years. European Journal of Criminology. 2005;2(1):39–66. http://doi.org/10.1177/1477370805048629. [Google Scholar]
  43. LeBlanc M. A generic control theory of the criminal phenomenon, the structural and the dynamical statements of an integrative multilayered control theory. In: Thornberry TP, editor. Developmental Theories of Crime and Delinquency. Vol. 7. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction; 1997. pp. 215–286. [Google Scholar]
  44. LeBlanc M, Frechette M. Male criminal activity from childhood through youth: Multilevel and developmental perspectives. New York: Springer-Verlag; 1989. [Google Scholar]
  45. Lussier P, Tzoumakis S, Cale J, Amirault J. Criminal Trajectories of Adult Sex Offenders and the Age Effect: Examining the Dynamic Aspect of Offending in Adulthood. International Criminal Justice Review. 2010;20(2):147–168. http://doi.org/10.1177/1057567710368360. [Google Scholar]
  46. Magnusson D. Individual development from an interactional perspective: A longitudinal study. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1988. [Google Scholar]
  47. Massoglia M. Desistance or Displacement? The Changing Patterns of Offending from Adolescence to Young Adulthood. Journal of Quantitative Criminology. 2006;22(3):215–239. http://doi.org/10.1007/s10940-006-9009-8. [Google Scholar]
  48. Mata AD, van Dulmen MHM. Adult-Onset Antisocial Behavior Trajectories Associations With Adolescent Family Processes and Emerging Adulthood Functioning. Journal of Interpersonal Violence. 2012;27(1):177–193. doi: 10.1177/0886260511416467. http://doi.org/10.1177/0886260511416467. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. McCord J. A thirty-year follow-up of treatment effects. American Psychologist. 1978;33(3):284–289. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.33.3.284. http://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.33.3.284. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. McGee TR, Farrington DP. Are There Any True Adult-Onset Offenders? British Journal of Criminology. 2010;50(3):530–549. http://doi.org/10.1093/bjc/azq008. [Google Scholar]
  51. Meier MH, Caspi A, Houts R, Slutske WS, Harrington H, Jackson KM, Moffitt TE. Prospective developmental subtypes of alcohol dependence from age 18 to 32 years: Implications for nosology, etiology, and intervention. Development and Psychopathology. 2013;25(03):785–800. doi: 10.1017/S0954579413000175. http://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579413000175. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Moffitt TE. Adolescence-limited and life-course-persistent antisocial behavior: A developmental taxonomy. Psychological Review. 1993;100(4):674–701. http://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.100.4.674. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Moffitt TE, Caspi A, Rutter M, Silva PA. Sex Differences in Antisocial Behaviour: Conduct Disorder, Delinquency, and Violence in the Dunedin Longitudinal Study. Cambridge University Press; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  54. Mosher CJ, Hart TC, Miethe TD. The Mismeasure of Crime. 2nd. SAGE; 2011. [Google Scholar]
  55. Mumola CJ, Karberg JC. Drug Use and Dependence, State and Federal Prisoners, 2004. Bureau of Justice Statistics; 2006. (Special Report No. NCJ 213530). [Google Scholar]
  56. National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism. Alcohol research: A lifespan perspective. Rockville, MD: 2008. (No. Alcohol Alert No. 74). [Google Scholar]
  57. Nilsson A, Bäckman O, Estrada F. Involvement in crime, individual resources and structural constraints processes of cumulative (dis)advantage in a Stockholm birth cohort. British Journal of Criminology. 2013;53(2):297–318. http://doi.org/10.1093/bjc/azs068. [Google Scholar]
  58. Osgood DW, Foster EM, Courtney ME. Vulnerable Populations and the Transition to Adulthood. The Future of Children. 2010;20(1):209–229. doi: 10.1353/foc.0.0047. http://doi.org/10.1353/foc.0.0047. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Owens JG, Slocum LA. Abstainers in Adolescence and Adulthood Exploring the Correlates of Abstention Using Moffitt’s Developmental Taxonomy. Crime & Delinquency. 2015;61(5):690–718. http://doi.org/10.1177/0011128711427234. [Google Scholar]
  60. Piquero AR. Taking Stock of Developmental Trajectories of Criminal Activity over the Life Course. In: Liberman AM, editor. The Long View of Crime: A Synthesis of Longitudinal Research. New York: Springer; 2008. pp. 23–78. Retrieved from http://link.springer.com/chapter/10.1007/978-0-387-71165-2_2. [Google Scholar]
  61. Piquero AR, Oster RP, Mazerolle P, Brame R, Dean CW. Onset Age and Offense Specialization. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency. 1999;36(3):275–299. http://doi.org/10.1177/0022427899036003002. [Google Scholar]
  62. Polk K, Adler CM, Bazemore G, Blake G, Cordray S, Coventry G, Temple M. Becoming adult: an analysis of maturational development from age 16 to 30 of a cohort of young men. Eugene: University of Oregon; 1981. (Final report of the Marion County Youth Study) [Google Scholar]
  63. Poulton R, Moffitt TE, Silva PA. The Dunedin Multidisciplinary Health and Development Study: overview of the first 40 years, with an eye to the future. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology. 2015;50(5):679–693. doi: 10.1007/s00127-015-1048-8. http://doi.org/10.1007/s00127-015-1048-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Pulkkinen L, Lyyra A-L, Kokko K. Life success of males on nonoffender, adolescence-limited, persistent, and adult-onset antisocial pathways: follow-up from age 8 to 42. Aggressive Behavior. 2009;35(2):117–135. doi: 10.1002/ab.20297. http://doi.org/10.1002/ab.20297. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Robins LN. Deviant children grown up. A Sociological and psychiatric study of sociopathic personality. Baltimore: Williams and Wilkins; 1966. [Google Scholar]
  66. Sampson RJ, Laub JH. Crime in the making: Pathways and turning points through life. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 1993. [Google Scholar]
  67. Shannon LW. Criminal career continuity: Its social context. New York: Human Sciences Press; 1988. [Google Scholar]
  68. Shannon LW. Alcohol and drugs, delinquency and crime: Looking back to the future. New York: St. Martin’s Press; 1998. [Google Scholar]
  69. Simpson SS, Yahner JL, Dugan L. Understanding Women’s Pathways to Jail: Analysing the Lives of Incarcerated Women. Australian & New Zealand Journal of Criminology. 2008;41(1):84–108. http://doi.org/10.1375/acri.41.1.84. [Google Scholar]
  70. Skardhamar T. Lifetime conviction risk—a synthetic cohort approach. Journal of Scandinavian Studies in Criminology and Crime Prevention. 2014;15(1):96–101. http://doi.org/10.1080/14043858.2014.883175. [Google Scholar]
  71. Sohoni T, Paternoster R, McGloin JM, Bachman R. “Hen”s teeth and horse’s toes’: the adult onset offender in criminology. Journal of Crime and Justice. 2014;37(2):155–172. http://doi.org/10.1080/0735648X.2012.759884. [Google Scholar]
  72. Stattin H, Magnusson D. Antisocial development: A holistic approach. Development and Psychopathology. 1996;8(04):617–645. http://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579400007331. [Google Scholar]
  73. Stattin H, Magnusson D, Reichel H. CRIMINAL ACTIVITY AT DIFFERENT AGES A Study Based on a Swedish Longitudinal Research Population. British Journal of Criminology. 1989;29(4):368–385. [Google Scholar]
  74. Steffensmeier DJ, Allan EA, Harer MD, Streifel C. Age and the Distribution of Crime. American Journal of Sociology. 1989;94(4):803–831. [Google Scholar]
  75. Theobald D, Farrington DP. Why do the Crime-reducing Effects of Marriage Vary with Age? British Journal of Criminology. 2011;51(1):136–158. http://doi.org/10.1093/bjc/azq060. [Google Scholar]
  76. Theobald D, Farrington DP. Onset of Offending. In: Bruinsma G, Weisburd D, editors. Encyclopedia of Criminology and Criminal Justice. New York: Springer; 2014. pp. 3332–3342. Retrieved from http://link.springer.com/referenceworkentry/10.1007/978-1-4614-5690-2_86. [Google Scholar]
  77. Thompson CM, Stewart A, Allard T, Chrzanowski A, Luker C, Sveticic J. Examining adult-onset offending: A case for adult cautioning. [Retrieved July 1, 2015];2014a Oct; from http://search.informit.com.au/documentSummary;dn=702770960580137;res=IELHSS. [Google Scholar]
  78. Thompson CM, Stewart AL, Allard TJ, Chrzanowski A, Luker C, Sveticic J. Understanding the extent and nature of adult-onset offending: Implications for the effective and efficient use of criminal justice and crime reduction resources. (No. CRG 09/11-12). Criminology Research Grants. 2014b Retrieved from http://www.criminologyresearchcouncil.gov.au/reports/1415/09-1112-FinalReport.pdf.
  79. Thornberry TP. Toward an interactional theory of delinquency. Criminology. 1987;25(4):863–892. http://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-9125.1987.tb00823.x. [Google Scholar]
  80. Thornberry TP. Explaining Multiple Patterns of Offending across the Life Course and across Generations. The ANNALS of the American Academy of Political and Social Science. 2005;602(1):156–195. http://doi.org/10.1177/0002716205280641. [Google Scholar]
  81. Thornberry TP, Krohn MD. Peers, druge use, and delinquency. In: Stoff DM, Breiling J, Maser JD, editors. Handbook of Antisocial Behavior. New York: John Wiley & Sons; 1997. pp. 218–233. [Google Scholar]
  82. Thornberry TP, Krohn MD. Integrated Developmental and Life-Course Theories of Offending. 2nd. Vol. 14. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Publishers; 2011. Applying interactional theory to the explanation of continuity and change in antisocial behavior; pp. 183–210. [Google Scholar]
  83. Tillyer MS, Tillyer R. Maybe I Should Do This Alone: A Comparison of Solo and Co-offending Robbery Outcomes. Justice Quarterly. 2014;0(0):1–25. http://doi.org/10.1080/07418825.2014.883422. [Google Scholar]
  84. Tracy PE, Kempf-Leonard K. Continuity and discontinuity in criminal careers. New York: Plenum; 1996. [Google Scholar]
  85. van der Geest V, Blokland A, Bijleveld C. Delinquent Development in a Sample of High-Risk Youth Shape, Content, and Predictors of Delinquent Trajectories from Age 12 to 32. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency. 2009;46(2):111–143. http://doi.org/10.1177/0022427808331115. [Google Scholar]
  86. Vitaro F, Tremblay RE, Bukowski WM. Conduct Disorders in Childhood and Adolescence. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2000. Friends, friendships and conduct disorders; pp. 346–378. [Google Scholar]
  87. Wagner FA, Anthony JC. From First Drug Use to Drug Dependence: Developmental Periods of Risk for Dependence upon Marijuana, Cocaine, and Alcohol. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2002;26(4):479–488. doi: 10.1016/S0893-133X(01)00367-0. http://doi.org/10.1016/S0893-133X(01)00367-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Wechsler D. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised. New York: Psychological Corporation; 1974. [Google Scholar]
  89. Werner E, Smith RS. Overcoming the odds: High risk children from birth to adulthood. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press; 1992. [Google Scholar]
  90. What Age Should Young Criminals Be Tried as Adults? [Retrieved February 24, 2016];2015 Dec 14; from http://www.nytimes.com/roomfordebate/2015/12/14/what-age-should-young-criminals-be-tried-as-adults. [Google Scholar]
  91. Wikström P. Crime as alternative: Towards a cross-level situational action theory of crime causation. In: McCord J, editor. Beyond Empiricism: Institutions and Intentions in the Study of Crime. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction; 2004. pp. 1–38. [Google Scholar]
  92. Wikström P. Integrated Developmental and Life-Course Theories of Offending. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction; 2005. The social origins of pathways in crime: Towards a developmental ecological action theory of crime involvement and its changes; pp. 211–245. [Google Scholar]
  93. Wolfgang ME, Thornberry TP, Figlio RM. From boy to man, from delinquency to crime. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 1987. [Google Scholar]
  94. World Bank, International Labour Organization. Unemployment, youth total (% of total labor force ages 15–24) (modeled ILO estimate) 2016 Retrieved from http://data.worldbank.org/indicator/SL.UEM.1524.ZS.
  95. Zara G, Farrington DP. Childhood and Adolescent Predictors of Late Onset Criminal Careers. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 2009;38(3):287–300. doi: 10.1007/s10964-008-9350-3. http://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-008-9350-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Zara G, Farrington DP. A longitudinal analysis of early risk factors for adult-onset offending: What predicts a delayed criminal career? Criminal Behaviour and Mental Health. 2010;20(4):257–273. doi: 10.1002/cbm.763. http://doi.org/10.1002/cbm.763. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Zara G, Farrington DP. Assessment of risk for juvenile compared with adult criminal onset implications for policy, prevention, and intervention. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law. 2013;19(2):235–249. http://doi.org/10.1037/a0029050. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES