Abstract
Neuronal pattern separation is thought to enable the brain to disambiguate sensory stimuli with overlapping features thereby extracting valuable information. In the olfactory system, it remains unknown whether pattern separation acts as a driving force for sensory discrimination and the learning thereof. Here we show that overlapping odor-evoked input patterns to the mouse olfactory bulb (OB) are dynamically reformatted in the network at the timescale of a single breath, giving rise to separated patterns of activity in ensemble of output neurons (mitral/tufted cells; M/T). Strikingly, the extent of pattern separation in M/T assemblies predicts behavioral discrimination performance during the learning phase. Furthermore, exciting or inhibiting GABAergic OB interneurons, using optogenetics or pharmacogenetics, altered pattern separation and thereby odor discrimination learning in a bidirectional way. In conclusion, we propose that the OB network can act as a pattern separator facilitating olfactory stimuli distinction, a process that is sculpted by synaptic inhibition.
Keywords: Population coding, in-vivo extracellular recording, head-restrained behavior, optogenetics, odorant mixture
Introduction
The optimal disambiguation of overlapping sensory stimuli by neuronal networks is an essential process to build internal representations of the external world. Along sensory pathways, precortical1─5 and cortical5─8 networks have been proposed to drive this disambiguation through pattern separation, also known as pattern decorrelation9,10. This process is thought to increase the formation of discrete representations and is considered to be useful for discrimination and memory storage9─11. Despite these observations, the timescales and the behavioral relevance of pattern separation remain elusive.
In the rodent OB, odorants evoke discrete spatiotemporal patterns of activated glomeruli in awake mice12,13. The spatial segregation helps separating channels of information and thereby increasing contrast between odor-evoked activities. However, similar stimuli such as binary mixtures evoke overlapping representations of activated glomeruli14─17. Yet, these odorant stimuli are still discriminable by animals14,18─20, indicating that overlapping sensory information is further refined along the sensory pathway as observed in the piriform cortex (PC)5,21,22. In the zebrafish OB, this refinement, namely pattern separation, acts to reduce overlapping odorant-evoked activity patterns in the population of output neurons (mitral/tufted cells, M/T)1,2,23. But the existence of a similar process in the mammalian olfactory system has not been clearly established. More importantly, the behavioral relevance of pattern separation is still debated24 and thus far no studies have addressed this question. Therefore it remains unknown whether pattern separation is a driving force for sensory discrimination and the learning thereof.
Here we show, using functional imaging, tetrode recordings, optogenetic and pharmacogenetic manipulations in awake mice, that pattern separation in the OB plays an important role in olfactory discrimination learning. Similar odorant stimuli evoked overlapping input patterns of olfactory sensory neuron (OSN) axons that were dynamically reformatted in ensembles of OB output neurons (mitral/tufted cells; M/T) at the timescale of a single sniff, helping the separation of such overlapping inputs. Strikingly, the extent of pattern separation in the OB predicted the mice’ ability to discriminate olfactory stimuli during the early learning phase. To further investigate the causality between pattern separation and discrimination performance, odor-evoked M/T cells responses were modulated by enhancing or suppressing inhibitory interneuron activity in the granule cell layer (GCL) using channelrhodopsin (ChR2) photostimulation or a designer receptor exclusively activated by designer drugs (hM4Di Gi-DREADD), respectively. Enhancing inhibition of M/T cells increased odor-evoked output pattern separation and improved odor discrimination learning. In contrast, silencing GCL neurons with Gi-DREADD caused a decrease of pattern separation and a deterioration of odor discrimination learning. In conclusion, our data indicate that the OB network acts as a pattern separator to facilitate olfactory stimuli discriminations, a process that can be controlled by OB GABAergic neurons.
RESULTS
Similar odors evoke correlated input patterns to the OB
To test how efficiently the olfactory system may separate similar odor-evoked representations, we aimed at generating a large set of odorant stimuli evoking input patterns that vary in similarity. We investigated the complex representation of OB sensory inputs by imaging odor-evoked Ca2+ responses in the glomerular layer of awake head-restrained transgenic mice expressing the genetically encoded calcium indicator GCaMP3 in olfactory sensory neurons13 (OSNs, Fig. 1a and Supplementary Fig. 1). We used two groups of binary mixtures – amyl acetate/ethyl butyrate (AA/EB) and ethyl butyrate/3-hexanone (EB/HX) – composed of variable relative ratios of individual components mixed either in gaseous or liquid phases. The gaseous and liquid mixtures having the same presumed ratios differed consistently and could be attributed to the chemical properties of resulting mixtures such as relative composition and total concentration. This was highlighted by the analytical studies of binary mixtures using gas chromatography combined to flame ionization detection (GC-FID, see methods and Supplementary Table 1; total FID area concentrations ranged from ~1000 to 10000). Nevertheless, related mixtures evoked similar spatiotemporal patterns of glomerular activity (Fig. 1a,b). To quantify the similarity, we combined the fluorescence change of activated glomeruli in a population vector binned over an entire breath and further computed the Pearson correlation coefficient between vectors of activity evoked by each mixture, either for each mouse or for all animals (yielding similar results; Fig. 1c,d and Supplementary Fig. 1). The systematic comparison of all possible mixtures to each other was reported in a correlation matrix which revealed that binary mixtures with identical components at different ratios (either AA/EB or EB/HX mixtures) evoked highly correlated patterns of activity, whereas binary mixtures with different components (AA/EB vs. EB/HX mixtures) evoked less correlated patterns (Fig. 1c-e and Supplementary Fig. 1f). This observation was consistent throughout the time course of the first breath following odor presentation onset (Fig. 1e-f and Supplementary Fig. 1g).
Importantly, input correlation was not correlated to the amplitude of glomerular responses (Supplementary Fig. 2). Moreover, the odorant concentration used is the study did not saturate OSN response (Supplementary Fig. 3). Finally, input correlation across mixtures was similar for weakly and strongly activated glomeruli (Supplementary Fig. 4), indicating that strong glomerular responses do not impose the level of input correlation. The differences between input representations are therefore neither due to saturation of the calcium dye nor due to strongly and potentially saturating OSN responses, which may have both compressed the dynamic range of the calcium imaging readout.
Taken together, these data suggest that binary mixtures sharing the same pre-determined component ratios evoke highly correlated input patterns to the OB, irrespective of the subtle differences caused by gaseous and liquid phase mixing.
OB reformatting of odor representations in a single breath
Despite similarities between odorant-evoked input patterns, trained rodents can discriminate monomolecular odorants or mixtures in less than 400 ms, which corresponds to the duration of one or two breaths14,18─20,25─27. The olfactory bulb network may thus process sensory inputs in order to separate overlapping information. To test this assumption, single M/T cell units (n = 377 cells from 18 mice) were recorded in the OB of awake head-restrained mice while presenting various odorants13,28,29 (Fig. 2a). Neurons displayed complex responses to odorants such as phasic, tonic, excitatory, inhibitory or bimodal (n = 1248 odor-cell pairs in 6 mice, Fig. 2b-e and n = 1859 and 1430 odor-cell pairs in 4 and 8 mice, respectively, Supplementary Fig. 5a-d; different datasets were used to ascertain the reproducibility of population correlation in different animal groups). In order to compare the similarity of output patterns evoked by different mixtures, we computed the Pearson correlation between population vectors of M/T cell activity averaged over the first breath duration after odor onset evoked by different mixtures. Notably, the output correlation matrix differed from the one computed at the input level, suggesting that sensory representations are indeed reformatted within the OB synaptic pathway (compare Fig. 2f,g to Fig. 1c,d). Some groups of mixtures that were correlated at the input level became decorrelated at the output level whereas others remained unchanged (compare Fig. 2g to Fig. 1d). Though there was an overall reduction of output correlation, few mixtures evoked more correlated patterns at the output level than at the input level (points above the diagonal in Fig. 2h), suggesting a non-linear reduction of input to output correlations over all odors.
We then analyzed the output correlation at a finer time scale. Interestingly, the level of correlation between output patterns significantly evolved over the time course of the first and subsequent breaths, groups of mixtures becoming significantly decorrelated over time (Fig. 3a,b, Supplementary Fig. 5e-h and Supplementary Fig. 6). For some mixtures, decorrelation was rapid (e.g. red and blue curves in Fig. 3b,c and Supplementary Fig. 5e,f), which is consistent with some mitral cells responding to the early glomerular response (i.e. first 80ms in Fig. 1e,f). In contrast, correlation was increasing in the first 120 ms and then decreasing in the remaining part of the breath for other mixtures (e.g. cyan and magenta curves in Fig. 3b,c and Supplementary Fig. 5g,h), which likely relates to the increase of input correlation seen in Fig. 1f. Nevertheless, a significant decorrelation was observed for all subgroups of mixtures during the first breath (Fig. 3c). A similar decorrelation was observed after subtracting baseline firing activity (Fig. 3d,e), indicating that the process is independent from sniff-related baseline dynamics and from non-responding cells. Since input correlation reached a maximum around 120 ms, we further analyzed the output correlation from this time point of the first breath. We observed a significant decorrelation process taking place between 126 and 252 ms (Fig. 3e). Furthermore, increasing the temporal resolution by averaging mitral/tufted firing over a 21 ms time window revealed that output patterns were significantly decorrelated in ~80─100 ms after the peak of input correlation (Fig. 3f and Supplementary Fig. 7a).
Interestingly differences in the population firing rate evoked by different mixtures could not explain the observed correlation differences (Fig. 4). Furthermore the extent and dynamics of pattern correlation for different groups of mixtures depended neither on cell sampling nor on single cell response property since similar correlation behavior was observed for independently recorded datasets for which neurons displayed dissimilar response profiles (compare Figs. 2,3 and Supplementary Fig. 5). The main differences of correlation observed for different mixtures might instead be due to the variation of component concentration and absolute number of molecules in the mixtures, as reported by our GC-FID analysis (Supplementary Table 1 and see methods). Taken together, these results suggest that a reformatting of activity occurs in the OB, which tends to increase the separation of representations evoked by similar mixtures over a breathing cycle.
Output pattern similarity predicts learning performances
Does pattern separation taking place over the time course of a breath help animals to discriminate related odorants in a behavioral task? To address this question, we trained mice on a head-restrained go/no-go operant discrimination paradigm19 and tested eleven pairs of odorants that we selected based on the amount of output correlation they elicited (Fig. 5a,b). While the extent of input correlation over the first post-odor breath was not a good predictor of discrimination performances, we observed in contrast a significant correlation between M/T ensemble correlation and discrimination performances during the initial learning phase, defined here as the average of the first 300 trials (Fig. 5c─e). Similar results were observed when considering the mean and the minimum of the output correlation (Fig. 5d,e) or when output correlation was quantified only after the maximum of input correlation had been reached (Supplementary Fig. 7b─e). These data suggest that the extent of pattern separation in the OB predicts the ability of mice to discriminate between two odorants: the more the patterns are separated; the faster the odor discrimination learning takes place.
GABA neurons modulate pattern separation and learning
To further test the causal relationship between pattern separation and odor discriminability, we aimed to manipulate the OB outputs using optogenetics. Theoretical works suggest that pattern decorrelation may depend on OB inhibitory interneurons23,30─32. Therefore, we expressed channelrhodopsin2-YFP (ChR2) in interneurons of the granule cell layer using adeno-associated viral (AAV) vectors18 (Fig 6a,b). Immunohistochemical analysis of the infected brains revealed an infection exclusively restricted to the GCL in which ~35% of the neurons were transfected (Supplementary Fig. 8). We further performed extracellular recordings of M/T cells using optrodes (n = 38 cells from 6 mice; Fig 6c,d). Photostimulation significantly inhibited 38% of the total odor-M/T cell pairs (n = 228, χ2 test, P = 0.01). Light-evoked inhibition was cell-specific and M/T cell responses evoked by each odorant were not linearly inhibited (Fig. 6e,f). Together, these results suggest that the photostimulation triggered a specific modulation of tuning properties rather than an unspecific gain control. Though the average population rate was significantly reduced by light stimulation either during air or odor presentation (Fig. 6g), photostimulation enhanced pattern decorrelation over the breath time course only during the odor epoch for mixtures (Fig. 6h). The light-induced decorrelation was visible across the breathing cycle (Fig. 6i). In summary, photoactivation of inhibitory neurons in the GC layer modulates individual M/T cells activity and promotes pattern decorrelation.
Considering our observations that the extent of pattern separation correlates with odor-discrimination learning (Fig. 5), one would expect that enhancement of decorrelation should improve odor-discrimination performance. To test this hypothesis, we trained two cohorts of mice (expressing either enhanced green fluorescence protein [EGFP] or ChR2-YFP in neurons of the granule cell layer) on different mixture discrimination tasks. Head-restrained mice were photostimulated using a LED implanted on a cranial window overlaying both olfactory bulbs (Fig. 6j, Supplementary Fig. 9a,b and see methods). At high light power, animals strongly reduced their licking behavior in an olfactory association task (Supplementary Fig. 9c,d), suggesting a global silencing of M/T cells. In order to avoid such effect, we gradually reduced the light power until complete recovery of the normal licking behavior. This optimized power was then used during odor discrimination tasks. ChR2-expressing mice learned mixture discriminations faster than control mice but reached similar final performance levels (AA/EB and EB/HX mixtures in Fig. 6k). This effect was specific to photostimulation since the discrimination performance evolved similarly in both cohorts when two other mixtures were subsequently tested in the absence of light stimulation (C+/C− and AC/LI mixtures in Fig. 6k). Then, in order to reconfirm the photostimulation effect, we trained the same mice on yet another binary mixture pair. Again, light stimulation significantly enhanced learning performances without altering the final performance levels in the ChR2-expressing mice (HO/HA mixtures in Fig. 6k).
We next questioned whether stimulating GCL neurons would have similar impact on simple odor discrimination tasks. Interestingly M/T cell correlation for the monomolecular compounds present in the mixtures was not affected by ChR2 stimulation (Supplementary Fig.10a). Consistent with the electrophysiology, ChR2 stimulation did not affect the learning pace for simple odor discrimination task in an additional cohort of mice (Supplementary Fig. 10b). In contrast, but consistent with our first cohort, learning pace for difficult discrimination tasks (mixtures) was improved by photostimulation of GCL neurons (Supplementary Fig. 10c). Likewise, photostimulation specifically enhanced reaction times for difficult discrimination tasks (Supplementary Fig. 10d). These results confirm that pattern separation is a process mainly required to disambiguate overlapping OSN inputs representation.
We finally tested whether inhibiting GCL neurons would have an opposite effect on pattern separation and odor discrimination learning. We thus tested the effect of silencing the GCL population using inhibitory pharmacogenetic manipulation (hM4Di Gi-DREADD cohort). We recorded odor-evoked responses of M/T cells before and after DREADD activation by clozapine-N-oxide (CNO) injection. CNO injection increased odor-evoked firing rates in DREADD infected mice but not in ChR2 expressing mice (used as controls, Fig. 7a,b), leading to a specific increase of ensemble correlation in DREADD mice (Fig. 7c). We finally tested whether such decrease in pattern separation would affect odor discrimination. As predicted, silencing granule cell layer neurons using pharmacogenetics decreased odorant discrimination learning pace for complex odors (Fig. 7d).
In conclusion, these results demonstrate that bidirectional manipulation of GABAergic neurons of the GCL modulates the decorrelation of overlapping M/T cell activity patterns, which controls the ability of animals to learn to discriminate between similar odorants.
DISCUSSION
Our work provides evidence that overlapping glomerular maps evoked by related odorants can be separated by the OB network (up to certain level of similarity). This neural reformatting, which occurs during a single sniff, helps to disambiguate similar odorants and improves odorant discrimination learning. Finally, pattern separation can be promoted by the interplay between M/T cells and GABAergic neurons.
Overlapping OSN inputs can be decorrelated into separated patterns of M/T cell activity in a single breath (Figs. 1-3). Though we observed an overall decrease of correlation between input and output patterns, some mixtures evoked output patterns that remained highly correlated or even exhibited an increase of correlation compared to input patterns (Fig. 2). We do not think that the input/output transformation can simply be explained by the use of different recording techniques. Indeed, though the differences in sensitivity and temporal resolution between the two techniques may limit the direct comparison of correlation at 40 ms time scale, such limitations are no longer valid when the patterns of activity are averaged and compared over the complete breath duration. Furthermore, higher correlation observed at the input level for some mixtures cannot be explained by overestimated correlation resulting from possible calcium dye saturation for strongly activated glomeruli (Supplementary Figs. 2─4). In conclusion, odorant stimuli undergo an input/output reformatting in the OB involving a nonlinear reduction of correlation.
While in the past several groups have analyzed coding properties in the rodent olfactory bulb, the existence of pattern separation was often eluded, probably because of the use of a limited number of related odorant stimuli or the absence of neuronal population analyses. Yet, some authors questioned the role of OB (in comparison to the PC) to compute pattern separation using odor blends4,5. They reported a drop of correlation when comparing across-trials correlation vs. across-mixtures correlation, which in fact does not correspond to a pattern separation process. In addition, they did not observe any significant decorrelation between similar mixtures. In contrast, here we show that decorrelation of similar mixtures occurred in the OB with temporal dynamics that varied for different subgroups of mixtures (Fig. 3 and Supplementary Fig. 5), probably reflecting temporal patterning of M/T cell spiking13,25,28,33. Strikingly, decorrelation occurs in 80-100 ms after the peak of input correlation within the first breath after odor onset, providing a relevant timescale for the reaction time measured during olfactory-driven behaviors14,25─27 (Supplementary Fig. 10).
In both insects and vertebrates, the anatomical segregation of glomeruli constitutes the first step to differentiate channels of information and help animals to discriminate between odorants. Though discrimination behavior can be predicted to some extent by the similarity of glomerular responses14,34,35, this prediction is not efficient at the early learning phase of novel odorants14. Here using a large set of mixtures varying in similarity, we could confirm that glomerular input correlation is not a good predictor of discrimination learning for novel odorants (Fig. 5). In contrast, initial behavioral performances were clearly predicted by the output correlation during the complete breath (Fig. 5). Interestingly, the fast decorrelation process taking place following the peak of input correlation also significantly predicts behavioral performances (Supplementary Fig. 7). These results support the idea that overlapping activity patterns are separated at the level of the OB network. Importantly, optogenetic and pharmacogenetic manipulations of the OB network further support this view (Figs 6 and 7). It is noteworthy that learning performances for simple odorants were not affected by optogenetic manipulations in contrast to the learning of mixtures (Supplementary Fig. 10). In conclusion, while very different glomerular patterns would not require further separation in the OB network, as observed in the case of monomolecular odorants (Supplementary Fig. 10), our results emphasize the functional relevance of OB-mediated pattern separation in processing similar odorants.
In summary, as it has been hypothesized by experimental1,36,37 and modelling studies23 for a long time but never demonstrated, our work provide evidence for a functional role of pattern separation at a pre-cortical level.
Using optogenetic and pharmacogenetic manipulations, we provide evidence that the interplay between M/T cells and GCL neurons affects pattern separation (Figs. 6 and 7). Interestingly, the odor specificity of evoked inputs matters since ChR2-mediated GC recruitment does not linearly reduce the tuning curve of M/T cells but rather changes its shape (Fig. 6). These data suggest that GC layer photostimulation preferentially changes the odor selectivity rather than the gain, in contrast to other GABAergic interneurons such as such as parvalbumin cells of the external plexiform layer38,39.
Consistent with our observations, computational and in vitro works speculated that GC-dependent gating of M/T cells should enhance channel decorrelation, reduce spiking similarities between pairs of M/T cells and further improve pattern separation30,31. Supported by recent in vivo work, other models speculated in contrast that periglomerular cells (PG) may reduce similarity between odor-evoked M/T cells representations through non-topographical contrast enhancement40. GC would hence preferentially modulate fast gamma oscillations without substantial change of M/T cells firing rate41. However, our optogenetic and pharmacogenetic manipulations of GCL neurons contrasts with this idea, showing that those neurons can in fact enhance contrast between related odorants. As channel decorrelation or non-topographical contrast enhancement can occur separately from pattern separation42, it is possible that they constitute different or complementary processes10. Nevertheless, future work will be needed to test the possible contribution of other subpopulations of OB interneurons38,43, adult-born interneurons44,45 or other mechanisms such as intrinsic biophysical properties46 of M/T cells to pattern separation.
How the GC to M/T cell inhibition is recruited is critical for the control of olfactory discrimination behavior. Indeed, the deletion of the AMPA receptor subunit GluA2 in GCs, while boosting synaptic Ca2+ influx and thus M/T cell dendrodendritic inhibition, does not change odor discrimination learning pace18. In the present work, ChR2-GC layer photostimulation may force a more global, but still specific, GABA release mimicking an increase of synaptic weight23, favoring pattern decorrelation and improving the learning pace. The ChR2 condition might relate to M/T cells inhibition induced by cortical feedback projections onto granule cells47,48.
What is the relevance of pattern separation for downstream networks? In the recorded neuronal population, pattern separation is mainly processed by M/T cells responding with temporal changes of firing (Fig. 2 and Supplementary Fig. 5), possibly enhancing fine temporal differences between overlapping odor-evoked patterns. As the PC is able to integrate temporally shifted OB outputs49, pattern separation may broaden the range of PC neurons that are sensitive to different temporal integration windows.
While our data showed that pattern separation occurs in the OB, a previous work indicates that this process occurs downstream, in the PC5. It is possible that, after an extensive training, the PC separates patterns of activity evoked by similar stimuli that the OB fails to segregate5. Alternatively, the PC may preferentially process pattern completion5. Indeed, the PC, like the hippocampus, is an auto associative area, which is thought to process pattern storage and recall11,50. The OB and the dentate gyrus may comparably reduce correlated inputs to the PC and CA3 respectively in order to prevent misclassification of patterns and facilitate memory formation.
In conclusion, pattern separation might constitute an efficient network feature shared by different brain structures to properly identify stimuli eliciting a combinatorial of overlapping inputs and thereby promoting better discrimination.
ONLINE METHODS
Animals and initial preparation
Behavior and electrophysiology experiments were performed at the beginning of experiments on 8 to 16 week-old male C57BL/6J mice (Janvier, France). Imaging was done on 8 week-old Ompcre/+Rosaflox-stop-flox-GCaMP3/+ male mice (i.e. Omptm4(cre)Mom 51; JAX 006668 and Gt(ROSA)26Sortm38(CAG-GCaMP3)Hze/J 52; JAX 014538, respectively). None of the experiments were blind of the genotype. Mice were housed in groups of 3─5 in a state of the art animal facility (12hours light/dark cycles). All experiments were done during daytime. All experiments were done in accordance with the Swiss Federal Act on Animal Protection and Swiss Animal Protection Ordinance. Experiments were approved by the University of Geneva and the Geneva state ethics committees (authorizations 1007/3387/2 and GE/156/14).
Mice were anesthetized with isofluorane (3─4 % induction, 1─2% maintenance). The skin overlaying the skull was removed under local anaesthesia using carbostesin (AstraZeneca, Zug, Switzerland). A steel head-post was then fixed on the bone by embedding its base in dental cement (Omni-Etch Dentin, OmniDent). The rest of the skull was also covered with dental cement except the part overlaying the OB. Animals were then put back to their cage and allowed to recover for couple of days.
Few days after recovery, mice were trained to be head-restrained. They were placed in a plastic tube and head-fixed by screwing the head-post on a custom-made holder fixed on the air table (Fig. 2a). Mice were trained to this restraining condition for 2─4 sessions (30─60 min each) done in 2─3 days.
Odor delivery and experimental protocol
All odorants (amyl acetate: AA, ethyl butyrate: EB, 3-hexanone: HX, carvone +: C+, carvone −: C−, acetophenone: AC, limonene: LI, heptanal: HA, hexanol: HO) were from Sigma-Aldrich. We used the following mixtures: AA/EB 80%/20%, 60%/40%, 40%/60%, 20%/80%, EB/HX 80%/20%, 60%/40%, 40%/60%, 20%/80% but also simple component in some case such as AA/air 60%/40%, EB/air 60%/40% and HX/air 60%/40%.
4 mL of pure odorant or mixtures of odorants were placed in glass vials. Binary mixtures were made by mixing the odorant plumes in gaseous phase or by directly mixing in liquid phase. Varying the relative flow of independent stream of odorized air allowed mixing of odors in gaseous phase. Unexpectedly, the way mixtures were prepared had an impact on the electrophysiology and behavioral read-outs (see next section).
Odorants were delivered for 1.5─2 seconds through a custom made olfactometer as described previously13,28,33,53. The odorant onset was set at the end of an inspiration. Airflow passed through the vials containing the odorants and was further diluted 20 times with clean dry air before being sent to the nose. The total flow was constant (400 sccm, Standard Cubic Centimeter per Minute). To maintain a stable odor concentration during the entire stimulus application, we ensured that flows were stationary with a 3 s preloading before the odorant was delivered to the animal.
Measurement of ratios and concentrations in mixtures
We found in this study that the way mixtures were prepared had a strong impact on the electrophysiology and behavioral read-outs. The purpose of this study is not to explain the physico-chemical parameters leading to such difference. However, to gain more insight into the possible difference in composition of various mixtures, we estimated the composition of different mixtures using gas chromatography-flame ionization detector system (GC-FID). We observed that the various mixtures were indeed different. We report the quantification done with the GC-FID which measures two parameters: the relative ratio between two components and the mass of compounds reaching the detector that we can relate to the total amount of molecules (Supplementary Table 1). Since we could not sort the correlation profile based on only one of these parameters, we decided to keep the mixing procedure with theoretical values, which also preserved coherent groups of correlated mixtures. Most likely, the combination of monomolecular components identity, relative ratio and total amount of molecules (i.e. concentration) is determining the neural responses and correlation behaviors, but further experiments are needed to clarify which physico-chemical parameter is critical in forming the percepts.
The GC-FID analysis involved two steps:
-
Collection of binary mixture vapors from the output of olfactometer using NeedlEx
NeedlEx is a luer lock needle with side hole: inner diameter 0.5 mm, outer diameter 0.7 mm and length 85 mm (Shinwa Chemical Industries Ltd, Japan). We used the NeedlEx specific for organic solvents, which is packed with a layer of a polymer material (a copolymer of methacrylic acid and ethylene glycol dimethacrylate). Prior to odorant collection, the NeedlEx was conditioned at 200 °C in a gas chromatograph injection port (set to 100 kPa of Helium with a 200:1 split) for 1 hour before the very first usage of NeedlEx and for 5 minutes before each sample collection by keeping the top of the needle open to eliminate all possible contaminants. The NeedlEx was protected from contamination by closing both ends using Teflon caps when not in use. After conditioning, samples from the olfactometer gas flow were collected by actively pumping 50mL of gas through the Needlex using a calibrated pump.
-
Analysis of binary mixtures using GC-FID
Sixty samples corresponding to 2s odor presentation were cumulatively trapped by Needlex to get a consistent readout from GC-FID analysis. Immediately after sampling, the Needlex was transferred to the hot injection port of the GC for desorbing the compounds. Desorption time was set to 3 minutes. The desorbed vapors were then analyzed by the FID connected to the GC. In order to get robust values, we repeated the experiment 5─8 times for each mixture. The amount of molecules was quantified using the area of the GC-FID peak corrected by their relative response factor relative to EB. Response factors relative to EB have been determined by injecting mixtures of EB and AA or EB and HX at pre-determined concentration in the GC-FID.
Flame ionization detector (FID) is a robust and highly linear mass dependent detector widely used in gas chromatography. Its response is strongly correlated to the number of carbon atoms of organic molecules and the corresponding mass to peak area proportionality coefficient is called response factor (RF). However, RF is notably influenced by heteroatoms54 and can vary substantially between molecules. Consequently, considering two molecules, their FID peak area ratio may not reflect accurately their mass ratio. To correct for this difference of RF, one can inject a concentration-controlled mixture of a standard together with a molecule of interest. It is known that the ratio of peak area and the ratio of mass of compound and standard are proportional. The proportionality coefficient corresponds to the ratio of RF and is called the Relative Response Factor (RRF). Applying these RRFs to all studied molecules led to accurate relative mass comparisons. In our study, we considered one of the three compounds to be the standard (EB) since it was a shared component of all mixtures.
The actual amount of molecules has not been determined. The percentage of each component in a binary mixture, for example AA-EB mixture, was calculated as following:
After correction using response factors (RRFHX = 0.777 and RRFAA = 0.854), the total intensity of the stimuli was calculated by summing the corrected FID areas corresponding to the individual components.
In vivo imaging
On the day of the imaging session, animals were anesthetized with isoflurane and the skin atop the olfactory bulb was removed after a topical injection of carbostesin. The skull was thinned to allow optimal optical access to the left OB. Imaging started after at least a 30 min recovery period following the end of anesthesia. Images were acquired at 25Hz using the Micam Ultima system (Brainvision, Tokyo, Japan) mounted on a custom build microscope (Navitar 17 mm, bottom lens, Nikon 135mm, upper lens; total magnification 7.9×)12,13. Pixel values of collected images were computed as ΔF/F. All odorants were presented 10 times in a pseudo-randomized order. The first trial was discarded from the analysis due to fast sniffing causing motion artefacts in the images. For all experiments, respiration was monitored using a bidirectional air flow sensor (AWM2100V, Honeywell, MN) placed in front of the mouse nose. The device, though close to one nostril, did not prevent the odorant to reach the other nostril (connected to the recorded OB).
For analysis, absolute ΔF/F values were analyzed as a proxy for neural activity in OSN terminals. Analysis have been performed either on manually selected regions of interest (ROI) or on all pixels covering the OB surface, giving similar results (Supplementary Fig. 1). For the ROI analysis, ovoid regions displaying sustained increase in fluorescence during consecutive breaths following odorant application were manually delineated. All ROI activated by at least one mixture (at least 30 ROI per animal, n = 4 animals) were included in the analysis. To calculate the correlation between odorant-evoked patterns of glomeruli, change in fluorescence was calculated for each ROI either over the first breath after odorant application or in consecutive 40ms time windows during the first breath (Fig. 1c─e). Vectors of ROI activity were generated for each mixture and used to calculate correlations and further create correlation matrices. The correlations were computed either on single animal (correlation matrices being then averaged across animals, Fig. 1c─e, Supplementary Fig. 1a─d) or by concatenating all ROI coming from different mice (Supplementary Fig. 1e─g), giving similar results. For the pixel analysis, in each animal, after realigning all image series acquired for different mixtures using the resting fluorescence, a ROI encompassing the entire dorsal olfactory bulb was drawn. All individual pixels in the ROI were used to calculate correlation between patterns of activity evoked by different mixtures as mentioned above for the ROI analysis.
In vivo electrophysiological recordings and spike sorting
On the day of the experiment, mice were head-restrained and anesthetized with isoflurane (3% induction, 0.75─1% maintenance). A 1─2mm window was made by drilling the skull above the olfactory bulb and a silicon-based recording electrode (A─4×2─Tet─5mm─150─200─312, NeuroNexus Technologies, Ann Arbor, MI, USA) was inserted (Fig. 2a). The skull cavity was filled with an ophthalmic gel (Lacryvisc, Alcon) to protect the brain from drying. A silver wire contacting the gel was connected to the air table to ground the preparation. Electrodes were lowered in the target zone until reaching the medial mitral/tufted cell layer (MCL). We waited for the complete recovery of animals from anaesthesia. This period lasted for around 45─60 minutes, which also allowed the tissue to recover from electrodes penetration. For all experiments, respiration was monitored using a bidirectional air flow sensor (AWM2100V, Honeywell, MN) placed in front of the mouse nose.
For the recording during optogenetic manipulation, we used a 32 channels optrode (Neuronexus technology, A1×32─Poly3─OA32; Fig. 6). We chose three binary mixtures (all mixing done in gaseous phase), AA/EB 60/40 and 40/60, EB/HX 60/40 and 40/60 and hexanol/heptanal (HO/HA) 60/40 and 40/60. Before each experiment, the power was set to ~1─30mW with a photodiode sensor (Thorlabs, Germany) coupled to a power meter (PM100D, Thorlabs) and measured at end of a 200 μm fiber optic going out from the 473nm laser driver (Shanghai Dream Laser; SDL─473–050MFL; China). We set the maximum light intensity that shut down the M/T cells baseline activity. We then gradually reduced the light power until recovering partially the spike rate. As the inhibitory effect varied depending on the electrode location and ChR2 expression, power varied from 24 to 95 mW/mm2 (integrated power for 2s at the tip of the 105 μm diameter fiber glued on the optrode). Light activation was triggered with the odorant onset that is to say at end of an expiration. During odor presentation, transfected neurons of the GCL were photostimulated with eighty 5 ms-long pulses at 40 Hz 44. This stimulation ranges in the gamma band and is optimal to drive GCs30,55,56.
For the DREADD positive mice, we used the same odors as for the ChR2 positive mice. 10 trials of the different stimuli were presented pseudorandomly before CNO injection. After these trials, CNO was injected (2 mg/kg) 15─30 min before launching the next trials. After this period, 10 trials of the different stimuli were again applied pseudorandomly. CNO was injected either IP after anesthetizing shortly the mouse with isofluorane or either sub-cutaneous using a homemade cannula fixed on the skin using superglue. In the latter case, CNO was injected while the mouse was awake. We used the exact same procedure for the ChR2 positive mice that we used as a control for the DREADD positive mice.
Further details about recording and spike sorting have been described extensively elsewhere13,28,33. In brief, wide-band field potentials were amplified (100×) and band-pass filtered (0.1 Hz to 9 kHz). All data was digitized at 32556 Hz with the Cheetah Digital Lynx system (Neuralynx, Tucson, AZ). Spikes were detected by a threshold on the high-pass filtered signal, decomposed in 16 features, and automatically clustered. Individual neurons were finally identified if the clusters showed a clean refractory period in their autocorrelograms. 78 neurons from 6 mice were isolated for dataset 1 (Supplementary table 2). 169 isolated neurons were isolated from 4 mice for dataset 2. 130 were isolated from 8 mice for dataset 3. 38 cells were recorded from 6 ChR2 positive mice (dataset 4). 55 cells were recorded from 5 DREADD positive mice (dataset 5). 40 cells were recorded from the ChR2-positive mice that were used as a control for the DREADD mice (dataset 6). 39 cells were recorded from 7 ChR2-positive mice for monomolecular odorants (dataset 7). The number of cells recorded per animal ranged from 1 to 43. Each stimulus was presented 5 (dataset 3) or 10 (datasets 1, 2, 4, 5, 6 and 7) times. Trials of different odorant stimuli were presented in a pseudorandom manner for all datasets except for the DREADD mice and their control (datasets 5 and 6) as explained above.
Electrophysiological data analysis
All subsequent analyses and statistics were done using custom routines written for Matlab (MathWorks, Inc., Natick, MA).
Breathing cycle realignment
In order to analyze the consistent neural responses to odors across trials, the beginning of each cycle was temporally realigned to each other as previously described 28. In brief, all breathing cycles in awake animals were artificially matched to the mean breathing duration averaged over all trials: longer cycles were cut and shorter ones were prolonged. We observed an average breathing duration of 336 ± 100 ms for dataset 1, 296 ± 94 ms for dataset 2, 362 ms ± 179 for the dataset 3, 362 ± 100 ms for the ChR2 expressing mice (dataset 4), 334 ± 144 for DREADD expressing mice (dataset 5), 350 ± 145 for ChR2-expressing mice used as control for DREADD (dataset 6) and 308 ± 112 for the ChR2-expressing mice tested with simple odors (dataset 7, all values mean ± SD). Corresponding spike timings were realigned with the same method. Importantly, relative action potential timings in spike trains were not affected by this method.
Statistical analysis of the rate change for single cell responses
Change of odor-evoked firing rate over the breathing cycle duration was assessed, relative to baseline, by the non-parametric Wilcoxon rank sum (WRS) test repeated in each respiratory cycles spanning stimulus presentation and for all cells and stimuli. Three breathing cycles in the baseline activity were used as a template. The template was compared subsequently to the first three breathing cycles after odor onset. In a particular cycle, a cell was considered as responsive if at least one odorant stimulus evoked a significant change in firing in comparison to baseline. We set the P-value to 0.05.
For each significant change, a response was considered as excited if the average firing rate over the complete breathing cycle was significantly higher from the baseline and inhibited if it was significantly lower.
Statistical analysis of the temporal change for single cell responses
Change in the spike timing distribution during odor presentation was assessed by comparing the spike timing relative to the onset of the inspiration before and after odorant presentation using a Kolmogorov-Smirnov (KS) test repeated for each respiratory cycles for all cells and stimuli. Three breathing cycles in the baseline activity were used as a template. The template was compared each consecutive breathing cycles (3 in total) after the odor onset. In a particular cycle, a cell was considered as responsive if at least one odorant stimulus evoked a significant change in firing in comparison to baseline. We set the p value to 0.05.
Population vector construction
We pooled all M/T cells recorded in different animals. The activity of the 78, 169, 130, 38, 55, 40 and 39 (depending on the data set) neurons were organized in 78, 169, 130, 38, 55, 40 and 39 dimensional vectors respectively, containing in each dimension the average firing rate of a recorded cell computed over a certain time bin. Population vectors were built using 1 and 8 bins per breathing cycles (on average, awake: ~42, ~37, ~44, ~36, ~41, ~43ms and ~30 time bins for datasets 1 to 4, respectively).
To test weather noise induced by baseline activity might influence the decorrelation process, we normalized the population vector by subtracting it with averaged activity across 6 breaths during the baseline before the odor onset.
Correlation matrix
The matrix of correlation was built by computing the Pearson coefficient of correlation between pairs of population vectors averaged over the trials evoked by two different odors. The dimensions of the matrix correspond to n × n, where n is the number of odors. Each square corresponds to the Pearson coefficient of correlation of a particular odor against another. The odorants were ranked based on the theoretical ratios of components.
For more accurate detection of the minimum (Fig. 5e), correlations were computed on 21 ms time windows (i.e. 16 bins).
Head-restrained behavioral paradigm
Mice were trained to discriminate odorants under head-fixed paradigm as described previously19. In brief, before each behavioral session, a mouse was head-restrained in a plastic tube by screwing the head-post on a custom made holder fixed on a platform (Fig. 5a and Supplementary fig. 9). All behavior experiments were performed using a custom built olfactometer (similar to the ones used for imaging and electrophysiology), which was synchronized to a custom built lickometer. 11 odorant pairs were used for the behavioral training, which was selected from the panel of odorants used for the electrophysiology based on the level of correlation computed on M/T cell population responses.
Habituation task
Beginning 1–3 days after starting the water restriction, animals were trained in an associative task using an operant conditioning procedure. In a first pre-training session, a water drop (2 μL) was presented to animals (40 trials) 3s after a 200 ms-long warning tone (two different frequencies, 5 KHz and 6 KHz, were used on two different setups in the same room). The warning tone was used to make the mice alert about the following stimuli. During this stage, water was delivered to mice without analyzing their licking behavior. During the second stage, following the tone, licking was recorded during the baseline (1 s) as well as during the odor presentation (in this experiment, 1% methyl benzoate). Odors were presented for 2 s. If animals were not licking during the baseline, we implemented the criteria for water delivery based on their licking time during odor presentation. The total licking time required during odor presentation to trigger water reward was gradually increased in each step from 40 ms up to 240 ms (40 ms – 30 trials, 80 ms – 30 trials, 120 ms – 30 trials, 160 ms – 50 trials, 240 ms – 50 trials). If animals were licking during the baseline before odor presentation, the required licking time kept increasing from 100% (same amount of licking as during the baseline) up to 200% (100% – 30 trials, 125% – 30 trials, 150% – 30 trials, 175% – 50 trials, 200% – 50 trials). Most animals learned this task in 2─3 days (4-─6 sessions of 30 min each).
Olfactory discrimination task
The inter-trial interval was constant (13.2 s including the preloading of 3.2 s). Mice were trained to discriminate between two odorants, one being rewarded (S+) and the other being unrewarded (S─). The criterion to get a water reward was a 80 ms total lick time in three out of four 500 ms time bins during the 2 s odor presentation. If mice licked before odorant presentation, they had to lick double amount of time during the odor presentation to get water reward. Trials were counted as correct if the animals met the criteria mentioned above for rewarded trials. For unrewarded trials, the criterion for a correct trial was a maximum 80 ms lick time in one out of four time bin of 500 ms during the 2 s odor presentation. If mice licked during the baseline of an unrewarded trial, the trial was counted as correct, if the total licking time during 2 s odor presentation did not exceed 25% of their baseline licking. Generally most of the mice did not lick for unrewarded trials and they consistently licked for rewarded trials after the task acquisition. No punishment was given to the mice for incorrect trials.
Odors were presented in a pseudo-randomized order (no more than 2 successive presentations of the same odor, equal numbers within each block of 20 trials, ensuring different order of presentations for S+ and S─ trials within each 20 trial blocks). No intrinsic preference toward any of the odors was observed. Bias caused by odor preferences was generally avoided by assigning the same odor as S+ or S─ stimulus for the same number of animals within the experimental group. A total of 80─160 trials per session per animal and a total of 150─300 trials per day per animal were performed in order to keep animal motivated. Motivation was checked by monitoring the licking. Mice stopped licking when they lost motivation, in which case the session was interrupted. No animals were excluded for failing to acquire the task. Reaction times were calculated as previously described 19.
The following sequence of tasks were done (Fig. 5): AA vs. EB (2 days), break (19 days), AA60/EB40 vs. AA40/EB40 in air (2 days), break (55 days), HX vs. EB (2 days), break (4 days), HX60/EB40 vs. HX40/EB60 in air (3days), break (4 days), HX60/EB40 vs. HX40/EB60 in liquid (2days), break (6 days), HX60/EB40 vs. AA60/EB40 in air (2days), break (90 days), HX80/EB20 vs. HX20/EB80 in air (2 days), break (81 days), AA80/EB20 vs. AA20/EB80 in air (2days), break (1 day), HX80/EB20 vs. AA80/EB20 in air (1 day), break (2 days), HX40/EB60 vs. AA60/EB40 in air (2 days), break (1 day), AA60/EB40 vs. AA40/EB60 in liquid (2 days).
AAV injection
The skin atop the skull was removed. Using a biopsy punch of 2.5 mm diameter (Harris, Uni-Core, Canada), a piece of bone was removed above the surface of both OBs. While removing the piece of bone, the skull was maintained wet to prevent the dura from sticking to the bone.
Virus injections were done in the granule cell layer of the olfactory bulb following the method described previously18. In order to avoid contamination in other cell types or brain regions, we restrained the injection site in the anterior portion of the OB and at an intermediate depth. The zero point was set with respect to the center of the dorsal surface of the OB (see 18). Starting from this point, four injections were done in the following order: (1) AP: +500 μm, DV: −1000 μm; (2) +300 μm, DV: −1300 μm; (3) +100 μm; DV: −1400 μm; (4) +100 μm; DV: −2000 μm. 100─150 μL of AAV1─ACAGW─ChR2─Venus, AAV5─EF1─eGFP.WRPE.RBG or rAAV2─hSyn─HA─hM4D(Gi)─IRES─mCitrine (University of North Carolina vector core facility) were injected by keeping the injection time around 2─3 min. We waited at least 5 min before removing the injection pipette in order to avoid diffusion of the virus toward the surface.
Optogenetic and pharmacogenetic manipulation of behavior
Cranial window implantation
The skull surrounding the hole was thinned to fit a cover glass. A drop of dexamethasone, a glucocorticoid steroid, was applied on the surface of the dura to minimize possible inflammation. After few minutes, the dexamethasone was cleaned with cortex buffer (containing in mM 125 NaCl, 10 glucose, 10 HEPES, 5 KCl, 2 CaCl2, 2 MgCl2). A drop of clean cortex buffer was put on the OB surface before placing the cover glass. A 3mm diameter cover glass was fitted in the hole on top of OB. Excessive cortex buffer was dried around the window until the surrounding bone was completely dry. The edge of the window was sealed with dental cement (Palladur mixed with Palapress vario, Pala). Once the window was fixed, we implanted the head-post as previously described13,18,28. A delay of at least 3 weeks was respected in order to obtain an optimal ChR2 expression. After this period, we checked the transparency of the cranial windows and a ~2mm diameter blue LED (blue LED, NFSB036BT) with the connector (ED8250-ND, connector strip header 50POS .050, Millmax) was implanted on top of the window with dental cement (Palladur mixed with Palapress vario, Pala). The LED illuminated a surface of ~5.3 mm2 covering both bulbs.
Settings of LED power
During the optogenetic behavior experiments, the implanted LEDs were driven with a high power LED driver (LEDD1B, T-Cube LED Driver 1200 mA, ThorLabs). The goal of the experiment was to evaluate whether the photoactivation of GCL neurons could enhance odorant discrimination. However, overstimulation of the inhibitory network may shut down the M/T cells activity. As the degree of ChR2 infection could vary between different animals, the maximum power needed to shut down OB activity may also vary. For this reason, we set a maximum light power that would be optimal for each mouse. We pre-trained the animals until they learned to lick sufficiently during odorant application19. We then photo-activated the network during odor application using a light power that blocked the licking behavior (Supplementary Fig. 9). The maximum power needed to shut down the odor-evoked licking behavior was 23 mW (7.3 mW/mm2) for ChR2-expressing mice. Finally, we applied the same sequence of blocks by gradually reducing the light power for each individual mouse until they showed a similar licking pattern as observed without light stimulation. This power varied for individual mice and was used for the stimulation of granule cells during the go/no-go discrimination tasks.
Light driven discrimination behavior paradigm
During optogenetic behavior experiments, we monitored the breathing cycles using an air flow sensor (AWM2100V, Honeywell, MN) coupled with a home-made amplifier in order to precisely set odorant and light onset based on the respiratory time course. The odorant onset began at the end of the mouse inspiration and lasted for 2 seconds. The light onset started at the beginning of inspiration along with odorant trigger. We used 80 pulses of 5ms at a frequency of 40hz. As mentioned in the previous section, the power was individually set for each mouse. The discrimination training was carried out as described previously.
Injection of clozapine-N-oxide (CNO)
CNO diluted in saline was prepared freshly every day. After isoflurane anesthesia, each mouse (DREADD and ChR2 expressing mice) was injected with 2 mg/kg 15─20 minutes before the animal started the discrimination paradigm. Mice performed the task for a maximum of 60─75 min after the injection.
Immunohistochemistry and quantification
Animals were anaesthetized by intraperitoneal injection of urethane 20% in 0.9% NaCl and perfused transcardially with 40 ml of saline followed by 100 ml of 4% paraformaldehyde (PFA) in 0.1M phosphate buffer at 4°C (PBS, pH 7.3). The brains were then removed and fixed overnight in 4% PFA. After embedding brains in 4% agarose, 40 μm coronal slices were cut with a vibratome (Leica VT S1000) and collected in PBS (0.1 M). For immunostaining, we first rinsed in TBST (Tris-buffered saline with tween) and TBSTT (Tris-buffered saline with tween and triton). We incubated the slices with 10% bovine serum for 1h at room temperature and then with the primary antibodies overnight at 4°C. We used a rabbit anti-GFP antibody29 (1:1000, Invitrogen A11122), a mouse anti-Reelin antibody29 (1:1000, Abcam 78540) and a mouse anti-NeuN antibody29 (1:500, Millipore MAB377). The day after, slices were rinsed with TBST and incubated with Alexa 488 anti-rabbit IgG29 (1:200, Invitrogen, A21206) and Alexa 546 anti-mouse IgG29 (1:200, Invitrogen, A21123) for 1h at room temperature. Slices were counterstained with Hoechst 154 (1:5000, Invitrogen, H3570) and mounted with Vectashield (Vectors Laboratories).
Images were acquired with a confocal laser scanning microscope (Zeiss LSM 510META and Leica SPM5) with a 40× or 63× oil-immersion objectives. Fluorescent channels were acquired sequentially to separate wavelength and minimize possible cross-talk. In the MCL and GL, all slices were visually inspected in order to find GFP/mCitrine positive neurons. Since we could not find any, only few slices were scanned for illustration. In the GCL, counting was done manually with ImageJ on 75 and 42 representative regions of 26 and 22 slices in the anterior, medial and posterior part of the OB for ChR2 and Gi-DREADD infected mice, respectively (n = 6 animals for each group; Supplementary Fig. 8). In total, ~25000 cells were analyzed.
Statistics
All analyses were performed using Matlab (MathWorks, Inc., Natick, MA), Origin pro, Statistica or Prism. In this study, we used ANOVA and post-hoc tests, χ2 test and different non-parametric tests (see text and legends). All tests were 2-sided. Shapiro-Wilk test was used to assess normality of the data. For all parametric ANOVA, homogeneity of variance was tested using Levene’s test or a test of sphericity (for one-way repeated measures ANOVA). No statistical methods were used to predetermine sample sizes, but our sample sizes were similar to those reported in previous publications13,19,28,29. Data collection and animal assignation to the various experimental groups were randomized.
Supplementary Material
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
We thank Simon Barthelme, Brice Bathellier, Rainer Friedrich, Anthony Holtmaat, Foivos Markopoulos, Alexandre Pouget and members of A.C. and I.R. laboratories for helpful discussions and/or comments on the manuscript. We thank Joëlle Bourquin and Sebastien Pellat for technical assistance. This research was supported by the University of Geneva, the Geneva neuroscience center (common grant to A.C. and I.R.), the Swiss National Science Foundation (grant numbers: 31003A_153410 to A.C., CR33I13_143723 to A.C. and I.R. and 310030E_135910 to I.R.), the National Center of Competence in Research (NCCR) “SYNAPSY - The Synaptic Bases of Mental Diseases” financed by the Swiss National Science Foundation (n° 51AU40_125759, A.C.), the European Research Council (contract number ERC─2009─StG─243344─NEUROCHEMS, A.C.) and the European Molecular Biology Organization (young investigator program, A.C; long-term postdoctoral fellowship, N.A).
Footnotes
A supplementary methods checklist is available
COMPETING INTERESTS
The authors declare that no competing interests exist.
REFERENCES
- 1.Friedrich RW, Laurent G. Dynamic optimization of odor representations by slow temporal patterning of mitral cell activity. Science (New York, N.Y) 2001;291:889–894. doi: 10.1126/science.291.5505.889. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Friedrich RW, Laurent G. Dynamics of olfactory bulb input and output activity during odor stimulation in zebrafish. Journal of neurophysiology. 2004;91:2658–2669. doi: 10.1152/jn.01143.2003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Niessing J, Friedrich RW. Olfactory pattern classification by discrete neuronal network states. Nature. 2010;465:47–52. doi: 10.1038/nature08961. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Barnes DC, Hofacer RD, Zaman AR, Rennaker RL, Wilson DA. Olfactory perceptual stability and discrimination. Nature neuroscience. 2008;11:1378–1380. doi: 10.1038/nn.2217. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Chapuis J, Wilson DA. Bidirectional plasticity of cortical pattern recognition and behavioral sensory acuity. Nature neuroscience. 2012;15:155–161. doi: 10.1038/nn.2966. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Leutgeb JK, Leutgeb S, Moser MB, Moser EI. Pattern separation in the dentate gyrus and CA3 of the hippocampus. Science (New York, N.Y) 2007;315:961–966. doi: 10.1126/science.1135801. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.McHugh TJ, et al. Dentate gyrus NMDA receptors mediate rapid pattern separation in the hippocampal network. Science (New York, N.Y) 2007;317:94–99. doi: 10.1126/science.1140263. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Nakashiba T, et al. Young dentate granule cells mediate pattern separation, whereas old granule cells facilitate pattern completion. Cell. 2012;149:188–201. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2012.01.046. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Friedrich RW. Neuronal computations in the olfactory system of zebrafish. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2013;36:383–402. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-062111-150504. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Friedrich RW, Wiechert MT. Neuronal circuits and computations: pattern decorrelation in the olfactory bulb. FEBS Lett. 2014;588:2504–2513. doi: 10.1016/j.febslet.2014.05.055. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Rolls ET. The mechanisms for pattern completion and pattern separation in the hippocampus. Front Syst Neurosci. 2013;7:74. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2013.00074. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Vincis R, Gschwend O, Bhaukaurally K, Beroud J, Carleton A. Dense representation of natural odorants in the mouse olfactory bulb. Nature neuroscience. 2012;15:537–539. doi: 10.1038/nn.3057. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Patterson MA, Lagier S, Carleton A. Odor representations in the olfactory bulb evolve after the first breath and persist as an odor afterimage. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2013;110:E3340–3349. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1303873110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Abraham NM, et al. Maintaining accuracy at the expense of speed; stimulus similarity defines odor discrimination time in mice. Neuron. 2004;44:865–876. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.11.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Linster C, Johnson BA, Morse A, Yue E, Leon M. Spontaneous versus reinforced olfactory discriminations. J Neurosci. 2002;22:6842–6845. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-16-06842.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Linster C, et al. Perceptual correlates of neural representations evoked by odorant enantiomers. J Neurosci. 2001;21:9837–9843. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-24-09837.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Rubin BD, Katz LC. Spatial coding of enantiomers in the rat olfactory bulb. Nature neuroscience. 2001;4:355–356. doi: 10.1038/85997. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Abraham NM, et al. Synaptic inhibition in the olfactory bulb accelerates odor discrimination in mice. Neuron. 2010;65:399–411. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.01.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Abraham NM, Guerin D, Bhaukaurally K, Carleton A. Similar odor discrimination behavior in head-restrained and freely moving mice. PLoS One. 2012;7:e51789. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0051789. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Abraham NM, Vincis R, Lagier S, Rodriguez I, Carleton A. Long term functional plasticity of sensory inputs mediated by olfactory learning. eLife. 2014;3:e02109. doi: 10.7554/eLife.02109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Stettler DD, Axel R. Representations of odor in the piriform cortex. Neuron. 2009;63:854–864. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.09.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Miura K, Mainen ZF, Uchida N. Odor representations in olfactory cortex: distributed rate coding and decorrelated population activity. Neuron. 2012;74:1087–1098. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.04.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Wiechert MT, Judkewitz B, Riecke H, Friedrich RW. Mechanisms of pattern decorrelation by recurrent neuronal circuits. Nature neuroscience. 2010;13:1003–1010. doi: 10.1038/nn.2591. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Schaefer AT, Margrie TW. Spatiotemporal representations in the olfactory system. Trends Neurosci. 2007;30:92–100. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2007.01.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Cury KM, Uchida N. Robust odor coding via inhalation-coupled transient activity in the mammalian olfactory bulb. Neuron. 2010;68:570–585. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.09.040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Rinberg D, Koulakov A, Gelperin A. Speed-accuracy tradeoff in olfaction. Neuron. 2006;51:351–358. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.07.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Uchida N, Mainen ZF. Speed and accuracy of olfactory discrimination in the rat. Nature neuroscience. 2003;6:1224–1229. doi: 10.1038/nn1142. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Gschwend O, Beroud J, Carleton A. Encoding odorant identity by spiking packets of rate-invariant neurons in awake mice. PLoS ONE. 2012;7:e30155. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0030155. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Tatti R, et al. A population of glomerular glutamatergic neurons controls sensory information transfer in the mouse olfactory bulb. Nature communications. 2014;5:3791. doi: 10.1038/ncomms4791. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Arevian AC, Kapoor V, Urban NN. Activity-dependent gating of lateral inhibition in the mouse olfactory bulb. Nature neuroscience. 2008;11:80–87. doi: 10.1038/nn2030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Giridhar S, Doiron B, Urban NN. Timescale-dependent shaping of correlation by olfactory bulb lateral inhibition. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2011;108:5843–5848. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1015165108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Koulakov AA, Rinberg D. Sparse incomplete representations: a potential role of olfactory granule cells. Neuron. 2011;72:124–136. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.07.031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Bathellier B, Buhl DL, Accolla R, Carleton A. Dynamic ensemble odor coding in the mammalian olfactory bulb: sensory information at different timescales. Neuron. 2008;57:586–598. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.02.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Guerrieri F, Schubert M, Sandoz JC, Giurfa M. Perceptual and neural olfactory similarity in honeybees. PLoS Biol. 2005;3:e60. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0030060. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Youngentob SL, et al. Experience-induced fetal plasticity: the effect of gestational ethanol exposure on the behavioral and neurophysiologic olfactory response to ethanol odor in early postnatal and adult rats. Behav Neurosci. 2007;121:1293–1305. doi: 10.1037/0735-7044.121.6.1293. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Friedrich RW, Habermann CJ, Laurent G. Multiplexing using synchrony in the zebrafish olfactory bulb. Nature neuroscience. 2004;7:862–871. doi: 10.1038/nn1292. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Yaksi E, Judkewitz B, Friedrich RW. Topological reorganization of odor representations in the olfactory bulb. PLoS Biol. 2007;5:e178. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0050178. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Kato HK, Gillet SN, Peters AJ, Isaacson JS, Komiyama T. Parvalbumin-expressing interneurons linearly control olfactory bulb output. Neuron. 2013;80:1218–1231. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.08.036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Miyamichi K, et al. Dissecting local circuits: parvalbumin interneurons underlie broad feedback control of olfactory bulb output. Neuron. 2013;80:1232–1245. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.08.027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Cleland TA, Sethupathy P. Non-topographical contrast enhancement in the olfactory bulb. BMC neuroscience. 2006;7:7. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-7-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Fukunaga I, Herb JT, Kollo M, Boyden ES, Schaefer AT. Independent control of gamma and theta activity by distinct interneuron networks in the olfactory bulb. Nature neuroscience. 2014;17:1208–1216. doi: 10.1038/nn.3760. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Chow SF, Wick SD, Riecke H. Neurogenesis drives stimulus decorrelation in a model of the olfactory bulb. PLoS Comput Biol. 2012;8:e1002398. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1002398. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Linster C, Cleland TA. Decorrelation of Odor Representations via Spike Timing-Dependent Plasticity. Frontiers in computational neuroscience. 2010;4:157. doi: 10.3389/fncom.2010.00157. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Alonso M, et al. Activation of adult-born neurons facilitates learning and memory. Nature neuroscience. 2012;15:897–904. doi: 10.1038/nn.3108. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Carleton A, et al. Making scents of olfactory neurogenesis. J Physiol Paris. 2002;96:115–122. doi: 10.1016/s0928-4257(01)00087-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Padmanabhan K, Urban NN. Intrinsic biophysical diversity decorrelates neuronal firing while increasing information content. Nature neuroscience. 2010;13:1276–1282. doi: 10.1038/nn.2630. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Boyd AM, Sturgill JF, Poo C, Isaacson JS. Cortical feedback control of olfactory bulb circuits. Neuron. 2012;76:1161–1174. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.10.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Markopoulos F, Rokni D, Gire DH, Murthy VN. Functional properties of cortical feedback projections to the olfactory bulb. Neuron. 2012;76:1175–1188. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.10.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Haddad R, et al. Olfactory cortical neurons read out a relative time code in the olfactory bulb. Nature neuroscience. 2013;16:949–957. doi: 10.1038/nn.3407. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Wilson DA, Sullivan RM. Cortical processing of odor objects. Neuron. 2011;72:506–519. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.10.027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Supplementary references
- 51.Li J, Ishii T, Feinstein P, Mombaerts P. Odorant receptor gene choice is reset by nuclear transfer from mouse olfactory sensory neurons. Nature. 2004;428:393–399. doi: 10.1038/nature02433. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Zariwala HA, et al. A Cre-dependent GCaMP3 reporter mouse for neuronal imaging in vivo. J Neurosci. 2012;32:3131–3141. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4469-11.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Bathellier B, Van De Ville D, Blu T, Unser M, Carleton A. Wavelet-based multi-resolution statistics for optical imaging signals: Application to automated detection of odour activated glomeruli in the mouse olfactory bulb. Neuroimage. 2007;34:1020–1035. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.10.038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.de Saint Laumer J-Y, Cicchetti E, Merle P, Egger J, Chaintreau A. Quantification in Gas Chromatography: Prediction of Flame Ionization Detector Response Factors from Combustion Enthalpies and Molecular Structures. Analytical Chemistry. 2010;82:6457–6646. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Lagier S, Carleton A, Lledo PM. Interplay between local GABAergic interneurons and relay neurons generates gamma oscillations in the rat olfactory bulb. J Neurosci. 2004;24:4382–4392. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5570-03.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Lagier S, et al. GABAergic inhibition at dendrodendritic synapses tunes gamma oscillations in the olfactory bulb. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2007;104:7259–7264. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0701846104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.