Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2017 Apr 1.
Published in final edited form as: Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2016 Mar 19;40(4):672–685. doi: 10.1111/acer.13004

The Behavioral Economics and Neuroeconomics of Alcohol Use Disorders

James MacKillop 1,2
PMCID: PMC4846981  NIHMSID: NIHMS750839  PMID: 26993151

Abstract

Background

Behavioral economics and neuroeconomics bring together perspectives and methods from psychology, economics, and cognitive neuroscience to understand decision making and choice behavior. Extending an operant behavioral theoretical framework, these perspectives have increasingly been applied to understanding alcohol use disorders (AUDs) and this review surveys the theory, methods, and findings from this approach. The focus is on three key behavioral economic concepts: delay discounting (i.e., preferences for smaller immediate rewards relative to larger delayed rewards), alcohol demand (i.e., alcohol reinforcing value), and proportionate alcohol-related reinforcement (i.e., relative amount of psychosocial reinforcement associated with alcohol use).

Findings

Delay discounting has been linked to AUDs in both cross-sectional and longitudinal studies, and has been investigated cross-sectionally using neuroimaging. Alcohol demand and proportionate alcohol-related reinforcement have both been robustly associated with drinking and alcohol misuse cross-sectionally, but not over time. Both have also been found to predict treatment response to brief interventions. Alcohol demand has also been used to enhance the measurement of acute motivation for alcohol in laboratory studies. Interventions that focus on reducing the value of alcohol by increasing alternative reinforcement and response cost have been found to be efficacious, albeit in relatively small numbers of randomized controlled trials (RCTs). Mediators and moderators of response to these interventions have not been extensively investigated.

Future Directions

The application of behavioral economics and neuroeconomics to AUDs has given rise to an extensive body of empirical work, although significant gaps in knowledge remain. In particular, there is a need for more longitudinal investigations to clarify the etiological roles of these behavioral economic processes, especially alcohol demand and proportionate alcohol reinforcement. Additional RCTs are needed to extend and generalize the findings from reinforcement-based interventions and to investigate mediators and moderators of treatment success for optimization. Applying neuroeconomics to AUDs remains at an early stage and has been primarily descriptive to date, but has high potential for important translational insights in the future. The same is true for using these behavioral economic indicators to understand genetic influences on AUDs.

Keywords: Behavioral economics, neuroeconomics, alcohol, delay discounting, alcohol demand, proportionate alcohol-related reinforcement

Introduction

The fields of behavioral economics and neuroeconomics are increasingly being applied to the study of alcohol use disorders (AUDs), and the goal of this review is to provide an introduction to these perspectives. Most broadly, behavioral economics refers to the hybridization of concepts and methods from psychology and economics to understand the choices people make (Camerer 1999; Bickel and Vuchinich 2003), and neuroeconomics refers to the further integration of behavioral economics and cognitive neuroscience to understand the neural foundations of those choices (Zak 2004; Glimcher et al. 2009). For heuristic purposes, these perspectives can be understood in the context of the conventional branches of economics, with a shifting focus from the largest units of analysis to the smallest. Macroeconomics focuses on aggregated indicators of economic activity for large catchment areas (e.g., countries, regions), characterizing the transactions within and between large-scale economic actors. Microeconomics focuses on the allocation of financial resources by businesses, families, or individuals within their economic niches, examining transactions between smaller scale economic actors. One level of analysis deeper, behavioral economics focuses on how individual people allocate their behavioral resources, broadly defined to include money, time, and physical efforts, and investigates the psychological processes and environmental contingencies that systematically influence these transactions. Finally, neuroeconomics focuses on identifying the underlying aspects of brain structure and function that underlie the choices people make in their transactions with the world.

Beyond heuristics, however, behavioral economics and neuroeconomics are less like singular academic disciplines and more like a federation of scientific lines of inquiry from different intellectual traditions. There are some commonalities, but also surprisingly large differences in the foundational assumptions and phenomena of interest. Considering the multidisciplinary nature of the field, this is not surprising, but a consequence of the heterogeneity of perspectives is the importance of ‘connecting the dots’ from theory to methods to findings in a given area. Achieving that continuity in applying behavioral economics and neuroeconomics to understand AUDs from one specific perspective is the goal of the current article. Specifically, this review is intended to be a précis of the theory, methods, empirical findings, and active research questions in the operant behavioral formulation of behavioral economics. As such, it is not intended to be exhaustive and does not provide all existing economic perspectives on AUDs. In addition, the primary scope of this review is on AUDs and human studies; studies on other drugs or using preclinical animal models will only be included when pertinent. In terms of structure, the first section introduces the theoretical foundations; the second section describes three key processes – delay discounting, alcohol demand, and proportionate alcohol-related reinforcement – and provides a concise survey of the behavioral findings for each; the third section reviews clinical interventions that employ behavioral economic principles; the fourth section discusses progress in applying neuroeconomics to AUDs; and the fifth section identifies gaps in knowledge and current research priorities.

Theoretical Foundations

The theoretical basis for using behavioral economics and neuroeconomics to study AUDs comes from two sources, its conceptual fit with the foundations of normative behavioral economics and the extension of an operant learning approach to AUDs. In the first case, common themes in virtually all behavioral economic research are a focus on choice behavior and understanding rational and irrational preferences (Camerer 1999; Camerer et al. 2004). The focus on choice behavior is selections among mutually exclusive outcomes are among the smallest units of analysis in economic exchange (broadly defined to include the exchange of one commodity [e.g., money, time, effort] for another [e.g., good, service, or behavioral outcome]). The focus on rationality is to address the general assumption of a rational actor in economics in general (i.e., preferences that are consistent over time and maximize positive outcomes). As a result, a major theme in behavioral economic research is identifying the ways in which individuals systematically deviate from rationality. Probably the most famous example of this is prospect theory, which has revealed asymmetric (i.e., irrational) sensitivity for losses compared to gains (Kahneman 2003), insights for which Daniel Kahneman shared the 2002 Nobel Prize in Economic Sciences. These normative studies on behavioral economics reveal the way that irrational preferences are not simply stochastic, but reflect systematic deviations from rational actor models, what Ariely (2010) refers to as “predictably irrational” behavior. With regard to AUDs, these conceptual perspectives are highly relevant because choice behavior is a similarly critical antecedent of drinking. However internally conflicted or subjectively avolitional a person may feel, the behavior of an individual with an AUD when drinking is to physically reach out, lift a glass or bottle, and drink alcohol, a behavioral act of choice. In addition, at least from a clinical perspective, AUDs present as persistently irrational behaviors: continued drinking despite substantial and escalating negative consequences and strong personal desire to gain control over the behavior.

The second theoretical basis for behavioral economics comes from the tradition of operant learning theory. Using basic behavioral science as first principles, alcohol use (healthy or unhealthy) is theorized to be a prototypic operant behavior that is maintained by the reinforcing properties of the drug (Bigelow 2001; Higgins et al. 2004). The foundational studies that support an operant perspective on AUDs come from residential laboratory studies in the late 1960s and early 1970s. These early studies demonstrated that alcohol consumption could be examined under controlled experimental conditions and was not unique as a reinforcing commodity (e.g., Mello & Mendelson 1965; Mendelson & Mello 1966; Nathan et al. 1970). In addition, residential laboratory studies convincingly demonstrated that alcohol consumption was sensitive to increases in response cost and the presence of alternative reinforcers (e.g., Bigelow et al. 1972; Sanders et al. 1976; Griffiths et al. 1977; for a review, see Bigelow 2001), key predictions from operant theory. Using self-report measures, it is also clear that alcohol has both positively reinforcing properties that enhance an experience (e.g., stimulation, self-perception, social enhancement, gustatory properties) and negatively reinforcing properties that remove unpleasant experiences (e.g., anxiolysis, alleviation of depression) (Carey and Correia 1997; Darkes et al. 2004; Kuntsche et al. 2008; Corbin et al. 2011; Herschl et al. 2012). Importantly, these different forms of positive and negative reinforcement are not mutually exclusive and operate concurrently, reflecting an individual’s underlying motivational schemata for alcohol reinforcement. Figure 1 presents an integrative operant/behavioral economic theoretical perspective on AUDs at a conceptual level. Alcohol’s positively and negatively reinforcing properties, its punishing properties, the opportunity for alternative reinforcers, and the timing of provision are theorized to jointly determine the reinforcing value of alcohol, the putative final common pathway to drinking. These processes are theorized to be the proximal mechanisms by which other known risk factors contribute to drinking behavior.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

The critical catalyst for integrating operant learning principles and economic methods, however, was Herrnstein’s matching law (Herrnstein 1961; Herrnstein 1970), which sought to describe behavior in operant environments with more complex and dynamic reinforcement contexts. Specifically, the matching law proposes that, over time, behavioral rates of responding scale to (match) the available reinforcement schedules. In turn, selectively allocating finite amounts of behavior for differential outcomes was recognized as basically being a parallel to an economic environment (Hursh 1980; Hursh 1984). Beyond controlled experimental contexts, drinking in the natural environment reflects choices among diverse behavioral options with varying reinforcing properties and behavioral costs. This natural operant environment is highly complex, with many concurrent schedules of reinforcement, but collectively it too can ultimately be understood as a zero-sum behavioral microeconomy within which finite resources are mutually exclusively allocated. For this reason, microeconomic principles and methods have been imported to improve the characterization of operant behavior (Bickel et al. 1993; Hursh 1993; Bickel and Vuchinich 2003; Vuchinich and Heather 2003; Hursh et al. 2005). This approach represents the converse of behavioral economic studies that import psychological processes to understand economic behavior; it is the importation of economic concepts and methods to understand behavior.

A second important insight from the matching law was that delay to reward had a similar effect to response cost (Chung and Herrnstein 1967), indicating that the reinforcing value of an outcome was a function not only of its reinforcing properties, punishment, and alternatives, but the immediacy of access. For this reason, it is also included in Figure 1. In addition, it became clear that the temporal valuation of outcomes based on delay tended to be hyperbolic in form (i.e., differentially steep initially and then shallow subsequently)(Mazur 1987), and a mathematical implication of hyperbolic discounting was a preference reversal as access to a smaller immediate reward approached. In other words, although an individual might report preferring a $100 larger later reward in one month over $80 in three weeks, hyperbolic preferences would predict the smaller sooner reward becoming more appealing and being selected as time to receipt approached. Preference reversals are an exemplar of irrational preferences as they violate the assumption of consistency over time, but, more importantly, dynamic inconsistency in intertemporal choice was viewed as a potential model system for self-control failure in addiction (Rachlin and Green 1972; Ainslie 1975; Ainslie 2001). In turn, a prediction from this interpretation was that individuals with addictive disorders would exhibit particularly hyperbolic time preferences, addressed below.

A further point about the operant behavioral economic perspective is that it emphasizes the importance of a ‘molar’ perspective on behavior rather than a ‘molecular’ perspective (Vuchinich and Tucker 1988; Vuchinich 1995). This distinction refers to investigating temporally extended patterns of behavior as opposed to assessments that are only specific to a particular instance. The perspective is influenced by Herrnstein’s matching law to the extent that rates of responding matched rates of reinforcement, not necessarily instances. Also, more broadly, a molar framework seeks to identify behavioral regularities that are final causes of behavior, rather than the efficient causes of a single instance, one that is potentially not be representative. Although it should be acknoweldged that most of the empirical research does not characterize extended temporal sequences of behavior directly, a molar perspective nonetheles informs behavioral economic research on AUDs.

Finally, it is worth noting that the preceding theoretical perspective is entirely distinct from Becker and Murphy’s (1988) theory of rational addiction, which comes from the economic tradition of behavioral economics. Although a full discussion of this approach is beyond the scope of this review, the rational addiction perspective argues that addictive behavior, defined as consumption of goods that increase future consumption of the same good) can be fundamentally understood as being rational behavior in economic terms. This approach is commonly applied in economics, but not in psychology or psychiatry, and has been criticized on a number of its assumptions (Rogeberg 2004).

In summary, a behavioral economic approach to AUDs synthesizes concepts and methods from operant behavioral psychology and microeconomics to understand overconsumption of alcohol and other drugs. This is informed by both conceptual links to choice behavior in normative behavioral economics and an elaboration of the operant behavioral conceptualization of AUDs. In the following sections, three core behavioral economic assays will be introduced: delay discounting, alcohol demand, and proportionate alcohol-related reinforcement. These are not the only behavioral economic measures, but they are the ones that have been most extensively applied to AUDs.

Three Core Behavioral Economic Assays for Understanding Alcohol Use Disorders

Delay Discounting

Delay discounting is a measure of intertemporal choice that characterizes preferences for smaller immediate rewards versus larger delayed rewards (i.e., how much a reward is discounted by the delay to receipt). It is akin to the notion of capacity to delay gratification and is considered a behavioral economic index of impulsivity. Although delay discounting can be assessed for a variety of different commodities and in a number of different formats, the most common strategy is to use money as generalized reinforcer and either iterative permuted decision-making tasks or shorter measures with preconfigured items pertaining to certain levels of discounting. The iterative permuted tasks systematically pose choices between various smaller immediate rewards versus a uniform larger delayed reward across multiple delay periods (e.g., one day, one week, one month, and one year), sometimes using an adaptive format for efficiency. Across trials, an individual’s points of indifference (i.e., the point at which the smaller immediate reward is equal to the larger delayed reward) can be ascertained and an overall temporal discounting function can be generated, reflecting the devaluation gradients illustrated in Figure 2. The most commonly used discounting equation is Mazur’s hyperbolic model (1987), but several alternatives exist (e.g., Green and Myerson 2004; Rachlin 2006) and an atheoretical measure of area under the curve is also widely used (Myerson et al. 2001). Of the measures using preconfigured items, the Monetary Choice Questionnaire (MCQ) (Kirby et al. 1999) is the most widely used (MacKillop et al. 2011). These measures do not permit empirical modeling of the discounting curve, but use the items to infer an estimated discounting function using the hyperbolic model.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Using these various assessments, there is consistent evidence that individuals with AUDs exhibit steeper discounting of future rewards than matched controls (Petry 2001; Bjork et al. 2004; Kirby and Petry 2004; Mitchell et al. 2005). In a meta-analysis of categorical studies, this difference was significant across studies and the effect size was medium (d = .50) for studies using clinical samples and small (d = .26) for studies using subclinical samples (MacKillop et al. 2011). In studies using continuous designs, greater delay discounting has also been found to be significantly associated with drinking quantity and frequency (Field et al. 2007; MacKillop et al. 2010b; Christiansen et al. 2012), with medium effect sizes according to statistical conventions.

Etiological causality cannot be addressed in these cross-sectional studies, but an accumulating body of work suggests that steep discounting precedes AUDs and not the other way around. In retrospective studies, steeper discounting has been associated with earlier onset of AUD symptoms (Kollins 2003; Dom et al. 2006). A number of longitudinal studies have directly addressed delay discounting and drinking over time. For example, one longitudinal study found discounting generally predicted subsequent drinking over the course of adolescence, but drinking did not predict subsequent discounting (Fernie et al. 2013). This parallels a longitudinal study in adolescent smokers (Audrain-McGovern et al. 2009). In a second longitudinal investigation, steeper discounting mediated the predictive relationship between working memory and drinking behavior over adolescence (Khurana et al. 2013). Further evidence for longitudinal links between steep delay discounting and alcohol misuse comes from a series of studies using a naturalistic measure of delay discounting, the documented allocation of discretionary financial resources to alcohol (immediate smaller reward) versus savings (future delayed reward). Specifically, this measure has been found to predict natural resolution of drinking problems among untreated heavy drinking adults (Tucker et al. 2002; Tucker et al. 2006). This relationship was subsequently replicated in an ecological momentary assessment design using interactive voice response (Tucker et al. 2008; Tucker et al. 2012). Also related to etiology, a number of studies have found that family history of alcohol and other drug use are associated with steeper delay discounting (Acheson et al. 2011; Dougherty et al. 2014; VanderBroek et al. 2015), even among drug-naïve early adolescents, suggesting it may be an intermediate process for the risk conferred by family history, genetic and/or environmental.

Of note, a number of the aforementioned studies used the MCQ, for which there is an inherent loss of assessment resolution as a result of using preconfigured items. This is both in terms of characterizing exact participant temporal discounting functions and capacity to test alternative models of discounting. However, despite this, the measure has robustly linked addictive behavior and steep discounting (MacKillop et al. 2011), suggesting it is nonetheless capturing key variability in preferences. In addition, the MCQ has been shown to have robust psychometric properties, including extended test-retest reliability (Kirby 2009).

Alcohol Demand

Demand is a fundamental concept in economics and refers to the amount of a commodity that is sought or consumed at a given price. In this case, alcohol demand is an index of the individual’s value of alcohol as a reinforcer. Methodologically, demand can be assessed via either operant progressive-ratio or fixed-ratio schedules, or behavioral economic purchase tasks. For the former, participants earn access to alcohol via effortful behavior on an experimental manipulandum (e.g., spacebar presses) at varying levels of response requirement. For the latter, participants report how much alcohol (or other drug) they would consume at a variety of prices that start very low and escalate to very high levels; choices are either for entirely hypothetical outcomes or may be for a consequated outcome that the participant actually receives. For both operant schedules and purchase tasks, motivation for alcohol is conceptually measured by the effects on consumption in the context of the imposition of costs, akin to measuring the momentum of an object by quantifying the resistance needed to stop it. More specifically, the reinforcing value of alcohol can be operationalized in both methods using microeconomic demand curve analysis, which formally characterizes the relationship between consumption of a commodity and its cost. Demand curves yield multiple motivational indices and prototypic demand and expenditure curves are presented in Figure 3. These include consumption at minimum price (i.e., intensity), the price that reduces consumption to zero (i.e., breakpoint), maximum expenditure (Omax), and the aggregate slope of the demand curve (i.e., elasticity), which quantifies the degree to which intensity of demand is defended. Most of the preceding can be generated using observed participant responses, but elasticity is typically derived via nonlinear regression of an exponential model of demand (Hursh and Silberberg 2008). This single parameter model has largely replaced earlier two-parameter models (Hursh et al. 1988; Hursh and Winger 1995).

Figure 3.

Figure 3

These indices putatively capture important variability in the value of alcohol as a reinforcer for the individual, both being conceptually related to each other (all reflecting some aspect of alcohol reinforcing value) but also being distinct (reflecting different points on the demand and expenditure curve). Persistently high alcohol demand is theorized to be an important recursive etiological marker for the development of alcohol misuse in emerging adults (MacKillop et al. 2011; Murphy, Correia, et al. 2007; Murphy, MacKillop, et al. 2012). In other words, following initial exposure to alcohol, increasing alcohol involvement with reinforcing consequences is theorized to increase the reinforcing value of alcohol reciprocally, in a feedforward loop. Among recreational drinkers, alcohol reinforcing value theoretically terminates before significant negative consequences, but among individuals with AUDs, alcohol reinforcing value persists and escalates despite the adverse effects of drinking.

Considerable empirical research supports this hypothesis, primarily from studies using the purchase task paradigm. Numerous cross-sectional studies have found robust associations between alcohol demand and alcohol misuse in emerging adults. For example, in categorical comparisons, individuals at higher levels of alcohol misuse exhibit significantly higher demand for alcohol (Murphy and MacKillop 2006; Smith et al. 2010; Teeters et al. 2014; Teeters and Murphy 2015). These relationships are also evident in continuous designs, where indices of alcohol demand are significantly associated with levels of alcohol consumption, symptoms of alcohol dependence, and alcohol-related problems (Murphy et al. 2009; MacKillop et al. 2010a; Smith et al. 2010; Gray and MacKillop 2014a; Bertholet et al. 2015). Indeed, in meta-analyzing continuous associations in cross-sectional studies, indices of alcohol demand were highly significantly associated with alcohol misuse at moderate effect size magnitudes (MacKillop et al. 2015). Demand indices have also been applied to understand common comorbidities between AUDs and other forms of psychopathology. For example, young adult drinkers with depressive or PTSD symptoms exhibit higher alcohol demand (Murphy et al. 2013); in both cases, these relationships putatively reflect augmented alcohol demand as a result of heightened motivation for its negatively reinforcing properties, although that indirect effect has not been demonstrated empirically. Similarly, smokers exhibit elevated alcohol demand (Yurasek et al. 2013), potentially reflecting the known potentiating effects of concurrent alcohol and nicotine consumption (Funk et al. 2006; Doyon et al. 2013). Also of note, the indices of demand from the APT and other purchase tasks have been shown to have good psychometric properties, including test-retest and internal reliability (Murphy et al. 2009; Amlung and MacKillop 2012; Few et al. 2012).

The preceding relationships pertain to a trait-like perspective on alcohol demand (i.e., preferences that are generally stable over time, ceteris paribus), but a second application of alcohol demand has been in augmenting the assessment of state motivation in laboratory studies. Historically, the primary dependent variable in lab studies is subjective craving, which is subject to a number of measurement limitations (Sayette et al. 2000). The use of purchase tasks in lab studies has been intended to translate acute motivation into putatively more objective measures of consumption, expenditure, and price sensitivity. The findings to date have been promising. State-oriented alcohol purchase tasks have revealed acute increases in demand in response to alcohol cues (MacKillop, O’Hagen, et al. 2010) and stress manipulations (Amlung and MacKillop 2014; Owens et al. 2014), and similar findings have been reported regarding motivation for tobacco (MacKillop et al. 2012; Acker and MacKillop 2013) and food (Stojek et al. 2015). Although not using in vivo manipulations, two studies have used manipulations in the instructional sets of alcohol purchase tasks to explore differences in demand based on the presence of psychosocial consequences the following day (e.g., an exam in university drinkers) (Skidmore and Murphy 2011; Gentile et al. 2012). In both cases, these instructional changes were found to substantially affect demand and sensitivity to this manipulation was subsequently linked to the presence of a family history of alcohol (Murphy et al. 2014). Collectively, this work illustrates the viability of examining alcohol demand as assessed via a purchase task as an individual difference measure (effectively an independent variable), an outcome measure (a dependent variable), and as a platform for implementing experimental manipulations.

In the forgoing work, alcohol demand was often referred to as a superordinate construct, generally reflecting alcohol reinforcing value, but it is important to note that the demand indices are considered distinct from one another. It is beyond the scope of this review unpack the specific findings on an index-by-index basis, but, in general, the largest effect sizes in the studies to date have tended to be for intensity and Omax, with somewhat smaller relationships for breakpoint and elasticity (e.g., MacKillop et al. 2015). In terms of interrelationships, the correlations among the indices vary considerably, ranging from negligible to very high (e.g., Murphy and MacKillop 2006; Herschl et al. 2012; Murphy et al. 2013). However, two studies examined the latent structure of these relationships and have found a binary factor structure to the data, with intensity and Omax loading on one factor and elasticity, breakpoint, and Omax loading on the other (Omax loading on both) (MacKillop et al. 2009; Bidwell et al. 2012). These factors were named Amplitude (i.e., maximum amount of alcohol consumed and money spent) and Persistence (i.e., sensitivity to escalating prices), respectively, and broadly scaled to the x and y axes of the demand curve. This suggests that there some natural latent interrelationships among the indices and, based on effect sizes, it appears that the indices comprising Amplitude measures are most salient to AUDs. However, it would be premature to identify any demand indices as being clearly preferable or superior at this point. Indeed, it is unclear if that would ever be desirable as a cardinal advantage of using full alcohol demand curve analysis is the availability of high resolution and precision in describing different aspects of reinforcement.

Proportionate Alcohol-related Reinforcement

The third behavioral economic index that is highly relevant to alcohol misuse is a molar measure of proportionate alcohol-related reinforcement. This is defined as the relative allocation of time and enjoyment that is associated with alcohol use compared to alcohol-free behavior (Murphy et al. 2005; Murphy et al. 2006; Murphy et al. 2007a). High proportionate alcohol reinforcement is theorized to be an indicator of disproportionate reliance on alcohol-related reinforcement compared to alternative reinforcers. This parallels neurobiological allostatic dysregulation models of addiction in which diminished dopamine response to naturally occurring substance-free rewards such as food or pleasant events is viewed as a key feature (Koob and Le Moal 2008; Koob and Volkow 2010; Volkow and Baler 2014; Koob 2015). In practice, it is assessed via the Alcohol-Related Reinforcement Scale (ARRS)(Correia et al. 1998), which quantifies engagement in a variety of different reinforcing activities and the extent to which alcohol is involved in each. On one hand, alcohol use may be high, but restricted to specific contexts and part of an extensive portfolio of reinforcing psychosocial activities; on the other, alcohol may be a part of all reinforcing activities in a person’s life, meaning changing drinking will generally attenuate reinforcing recreational activities. Figure 4 illustrates the nature of the index.

Figure 4.

Figure 4

There is considerable empirical evidence that proportionate alcohol-related reinforcement plays an important role in AUDs. For example, numerous human laboratory studies have shown that substance use decreases if access to alternative reinforcers is increased (Bigelow 2001; Higgins et al. 2004). Similarly, in preclinical models, contexts devoid of substance-free sources of reinforcement have been consistently found to promote high rates of substance use (Carroll et al. 2009). Pertaining to alcohol misuse in young adults, there is evidence that heavy drinkers report less reinforcement from nondrug activities compared to matched controls (Correia et al. 1998; Correia et al. 2003). In continuous designs, alcohol-free reinforcement is significantly negatively associated with alcohol misuse and vice versa (Correia et al. 1998; Correia et al. 2002; MacKillop & Murphy 2007; Murphy et al. 2005; Murphy et al. 2009). Longitudinally, alcohol-related reinforcement has been shown to predict ongoing drinking and problem resolution in middle-aged AUD+ adults (Tucker et al. 1995; Tucker et al. 2002; Vuchinich & Tucker 1996; Tucker et al. 2008; Tucker et al. 2009).

With regard to the overlap between alcohol reinforcing value as measured by alcohol demand and proportionate alcohol-related reinforcement, the two are related but nonetheless distinct. In psychological terms, it can be thought of as the drug’s endogenous motivational significance for the individual. In contrast, proportionate alcohol-related reinforcement is an assay of the individual’s allocation of behavior across diverse environmental reinforcement contingencies. It characterizes the reinforcement context within which drinking takes place. In psychological terms, it is the molar reinforcement context of alcohol use for the individual. In addition to these conceptual differences, several empirical studies indicate that the two are generally quantitatively distinct (Murphy et al. 2009; Skidmore et al. 2014; MacKillop & Murphy 2007).

It is also worth noting that the assessment of proportionate alcohol-related reinforcement is qualitatively different from alcohol demand or delay discounting, which are both measured by tasks that permit the quantitative analysis of behavior. In addition, demand and discounting tasks both permit the assessment of the process of decision making in vivo. In contrast, the ARRS is an entirely retrospective report of activities with and without the presence of alcohol and the associated reinforcement. As such, it is more similar to traditional retrospective self-report inventories than behavioral tasks. To be fair, trait-level alcohol demand often uses the instructional set of drinking during a typical episode, which involves some level of retrospection also, and proportionate alcohol-related reinforcement is quantitatively derived as a ratio rather than being exclusively self-attributions. Nonetheless, proportionate alcohol-related reinforcement as measured by the ARRS is fundamentally a historical measure and there would be value in the development of a task that validly captures the construct (e.g., a concurrent reinforcement schedule pitting alcohol reinforcement against diverse forms of alternative reinforcement).

Clinical Applications

From a behavioral economic theoretical perspective, altering the reinforcement contingencies for drinking is a critical strategy in treating AUDs and this has been directly investigated using the Community Reinforcement Approach (CRA) (Hunt and Azrin 1973) and Contingency Management (CM) (Stitzer and Petry 2006). Given the deleterious consequences of drinking, it is not uncommon for an individual with an AUD to have substantially impoverished reinforcing opportunities beyond drinking. Loss of recreational activities, jobs, and relationships can create a ‘vicious cycle’ of drinking because of dwindling options, until none remain. Based on this, the CRA’s strategy is to restructure the environmental contingencies to increase the value of sobriety via mutually exclusive alternative reinforcers and thereby also increase the costs of drinking. By doing so, the goal is to make abstinence more reinforcing than drinking. In terms of clinical outcomes, initial trials of the CRA were very positive (Hunt and Azrin 1973; Azrin 1976; Meyers and Miller 2001) and these outcomes have subsequently generalized from a traditional adult inpatient sample to homeless individuals with AUDs (Smith et al. 1998) and a young adult AUD sample (Smith et al. 2011). Across studies, the CRA has been found to be efficacious in treating AUDs (Miller and Wilbourne 2002; Roozen et al. 2004) and also other addictive disorders (for a comprehensive review, see Meyers et al. 2011). An adaptation of the CRA for family members has also been developed, termed Community Reinforcement Approach Family Training (CRAFT). The rationale behind CRAFT is that many individuals with AUDs are ambivalent or unmotivated to modify their drinking and family members may benefit from strategies for modifying the reinforcement contingencies around drinking to encourage treatment. Although the number of randomized controlled trials is small, CRAFT outcomes have been positive for both AUDs (Sisson and Azrin 1986; Miller et al. 1999; Dutcher et al. 2009) and other substance use disorders (Meyers et al. 2011).

In contrast to the macrocosmic perspective of the CRA, CM uses incentives to directly reinforce pro-treatment outcomes, such as attendance and sobriety (Stitzer and Petry 2006). From a theoretical standpoint, this strategy increases the opportunity cost of using, thus increasing the reinforcing value of pro-treatment behavior. Given steep delay discounting among individuals with AUDs, an added advantage is that these contingencies are in high temporal proximity. A recent meta-analysis reported CM to be generally efficacious for SUDs (Prendergast et al. 2006). Only two studies of CM have been conducted on AUDs, but both have been supportive (Petry et al. 2000; Petry et al. 2012). A challenge in implementing CM for AUDs is that breath alcohol is the standard biomarker for drinking and is highly transient, but recent studies have used continuous transdermal monitoring systems and ethyl-glucuronide (EtG), a longer-lasting urinary biomarker, to verify contingencies over longer periods of time (Barnett et al. 2011; McDonell et al. 2012).

The third area where progress has been made applying behavioral economic theory in clinical settings is as an enhancement to brief motivational interventions (BMIs) for young adult drinkers. Typical BMIs are single session interventions that provide personalized feedback within a motivational interviewing framework. Several studies have evaluated the addition of a behavioral economic supplement that focuses on reducing the value of alcohol by fostering increases in substance-free reinforcement and increasing the salience of delayed rewards. In an initial within-subjects pilot study, the supplement was associated with robust reductions in drinking (Murphy et al. 2012). Subsequently, in an RCT using an active control group, the supplement was again associated with significantly greater reductions in alcohol problems (Murphy, Dennhardt, et al. 2012). However, a version of the supplement that was attenuated and focused on both alcohol and other drugs has subsequently not been found to enhance drinking outcomes, suggesting the full supplement may be necessary (Yurasek et al. 2015). A promising aspect of this latter study was that the behavioral economic supplement did significantly reduce marijuana use compared to the control condition.

A final notable application of behavioral economics in clinical studies of alcohol is in mechanistic research, either using indices for predicting treatment response or directly examining treatment effects. This work has largely taken place in the context of BMIs for young adult drinkers. Early studies using within-subjects designs reported that both indices of alcohol demand and proportionate alcohol-related reinforcement were significantly associated with intervention response (Murphy et al. 2005; MacKillop & Murphy 2007). In both cases, higher pre-treatment reinforcing value was associated with poorer follow-up drinking. Taking a different tack, one investigation has used indices of alcohol demand to investigate whether one of the mechanisms of naltrexone was via alternations to the reinforcing value of alcohol (Bujarski et al. 2012). In that study, naltrexone did indeed significantly reduce intensity, Omax and breakpoint compared to placebo. More recently, Dennhardt et al. (2015) reported that a BMI acutely reduced alcohol demand (intensity, Omax, elasticity) and the acute reductions in demand were predictive of 6-month outcomes. Similarly, a second study recently reported that two baseline demand indices (intensity and Omax) significantly predicted follow-up drinking and alcohol problems, that these indices exhibited acute reductions in response to the interventions, and that those changes were predictive of intervention response (Murphy et al. 2015). In addition, this study also assessed relative discretionary expenditures on alcohol and found that this index predicted baseline alcohol problems and follow-up drinking, that reductions in expenditure predicted follow-up outcomes. These findings collectively suggest behavioral economic indices are highly promising prognostic variables and mechanisms of behavior change.

The Neuroeconomics of Alcohol Use Disorders

The field of neuroeconomics has existed for just over a decade and started primarily with proof-of-concept studies demonstrating that key decision making phenomena could be characterized at all using neuroimaging (e.g., Sanfey et al. 2003; McClure et al. 2004). Increasingly, neuroeconomic approaches have been applied to clinical populations, including individuals with AUDs. The dominant methodology is functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), which indirectly characterizes neural activity via the blood-oxygenation-level-dependent (BOLD) signal, and the bulk of the empirical neuroeconomic research on AUDs has focused on the neural correlates of delay discounting. In an early case-control study of delay discounting and AUDs, individuals with AUDs exhibited steeper discounting behaviorally and greater neural activity in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC), posterior parietal cortex (PPC), and parahippocampal gyrus (PHG) (Boettiger et al. 2007). Similar findings were present in a subsequent study using the same design, with individuals with AUDs exhibiting greater devaluation of future rewards behaviorally and significantly greater activity in bilateral dlPFC (Amlung et al. 2012). Differences in PPC were not observed in that study, although significantly greater activity in the precuneus and parietal cortex was present. In general, these findings are similar to studies of individuals with methamphetamine and nicotine dependence compared to controls that found significantly greater frontoparietal activity in the addiction criterion groups (Monterosso et al. 2007; Clewett et al. 2014) In contrast, in a study using a continuous design, significant associations were detected between severity of AUD and neural activity in the anterior insula, inferior frontal gyrus, anterior cingulate, cuneus and inferior parietal lobule (Claus et al. 2011). Moving away from region-of-interest-based designs, a recent study examined functional connectivity in individuals with AUDs and controls, finding significantly greater connectivity in salience, default mode, orbitofrontal, and executive networks in the AUD group (Zhu et al. 2015). In addition, in individuals with AUDs, level of delay discounting was significantly associated with connectivity in the executive control, default mode and salience networks. Although only a small number of studies have been conducted, one common theme has been significant differences in frontoparietal regions, suggesting important differences in the interactions between executive faculties and prospective thinking/consciousness. In addition, an interesting theme is that the significant findings to date have revealed greater brain activity and functional connectivity, not the other way around. Although intuition might predict deficits in activity during delay discounting, the studies to date suggest greater activity and functional connectivity, despite more impulsive behavioral output. Although definitive conclusions cannot be drawn given the relatively small number of studies to date, these studies provide potentially important initial insights into this disrupted form of decision making in AUDs.

Beyond delay discounting, one study has investigated the neural correlates of alcohol demand decision making (MacKillop et al. 2014). In that study, male heavy drinkers underwent an extended protocol combining an fMRI scan with a bar laboratory self-administration session. Participants completed an adapted demand paradigm in the scanner, choosing how many drinks they would consume at various prices within a $15 ‘bar tab.’ (Immediately following the scan, the participants received one of their choices and the associated money and alcohol in a bar laboratory.) The study characterized the neural activity associated with three different types of choices: inelastic choices (maximum alcohol consumption preferred across prices), elastic choices (some alcohol consumption preferred, but lower than maximum), and suppressed demand (no alcohol consumption preferred). Decisions to drink in general (inelastic and elastic) were associated with frontoparietal activity, putatively reflecting executive functioning, deliberation, and prospective thinking. Elastic demand decisions, reflecting reductions in preferences but not complete suppression, elicited the largest magnitude differences in activation. These choices were characterized by differential frontostriatal activity and insular activity, appearing to capture the interplay among subjective reward value in the striatum, interoceptive visceral processing via the insula, and the external contingencies represented in dlPFC. In addition, elastic demand drinking decisions were associated with significant deactivation in regions associated with default mode network activity. This apparent DMN suppression appears to reflect the greater cognitive load associated with these decisions. Although this is clearly just one study, it provides proof-of-concept that alcohol demand can be characterized in a neuroimaging environment.

Gaps in Knowledge and Future Directions

The preceding sections reveal the numerous ways behavioral economics and neuroeconomics have been applied to AUDs, but significant gaps in knowledge remain. In the case of the behavioral studies on delay discounting, alcohol demand, and proportionate alcohol reinforcement, a significant limitation is that the vast majority of the work is cross-sectional in nature. In particular, no longitudinal risk studies have leveraged indices of alcohol demand or proportionate alcohol reinforcement, and only a small number of investigations have focused on delay discounting. This leaves unanswered significant scientific questions about whether these processes are causal or consequential, and undermines the unique molar theoretical orientation of behavioral economics. A second gap in the behavioral literature is that although behavioral economic variables appear to reveal novel aspects of acute motivation in laboratory studies, the incremental value of these additional facets remains an open question.

At the clinical end of the spectrum, although behavioral economic interventions are well-supported, the total number of trials on CRA, CM, and augmented BMIs is small and generalizing the efficacy of these approaches to additional populations is a natural step forward. In addition, with the exception of the brief intervention studies, the previous CRA and CM trials have not systematically examined behavioral economic mediators and moderators of treatment effects to address whether the positive effects are for the predicted reasons and whether outcomes systematically differ based on task performance. For example, proportionate alcohol-related reinforcement is an obvious putative mechanism for positive CRA effects, given the program’s theorized influence on non-alcohol related reinforcement. Another future clinical direction is a focus on delay discounting as a novel treatment target. Several recent studies have identified methods for acutely attenuating steep discounting (e.g., Bickel et al. 2011; Radu et al. 2011; Daniel et al. 2013) and, although the generalizability is not clear at this point, these strategies may contribute to improving treatment response. However, a recent review reported evidence of a rate-dependent effect in studies attempting to reduce delay discounting behaviorally (Bickel et al. 2015). This effect was such that the most robust effects were present for the most impulsive individuals, suggesting an individual’s discounting rate itself is a potentially powerful moderating variable.

Substantial gaps in knowledge also remain at the more basic end of the translational spectrum. The neuroimaging studies on delay discounting have provided novel and unique perspectives on the neural correlates of this form of maladaptive decision making, but the literature is largely descriptive, not predictive in terms of change over time or prescriptive in terms of identifying specific forms of neural activity to target. In other words, these approaches need to be integrated into etiological and clinical research more fully. Similarly, the recent study using fMRI to study alcohol demand is not an end unto itself, but rather part of the foundation for future questions, such as differences in neural processing of alcohol demand among individuals with AUDs versus social drinkers or in response to experimental manipulations. Similarly, there is a line of inquiry about delay discounting is whether it is a mechanism by which genetic variation confers risk for addictive disorders (i.e., an endophenotype)(Gottesman and Gould 2003; MacKillop and Munafò 2013). Recent studies using animal models and twin designs have established that delay discounting appears to be relatively robustly heritable (Wilhelm and Mitchell 2009; Anokhin et al. 2011; Richards et al. 2013; Anokhin et al. 2014). In addition, a small number of molecular genetic studies have reported associations between steep discounting and dopamine-related polymorphisms (Boettiger et al. 2007; Eisenberg et al. 2007; Paloyelis et al. 2010; Gray and MacKillop 2014b; MacKillop et al. 2015) and with some inconsistency of findings. This is a nascent area of the field and considerably further progress will be needed to characterize genetic influences on delay discounting and, ultimately, whether discounting mediates the relationship between specific forms of genetic variation and risk for AUDs. It is also possible that alcohol demand may serve as a mechanism linking genetic variation and AUD risk, but this has received only very limited investigation to date (Wahlstrom et al. 2012; Lamb and Daws 2013; Owens et al. 2014).

Conclusions

Considered collectively, behavioral economics and neuroeconomics provide a powerful framework for studying the nature of AUDs. Importing theories and tools from a variety of disciplines, these approaches attempt to conceptualize the problem from a unique vantage point, the value of which is supported by a substantial and robust empirical literature. The body of work to date provides a deep and broad foundation upon which to build and addressing the gaps identified has the potential to make major contributions to understanding AUDs and other addictive disorders.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by NIH Grants R21AA023669, P30 DA027827, and R01 DA032015, and K23 AA016936, and the Peter Boris Chair in Addictions Research. The author is grateful to Tashia Petker for editorial assistance.

Footnotes

Conflicts of Interest: None

References

  1. Acheson A, Vincent AS, Sorocco KH, Lovallo WR. Greater discounting of delayed rewards in young adults with family histories of alcohol and drug use disorders: studies from the Oklahoma family health patterns project. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2011;35:1607–13. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2011.01507.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Acker J, MacKillop J. Behavioral Economic Analysis of Cue-Elicited Craving for Tobacco: A Virtual Reality Study. Nicotine Tob Res. 2013 doi: 10.1093/ntr/nts341. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Ainslie G. Specious reward: a behavioral theory of impulsiveness and impulse control. Psychol Bull. 1975;82:463–496. doi: 10.1037/h0076860. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Ainslie G. Breakdown of Will. Cambridge University Press; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  5. Amlung M, MacKillop J. Understanding the effects of stress and alcohol cues on motivation for alcohol via behavioral economics. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2014;38:1780–1789. doi: 10.1111/acer.12423. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Amlung M, MacKillop J. Consistency of self-reported alcohol consumption on randomized and sequential alcohol purchase tasks. Front psychiatry. 2012;3:65. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2012.00065. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Amlung M, Sweet LH, Acker J, et al. Dissociable brain signatures of choice conflict and immediate reward preferences in alcohol use disorders. Addict Biol. 2012 doi: 10.1111/adb.12017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Anokhin AP, Golosheykin S, Grant JD, Heath AC. Heritability of delay discounting in adolescence: a longitudinal twin study. Behav Genet. 2011;41:175–183. doi: 10.1007/s10519-010-9384-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Anokhin AP, Grant JD, Mulligan RC, Heath AC. The Genetics of Impulsivity: Evidence for the Heritability of Delay Discounting. Biol Psychiatry. 2014 doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.10.022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Ariely D. Predictably Irrational, Revised and Expanded Edition: The Hidden Forces That Shape Our Decisions, First. Exp. Harper Perennial; New York, NY US: 2010. [Google Scholar]
  11. Audrain-McGovern J, Rodriguez D, Epstein LH, et al. Does delay discounting play an etiological role in smoking or is it a consequence of smoking? Drug Alcohol Depend. 2009;103:99–106. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2008.12.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Azrin NH. Improvements in the community-reinforcement approach to alcoholism. Behav Res Ther. 1976;14:339–348. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(76)90021-8. doi: 0005-7967(76)90021-8 [pii] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Barnett NP, Tidey J, Murphy JG, et al. Contingency management for alcohol use reduction: a pilot study using a transdermal alcohol sensor. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2011;118:391–9. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2011.04.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Becker G, Murphy K. A theory of rational addiction 1988 [Google Scholar]
  15. Bertholet N, Murphy JG, Daeppen J-B, et al. The alcohol purchase task in young men from the general population. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2015;146:39–44. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2014.10.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Bickel WK, DeGrandpre RJ, Higgins ST. Behavioral economics: A novel experimental approach to the study of drug dependence. Drug Alcohol Depend. 1993;33:173–192. doi: 10.1016/0376-8716(93)90059-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Bickel WK, Quisenberry AJ, Moody L, Wilson AG. Therapeutic Opportunities for Self-Control Repair in Addiction and Related Disorders: Change and the Limits of Change in Trans-Disease Processes. Clin Psychol Sci a J Assoc Psychol Sci. 2015;3:140–153. doi: 10.1177/2167702614541260. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Bickel WK, Vuchinich RE. Reframing health behavior change with behavioral economics 2003 [Google Scholar]
  19. Bickel WK, Yi R, Landes RD, et al. Remember the future: working memory training decreases delay discounting among stimulant addicts. Biol Psychiatry. 2011;69:260–265. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.08.017. doi: S0006-3223(10)00852-8 [pii] 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.08.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Bidwell LC, MacKillop J, Murphy JG, et al. Latent factor structure of a behavioral economic cigarette demand curve in adolescent smokers. Addict Behav. 2012;37:1257–1263. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2012.06.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Bigelow G. An operant behavioral analysis of alcohol abuse and dependence. In: Heather N, Peters TJ, Stockwell T, editors. International Handbook of Alcohol Dependence and Problems. John Wiley & Sons; Chichester, UK: 2001a. pp. 299–315. [Google Scholar]
  22. Bigelow G, Cohen M, Liebson I, Faillace LA. Abstinence or moderation? Choice by alcoholics. Behav Res Ther. 1972;10:209–214. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(72)90037-x. doi: 0005-7967(72)90037-X [pii] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Bigelow GE. An Operant Behavioral Perspective on Alcohol Abuse and Dependence. In: Heather N, Peters TJ, Stockwell T, editors. International Handbook of Alcohol Dependence and Problems. John Wiley & Sons, LTD; Chicester: 2001b. pp. 299–315. [Google Scholar]
  24. Bjork JM, Hommer DW, Grant SJ, Danube C. Impulsivity in abstinent alcohol-dependent patients: relation to control subjects and type 1-/type 2-like traits. Alcohol. 2004;34:133–150. doi: 10.1016/j.alcohol.2004.06.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Boettiger CA, Mitchell JM, Tavares VC, et al. Immediate reward bias in humans: fronto-parietal networks and a role for the catechol-O-methyltransferase 158(Val/Val) genotype. J Neurosci. 2007;27:14383–14391. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2551-07.2007. doi: 27/52/14383 [pii] 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2551-07.2007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Bujarski S, MacKillop J, Ray LA. Understanding Naltrexone Mechanism of Action and Pharmacogenetics in Asian Americans via Behavioral Economics: A Preliminary Study. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2012 doi: 10.1037/a0027379. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Camerer C. Behavioral economics: reunifying psychology and economics. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 1999;96:10575–10577. doi: 10.1073/pnas.96.19.10575. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Camerer C, Loewenstein G, Rabin M. Advances in Behavioral Economics 2004 [Google Scholar]
  29. Carey KB, Correia CJ. Drinking motives predict alcohol-related problems in college students. J Stud Alcohol. 1997;58:100–105. doi: 10.15288/jsa.1997.58.100. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Carroll ME, Anker JJ, Perry JL. Modeling risk factors for nicotine and other drug abuse in the preclinical laboratory. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2009;104(Suppl):S70–8. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2008.11.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Christiansen P, Cole JC, Goudie AJ, Field M. Components of behavioural impulsivity and automatic cue approach predict unique variance in hazardous drinking. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2012;219:501–10. doi: 10.1007/s00213-011-2396-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Chung SH, Herrnstein RJ. Choice and delay of reinforcement. J Exp Anal Behav. 1967;10:67–74. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1967.10-67. 10, 67–74. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Claus ED, Kiehl KA, Hutchison KE. Neural and behavioral mechanisms of impulsive choice in alcohol use disorder. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2011;35:1209–19. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-0277.2011.01455.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Clewett D, Luo S, Hsu E, et al. Increased functional coupling between the left fronto-parietal network and anterior insula predicts steeper delay discounting in smokers. Hum Brain Mapp. 2014;35:3774–87. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22436. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Corbin WR, Iwamoto DK, Fromme K. Broad social motives, alcohol use, and related problems: Mechanisms of risk from high school through college. Addict Behav. 2011;36:222–230. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2010.11.004. doi: S0306-4603(10)00323-0 [pii] 10.1016/j.addbeh.2010.11.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Correia CJ, Carey KB, Borsari B. Measuring substance-free and substance-related reinforcement in the natural environment. Psychol Addict Behav. 2002;16:28–34. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Correia CJ, Carey KB, Simons J, Borsari BE. Relationships between binge drinking and substance-free reinforcement in a sample of college students: a preliminary investigation. Addict Behav. 2003;28:361–368. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4603(01)00229-5. doi: S0306460301002295 [pii] [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Correia CJ, Simons J, Carey KB, Borsari BE. Predicting drug use: application of behavioral theories of choice. Addict Behav. 1998;23:705–709. doi: S0306-4603(98)00027-6 [pii] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Daniel TO, Stanton CM, Epstein LH. The future is now: Comparing the effect of episodic future thinking on impulsivity in lean and obese individuals. Appetite. 2013;71:120–125. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2013.07.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Darkes J, Greenbaum PE, Goldman MS. Alcohol expectancy mediation of biopsychosocial risk: complex patterns of mediation. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2004;12:27–38. doi: 10.1037/1064-1297.12.1.27. doi: 10.1037/1064-1297.12.1.27 2004-10475-007 [pii] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Dennhardt AA, Yurasek AM, Murphy JG. Change in delay discounting and substance reward value following a brief alcohol and drug use intervention. J Exp Anal Behav. 2015;103:125–40. doi: 10.1002/jeab.121. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Dom G, D’Haene P, Hulstijn W, Sabbe B. Impulsivity in abstinent early- and late-onset alcoholics: differences in self-report measures and a discounting task. Addiction. 2006;101:50–59. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2005.01270.x. doi: ADD1270 [pii] 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2005.01270.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Dougherty DM, Charles NE, Mathias CW, et al. Delay discounting differentiates pre-adolescents at high and low risk for substance use disorders based on family history. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2014;143:105–111. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2014.07.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Doyon WM, Thomas AM, Ostroumov A, et al. Potential substrates for nicotine and alcohol interactions: a focus on the mesocorticolimbic dopamine system. Biochem Pharmacol. 2013;86:1181–93. doi: 10.1016/j.bcp.2013.07.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Dutcher LW, Anderson R, Moore M, et al. Community reinforcement and family training (CRAFT): An effectiveness study. J Behav Anal Health Sports Fit Med. 2009;2:80–93. doi: 10.1037/h0100373. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Eisenberg DTA, MacKillop J, Modi M, et al. Examining impulsivity as an endophenotype using a behavioral approach: A DRD2 TaqI A and DRD4 48-bp VNTR association study. Behav Brain Funct. 2007 doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-3-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Fernie G, Peeters M, Gullo MJ, et al. Multiple behavioural impulsivity tasks predict prospective alcohol involvement in adolescents. Addiction. 2013;108:1916–23. doi: 10.1111/add.12283. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Few LR, Acker J, Murphy C, MacKillop J. Temporal stability of a cigarette purchase task. Nicotine Tob Res. 2012;14:761–5. doi: 10.1093/ntr/ntr222. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Field M, Christiansen P, Cole J, Goudie A. Delay discounting and the alcohol Stroop in heavy drinking adolescents. Addiction. 2007;102:579–586. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2007.01743.x. doi: ADD1743 [pii] 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2007.01743.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Funk D, Marinelli PW, Lê AD. Biological processes underlying co-use of alcohol and nicotine: neuronal mechanisms, cross-tolerance, and genetic factors. Alcohol Res Health. 2006;29:186–92. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Gentile ND, Librizzi EH, Martinetti MP. Academic constraints on alcohol consumption in college students: a behavioral economic analysis. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2012;20:390–9. doi: 10.1037/a0029665. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Glimcher PW, Camerer C, Fehr T, Poldrack RA. Neuroeconomics: Decision Making and the Brain. Academic Press; London: 2009. [Google Scholar]
  53. Gottesman II, Gould TD. The endophenotype concept in psychiatry: etymology and strategic intentions. Am J Psychiatry. 2003;160:636–645. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.4.636. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Gray J, MacKillop J. Interrelationships among Individual Differences in Alcohol Demand, Impulsivity, and Alcohol Misuse. Psychol Addict Behav. 2014a;28:282–287. doi: 10.1037/a0032766. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Gray JC, MacKillop J. Genetic basis of delay discounting in frequent gamblers: examination of a priori candidates and exploration of a panel of dopamine-related loci. Brain Behav. 2014b doi: 10.1002/brb3.284. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Green L, Myerson J. A Discounting Framework for Choice With Delayed and Probabilistic Rewards. Psychol Bull. 2004;130:769–792. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.130.5.769. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Griffiths RR, Bigelow G, Liebson I. Comparison of social time-out and activity time-out procedures in supressing ethanol self-administration in alcoholics. Behav Res Ther. 1977;15:329–336. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(77)90064-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Herrnstein RJ. Relative and absolute strength of response as a function of frequency of reinforcement. J Exp Anal Behav. 1961;4:267–272. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1961.4-267. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Herrnstein RJ. On the law of effect. J Exp Anal Behav. 1970;13:243–266. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1970.13-243. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Herschl LC, McChargue DE, MacKillop J, et al. Implicit and explicit alcohol-related motivations among college binge drinkers. Psychopharmacol. 2012;221:685–692. doi: 10.1007/s00213-011-2613-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Higgins ST, Heil SH, Lussier JP. Clinical implications of reinforcement as a determinant of substance use disorders. Annu Rev Psychol. 2004;55:431–461. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.142033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Hunt GM, Azrin NH. A community-reinforcement approach to alcoholism. Behav Res Ther. 1973;11:91–104. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(73)90072-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Hursh SR. Behavioral economics. J Exp Anal Behav. 1984;42:435–452. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1984.42-435. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Hursh SR. Behavioral economics of drug self-administration: an introduction. Drug Alcohol Depend. 1993;33:165–172. doi: 10.1016/0376-8716(93)90058-x. doi: 0376-8716(93)90058-X [pii] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Hursh SR. Economic concepts for the analysis of behavior. J Exp Anal Behav. 1980;34:219–238. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1980.34-219. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Hursh SR, Galuska CM, Winger G, Woods JH. The economics of drug abuse: a quantitative assessment of drug demand. Mol Interv. 2005;5:20–28. doi: 10.1124/mi.5.1.6. doi: 5/1/20 [pii] 10.1124/mi.5.1.6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Hursh SR, Raslear TG, Shurtleff D, et al. A cost-benefit analysis of demand for food. J Exp Anal Behav. 1988;50:419–440. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1988.50-419. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Hursh SR, Silberberg A. Economic demand and essential value. Psychol Rev. 2008;115:186–198. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.115.1.186. doi: 2008-00265-008 [pii] 10.1037/0033-295X.115.1.186. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Hursh SR, Winger G. Normalized demand for drugs and other reinforcers. J Exp Anal Behav. 1995;64:373–384. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1995.64-373. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Kahneman D. A perspective on judgment and choice: Mapping bounded rationality. Am Psychol. 2003;58:697–720. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.58.9.697. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Khurana A, Romer D, Betancourt LM, et al. Working memory ability predicts trajectories of early alcohol use in adolescents: the mediational role of impulsivity. Addiction. 2013;108:506–15. doi: 10.1111/add.12001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Kirby KN. One-year temporal stability of delay-discount rates. Psychon Bull Rev. 2009;16:457–462. doi: 10.3758/PBR.16.3.457. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Kirby KN, Petry NM. Heroin and cocaine abusers have higher discount rates for delayed rewards than alcoholics or non-drug-using controls. Addiction. 2004;99:461–471. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2003.00669.x. 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2003.00669.x ADD669 [pii] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Kirby KN, Petry NM, Bickel WK. Heroin addicts have higher discount rates for delayed rewards than non-drug-using controls. J Exp Psychol Gen. 1999;128:78–87. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.128.1.78. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Kollins SH. Delay discounting is associated with substance use in college students. Addict Behav. 2003;28:1167–1173. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4603(02)00220-4. doi: S0306460302002204 [pii] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Koob GF. The dark side of emotion: the addiction perspective. Eur J Pharmacol. 2015;753:73–87. doi: 10.1016/j.ejphar.2014.11.044. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Koob GF, Le Moal M. Addiction and the brain antireward system. Annu Rev Psychol. 2008;59:29–53. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093548. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Koob GF, Volkow ND. Neurocircuitry of addiction. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35:217–238. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.110. doi: npp2009110 [pii] 10.1038/npp.2009.110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Kuntsche E, Stewart SH, Cooper ML. How stable is the motive-alcohol use link? A cross-national validation of the Drinking Motives Questionnaire Revised among adolescents from Switzerland, Canada, and the United States. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 2008;69:388–96. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2008.69.388. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Lamb RJ, Daws LC. Ethanol self-administration in serotonin transporter knockout mice: unconstrained demand and elasticity. Genes Brain Behav. 2013;12:741–7. doi: 10.1111/gbb.12068. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. MacKillop J, Amlung MT, Acker J, et al. The neuroeconomics of alcohol demand: an initial investigation of the neural correlates of alcohol cost-benefit decision making in heavy drinking men. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2014;39:1988–95. doi: 10.1038/npp.2014.47. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. MacKillop J, Amlung MT, Few LR, et al. Delayed reward discounting and addictive behavior: A meta-analysis. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2011a;216:305–321. doi: 10.1007/s00213-011-2229-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. MacKillop J, Amlung MT, Murphy CM, et al. A Behavioral Economic Approach to Health Behavior. In: DiClemente R, Crosby LFS RA, editors. Theory and Practice for a New Public Health. Jones and Bartlett Publishers, LLC; 2011b. pp. 131–162. [Google Scholar]
  84. MacKillop J, Brown CL, Stojek MK, et al. Behavioral economic analysis of withdrawal- and cue-elicited craving for tobacco: an initial investigation. Nicotine Tob Res. 2012;14:1426–34. doi: 10.1093/ntr/nts006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. MacKillop J, Gray JC, Bidwell LC, et al. Genetic influences on delay discounting in smokers: examination of a priori candidates and exploration of dopamine-related haplotypes. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2015;232:3731–9. doi: 10.1007/s00213-015-4029-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. MacKillop J, Miranda RJ, Monti PM, et al. Alcohol demand, delayed reward discounting, and craving in relation to drinking and alcohol use disorders. J Abnorm Psychol. 2010a;119:115–125. doi: 10.1037/a0017513. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. MacKillop J, Miranda R, Monti PM, et al. Alcohol demand, delayed reward discounting, and craving in relation to drinking and alcohol use disorders. J Abnorm Psychol. 2010b;119:106–14. doi: 10.1037/a0017513. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. MacKillop J, Munafò M. An intermediate phenotype approach to addiction genetics. MIT Press; Cambridge, MA, US: 2013. [Google Scholar]
  89. MacKillop J, Murphy JG. A behavioral economic measure of demand for alcohol predicts brief intervention outcomes. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2007a;89:227–233. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2007.01.002. doi: S0376-8716(07)00022-1 [pii] 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2007.01.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. MacKillop J, Murphy JG. A behavioral economic measure of demand for alcohol predicts brief intervention outcomes. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2007b;89:227–33. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2007.01.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. MacKillop J, Murphy JG, Tidey JW, et al. Latent structure of facets of alcohol reinforcement from a behavioral economic demand curve. Psychopharmacol. 2009;203:33–40. doi: 10.1007/s00213-008-1367-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. MacKillop J, O’Hagen S, Lisman SA, et al. Behavioral economic analysis of cue-elicited craving for alcohol. Addiction. 2010c;105:1599–1607. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2010.03004.x. doi: ADD3004 [pii] 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2010.03004.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. MacKillop J, Jackson JN, Murphy JG, Amlung MT. Associations between individual differences in alcohol’s relative reinforcing value and alcohol misuse: A meta-analysis. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2015;39:42A. doi: 10.1111/acer.12741. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  94. Mazur JE. An adjusting procedure for studying delayed reinforcement. In: Commons ML, Mazur JE, Nevin JA, editors. The Effect of Delay and of Intervening Event on Reinforcement Value. Quantitative Analyses of Behavior. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc; Hillsdale, NJ: 1987. pp. 55–73. [Google Scholar]
  95. McClure SM, Laibson DI, Loewenstein G, Cohen JD. Separate neural systems value immediate and delayed monetary rewards. Science. 2004;306:503–7. doi: 10.1126/science.1100907. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. McDonell MG, Howell DN, McPherson S, et al. Voucher-based reinforcement for alcohol abstinence using the ethyl-glucuronide alcohol biomarker. J Appl Behav Anal. 2012;45:161–5. doi: 10.1901/jaba.2012.45-161. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Mello NK, Mendelson JH. Operant Analysis of Drinking Patterns of Chronic Alcoholics. Nature. 1965;206:43–46. doi: 10.1038/206043a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Mendelson JH, Mello NK. Experimental analysis of drinking behavior of chronic alcoholics. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1966;133:828–845. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1966.tb50930.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Meyers RJ, Miller WR. A community reinforcement approach to addiction treatment. Cambridge University Press; New York, NY: 2001. [Google Scholar]
  100. Meyers RJ, Roozen HG, Smith JE. The community reinforcement approach: an update of the evidence. Alcohol Res Health. 2011;33:380–8. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Miller WR, Meyers RJ, Tonigan JS. Engaging the unmotivated in treatment for alcohol problems: a comparison of three strategies for intervention through family members. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1999;67:688–697. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.67.5.688. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Miller WR, Wilbourne PL. Mesa Grande: a methodological analysis of clinical trials of treatments for alcohol use disorders. Addiction. 2002;97:265–277. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.2002.00019.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Mitchell JM, Fields HL, D’Esposito M, Boettiger CA. Impulsive responding in alcoholics. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2005;29:2158–2169. doi: 10.1097/01.alc.0000191755.63639.4a. doi: 00000374-200512000-00010 [pii] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Monterosso JR, Ainslie G, Xu J, et al. Frontoparietal cortical activity of methamphetamine-dependent and comparison subjects performing a delay discounting task. Hum Brain Mapp. 2007;28:383–393. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20281. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Murphy JG, Barnett NP, Colby SM. Alcohol-related and alcohol-free activity participation and enjoyment among college students: a behavioral theories of choice analysis. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2006;14:339–349. doi: 10.1037/1064-1297.14.3.339. doi: 2006-10166-008 [pii] 10.1037/1064-1297.14.3.339. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Murphy JG, Barnett NP, Goldstein AL, Colby SM. Gender moderates the relationship between substance-free activity enjoyment and alcohol use. Psychol Addict Behav. 2007a;21:261–265. doi: 10.1037/0893-164X.21.2.261. doi: 2007-08148-018 [pii] 10.1037/0893-164X.21.2.261. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Murphy JG, Correia CJ, Barnett NP. Behavioral economic approaches to reduce college student drinking. Addict Behav. 2007b;32:2573–2585. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2007.05.015. doi: S0306-4603(07)00156-6 [pii] 10.1016/j.addbeh.2007.05.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Murphy JG, Correia CJ, Colby SM, Vuchinich RE. Using behavioral theories of choice to predict drinking outcomes following a brief intervention. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2005;13:93–101. doi: 10.1037/1064-1297.13.2.93. doi: 2005-05782-002 [pii] 10.1037/1064-1297.13.2.93. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Murphy JG, Dennhardt AA, Skidmore JR, et al. A randomized controlled trial of a behavioral economic supplement to brief motivational interventions for college drinking. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2012a;80:876–886. doi: 10.1037/a0028763. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. Murphy JG, Dennhardt AA, Yurasek AM, et al. Behavioral Economic Predictors of Brief Alcohol Intervention Outcomes. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2015 doi: 10.1037/ccp0000032. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Murphy JG, MacKillop J. Relative reinforcing efficacy of alcohol among college student drinkers. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2006;14:219–227. doi: 10.1037/1064-1297.14.2.219. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Murphy JG, MacKillop J, Skidmore JR, Pederson AA. Reliability and validity of a demand curve measure of alcohol reinforcement. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2009;17:396–404. doi: 10.1037/a0017684. doi: 2009-23091-004 [pii] 10.1037/a0017684. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. Murphy JG, MacKillop J, Vuchinich RE, Tucker JA. Behavioral Economic Concepts in the Analysis of Substance Abuse. In: Walters S, Rotgers F, M J, editors. Treating Substance Abuse, Theory and Technique. Guilford Press; New York: 2012b. [Google Scholar]
  114. Murphy JG, Skidmore JR, Dennhardt AA, et al. A behavioral economic supplement to brief motivational interventions for college drinking. Addict Res Theory. 2012c;20:456–465. doi: 10.3109/16066359.2012.665965. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Murphy JG, Yurasek AM, Dennhardt AA, et al. Symptoms of depression and PTSD are associated with elevated alcohol demand. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2013;127:129–36. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2012.06.022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Murphy JG, Yurasek AM, Meshesha LZ, et al. Family history of problem drinking is associated with less sensitivity of alcohol demand to a next-day responsibility. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 2014;75:653–63. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2014.75.653. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Myerson J, Green L, Warusawitharana M. Area under the curve as a measure of discounting. J Exp Anal Behav. 2001;76:235–243. doi: 10.1901/jeab.2001.76-235. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  118. Nathan PE, Titler NA, Lowenstein LM, et al. Behavioral analysis of chronic alcoholism. Interaction of alcohol and human contact. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1970;22:419–430. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1970.01740290035005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  119. Owens MM, Ray LA, MacKillop J. Behavioral economic analysis of stress effects on acute motivation for alcohol. J Exp Anal Behav. 2014 doi: 10.1002/jeab.114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. Paloyelis Y, Asherson P, Mehta MA, et al. DAT1 and COMT effects on delay discounting and trait impulsivity in male adolescents with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder and healthy controls. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2010;35:2414–2426. doi: 10.1038/npp.2010.124. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  121. Petry NM. Delay discounting of money and alcohol in actively using alcoholics, currently abstinent alcoholics, and controls. Psychopharmacol. 2001;154:243–250. doi: 10.1007/s002130000638. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  122. Petry NM, Alessi SM, Ledgerwood DM. A randomized trial of contingency management delivered by community therapists. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2012;80:286–298. doi: 10.1037/a0026826. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. Petry NM, Martin B, Cooney JL, Kranzler HR. Give them prizes and they will come: Contingency management for treatment of alcohol dependence. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2000;68:250–257. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.68.2.250. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  124. Prendergast M, Podus D, Finney J, et al. Contingency management for treatment of substance use disorders: a meta-analysis. Addiction. 2006;101:1546–60. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2006.01581.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  125. Rachlin H. Notes on discounting. J Exp Anal Behav. 2006;85:425–35. doi: 10.1901/jeab.2006.85-05. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  126. Rachlin H, Green L. Commitment, choice and self-control. J Exp Anal Behav. 1972;17:15–22. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1972.17-15. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  127. Radu PT, Yi R, Bickel WK, et al. A mechanism for reducing delay discounting behavior by altering temporal attention. J Exp Anal Behav. 2011;96:363–385. doi: 10.1901/jeab.2011.96-363. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  128. Richards JB, Lloyd DR, Kuehlewind B, et al. Strong genetic influences on measures of behavioral-regulation among inbred rat strains. Genes Brain Behav. 2013;12:490–502. doi: 10.1111/gbb.12050. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  129. Rogeberg O. Taking Absurd Theories Seriously: Economics and the Case of Rational Addiction Theories. Philos Sci. 2004;71:263–285. [Google Scholar]
  130. Roozen HG, Boulogne JJ, van Tulder MW, et al. A systematic review of the effectiveness of the community reinforcement approach in alcohol, cocaine and opioid addiction. Drug Alcohol Depend. 2004;74:1–13. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2003.12.006. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2003.12.006 S0376871603003405 [pii] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  131. Sanders RM, Nathan PE, O’Brien JS. The performance of adult alcoholics working for alcohol: A detailed operant analysis. Br J Addict to Alcohol Other Drugs. 1976;71:307–319. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.1976.tb00101.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  132. Sanfey AG, Rilling JK, Aronson JA, et al. The neural basis of economic decision-making in the Ultimatum Game. Science (80-) 2003;300:1755–1758. doi: 10.1126/science.1082976. doi: 10.1126/science.1082976 300/5626/1755 [pii] [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  133. Sayette MA, Shiffman S, Tiffany ST, et al. The measurement of drug craving. Addiction. 2000;95(Suppl 2):S189–210. doi: 10.1080/09652140050111762. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  134. Sisson RW, Azrin NH. Family-member involvement to initiate and promote treatment of problem drinkers. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. 1986;17:15–21. doi: 10.1016/0005-7916(86)90005-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  135. Skidmore JR, Murphy JG. The effect of drink price and next-day responsibilities on college student drinking: a behavioral economic analysis. Psychol Addict Behav. 2011;25:57–68. doi: 10.1037/a0021118. doi: 2010-25606-001 [pii] 10.1037/a0021118. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  136. Skidmore JR, Murphy JG, Martens MP. Behavioral economic measures of alcohol reward value as problem severity indicators in college students. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2014;22:198–210. doi: 10.1037/a0036490. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  137. Smith AE, Martens MP, Murphy JG, et al. Reinforcing efficacy moderates the relationship between impulsivity-related traits and alcohol use. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2010;18:521–529. doi: 10.1037/a0021585. doi: 2010-26426-007 [pii] 10.1037/a0021585. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  138. Smith DC, Godley SH, Godley MD, Dennis ML. Adolescent Community Reinforcement Approach outcomes differ among emerging adults and adolescents. J Subst Abuse Treat. 2011;41:422–30. doi: 10.1016/j.jsat.2011.06.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  139. Smith JE, Meyers RJ, Delaney HD. The community reinforcement approach with homeless alcohol-dependent individuals. J Consult Clin Psychol. 1998;66:541–548. doi: 10.1037//0022-006x.66.3.541. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  140. Stitzer M, Petry N. Contingency management for treatment of substance abuse. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2006;2:411–434. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.2.022305.095219. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  141. Stojek MK, Fischer S, MacKillop J. Stress, cues, and eating behavior. Using drug addiction paradigms to understand motivation for food. Appetite. 2015;92:252–60. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2015.05.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  142. Teeters JB, Murphy JG. The behavioral economics of driving after drinking among college drinkers. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2015;39:896–904. doi: 10.1111/acer.12695. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  143. Teeters JB, Pickover AM, Dennhardt AA, et al. Elevated alcohol demand is associated with driving after drinking among college student binge drinkers. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2014;38:2066–72. doi: 10.1111/acer.12448. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  144. Tucker JA, Foushee HR, Black BC. Behavioral economic analysis of natural resolution of drinking problems using IVR self-monitoring. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2008a;16:332–340. doi: 10.1037/a0012834. doi: 2008-10619-008 [pii] 10.1037/a0012834. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  145. Tucker JA, Foushee HR, Black BC. Behavioral economic analysis of natural resolution of drinking problems using IVR self-monitoring. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2008b;16:332–340. doi: 10.1037/a0012834. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  146. Tucker JA, Roth DL, Huang J, et al. Effects of interactive voice response self-monitoring on natural resolution of drinking problems: utilization and behavioral economic factors. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 2012;73:686–698. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2012.73.686. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  147. Tucker JA, Roth DL, Vignolo MJ, Westfall AO. A behavioral economic reward index predicts drinking resolutions: moderation revisited and compared with other outcomes. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2009;77:219–228. doi: 10.1037/a0014968. doi: 2009-03774-003 [pii] 10.1037/a0014968. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  148. Tucker JA, Vuchinich RE, Black BC, Rippens PD. Significance of a behavioral economic index of reward value in predicting drinking problem resolution. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2006;74:317–326. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.74.2.317. doi: 2006-05351-012 [pii] 10.1037/0022-006X.74.2.317. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  149. Tucker JA, Vuchinich RE, Pukish MA. Molar environmental events surrounding recovery from alcohol problems in treated and untreated problem drinkers. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 1995;3:195–204. [Google Scholar]
  150. Tucker JA, Vuchinich RE, Rippens PD. Predicting natural resolution of alcohol-related problems: a prospective behavioral economic analysis. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2002;10:248–257. doi: 10.1037//1064-1297.10.3.248. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  151. VanderBroek L, Acker J, Palmer AA, et al. Interrelationships among parental family history of substance misuse, delay discounting, and personal substance use. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2015 doi: 10.1007/s00213-015-4074-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  152. Volkow ND, Baler RD. Addiction science: Uncovering neurobiological complexity. Neuropharmacology. 2014;76(Pt B):235–49. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2013.05.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  153. Vuchinich RE. Alcohol abuse as molar choice: An update of a 1982 proposal. Psychol Addict Behav. 1995;9:223–235. doi: 10.1037/0893-164x.9.4.223. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  154. Vuchinich RE, Heather N. Choice, Behavioral Economics and Addiction. 2003;438 [Google Scholar]
  155. Vuchinich RE, Tucker JA. Contributions from behavioral theories of choice to an analysis of alcohol abuse. J Abnorm Psychol. 1988;97:181–195. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.97.2.181. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  156. Vuchinich RE, Tucker JA. Alcoholic relapse, life events, and behavioral theories of choice: A prospective analysis. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 1996;4:19–28. [Google Scholar]
  157. Wahlstrom LC, McChargue DE, Mackillop J. DRD2/ANKK1 TaqI A genotype moderates the relationship between alexithymia and the relative value of alcohol among male college binge drinkers. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2012;102:471–476. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2012.06.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  158. Wilhelm CJ, Mitchell SH. Strain differences in delay discounting using inbred rats. Genes Brain Behav. 2009;8:426–34. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2009.00484.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  159. Yurasek AM, Dennhardt AA, Murphy JG. A randomized controlled trial of a behavioral economic intervention for alcohol and marijuana use. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2015;23:332–8. doi: 10.1037/pha0000025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  160. Yurasek AM, Murphy JG, Clawson AH, et al. Smokers Report Greater Demand for Alcohol on a Behavioral Economic Purchase Task. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 2013;74:626. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2013.74.626. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  161. Zak PJ. Neuroeconomics. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2004;359:1737–1748. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2004.1544. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  162. Zhu X, Cortes CR, Mathur K, et al. Model-free functional connectivity and impulsivity correlates of alcohol dependence: a resting-state study. Addict Biol. 2015 doi: 10.1111/adb.12272. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES