Abstract
The goal of the present study was to evaluate the dependence of perceptual learning gains on initial visual acuity (VA), in a large sample of subjects with a wide range of VAs. A large sample of normally sighted and presbyopic subjects (N = 119; aged 40 to 63) with a wide range of uncorrected near visual acuities (VA, −0.12 to 0.8 LogMAR), underwent perceptual learning. Training consisted of detecting briefly presented Gabor stimuli under spatial and temporal masking conditions. Consistent with previous findings, perceptual learning induced a significant improvement in near VA and reading speed under conditions of limited exposure duration. Our results show that the improvements in VA and reading speed observed following perceptual learning are closely linked to the initial VA, with only a minor fraction of the observed improvement that may be attributed to the additional sessions performed by those with the worse VA.
Presbyopia results from a gradual decrease in accommodative power during normal ageing1, leading to reduced near visual acuity and contrast sensitivity2,3,4 and slower visual processing speed5,6. In addition, visual performance, measured as percent of correct responses for a constant stimulus level or sensitivity (d-prime) is reduced under crowded conditions in the fovea of presbyopic subjects7,8,9. Thus, presbyopia negatively affects the quality of vision for near tasks such as reading. Indeed, near VA declines by about one line (0.1 log unit) on an ETDRS letter chart roughly every five years after the age of 30 years9, imposing constraints on near vision even in pre-presbyopic subjects. Because contrast is important for driving neural responses in the visual cortex, blurred input results in weaker and slower neuronal responses in the visual cortex3,10.
Visual performance on a variety of tasks benefits from practice, in both young6,8,11 and older adults4,8,12,13,14, resulting in long-term improvements. These long-term effects of repeated practice on a demanding task are termed perceptual learning, for recent reviews, see15,16,17. Visual performance has been shown to improve following practice under crowded conditions, both in normal peripheral vision18,19,20,21,22 and in the amblyopic fovea22,23,24,25,26,27, for a review see28. The specificity of the training effects depends on the amount of training29, and the training gains after numerous trials generalize to untrained stimulus parameters, such as untrained spatial separations between stimuli30,31, for a review, see32.
Perceptual learning using a detection paradigm with flankers that facilitate collinear lateral interactions33,34,35,36,37,38 has been shown to improve foveal visual acuity in young normally sighted adults6,8, and in adults with amblyopia3,25,26,39. Similar training, combined with backward masking, resulted in shorter processing times needed for target detection in young normally sighted adults, as indicated by the shorter latency of a specific neuronal marker40. A similar training paradigm also resulted in robust improvements in near VA in adults with presbyopia, with no changes in the optics of the eye4. Importantly, the gains following training on near-detection-threshold Gabor stimuli generalized to improved performance on high contrast letter stimuli4,6,8.
The goal of the present study was to evaluate the dependency of perceptual learning gains on initial VA, in a large sample of subjects with a wide range of VAs. In uncorrected presbyopia, it is widely acknowledged that the main challenge is in reading small fonts that are beyond the functional reading acuity under the conditions of blur and illumination associated with it. The standard clinical measurements extracted from visual acuity charts reflect acuity for static widely spaced letters. Similarly, the MNREAD chart41 provides information relating to the maximal reading speed and its corresponding acuity for font sizes much larger than the effective minimal font size a subject is able to read. Therefore, such standard chart measurements may not be sensitive enough to fully reflect either the real functional performance of a normally sighted adult or the reduction in performance produced by presbyopia. Thus, in addition to the standard measures, we evaluated reading speed with the minimal readable font size on the MNREAD chart.
Consistent with earlier findings4,6,8,27, perceptual learning induced a significant improvement in near VA, proportional to the initial VA, with higher gains observed for more advanced presbyopia. Moreover, the transfer of perceptual learning to untrained tasks resulted in improved reading speed, with the gain closely linked to the initial VA as well.
Results
Consistent with our earlier findings4,6,8, there was a significant improvement in VA following training (Fig. 1a,b, the main effect of training (F1,111 = 320.5, p < 0.001; VA of 0.313 ± 0.002 at pretest, mean ± se; VA of 0.134 ± 0.001 at posttest).
The improvement in visual acuity was significantly correlated with the initial VA (r = 0.63, p < 0.0001). Subjects with more advanced presbyopia and thus poorer initial VA showed the greatest improvement in VA (Fig. 1b; interaction: group × training, F7,111 = 8.7, p < 0.001). This pattern of results shows that improvements after perceptual learning are dependent on the initial acuity26,42. The main effects of the device type covariate and its interaction with training were both insignificant. The main effects of the session length covariate and its interaction with training were both insignificant.
The improvement in VA was not simply a result of learning to take the test. To evaluate the effect of test-retest reliability, a control group took the pretest and the posttest with no perceptual learning in between (N = 10, age 53 ± 8.75, years old, mean ± SD, ranging between 43 to 68 years old, with VA of 0.17 ± 0.08 for first testing and 0.18 ± 0.08 for the second testing, mean ± se, ranging between −0.12 to 0.58 LogMAR, thus matching the main group in both age range and VA range). This test-retest control group was included in a separate repeated measures ANOVA with a between-subject factor of paradigm (2 levels: with perceptual learning and without). There was a significant effect of perceptual learning as opposed to a simple test-retest effect, evident from the significant interaction between training and paradigm (F1, 127 = 23.7, p < 0.001, also see Supplementary information). The effect size measurements indicate there was a large effect of training and its interaction with the initial VA, whereas there was a very small effect of the interaction between training and the device type (partial eta squared values are: 0.31 for the main effect of training, 0.355 for the training × group interaction, and 0.004 for the training × device type interaction and 0.016 for the training × session length interaction). There was a large effect of perceptual learning as opposed to a simple test-retest (partial eta squared value of 0.157 for the training × paradigm interaction).
A subgroup of 63 subjects (age 48.7 ± 3.4 years old, mean ± SD, ranging between 40 and 58, with initial VA 0.14 ± 0.02 LogMAR, mean ± se, ranging between −0.12 and 0.58 LogMAR) underwent a reading performance measurement before and after training, using an MNREAD chart41. Consistent with our earlier findings4, there was a significant improvement in the reading performance following the training (Fig. 2). On average, reading speed for the minimal font measured at pretest improved by 50%, from 52 ± 2.8 to 77 ± 4.2 words per minute (wpm), gaining 25 wpm (Fig. 3; F1,60 = 40.61, p < 0.001). For the non-presbyopic group specifically (N = 20, initial VA ranging between −0.12 to 0 LogMAR), reading speed improved by 26%, from 53 ± 3.8 to 67 ± 5.8 wpm, gaining 14 wpm, measured for the letter size of 0.02 ± 0.02 LogMAR, mean ± se. For early presbyopia (N = 21, initial VA ranging between 0.06 to 0.18 LogMAR), reading speed improved by 43%, from 55 ± 5.7 to 77 ± 6 wpm, gaining 22 wpm, measured for the letter size of 0.17 ± 0.02 LogMAR, mean ± se. For advanced presbyopia (N = 22, initial VA ranging between 0.2 to 0.58 LogMAR), reading speed improved by 81%, from 47 ± 4.8 to 85 ± 9 wpm, gaining 38 wpm, measured for the letter size of 0.23 ± 0.03 LogMAR, mean ± se. The improvement was also correlated with the initial VA (r = 0.33, p = 0.008), with higher gains observed for those with poorer initial VA (Fig. 2c) and advanced presbyopia levels (Fig. 3; interaction: group × training, F2,60 = 3.34, p = 0.042). These results show that, similar to the VA gains, improvement following training in reading speed was also dependent on the initial VA. The poorer the initial VA, the greater the improvement in reading speed.
Discussion
Consistent with earlier findings4,6,8,27, perceptual learning induced a significant improvement in near VA, which was dependent on initial VA, with higher gains observed for those with poorer initial VA and more advanced presbyopia. Similar to the VA gains, improvement following training in reading speed was also correlated with the initial VA.
Our results suggest that the improvements following perceptual learning are tied to the initial VA threshold in a lawful way, with higher gains observed for more advanced presbyopia. Interestingly, our data also show very clearly that the improvement is on average 0.18 ± 0.014 LogMAR, independent of the final (post-training) acuity (Fig. 4a). This finding suggests that there may be a fundamental limit (based on optics), and that what is learned is to more efficiently interpret the blurred image - the benefit is that the improvement is a constant fraction (≈a factor of 1.5) of the post training acuity. These results are reminiscent of recent work in normal young observers suggesting that the improvement following perceptual learning is a constant proportion of the initial performance - analogous to Weber’s law42. Our results and those of Li, Klein and Levi and of Polat26,43 in amblyopic subjects suggest that the worse the initial performance, the greater the improvement. However, the Weber’s law behavior suggested by Astle et al. implies that the improvement as a function of initial performance should be constant on a log scale, whereas we found that the improvement (in LogMAR units), was not constant – rather the worse the initial performance, the greater the improvement (in LogMAR units). Surprisingly, we find that the improvement (in LogMAR units), is a constant fraction of the post training acuity (Fig. 4a). We suspect that one reason for the discrepancy is that the training in both the current study and that of Li was prolonged, enabling the subjects to reach asymptotic performance, whereas the Astle study used a fixed length of training (10 sessions). Indeed, in our study the algorithm adjusts the training program length automatically according the initial VA and the progress rate, thus the number of training sessions ranged between 25 and 65 and is correlated with the initial VA (r = 0.39, p < 0.001, Fig. 4b).
The fact that those participants with worse initial VA performed more sessions, may suggest the observed greater improvement in these subjects be attributed to the increased number of training sessions per se. This may be supported by the study of Li, Klein and Levi43 that used “kilo” trials for inducing further improvements. However, here, and as shown by Li and colleagues, the improvement on the training task levels reaches a plateau, which was used as an indication for termination of the training. Thus, we believe that only a minor fraction of the observed improvement (with low marginally significant correlation, r = 0.18, p = 0.05, Fig. 4c) may be attributed to the additional sessions performed by those with the worse VA.
Our results suggest that the gold-standard initial chart VA measurements are useful in assessing the outcome of perceptual learning for near visual acuity and reading speed. On the other hand, static clinical VA may not fully capture the visual losses that occur in presbyopia in a real-life dynamic environment, nor the expected gains in visual performance produced by perceptual learning. Elsewhere we have shown that more efficient temporal and spatial processing following perceptual learning results in faster processing speed and in higher contrast sensitivity3,6.
The finding that improvements are closely linked to their initial VA threshold in a lawful way might be taken to suggest that the same factors that impose limits on a visual threshold also constrain learning on a visual task42. On the other hand, subjects with the poorest performance initially have the most room to improve, while those with the best initial performance may be close to the limit of their performance on the task.
Methods
We used a previously described structured perceptual learning treatment method for improving visual functions3,4,6,8,40. Subjects were trained on contrast detection of Gabor targets under backward masking and crowding conditions, imposing both spatial and temporal constraints on the visual processing. The training covered a range of spatial frequencies and orientations that were modified in accordance with the performance improvement (training procedure is described in detail below).
Subjects were trained at a distance of 40 cm with both eyes open. All visual acuity and reading speed measurements were performed before (pre-test) and after (post-test) the treatment. Subjects practiced for at least 3 sessions of about 12–30 min per week over 2 to 4 months. The algorithm adjusts the training program length automatically according the initial VA and the progress rate, thus the number of training sessions ranged from 25 to 65.
Participants
A total of 119 of normally sighted and presbyopic subjects with no ocular pathology (age 49.28 ± 4.42 years old, mean ± SD, ranging between 40 and 63), with initial uncorrected near VA of 0.313 ± 0.002 LogMAR, mean ± se, ranging between −0.12 and 0.8 LogMAR) volunteered to participate in the study. Ten additional subjects served as controls, (age 52.95 ± 8.75 years old, mean ± SD, ranging between 43 and 68) with initial uncorrected near VA of 0.174 ± 0.07 LogMAR, (mean ± se, ranging between −0.12 and 0.58 LogMAR), participating in pre-and post-testing roughly 2 months apart, but with no intervening training. Thirty of the participants were tested at the University of California, Berkeley, the rest of participants were tested in Israel. Subjects were tested with their habitual distance correction and without any optical correction for near distance. The procedures were approved by the ethics committee of the UC Berkeley IRB and Assuta Medical Center. All participants gave informed written consent to participate in the study. All experimental protocols were performed in accordance with the guidelines provided by the committee approving the experiments.
Apparatus
Stimuli for the training sessions (Training procedure, see next) were displayed on either a PC (compaq presario CQ62-215DX Notebook) with effective screen resolution of 1366 × 768 pixels at a 60 Hz refresh rate, effective monitor size of 15.6″ (20 × 34 cm) subtending a visual angle of 28.07 × 46.05 degrees, or a mobile iOS device with a color monitor (iPhone and iPod). The effective iOS screen resolution was 640 by 640 pixels (5 cm in diameter), with a pixel size of 0.078 mm (“retina” display, which corresponds to VA of −0.18 LogMAR) and a refresh rate of 60 Hz. The mean display luminance was set to the maximal level (120 cd/m2) in an otherwise dark environment. The Gabor size was maintained constant across devices by appropriate scaling using the device pixel size as a reference.
Measurements before and after training
A complete eye examination was performed at both pre-test and post-test. The procedures were similar to those of our earlier study9. The exam included objective and subjective refraction, ETDRS acuity at a viewing distance of 40 cm, all made while wearing habitual optical correction for far distance and without any optical correction for near distance. All near-acuity measurements were made without any optical correction for the near distance. Standard static near VA was measured using a near ETDRS chart (Precision Vision) from 40 cm, attached to a stand to maintain the exact distance. The ETDRS chart is composed of lines containing 5 letters each with size increments of 0.1 log units and spacing of 1 letter. The measurements were performed in a single session by the same experienced, certified optometrist in the same room under standardized fixed light conditions using 2 variations of the same chart (to prevent participants from memorizing the order of the letters). The termination rules were based on a forced-choice paradigm and testing continued until the participant made a complete line of errors, or read all the letters on the chart44. The charts smallest font size is equal to VA of −0.3 LogMAR.
The MNREAD acuity charts are continuous-text reading acuity charts, which are used for subjects with normal and low vision41,45. These charts, developed for Low-Vision research, can be used to measure reading acuity and speed. The repeatability of MNREAD charts was demonstrated earlier in a study on 30 normal young adults (mean age, 23.3 years), including testing of the variability at different test distances (40 and 52 cm)46. The results of the repeated (i.e., test-retest) measurements show repeatability ±0.05 LogMAR for reading acuity and ±8.6 words per minute (wpm) for reading speed. Thus, the MNREAD acuity charts can be used to reliably measure reading acuity, reading speed, and critical print size in a normal adult population. When needed (i.e., for the participants tested in Israel), the MNREAD chart was converted into Hebrew using the original instructions by the author of the MNREAD chart, Prof. G. Legge, maintaining the characteristics of the English version. The task was to read aloud one sentence at a time (black print on white background), as the sentences were uncovered one by one from large to small print. The observers were asked to read as fast and as accurately as possible. Reading time and number of errors made for each sentence were recorded on a score sheet and were converted to reading speed in words per minute by the method described in the test instructions. Viewing distance was fixed at 40 cm.
Training procedure
The paradigm in this study is similar to that used in our earlier studies involving both young and presbyopic participants4,6,8,40 in terms of behavioral tasks and temporal parameters. A perceptual learning method for improving visual functions (using the GlassesOff application) was adapted individually for each user by setting the training parameters according to the user’s performance. Subjects who practiced on PC were trained according to the procedure presented in detail elsewhere4, whereas subjects who practiced on iOS devices followed a similar procedure presented in detail in another earlier publication8. Participants were trained on contrast detection and discrimination (on mobile devices) of Gabor targets under spatial masking, temporal masking, and spatial crowding conditions, in which spatial and temporal constraints were imposed on the visual processing. The training covered a range of spatial frequencies (2–8 cpd; the size of the Gabor patches ranged from 0.18 to 1 degrees) and included 4 orientations (0, 45, 90, and 135 degrees) that were modified in accordance with the improved performance: every 2 sessions for the orientation and when after reaching a predefined criterion of contrast detection threshold for the spatial frequency. The PC and mobile device screens had an image processing rate of 8 bits precision per color channel (256 unique grayscale tones). Under these specifications the smallest luminance increment at mean luminance is greater than 1% (for typical display gamma values ≥1.8)47. We have limited the lowest contrast one could reach to be above 1%, reaching this limit terminated the staircase. Moreover, we were using stimulus composed of Gabor patches and presbyopic subjects, therefore, contrast thresholds received during the training were always higher than this minimal available contrast.
Participants were instructed to train in a dark environment from a distance of 40 cm with both eyes open, while wearing their habitual far distance correction. The algorithm adjusted the stimulus parameters automatically and continuously according the initial VA and the progress rate. Participants performed between 25 and 65 training sessions, on different days, with each session lasting about 12 min (30 min for the subjects in UC Berkeley), with at least 3 sessions per week. At the beginning of training, the number of training sessions was determined by the initial VA, however it was shortened if the performance was better than initially assessed and vice versa. For all participants, training was stopped after performance improvement saturation, however with a minimum of 25 training sessions. The trial number for the participants that used iOS devices varied from ~5250 to 13650 trials (~210 trials per session for 25 to 60 sessions) with an average of ~9240 trials, depending on the individual progress. The trial number for the participants that used PC varied from ~8750 to 21000 trials (350 trials per session) with an average of 13000 trials depending on the personal progress. All of our subjects trained on the same procedure/tasks with two exceptions, the initial target spatial frequency which was determined according to the initial VA and the number of repetitions on some of the tasks that was varied according to the user’s performance. Auditory and visual feedback were provided after each trial. The experimental conditions were as follows: 1) A single Gabor target of a specific spatial frequency; 2) A Gabor target masked by two high-contrast (60%) collinear Gabor flankers with several target-flanker distances; 3) crowding: a Gabor patch masked by multiple (between 4 and 11) collinear or non-collinear iso-oriented flankers, with several target-flanker distances; and 4) temporal masking: backward masking applied the conditions above, composed of either flankers or the crowding condition, presented with varying time intervals (inter-stimulus intervals, ISI) of 68, 83, 116, or 150 msec. The duration of the mask was similar to the target stimulus presentation duration (34, 68, and 116 msec). The ISI, duration that the target and flanking Gabors were presented, their orientation, and spatial frequency were modified between sessions, one parameter at a time, according to the preceding session’s performance.
All measurements and training sessions were conducted without near distance optical corrections. Near visual acuity was measured before and after training. All subjects trained at their homes, with their compliance both monitored online using a back-office client with automatic reminders and notifications and by the study authors.
Sample Characteristic
The data were tested for normality using the SPSS software (IBM Corp. Released 2013. IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 22.0. Armonk, NY: IBM Corp). For each of the 16 dependent variables (i.e., per 8 acuity levels in pretest and posttest). A Shapiro-Wilk’s test (P > 0.05)48,49 and a visual inspection of the normal Q-Q plots showed that the acuity values were approximately normally distributed, for all acuity levels, both at pretest and posttest, with a skewness and kurtosis z-values within the range of ±1.96 (Table 1)50,51,52. Skewness of 0.826 and 0.836 and kurtosis of −0.78 and −0.035 for first and second testing, respectfully.
Table 1. Data normality testing.
VA levels (LogMAR) |
|||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
up to 0 | 0–0.1 | 0.1–0.2 | 0.2–0.3 | 0.3–0.4 | 0.4–0.5 | 0.5–0.6 | 0.6–0.8 | ||
Pretest | Skewness | −1.87 | 0.30 | 0.42 | −0.98 | −0.27 | 0.06 | −0.07 | 1.37 |
Kurtosis | 1.31 | −1.40 | −0.92 | −1.06 | −0.85 | −0.93 | −1.10 | 0.19 | |
Posttest | Skewness | −1.48 | 0.10 | −0.19 | −1.21 | 0.61 | −0.63 | −1.06 | 0.31 |
Kurtosis | 0.76 | −0.28 | −1.14 | 0.10 | −1.14 | −0.38 | −0.90 | −0.37 |
Skewness and kurtosis z-values are presented separately for the 8 acuity levels, at pretest and posttest. All z-values in the table fall within the range of ±1.96.
Data analysis
VA measurements were divided into 8 levels: bins of 0.1 LogMAR, ranging between below 0 and 0.8 LogMAR. SPSS repeated measures ANOVA with one within subject and one between subjects effect was performed, with a within-subject factor of training (2 levels: before and after perceptual learning) and a between-subject factor of initial VA, denoted as “group” (8 levels). The Huynh-Feldt Epsilon correction was applied. The device type was included as a covariate (3 levels: iPhone, iPod and PC). The session length was included as a covariate (2 levels: 12 and 30 minutes). MNREAD data were divided into 3 groups: non-presbyopia, early presbyopia and advanced presbyopia. We performed SPSS repeated measures ANOVA with one within subject and one between subjects effect, with a within-subject factor of training (2 levels: before and after perceptual learning) and a between-subject factor of initial VA (3 levels). The Huynh-Feldt Epsilon correction was applied.
Pearson correlation with a two-tailed probability values was used for all reported correlations.
Additional Information
How to cite this article: Yehezkel, O. et al. Gains following perceptual learning are closely linked to the initial visual acuity. Sci. Rep. 6, 25188; doi: 10.1038/srep25188 (2016).
Supplementary Material
Footnotes
U.P.’s work has been funded by GlassesOff, Inc. He has received compensation as a consultant and as a member of the scientific advisory board and owns stock in the company. O.Y., A.S., M.L. are GlassesOff, Inc. employees. D.L. declares no financial interests.
Author Contributions O.Y., A.S., M.L., D.L. and U.P. designed the study and analyzed the data, O.Y. and D.L. prepared the figures, O.Y., A.S., U.P. and D.L. were involved in writing the main manuscript text. All authors reviewed the manuscript.
References
- Koretz J. F., Kaufman P. L., Neider M. W. & Goeckner P. A. Accommodation and presbyopia in the human eye-aging of the anterior segment. Vision Res 29, 1685–1692 (1989). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holden B. A. et al. Global vision impairment due to uncorrected presbyopia. Arch Ophthalmol 126, 1731–1739, 10.1001/archopht.126.12.1731 (2008). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Polat U. Making perceptual learning practical to improve visual functions. Vision Res 49, 2566–2573, 10.1016/j.visres.2009.06.005 (2009). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Polat U. et al. Training the brain to overcome the effect of aging on the human eye. Sci. Rep 2, 278, 10.1038/srep00278 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Owsley C. Visual processing speed. Vision Res 90, 52–56, 10.1016/j.visres.2012.11.014 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lev M. et al. Training improves visual processing speed and generalizes to untrained functions. Sci. Rep. 4, 4067 10.1038/srep07251 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lev M., Yehezkel O. & Polat U. Uncovering foveal crowding? Sci. Rep 4, 4067, 10.1038/srep04067 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yehezkel O., Sterkin A., Lev M. & Polat U. Training on spatiotemporal masking improves crowded and uncrowded visual acuity. J Vis 15, 12, 10.1167/15.6.12 (2015). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yehezkel O., Sterkin A., Lev M. & Polat U. Crowding is proportional to visual acuity in young and aging eyes J Vis 15, 23, 10.1167/15.8.23 (2015). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Albrecht D. G. & Hamilton D. B. Striate cortex of monkey and cat: contrast response function. J Neurophysiol 48, 217–237 (1982). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tan D. T. & Fong A. Efficacy of neural vision therapy to enhance contrast sensitivity function and visual acuity in low myopia. J Cataract Refract Surg 34, 570–577 (2008). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andersen G. J., Ni R., Bower J. D. & Watanabe T. Perceptual learning, aging, and improved visual performance in early stages of visual processing. J Vis 10, 4, 10.1167/10.13.4 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bower J. D., Watanabe T. & Andersen G. J. Perceptual learning and aging: improved performance for low-contrast motion discrimination. Frontiers in psychology 4, 66, 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00066 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- DeLoss D. J., Watanabe T. & Andersen G. J. Improving vision among older adults: behavioral training to improve sight Psychol Sci 26, 456–466, 10.1177/0956797614567510 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fahle M. Perceptual learning: gain without pain? Nat Neurosci 5, 923–924 (2002). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sasaki Y., Nanez J. E. & Watanabe T. Advances in visual perceptual learning and plasticity. Nat Rev Neurosci 11, 53–60, 10.1038/nrn2737 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sagi D. Perceptual learning in Vision Research. Vision Res 51, 1552–1566, 10.1016/j.visres.2010.10.019 (2011). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bernard J. B., Arunkumar A. & Chung S. T. Can reading-specific training stimuli improve the effect of perceptual learning on peripheral reading speed? Vision Res 66, 17–25, 10.1016/j.visres.2012.06.012 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chung S. T. Learning to identify crowded letters: does it improve reading speed? Vision Res 47, 3150–3159, 10.1016/j.visres.2007.08.017 (2007). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chung S. T. Improving reading speed for people with central vision loss through perceptual learning. Invest ophth vis sci 52, 1164–1170, 10.1167/iovs.10-6034 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maniglia M. et al. Reducing crowding by weakening inhibitory lateral interactions in the periphery with perceptual learning. Plos one 6, e25568, 10.1371/journal.pone.0025568 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hussain Z., Webb B. S., Astle A. T. & McGraw P. V. Perceptual learning reduces crowding in amblyopia and in the normal periphery. J neurosci 32, 474–480, 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3845-11.2012 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levi D. M. & Klein S. A. Vernier acuity, crowding and amblyopia. Vision Res 25, 979–991 (1985). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li R. W., Provost A. & Levi D. M. Extended perceptual learning results in substantial recovery of positional acuity and visual acuity in juvenile amblyopia. Invest ophth vis sci 48, 5046–5051, 10.1167/iovs.07-0324 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Polat U., Ma-Naim T., Belkin M. & Sagi D. Improving vision in adult amblyopia by perceptual learning. P Natl Acadf Sci USA 101, 6692–6697 (2004). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Polat U. Restoration of underdeveloped cortical functions: evidence from treatment of adult amblyopia. Restor Neurol Neuros 26, 413–424 (2008). [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhou Y. et al. Perceptual learning improves contrast sensitivity and visual acuity in adults with anisometropic amblyopia. Vision Res 46, 739–750 (2006). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levi D. M. & Li R. W. Perceptual learning as a potential treatment for amblyopia: a mini-review. Vision Res 49, 2535–2549, 10.1016/j.visres.2009.02.010 (2009). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huckauf A. & Nazir T. A. How odgcrnwi becomes crowding: stimulus-specific learning reduces crowding. J Vis 7, 18 11–12, 10.1167/7.2.18 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chung S. T., Li R. W. & Levi D. M. Crowding between first- and second-order letter stimuli in normal foveal and peripheral vision. J Vis 7, 10 11–13, 10.1167/7.2.10 (2007). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chung S. T., Li R. W. & Levi D. M. Learning to identify near-acuity letters, either with or without flankers, results in improved letter size and spacing limits in adults with amblyopia. Plos one 7, e35829, 10.1371/journal.pone.0035829 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levi D. M. Crowding–an essential bottleneck for object recognition: a mini-review. Vision Res 48, 635–654, 10.1016/j.visres.2007.12.009 (2008). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Polat U. & Sagi D. Lateral interactions between spatial channels: suppression and facilitation revealed by lateral masking experiments. Vision Res 33, 993–999 (1993). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Polat U. & Sagi D. The architecture of perceptual spatial interactions. Vision Res 34, 73–78 (1994). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Polat U., Mizobe K., Pettet M. W., Kasamatsu T. & Norcia A. M. Collinear stimuli regulate visual responses depending on cell’s contrast threshold. Nature 391, 580–584 (1998). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Polat U. Functional architecture of long-range perceptual interactions. Spat. Vis. 12, 143–162 (1999). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kasamatsu T., Polat U., Pettet M. W. & Norcia A. M. Colinear facilitation promotes reliability of single-cell responses in cat striate cortex. Exp brain R 138, 163–172 (2001). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Levi D. M., Klein S. A. & Hariharan S. Suppressive and facilitatory spatial interactions in foveal vision: foveal crowding is simple contrast masking. J Vis 2, 140–166, 10:1167/2.2.2 (2002). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lev M. et al. Training-induced recovery of low-level vision followed by mid-level perceptual improvements in developmental object and face agnosia. Dev Sci, 10.1111/desc.12178 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sterkin A., Yehezkel O. & Polat U. Learning to be fast: Gain accuracy with speed. Vision Res 61, 115–124, 10.1016/j.visres.2011.09.015 (2012). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mansfield J. S., Ahn S. J., Legge G. E. & Luebker A. A new reading-acuity chart for normal and low vision. Ophthalmic. Vis Opt. 3, 232–235(1993). [Google Scholar]
- Astle A. T., Li R. W., Webb B. S., Levi D. M. & McGraw P. V. A Weber-like law for perceptual learning. Sci. Rep 3, 1158, 10.1038/srep01158 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li R. W., Klein S. A. & Levi D. M. Prolonged perceptual learning of positional acuity in adult amblyopia: perceptual template retuning dynamics. J neurosc 28, 14223–14229, 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4271-08.2008 (2008). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaiser P. K. Prospective evaluation of visual acuity assessment: a comparison of snellen versus ETDRS charts in clinical practice (An AOS Thesis). Trans Am Ophthal Soc 107, 311–324 (2009). [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brussee T., van Nispen R. M. & van Rens G. H. Measurement properties of continuous text reading performance tests. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt 34, 636–657, 10.1111/opo.12158 (2014). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Subramanian A. & Pardhan S. The repeatability of MNREAD acuity charts and variability at different test distances. Optometry vision sci 83, 572–576, 10.1097/01.opx.0000232225.00311.53 (2006). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dorr M., Lesmes L. A., Lu Z. L. & Bex P. J. Rapid and reliable assessment of the contrast sensitivity function on an iPad. Invest ophth vis sci 54, 7266–7273, 10.1167/iovs.13-11743 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shapiro S. S. & M. B. W. An analysis of variance test for normality (complete samples). Biometrika 52, 591–611 (1965). [Google Scholar]
- Razali N. M. & Wah Y. B. Power comparisons of shapiro-wilk, kolmogorov-smirnov, lilliefors and anderson-darling tests. Journal of Statistical Modeling and Analytics. 2(1), 21–33 (2011). [Google Scholar]
- Cramer D. In Fundamental statistics for social research: step-by-step calculations and computer techniques using SPSS for Windows (Routledge, London, 1998). [Google Scholar]
- Cramer D. & Howitt D. L. In the SAGA dictionary of statistics: A practical resource for students in the social sciences (SAGE, London, 2004). [Google Scholar]
- Doane D. P. & Seward L. E. Measuring skewness: a forgotten statistic. Journal of Statistics Education. 19(2), 1–18 (2011). [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.