Skip to main content
BMC Medical Research Methodology logoLink to BMC Medical Research Methodology
. 2016 Apr 29;16:48. doi: 10.1186/s12874-016-0148-9

The validity of socioeconomic status measures among adolescents based on self-reported information about parents occupations, FAS and perceived SES; implication for health related quality of life studies

P Svedberg 1, J M Nygren 1, C Staland-Nyman 1, M Nyholm 1,
PMCID: PMC4850630  PMID: 27130331

Abstract

Background

Research has shown inconsistencies in results and difficulties in conceptualization of assessment of socioeconomic status (SES) among adolescents. The aim of this study was thus to test the validity of self-reported information on SES in two age-groups (11–13 and 14–16 years old) in an adolescent population and to evaluate its relationship to self-reported health related quality of life (HRQOL). Different measures of SES commonly used in research in relation to HRQOL were tested in this study; parent’s occupations status, family material affluence status (FAS) and perceived SES.

Method

A cross-sectional study, with a sample of 948 respondents (n = 467, 11–13 years old and n = 481, 14–16 years old) completed questionnaires about SES and HRQOL. The adolescents’ completion rates were used, with chi2-test, to investigate differences between gender and age-group. Correlation was used for convergent validity and ANOVA for concurrent validity.

Results

We found a low completion rate for both fathers’ (41.7 %) and mothers' (37.5 %) occupation status, and a difference in completion rate between gender and age-groups. FAS had the highest completion rate (100 %) compared to parent's occupations status and perceived SES. The convergent validity between the SES-indicators was weak (Spearman correlation coefficient below 0.3), suggesting that the indicators measured different dimensions of SES. Both FAS and perceived SES showed a gradient in mean HRQOL between low and high SES in relation to HRQOL, this was significant only for perceived SES (p < 0.01, both age-groups).

Conclusion

This study indicates the need for considering different approaches to measures of SES among adolescences and when evaluating SES in relation to HRQOL. Further research is needed to investigate sustainable ways to measure SES, delineating the relevance of tangible measures of education, occupation and income in relation to the perceived socioeconomic status in comparison with others in immediate social networks and in society at large.

Keywords: Adolescents, Completion rate, Concurrent validity, Convergent validity health related Quality of Life (HRQOL), Measurement, Socioeconomic status

Background

Socioeconomic status (SES) is an important determinant of physical, psychological and social development and of inequalities in health related quality of life (HRQOL) among adolescents [16]. Adolescents’ with low socioeconomic status are more likely to have poorer health outcomes compared to adolescents from groups with higher socioeconomic status [710]. Research has shown that SES among adolescent differ between age and gender [11, 12] but the SES gradient in health outcomes among adolescents’ is inconsistent [13]. Among adults, the socioeconomic-gradient in HRQOL is well established, despite using different measures of SES ([1315], Goodman et al. 2001). This inconsistency in SES gradient in health outcomes among adolescents could be explained by methodological and measurement issues [10].

Self-reported SES among adolescents’ is not commonly included in health and medical research. Instead the relationship between SES and HRQOL is often derived from SES measures based on parents’ education and occupation [7, 1618], with information collected by parents or adolescent themselves. However, parents’ education and parents’ occupation as measures of SES have been debated as not being suitable proxies for self-reported SES among adolescents [4, 15, 19, 20]. This is due to potential disagreement between adolescents and proxy-respondents’ report of education and occupation, high level of non-responders among adolescents’ and hence uncertainties about the validity and meaning of the measurements [4, 15, 19]. Another approach is to measure family wealth and material affluence as a marker for socioeconomic status [9, 21, 22]. The Family Affluence Scale (FAS) measures material affluence and contains quantitative questions about the ownership of computers and cars, family holidays and having a private room in their home, which is aggregated to attain a sum score. This FAS-score has been shown to be associated with the parental social class [22]. Another way to measure SES among adolescents is by using the subjective ratings of social status, i.e. their perceived position within the social economic hierarchy within the population or community. Subjective SES can be measured through questions about adolescents’ perception of their family’s socio-economical, financial or social status [10, 23]. The most common measurements are using a 10-point ladder (for example MacArthur Scale) or a Likert scale (for example HBSC) to investigate subjective SES [13, 24, 25]. Measuring the subjective SES of adolescents’ seems to be less dependent on the use of specific measurements or scales, instead similar results has been yielded in relation to health outcomes [10]. There is a need for more research regarding the correlation between different types of SES measurements in adolescents and the validity of each of these instruments in order to increase the understanding about the pathways between SES and HRQOL The aim of this study was to explore the validity of different types of measurements of self-reported information on SES in two age-groups (11–13 and 14–16 years old) in a Swedish adolescence population. Furthermore, we investigated how these SES measures related to levels of mean self-reported HRQOL.

Methods

Study population

This cross-sectional study is based on data from the Halmstad Youth Quality of Life cohort collected in 2011 in Sweden. The sampling frame included pupils from all municipal schools (n = 7) that were centrally located in the city of Halmstad and each having a total of more than 100 pupils. In total 50 classes were invited to participate in the study, resulting in a sample of 24 classes with pupils 11–13 years old (n = 536 pupils, younger age group) and 25 classes with pupils 14–16 years old (n = 576 pupils, older age group). One class (14–16 years) decided not to participate. A sample of 948 respondents (n = 467 11–13 years old and n = 481 14–16 years old) completed the questionnaires yielding a response was rate of 87 and 84 % respectively (non-respondents were almost exclusively due to absence from school during the data collection). Adolescents answered a self-report questionnaire consisting of the Minneapolis Manchester Quality of Life instrument, (MMQL) and questions about socioeconomic status.

The principal at each school approved participation in the study. Prior to the data collection, the schools distributed written information to children and their parents about the purpose of the research and that participation was voluntary and that if the children or the parents declined participation, they could decide to not fill in the questionnaire without having to explain any reasons why. Questionnaires were distributed to each class following a brief introduction by the researchers (PS; JN) and with a continued possibility to ask questions about the study or specific questions during completion of the questionnaire. Completed questionnaires were returned by each respondent and collected by the researchers (PS; JN), except for two schools where teachers distributed and collected the questionnaires in return envelopes from each class. A total of 210 adolescents were excluded due to missing data and 741 adolescents participated in this study.

Measurements

Socioeconomic status (SES)

Parent’s occupation status

Parents occupation was based on the occupational status of the mother and father reported by the adolescents. The questions were; “Do you know what your father works with?” and “Do you know what your mother works with?” Adolescents whose answer to the question, were either not interpretable such as “at a company, a name of a shop or just at the Hospital” or blank, were analyzed together as missing data. Answers such as; “nurse at the hospital” or “police”, were categorised according to the Swedish socioeconomic classification index (SEI) – standardized by Statistics Sweden [26]. The SEI-groups were combined into five categories; high-level non-manual employees, medium level non-manual employees, low-level non-manual employees, skilled manual workers, unskilled manual workers and unemployed [26, 27]. The five categories were in this study further categorised into manual (white collar), non-manual (blue collar) and unemployed [28].

FAS- score

The Family Affluence Scale (FAS), used in this study, is one of the most common tools or standardized measurements to measure the objective SES of adolescents [22]. It was developed by the World Health Organization (WHO) as a measure of family wealth and comprises four items including; parental car ownership (“Does your family own a car, van, or a truck?” (0, 1, 2)), sharing or not sharing bedroom (“Do you have your own room?” (1, 0)), number of holidays per year (“During the past 12 months, how many times did you travel away on holiday with your family?” (0, 1, 2, 3)), having computers at home (“How many computers does your family own?” (0, 1, 2, 3)) [22]. The composite FAS score was calculated for each adolescent by adding of the four items (ranging from 0–9) and further categorised into low (0–5), medium (6–7) and high (8–9) [29].

Perceived SES

The perceived SES is a measurement that captures adolescents perception of their family’s socioeconomical, financial or social status [10, 23]. Perceived SES was measured with a question from the HBSC survey [30]; “How well off do you think your family is?” with five response alternatives; “not at all”, “not particularly”, “fairly”, “rather” and “very”. The five response alternatives were clustered into three categories; low (“not at all” and “not particularly”), middle (“fairly”) and high (“rather” and “very”).

Quality of life

Minneapolis Manchester Quality of Life instrument, (MMQL)

MMQL is a self-assessment instrument available in two age-appropriate versions, the MMQL-Youth form for children aged 8–12 years old and the MMQL-Adolescent form for children aged 13–20 years old [31, 32]. Both instruments have good psychometric properties and are available in versions translated and validated in a Swedish context [23, 24]. The MMQL-Youth form consists of four quality of life subscales; physical symptoms, physical functioning, psychological functioning and outlook on life/family dynamics divided into 32 items. The MMQL-Adolescent form consists of seven quality of life subscales; physical functioning, cognitive functioning, psychological functioning, body image, social functioning, intimate relations and outlook on life divided into 45 items. The responses to each item are given with a Likert-type scale from never to always: “Never” = 5; “Seldom” = 4, “Sometimes” = 3, “Mostly” = 2 and “Always” = 1. All item scores were aggregated and then divided with the number of item derive a total score of health related quality of life.

Statistical analysis

All analyses were performed using the whole sample and some analyses were repeated after stratifying for age group (11–13 years and 14–16 years) and gender. Statistical analyses were conducted using SPSS PC version 14.0. Statistical significance in all analyses was determined at P < 0.05. The general characteristics of the sample was described using mean and standard deviation (SD), numbers (n) and proportions (%). The response rates and rates of missing items for parent’s occupation status, FAS and perceived SES were described using numbers (n) and proportions (%). Differences in the rates of missing items between gender and age-groups were analysed using the chi2- test. Validity was tested with the following criteria [33, 34]: for convergent validity the computed Spearman correlation coefficient was applied between levels (low, medium and high) of parent’s occupations, FAS and perceived SES. Spearman correlation coefficients below 0.30 were considered weak, 0.30 to 0.59 moderate and ≥ 0.60 high [25]. Convergent validity was considered to be achieved when correlations were moderate or high [25]. For concurrent validity ANOVA was used to test the ability of the SES measures to yield parents occupations status (manual, non-manual and un-employed) and levels low, mediate and high of FAS and perceived SES in relation to the mean of the total-MMQL score [26].

Results

Of the 741 study participants, 50.1 % were girls in the younger age group and 47.8 % in the older age group. The mean (standard deviation) MMQL-score were 4.34 (0.37) in the younger age-group and 4.01 (0.42) in the older age group (Table 1).

Table 1.

General characteristics in 11–13 years old and 14–16 years old pupils participating in the Halmstad Youth Cohort (n = 741)

Age-group
11–13 years old 14–16 years old
(n = 377) (n = 364)
mean (SD) mean (SD)
HRQoL (total) 4.34 (0.37) 4.01 (0.42)
n (%) n (%)
Gender
Boys 188 (49.9) 190 (52.8)
Girls 189 (50.1) 174 (47.8)
Parents occupation status
Fathers occupation status
Non-manual workers (white collar) 101 (50.2) 117 (51.3)
Manual workers (blue collar) 91 (45.3) 103 (45.2)
Unemployed 9 (4.5) 8 (3.5)
Mothers occupation status
Non-manual workers (white collar) 140 (65.7) 159 (63.6)
Manual workers (blue collar) 58 (27.2) 75 (30.0)
Unemployed 15 (7.0) 16 (6.4)
Fas-score
High (8–9 items) 111 (29.2) 95 (26.1)
Medium (6–7 items) 160 (42.4) 191 (52.5)
Low (0–5 items) 106 (28.1) 78 (21.4)
Percieved SES
High 134 (36.7) 85 (24.5)
Medium 187 (51.2) 209 (60.1)
Low 44 (12.1) 54 (15.5)

Completion rates were lowest for parent occupations status, 41.7 % for fathers’ occupation status and 37.5 % for mothers’ occupations status (Table 2). There was a significant difference between the age groups, where the youngest age group had the highest proportion of non-responders compared to the older age group, mothers’ occupation status (p = 0.001) and fathers’ occupation status (p = 0.012). Girls had a higher completion rate than boys for the question on mothers’ occupation status (p = 0.004) and on fathers’ occupation status (p = 0.015). No such difference was seen for the perceived SES (Table 2).

Table 2.

Completion rate in parents occupation status, FAS and percieved SES

Fathers occupation status Mothers occupation status FAS-score Percieved SES
Respondent Non-respondent p* Respondent Non-respondent p* Respondent Non-respondent p* Respondent Non-respondent p*
n (%) n (%) n (%) n (%) n (%) n (%) n (%) n (%)
Total 432 (58.3) 309 (41.7) 463 (62.5) 278 (37.5) 741 (100) 713 (96.2) 28 (3.8)
Age-group
11–13 years 201 (53.3) 176 (46.7) 0.012 210 (55.7) 167 (44.3) 0.001 377 (100) - - 365 (96.8) 12 (3.2) 0.387
14–16 years 224 (61.5) 140 (38.5) 252 (69.2) 112 (30.8) 364 (100) - - 348 (95.6) 16 (4.4)
Gender
Boys 204 (53.7) 174 (46.3) 0.015 217 (57.4) 161 (42.6) 0.004 378 (100) - - 363 (96.0) 15 (4.0) 0.782
Girls 228 (62.8) 135 (37.2) 246 (67.8) 117 (32.2) 363 (100) - - 350 (96.4) 13 (3.6)

*P-value differenes in completion rate were analysed with CHI2 -test

Convergent validity was evaluated by Spearman’s correlation coefficient between the categorized levels of parents’ occupational status, FAS and perceived SES (Table 3). The highest significant correlation coefficient in the younger age group was seen between mothers’ and fathers’ occupational status r = 0.303 (p < 0.001) suggesting that these measures are measuring the same construct in this age group. This was not seen in the older age group, where the highest correlation coefficient was seen between FAS and perceived SES, the correlation was, however, weak (r = 0.292, p < 0.001) (Table 3). All other correlations in both age groups were considered weak (r below 0.300), suggesting that these indicators measure different dimensions of self-rated SES.

Table 3.

Convergent validity - Spearman correlations coefficient between parents occupation status, FAS and percieved SES

Measure Fathers occupation status Mothers occupation status FAS-score Percieved SES
14–16 years old
Fathers occupation status 1 0.225 0.197 0.113
Mothers occupation status 0.303 1 0.199 0.083
FAS-score 0.184 0.246 1 0.292
Percieved SES 0.143 0.063 0.262 1
11–13 years old

Boldface indicates P<0.05

For concurrent validity, each SES measure was used to differentiate between mean self-reported HRQOL of those having parents with manual, non-manual and unemployed occupational status and having high, medium or low levels of SES in FAS and perceived SES (Table 4). The analysis of validity showed no gradient of HRQOL when using fathers’ and mothers’ occupational status whereas when using FAS to grade the mean HRQOL the lowest level of mean HRQOL was found for those with lower SES. This analysis was neither significant in the younger age group (p = 0.052) nor in the older age group (p = 0.065), however, the perceived SES showed a significant gradient of mean HRQOL in the different levels of SES in both age-groups p < 0.001 (Table 4).

Table 4.

Concurrent validity - comparison of HRQoL total score in parents occupation status, FAS and percieved SES

Age-group
11–13 years old 14–16 years old
HRQol total score HRQol total score
mean (SD) mean (SD)
Fathers occupation status (n = 192) (n = 219)
Non-manual workers (white collar) 4.33 (0.35) 4.05 (0.38)
Manual workers (blue collar) 4.29 (0.41) 3.99 (0.46)
Unemployed 4.47 (0.47) 3.90 (0.39)
p-value 0.996 0.171
Mothers occupation status (n = 198) (n = 233)
Non-manual workers (white collar) 4.33 (0.39) 4.03 (0.40)
Manual workers (blue collar) 4.34 (0.39) 4.00 (0.43)
Unemployed 4.24 (0.37) 3.91 (0.38)
p-value 0.517 0.246
FAS-score (n = 377) (n = 365)
High 4.41 (0.34) 4.06 (0.39)
Medium 4.31 (0.38) 4.03 (0.41)
Low 4.30 (0.39) 3.92 (0.47)
p-value 0.052 0.065
Percieved SES (n = 365) (n = 347)
High 4.41 (0.36) 4.10 (0.42)
Medium 4.35 (0.35) 4.04 (0.36)
Low 4.07 (0.45) 3.77 (0.56)
p-value <0.001 <0.001

Discussion

Principal findings

We found a low convergent validity between the SES indicators, where weak correlations were found between parents’ occupational status, FAS and the perceived SES, suggesting that these indicators measure different reflections of SES among adolescents. In terms of concurrent validity we found that HRQOL, in relation to both FAS and perceived SES, showed a gradient between mean HRQOL, however this was significant only for perceived SES. Now such relation was seen when using measures of parents’ occupational status.

Results from this study showed low convergent validity between the three different SES indicators, which confirms that there are difficulties in relying on only one SES indicator when measuring adolescents’ SES. This is especially true when parents’ occupational status is used as a proxy for self-reported SES among adolescents’ [20, 34]. In line with earlier studies, difficulties in the assessment of parents’ occupation (i.e. parents’ socio-economic status) were found. There were respondents, in both age groups, who had difficulties in specifying parents’ occupation and were unable to answer this question which raise questions in terms of validity [35]. The proportion of non-responses was higher in the younger age group and boys were more likely than girls to be non-responders. Difficulties for young people in assessing parents’ occupation status may be due to re-call bias but probably also to the fact that contemporary working life comprises a variety of different types of work tasks and work places that may be difficult for children of younger ages to comprehend. For a smaller group of children and adolescents, non-responses may also reflect feelings of shame due to parents being unemployed or long-term sick which respondents may not consider to be socially acceptable [4].

FAS, on the other hand, reflect tangible material assets on a daily basis which is likely to explain the high completion rate. Despite the high completion rate and the widespread use of the FAS scale as an indicator of family SES in surveys with high face validity, there are limitations that need to be considered. In this study the FAS scale did not show any significant association with HRQOL. Since the development of FAS the average material standard in society has increased. It might be that material standard measured in terms of numbers of computers, bedrooms, cars and holiday trips does not sufficiently reflect socio-economic inequalities and thus can not adequately differentiate between degrees of material affluence in different groups of adolescents [22]. This could in particular be the case in a country that, in an international comparison, is characterized by a high economic and technological standard and a compressed income structure, particularly if there is a developed welfare system with general support systems aiming to reduce economic disparities in the society entailing less material difference between those well off and those less well off. It is likely that FAS can reveal differences in material standard between high socio-economic status groups versus economically deprived groups but less suitable for detecting differences in midrange groups. Since Sweden, in international surveys, stands out as a country were severe material deprivation is rare the inability of FAS to distinguish between socioeconomic groups could be somewhat questioned and our results therefore needs to be interpreted carefully [36]. It is also important to notice that the assessment of numbers of computers in a home may not always mirror an explicit affluence standard since certain companies or employers provide their staff with possibility to use office computers at home or provide home computers with reduced costs. Moreover, it is possible that a comprehensive introduction of various mobile devices in many respects replace the need for home computers. Another reason for the non-significant association to HRQOL may be found in the construction of the scale where FAS does not explicitly meet the terms for SES (education, occupation, income). Respondents can achieve their score on FAS in different ways not always closely connected to their parents’ absolute SES or affluence standard. High scores on FAS can be reported by young people in high-income families who can afford several cars, computers and holiday trips but the same FAS score may also be reported by an adolescent in a low-income family where material possessions are bought with for example loans. On the other hand, adolescents scoring not having a car may live in a well-off family with a centrally located apartment in a city with good public transportation. Even though this will generate a lower FAS score it does not reflect a lower material standard and in this case the validity of FAS will be adverse.

The perceived SES was the only significant indicator that showed a significant gradient of mean HRQOL in the different levels of SES in both age groups. Earlier studies have discussed that SES is a multidimensional construct and it is important to understand how the construct of subjective SES is conceptualized and understood among adolescents. In contrast to the affluence scale, perceived SES measures adolescents’ perception of their everyday life in terms of economic situation, and thus a wider concept of perceived social status. It has earlier been discussed that perceived social status not only assesses the situation at a certain time point but also captures a cumulative influence of different experiences throughout the life span adding to a person’s perceived relative position in the social hierarchy [37, 38]. Perceived SES is understood to assess more salient dimensions of adolescents’ perceptions of their social status than more objective measures (i.e. parents’ occupation status), such as their immediate social interactions with their closest and most important environment [39]. Earlier studies have also discussed that perceived SES includes a psychological dimension, not only objective resources, closely related to perceived social status. When assessing wealth in one’s family (i.e. social status) in comparison with others’ it is likely that adolescents refer to both proximal referents e.g. perceived richness in friends and class-mates and distal referent social groups e.g. comparisons with young people or other families in the society [40]. Due to the immense development of boundless Internet based social communication it is likely that there is an increased impact of referent groups on adolescents’ views about richness which influence perceptions of family wealth and position in the social hierarchy. In a review, perceived SES has been shown to predict health outcomes better than traditional measurements [41]. The perception of relative social position has also been suggested to mediate the association between economic inequality and material standard and population health [42]. With this understanding, perceived SES may be a more sensitive socio-economic measure than objective SES indicators (i.e. parents’ occupational status) in adolescents, which could be part of the explanation for the significant results on HRQOL in this study.

Strengths and limitation

A strength of this study was that the data was based on a sample of young people within their period of adolescenthood. To be able to explore possible associations between SES and HRQOL and to generate further age-related hypotheses, two age groups (11–13 and 14–16 years of age) were chosen. However, the data was a non-probability sampling, with an assumption of an even distribution of the characteristics within the population. A limitation of this might be the difficulties to identify all possible biases. The instruments used in this study are well established, internationally accepted and commonly used measures of SES and HRQOL. The key limitations of this study were the cross-sectional design and that the analyses were based only on self-rated data of adolescents. In this study, parents’ occupation status was classified into three categories in the analyses, according to the Swedish socioeconomic classification index [26]. This categorization could be a limitation as it might be too rough and as it does not capture all levels of the parents’ occupational status in the area such as in distinction between high, medium, and low non-manual employees and between skilled and unskilled manual workers. All component items in the FAS scale were given an equal weight in this study. It is possible that this way of handling the analysis reduced the scale’s ability to discriminate between different components. It has been discussed in work of Batista-Fouget and colleagues (2004) that different weights should be applied to the different items depending on their expected importance in different countries [31]. However, due to the considerations concerning the FAS scale highlighted above we do not believe that this was the decisive reason behind the results on the FAS scale in this study. Perceived SES in previous studies have been measured in different ways [10, 13, 2325], however this concept need to be more discussed and further investigated in face and content validity among adolescents between 11–16 years.

Conclusion

In conclusion, there was a low concurrent validity between the traditional indicators of SES, FAS and the subjective SES in this adolescent population. This might indicate that self-rated information on SES measures different dimensions of SES among adolescents. However, in relation to HRQOL, only subjective SES showed a significant gradient between the mean HRQOL of low and high SES. It can be suggested that subjective SES might measure a wider construct of perceived social status, where social networks and popularity have a stronger impact on adolescence HRQOL. It can also be implied from our findings that different approaches to measures of SES among adolescents should be considered when evaluating SES, at individual and family level as well as at area levels, in relation to HRQOL. Further research is needed to investigate sustainable ways to measure SES, delineating the relevance of tangible measures of education, occupation and income in relation to the perceived socioeconomic status in comparison with others in immediate social networks and in society at large.

Ethics and consent statement

The Regional Ethical Review Board in Lund, Sweden (Dnr 2013/55) formally approved the study. The participants and their guardians were given written and oral information about the aim of the study, what participation entailed, the confidentiality of the data and that they had right to withdraw from the study at any time. Consent was obtained from all participants and their guardians.

Availability of data and materials

The dataset supporting the conclusions of this article are available from the corresponding author on request.

Acknowledgments

This study was supported by funding from the municipality of Halmstad, Länsförsäkringar Halland, Ljungbergska foundation and Region Halland.

Funding

The funding sources had no role in the design, methods, and recruitment of participants, data collection, analysis, or paper preparation.

Abbreviations

FAS

family material affluence status

FOB 85

OCCUPATIONS in population and housing census 1985

HBSC

health behaviour in school-aged children

HRQOL

health related quality of life

MMQL

Minneapolis Manchester quality of life instrument

NYK

Nordicordic standard occupational classification

SCB

Stockholm Statistiska Centralbyrå Statistics-Sweden

SEI

the Swedish socioeconomic classification index

SES

socioeconomic status

SPSS

Statistical Package for Social Sciences

Footnotes

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

PS and JMN conceptualized and developed the instrument. MN, PS and JMN conceptualized the validation and reliability study. MN undertook statistical analysis and all authors (MN, PS, JMN, and CSN) reporting and evaluating the results. The paper was drafted by all authors and all authors contributed to subsequent writing and review. All authors approved version of the paper submitted to review, and all have approved the revised version.

References

  • 1.Marmot M. Social determinants of health inequalities. Lancet. 2005;365(9464):1099–104. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(05)74234-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.World. Social determinants of health and well-being among young people. Health Behavior in school-age children (HBSC) study. International report from the 2009/2010 survey. Health Policy for Children and Adolescents. 2012; No 6.
  • 3.Jianghong L, Mattes E, Stanely F, McMurray A, Hertzman C. Social determinants of child health and well-being. Health Sociol Rev. 2009;18(1):3–11. doi: 10.5172/hesr.18.1.3. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Von Rueden U, Gosch A, Rajmil L, Bisegger C, Ravens-Sieberer U. Socioeconomic determinants of health related quality of life in childhood and adolescences; results from a European study. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2006;60(2):130–5. doi: 10.1136/jech.2005.039792. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Case A, Lubotsky D, Paxson C. Economic status and health in childhood: the origins of the gradient. Am Econ Rev. 2002;95:1308–34. doi: 10.1257/000282802762024520. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Elgar FJ, Pförtner TK, Moor I, De Clercq B, Stevens GWJM, Currie C. Socioeconomic inequalities in adolescent health 2002–2010: a time series analyses of 34 countries participating in the health behaviour in school-aged children study. Lancet. 2015;385:385–95. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(14)61460-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Viner RM, Ozer EM, Denny S, Marmot M, Resnick M, Fatusi A, Currie C. Adolescence and social determinants of health. Lancet. 2012;379(12):1641–52. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(12)60149-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Power C, Stansfeld SA, Matthews S, Manor O, Hope S. Childhood and adulthood risk factors for socio-economic differentials in psychological distress: evidence from the 1958 British birth cohort. Soc Sci Med. 2002;55(11):1989–2004. doi: 10.1016/S0277-9536(01)00325-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Richter M, Erhart M, Vereecken CA, Zambon A, Boyce W, Gabhainn SN. The role of behavioral factors in explaining socio-economic differences in adolescent health: a multilevel study in 33 countries. Soc Sci Med. 2009;69(3):396–403. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2009.05.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Quon EC, McGrath JJ. Subjective socioeconomic status and adolescent health: a meta-analysis. Health Psychol. 2014;33(5):433–47. doi: 10.1037/a0033716. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Nuru-Jeter AM, Sarsour K, Jutte DP, Boyce WT. Socioeconomic predictors of health and development in middle childhood: variations by socioeconomic status measure and race. Issues Compr Pediatr Nurs. 2010;33(2):59–81. doi: 10.3109/01460861003663953. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Reiss F. Socioeconomic inequalities and mental health problems in children and adolescents: a systemic review. Soc Sci Med. 2013;90:24–31. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2013.04.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Goodman E1, Adler NE, Kawachi I, Frazier AL, Huang B, Colditz GA. Adolescents’ perceptions of social status: development and evaluation of a new indicator. Pediatrics. 2001;108(2) doi: 10.1542/peds.108.2.e31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Huguet N, Kaplan MS, Feeny D. Socioeconomic status and health-related quality of life among elderly people: results from the Joint Canada/United States Survey of Health. Soc Sci Med. 2008;66(4):803–10. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2007.11.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Trupin L, Katz PP, Balmes JR, Chen H, Yelin EH, Omachi, Blanc DP. Mediators of the socioeconomic gradient in outcomes of adult asthma and rhinitis. Am J Public Health. 2013;103(2):e31–8. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2012.300938. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Perna L, Bolte G, Mayrhofer H, Mielck A. The impact of social environment on children’s mental health in a prosperous city: an analysis with data from the city of Munich. BMC Public Health. 2010;10:199. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-10-199. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Butterfield MC, Williams AR, Beebe T, Finnie D, Liu H, Liesinger J, Wheeler PH, Yawn B, Juhn YJ. A two-county comparison of the HOUSES index on predicting self-rated health. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2011;65(4):254–9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 18.Juhn YJ, Beebe TJ, Finnie DM, Sloan J, Wheeler PH, Yawn B, Williams AR. Development and initial testing of a new socioeconomic status measure based on housing data. J Urban Health. 2011;88(55):933–44. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 19.Pardo-Crespo MR, Narla NP, Williams AR, Beebe TJ, Sloan J, Yawn BP, Wheeler PH, Juhn YJ. Comparison of individual-level versus area-level socioeconomic measures in assessing health outcomes of children in Olmsted County, Minnesota. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2013;67(4):305–10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 20.Lien N, Friestad C, Klepp KI. Adolescents’ proxy reports of parents’ socioeconomic status: How valid are they? J Epidemiol Community Health. 2001;55(1):731–7. doi: 10.1136/jech.55.10.731. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Torsheim T, Currie C, Boyce W, Kalnins I, Overpeck M, Haugland S. Material deprivation and self-rated health: a multilevel study of adolescents from 22 European and North American countries. Soc Sci Med. 2004;59(1):1–12. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2003.09.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Currie C, Molcho M, Boyce W, Holstein B, Torsheim T, Richter M. Researching health inequalities in adolescents: the development of the Health Behavior in School-aged Children (HBSC) family affluence scale. Soc Sci Med. 2008;66(6):1429–36. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2007.11.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Sweeting H, West P, Young R, Kelly S. Dimensions of adolescents subjective social status within the social community: description and correlations. J Adolesc. 2001;34(3):493–504. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2010.06.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Chen E, Paterson LQ. Neighbourhood, family and subjective socioeconomic status: How do they relate to adolescent health? Health Psychol. 2006;25(6):704–14. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.25.6.704. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Giatti L, do Valle Camelo L, de Castro Rodrigues JF, Maria Barreto S. Reliability of MacArthur scale of subjective social status – Brazilian Longitudinal study od adult health (ELSA-Brasil) BMC Public Health. 2012;12:1096. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-12-1096. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Statistics-Sweden. Reports on statistical Co-ordination 1989:5. Occupations in population and housing census 1985 (FOB 85) according to Nordic standard occupational classification (NYK) and Swedish socio-economic classification (Socioekonomisk indelning SEI). Stockholm Statistiska Centralbyrå (SCB); 1989.
  • 27.Statistics-Sweden . Reports on statistical co-cordination 1982;4 Swedish socio-economic classification. Örebro: Statistiska Centralbyrå (SCB); 1983. [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Rosvall M, Chaix B, Lynch J, Lindström M, Merlo J. Contribution of main causes of death to social inequalities in mortality in the whole population of Scania, Sweden. BMC Public Health. 2006;6:79. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-6-79. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Hutton K, Nyholm M, Nygren JM, Svedberg P. Self-rated mental health and socio-economic background: a study of adolescents in Sweden. BMC Public Health. 2014;23:14–394. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-14-394. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Simpson K, Janssen I, Craig WM, Pickett W. Multilevel analysis of associations between socio-economic status and injury among Canadian adolescents. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2005;59:1072–7. doi: 10.1136/jech.2005.036723. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Bhatia S, Jenney MEM, Wu E, Bogue MK, Rockwood TH, Feusner JH, Friedman DL, Robison LL, Kane RL: The Minneapolis-Manchester quality of life instrument: reliability and validity of the youth form. J Pediatr. 2004;145(1):39–46. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 32.Bhatia S, Jenney MEM, Wu E, Bogue MK, Rockwood TH, Feusner JH, Friedman DL, Robison LL, Kane RL: The Minneapolis-Manchester quality of life instrument: reliability and validity of the adolescent form. J Clin Oncol. 2002;20(24):4692–8. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 33.Einberg EL, Kadrija I, Brunt D, Nygren JN, Svedberg P. Psychometric evaluation of a Swedish version - Manchester quality of life-youth form and adolescent form. Health Qual Life Outcomes. 2013;11:79.5. doi: 10.1186/1477-7525-11-79. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Andresen EM. Criteria for assessing the tools of disability outcomes research. Arch Phys Rehabil. 2000;81(12 supp 2):S15–20. doi: 10.1053/apmr.2000.20619. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Cook DA, Beckman TJ. Current concepts in validity and reliability for psychometric instruments: theory and application. Am J Med. 2006;119(2):166.e7-16. 27. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 36.Åslund C, et al. Shaming experiences and the association between adolescent depression and psychosocial risk factors. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2007;16:298–304. doi: 10.1007/s00787-006-0564-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Social Europe: Aiming for inclusive growth. Annual report of the Social Protection Committee on the social situation in the European Union (2014). European Union. 2015; doi:10.2767/355771.
  • 38.Singh-Manoux A, Adler NE, Marmot MG. Subjective social status: its determinants and its association with measures of ill-health in the Whitehall II study. Soc Sci Med. 2003;56:1321–33. doi: 10.1016/S0277-9536(02)00131-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Pedersen CR, Madsen M. Parents’ labour market participation as a predictor of children’s health and wellbeing: a comparative study in five Nordic countries. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2002;56:861–7. doi: 10.1136/jech.56.11.861. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Cundiff J, Smith TW, Uchino B, Berg C. Subjective social status: construct validity and associations with psychosocial vulnerability and self-rated health. Int J Behav Med. 2013;20:148–58. doi: 10.1007/s12529-011-9206-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Euteneuer F. Subjective social status and health. Curr Opin Psychiatry. 2014;27(5):337–43. doi: 10.1097/YCO.0000000000000083. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Wilkinson RG. Income inequality, social cohesion, and health: clarifying the theory—a reply to muntaner and lynch. Int J Health Serv. 1999;29(3):525–43. doi: 10.2190/3QXP-4N6T-N0QG-ECXP. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Availability Statement

The dataset supporting the conclusions of this article are available from the corresponding author on request.


Articles from BMC Medical Research Methodology are provided here courtesy of BMC

RESOURCES