Skip to main content
F1000Research logoLink to F1000Research
. 2016 Apr 18;5:696. [Version 1] doi: 10.12688/f1000research.8428.1

Provisional Tic Disorder: What to tell parents when their child first starts ticcing

Kevin J Black 1,2,3,4,a, Elizabeth Rose Black 5, Deanna J Greene 1,3, Bradley L Schlaggar 1,2,3,4,6
PMCID: PMC4850871  PMID: 27158458

Abstract

The child with recent onset of tics is a common patient in a pediatrics or child neurology practice. If the child’s first tic was less than a year in the past, the diagnosis is usually Provisional Tic Disorder (PTD). Published reviews by experts reveal substantial consensus on prognosis in this situation: the tics will almost always disappear in a few months, having remained mild while they lasted. Surprisingly, however, the sparse existing data may not support these opinions.

PTD may have just as much importance for science as for clinical care. It provides an opportunity to prospectively observe the spontaneous remission of tics. Such prospective studies may aid identification of genes or biomarkers specifically associated with remission rather than onset of tics. A better understanding of tic remission may also suggest novel treatment strategies for Tourette syndrome, or may lead to secondary prevention of tic disorders.

This review summarizes the limited existing data on the epidemiology, phenomenology, and outcome of PTD, highlights areas in which prospective study is sorely needed, and proposes that tic disorders may completely remit much less often than is generally believed.

Keywords: Provisional Tic Disorder, Tourette syndrome, tic remission, tics

Introduction

  • Most parents want predictions of the future for their child. This, however, is one of the more challenging aspects of caring for the child with [tics] 1.

Pediatricians, child neurologists and child psychiatrists commonly encounter children with recent-onset tics. Many experts conclude that Provisional Tic Disorder (PTD)—tics in someone whose first tic was less than a year ago—is probably a different disease than chronic tic disorders including Gilles de la Tourette syndrome 17. Others conclude that all tic disorders lie on a continuum, with no appropriate arbitrary duration boundary, and may share the same causes 811. However, most experts agree that current PTD is usually mild and will likely go away in a few months 15, 1216 (p. 171). Surprisingly, the sparse existing prospective data may not support this prognosis. This article reviews the current state of knowledge about PTD, focusing on clinical relevance.

Definitions

Tics are abnormal, unwanted movements or vocalizations. They are distinguished from other movement disorders by several characteristics: they are repeated, stereotyped, discrete and nonrhythmic, most frequently involve the head and upper body, and (although sometimes described as involuntary) are perceived by most patients as an inevitable capitulation to an almost irresistible urge 17. Typical examples of tics include raising the eyebrows, turning the eyes, shaking the head, sniffing, snorting, and more complex phenomena such as repetitive touching or saying words or phrases 18. Tics almost always begin in childhood, about ages 3 to 9 years 1921, and on average tics are most severe around ages 9 to 11 22. Recent-onset tics caused by a systemic or other neurological illness are not considered further in this review, since most tic disorders are primary 23.

A brief digression into nosology is necessary to understand the primary literature. Differences between diagnostic criteria contribute to confusion about the patient with recent-onset tics, since various criteria sets differ on defining features, such as whether a minimum duration of ticcing is needed for diagnosis, or whether brief reappearance of tics after a remission of months to years confirm a single chronic disorder or constitute a second episode of transient tics. Similarly, several diagnostic schemata defined “transient tic disorder” (TTD), but construed the term “transient” in quite different senses. These points complicate interpretation of the literature, because many published reports used different rules than those they claimed to use. Appendix 1 discusses these points in more detail. The simplest definition in current use comes from DSM-5, which specifies that a child with primary tics that began less than a year ago is always diagnosed with Provisional Tic Disorder 12, 24. We will follow that nomenclature when referring generally to the child with recent-onset tics, but when reviewing the primary literature we attempt to specify the rules the various reports actually used for definition.

The common, idiopathic DSM-5 chronic tic disorders are Tourette’s Disorder (also called Tourette Syndrome) and Persistent (also called chronic) Motor (or Vocal) Tic Disorder. Hereinafter the terms Tourette Syndrome or Chronic Tic Disorders (TS/CTD) are used.

Search strategy and selection criteria

References for this review were identified by searches of PubMed through February, 2016, and references from relevant articles and books. Search terms included “tic disorder”, “transient tic disorder”, “Tourette NOT Tourette [AU]”, and “tic disorder [MAJR] AND (transient [TW] OR persistent [TW])”. The final reference list was generated based on relevance to the topic of this review.

Epidemiology of Provisional Tic Disorder

  • Tics are thought to be the most common movement disorder diagnosed in children 25.

Prevalence

Estimates of the prevalence of PTD span a wide range 2530. For example, at least one motor tic was observed in 47% of first graders over the course of one school year 31, while a study using questionnaires and interviews reported a 5% lifetime prevalence of PTD (the authors reported 2.6% as DSM-IV TTD, plus 2.5% in a separate category for children with current tics that had begun < 12 months prior) 32. Obviously at least one of these estimates must be wrong. Below we discuss the key issues in epidemiological studies of PTD before returning to a summary of the evidence.

Factors that contribute to the wide range of prevalence estimates.

A number of factors complicate estimation of PTD prevalence, and some of these deserve special attention. A more complete list and additional references appear in Appendix 2.

Point prevalence vs. lifetime prevalence. Cross-sectional studies are much easier to conduct than longitudinal studies, but provide very different information. Nevertheless, all prevalence studies of chronic tic disorders are relevant to the lifetime prevalence of PTD, because all old tics were once new. In other words, all children with TS or CTD started ticcing at least a year ago, and in the first few months after tic onset would have met criteria for PTD. So the total number of children with a lifetime diagnosis of any tic disorder constitutes a lower bound for the lifetime prevalence of PTD.

Furthermore, since by definition PTD has not lasted for a year, resampling even a short time later can substantially change the prevalence estimate. The most illustrative data come from Snider and colleagues 31, who visited an elementary school eight times over the course of a single school year. A trained rater observed the classroom for 1 hour on each visit, attending to each child individually for at least 3 minutes. A motor tic was observed at some point during the year in 47% of first graders [Table 1 in ref. 31]. However, the children in first grade in one year will be in second or third grade in subsequent years, and observers saw tics in children in all grades. One can calculate from their data that by sixth grade, at least 70% of students must have met the DSM-5 criteria for PTD at some point (for details, see Appendix 3).

Population. Prevalence of tic disorders depends heavily on the age of the population studied. A prospective, community-based study found that “the percentage of children with tic behaviors varied with age: preschool children (22.3%), elementary school children (7.8%), and adolescents (3.4%)” 33. Similarly, the 1-year period prevalence of tics with impairment or distress was 9% in children age 7–9, 6% age 10–12, and 5% age 13–15 34. In another grade school study, the prevalence of directly observed motor tics peaked in first grade (~age 6) 31. By contrast, a large study identified TS in only 12 of 28,037 teenagers screened at age 16–17 35.

Tics are much more common in children with intellectual disability compared to healthy children without developmental delay, and in special education classrooms than in regular classrooms 33, 3639. In Khalifa and von Knorring’s study, for instance, the 1-year prevalence of any DSM-IV tic disorder was 6.3% in children in regular classrooms but 46.3% in special school settings 34. Tics are also common in children with an autism spectrum diagnosis 40. Consequently, studies will underestimate prevalence if they do not take care to include children outside mainstream classrooms.

Sampling method

  • Generally speaking, few patients with tics seek medical advice, since tics are thought to remit spontaneously 41.

The classroom setting is a special case; more generally, any convenience sample is likely not to fairly represent the entire population. Specifically, children evaluated in any health care setting are more likely to have experienced severe tics and to have symptoms other than tics 42. In studies that use a staged approach to sample a population, an important concern is the false negative rate of the screening procedure. If, for example, 5% of the original population screen positive, then even if the true negative rate of the screening procedure is 90%, the study will underestimate the true prevalence by about two thirds. In perhaps the most telling example, eight screen-negative children—no tics reported by parent, teacher or self, and no tics observed by an expert visiting the classroom—came in for a 20-minute interview, and tics were observed in three of the eight 42a.

Sources of information. Several studies show that direct examination of children identifies tics that children, parents and teachers were all unaware of. This result is apparently due to true differences between sources rather than poor within-source reliability: even though “rates of motor tic frequency were found to be moderately stable across both days and school settings,” clinician ratings of tics correlated only moderately with tics observed by researchers or by teachers 43. Historical information is also crucial, because tics can be absent during an examination due to spontaneous fluctuations in severity, tic suppression, or effective treatment. In general, studies that employ a more comprehensive approach to identifying cases find a higher prevalence of tics 25, 4446.

Prevalence: conclusion. In summary, many factors complicate accurate estimation of tic prevalence, and most of these lead to underestimation ( Appendix 2). At a given moment in time, the fraction of children with either chronic or recent-onset tics constitutes a lower bound on the lifetime prevalence of PTD. Point prevalence depends strongly on age, with the highest rate probably about 20% at age 5–10. Lifetime prevalence is much higher; though longitudinal studies have been sparse, the available evidence supports the view that tics occur at some time or another in a large fraction of all children, probably over half. In most cases these tics never come to medical attention, and those that do are disproportionately more severe. Tics are even more common in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), obsessive compulsive disorder (OCD) or intellectual disability.

Incidence

  • It is almost impossible to conduct a prospective study of the onset of tics, as the majority of patients do not seek help for the tics but rather for other problems 47.

Incidence means the rate of new (first-episode) cases in a given period of time. Some of the prevalence studies cited above provide limited information on incidence. Spencer and colleagues 48 provide some of the most direct data, prospectively observing boys for 4 years. The rate of new-onset tic disorders over the 4-year follow-up period was 3% in boys without ADHD.

Spencer et al. also provide incidence data in boys with ADHD, many of whom had tics at baseline (17%, vs. 4% in boys without ADHD). Boys with ADHD were more likely than those without ADHD to develop new tic disorders over the 4-year follow-up period (20%, including 33% in the 6- to 8-year-old group). Law and Schachar 49 performed a large, 1-year-long randomized controlled trial of methylphenidate for ADHD, and monitored carefully for tics. Children who met the criteria for TS at baseline were excluded from participation, but (other) tics were common at baseline (30%). During the year of follow-up, “clinically significant tics for the first time ( i.e., moderate or worse)” occurred in 19.0% (12/63) of the children who had no tics at baseline. The relevant part of this study for this section is the high rate of new tic disorder in the placebo group: one sixth (16.7%, 2/12) of children with ADHD but no tics at baseline developed PTD of at least moderate severity in one year (see also Table 2 in ref. 49).

Carter and colleagues 50 prospectively followed first-degree relatives of TS probands for 2 to 4 years. Nine of 21 children free of tics initially but at least 4 years old at final assessment had developed tics: five were diagnosed with TS, two with chronic motor or vocal tic disorder, and two with DSM-III-R TTD (one of whom developed tics just before the last assessment). In other words, at least one third of children in this high-risk sample developed a chronic tic disorder over an average of only 3 years. In a similar study, nine of 29 children of a parent with TS had a tic disorder at baseline, and 10 more developed a new tic disorder over the next 2–5 years 51.

Clinical features of Provisional Tic Disorder

Given its prevalence, the knowledge base on PTD is surprisingly limited and scattered 9, 47. Some authors report that PTD that remits within the first year has a similar initial presentation as do chronic tic disorders 2, 52, 53. Certainly any clinical feature of TS can present first chronologically (when the diagnosis is still PTD). However, some features of TS are less typical in PTD: not all children with PTD progress to TS, and some features of TS are more common in older children. Many of the conclusions below should be taken as tentative, since cross-sectional studies not focused on recent tics supply most of the data.

Demographics

Boys are more likely to have PTD than girls, with reported sex ratios of 1.2–4.0 31, 41, 5456. The sex ratio in TS is generally reported as closer to 4. In this regard it may be relevant that children with tics spanning at least 3 months had a significantly higher ratio of boys to girls (7.5:1) than did the children with tics observed in only 1 month or 2 consecutive months (1.6:1) 31; perhaps tics persist more often in boys.

In a large epidemiological study, children with DSM-IV TTD had a later age of onset than children with TS/CTD 47. This finding is plausible, but a longitudinal study is required since chronicity is probably easier to recall in children with more severe or less socially acceptable tics 34.

Tics and other symptoms

Children with DSM-IV TTD had lower severity and a lower rate of vocal tics than children with TS or CTD 47. Similarly, in a school observation study, children with tics spanning at least 3 months had a significantly higher mean tic severity than children with tics observed in only 1 month or 2 consecutive months 31. In a report on over 300 children with ADHD, tic disorders (chronic and recent-onset together) were two to three times as common in combined type ADHD than in hyperactive-impulsive or inattentive ADHD 57. Case-control and family studies of OCD find an excess of tic disorders in OCD relatives, and suggest that tics are more common and more severe in relatives of boys with OCD beginning earlier in childhood 58, 59.

Treatment response

Treatment response in PTD has not been carefully studied, in part because treatment is often unnecessary. Clinical experience suggests that symptoms in PTD respond to treatment at least as well as in TS/CTD.

Etiology

Genetics

In 1987, Zausmer and Dewey opined that “Further research is needed to confirm that there is a connection between childhood tics and Gilles de la Tourette's syndrome, to establish that the predisposition to tics is familial, and, if so, whether there is a complex genetic mechanism involved, or some other environmental ætiology so far undisclosed” 60. However, the following year Kurlan et al. reported on two individuals with remitted PTD in TS families whom they concluded were “very likely obligate carriers of the TS gene” 61, and in fact most family studies of probands with TS find relatives with other tic disorders including PTD. Of 16 monozygotic twin probands with TS, 56% of co-twins also had TS, but 94% of co-twins had some tic disorder, suggesting strongly that TS and other tic disorders share a genetic predisposition 62. High rates of tic disorders including PTD in probands and family members of patients with ADHD and OCD have suggested that these clinical features may be “alternative manifestations of the same underlying illness” 7, 51, 59, 63.

Environmental factors

In the monozygotic twin study of TS cited in the previous paragraph, the lower birth-weight twin had more severe tics in 12 of 13 pairs, and “the magnitude of the intrapair birth-weight difference … strongly predicted the magnitude of the intrapair tic score difference” 62. These results suggest that environmental factors in utero may predict severity and outcome of tic disorders. Maternal smoking or other drug use during pregnancy may also be risk factors for TS 6467 and thus perhaps also for PTD. Although external contingencies do transiently modify ticcing in PTD 68, life events seem to have little to do with the onset of TS/CTD 69. Life events have not been evaluated systematically in PTD.

Outcome of PTD

  • Studies of prognosis are infrequent and difficult to conduct, and the conclusions are severely limited by diverse methodological shortcomings [ 16, p. 188].

One of the most important questions about PTD remains that of prognosis. Since prospective studies of PTD are so few, we examine cross-sectional and retrospective studies as well.

Outcome inferred from cross-sectional studies

Child epidemiological studies not focused primarily on tic disorders often identify too few cases to be useful for this context 70, 71.

Age and sex. Subjects with DSM-IV TTD later recalled their age of onset as 8.5 ± 3.0 years, vs. 4.6 ± 2.9 for TS, though onset for chronic motor or vocal tics also averaged 7–8 years, and a prospective study would be needed to rule out recall bias 47. In Snider et al.’s grade school observational study, the boy:girl ratio was higher in “the persistent group,” i.e. tics observed on at least three consecutive monthly visits or two nonconsecutive visits (7.5:1), than in the isolated group, i.e. tics observed only on one visit or two consecutive visits (1.6:1) 31.

Tics and other symptoms. In the Snider et al. “persistent group,” non-facial tics were twice as common as in the isolated group, and problem behaviors were also more associated with tics in the persistent group 31. In a large epidemiological study, TTD subjects had lower tic severity than children with TS/CTD, and were less likely to have vocal tics 47. In the same study, people with vocal tics (at least chronic vocal tics) had higher rates of comorbidity (58% vs. 12%), including ADHD (33% vs. 12%) and OCD (8% vs. 0%).

Duration at initial presentation. Editions of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual previous to DSM-5 had required a 4-week threshold for diagnosing TTD, but a recent expert consensus statement noted no basis for this threshold in data, concluding that “it is unknown whether tics of less than one month’s duration predicts a transient course or not” 24. In the Snider et al. “persistent group,” tics were significantly more severe than in the isolated group (mean 1.08, t = 2.7; P < .01)” 31.

Outcome based on retrospective data

Remschmidt and Remschmidt located 54 families of children who had been seen for tic disorders 72. After an average of 3 years, 11 (38%) of the 29 untreated children had completely remitted according to the family. Abe and Oda 73 obtained questionnaire data from the parents of 32 of 57 children of a parent with tics, and 94 of 178 control children, chosen from children seen at a well-child clinic when they were 3. Of the children in the former, high-risk group, 25% were reported to have tics at age 8, compared to 10% of the control children. Stárková reported the duration of clinical treatment in 131 tic patients hospitalized in a Czech child psychiatry department over a 20-year period 74. Duration of tics at onset is not provided, so many of these patients may have had TS/CTD at initial presentation, but 38% of the 63 patients aged 15–29 years at last follow-up had completely remitted.

Age and sex. At a one-day clinic in New York in 1977, members of 21 families with a GTS proband were examined. Many family members had current or past tics, and “among those with spontaneous clearing, females predominated” 75.

Tics and other symptoms. Wang and Kuo concluded that: “Cases in 4–6 years old children with multiple motor and vocal tics have poor prognosis” 76. Consistent with that opinion, six of 11 spontaneously remitting tic disorders in another study had only a winking tic at presentation, and only one had a vocal tic 72. By contrast, Chouinard and Ford concluded that “the appearance of the tic disorder, the course and prognosis, the family history of tic disorder, and the prevalence of OCD were found to be similar” in nine adult patients in whom a previous history of transient tics in childhood was eventually elicited, whereas a specific cause for the tic disorder (infection, trauma, cocaine use, or neuroleptic exposure) was more often found in 13 adult patients who apparently were truly presenting de novo 53. In a large study of adults, “a significantly greater proportion of adults with ADHD (12%) than those without ADHD (4%) had tic disorders. Tic disorders followed a mostly remitting course and had little impact on functional capacities” 77.

Remission at presentation. In the epidemiological study of Wang and Kuo, all subjects diagnosed with TTD in childhood had remained in remission 76. However, this conclusion, like so many discussed in the preceding sections, may depend on the thoroughness of the assessment. “Detailed questioning disclosed a history of previous childhood transient tic disorder” in nine of 22 patients presenting for medical care of a tic disorder for the first time after the age of 21 53. Similarly, children with tic disorders that had remitted before age 20 often experienced a recurrence later in life 78. Still, the remission rate for PTD is likely more favorable than that of TS; tics persisted into adulthood in 90% 79 or 100% 80 of patients with childhood-onset TS.

Outcome from prospective monitoring

Statistics on the rate of permanent spontaneous remission are difficult to obtain since these patients, characteristically, do not return for follow-up contacts 81.

The ideal design to address prognosis of PTD is a prospective study, lasting at least through the 1-year anniversary of the first tic. Unfortunately, few such studies are available. Shin and colleagues followed eight children (seven boys) with DSM-III or DSM-III-R TTD 82 identified by hospital records. Their age at symptom onset was 8.38 ± 3.60 years (mean ± SD), and symptom duration at presentation was 0.38 ± 0.37 years. At follow-up 3 to 18 years later, using a semi-structured interview by telephone or face to face, four had recovered completely and four had not improved at all. By comparison, of the 22 children in the same study who were initially diagnosed with TS or CTD, seven had remitted by follow-up, 11 had improved somewhat, and four had not improved.

Bruun and Budman reported follow-up by telephone (62%) or direct interview (38%) on 58 children who had tics lasting less than a year at presentation (DSM-III and ICD9 TTD) 9. After 2–14 years, tics had remained absent throughout the follow-up period in only 17%; 40% now met criteria for chronic motor or vocal tics and the remaining 43% “continued to have tics that were chronic and episodic (either TS or tic disorder ‘not otherwise specified’ by DSM-IV criteria)” 9.

Peterson et al. 83 followed for up to 15 years a large sample of children with chronic or recent-onset tics, diagnosed initially by parental report only. The follow-ups used direct examination for diagnosis. The number of children with motor tics declined substantially over time (Time 1, 17.7%; Time 2, 2.2%; Time 3, 2.1%; Time 4, 0.6%), but as only Times 2 and 3 used identical ascertainment methods, generalization about remission of PTD is difficult. From 43% to 84% of 32 children with a tic at presentation to an ophthalmology practice still had tics after an average follow-up of 6.1 ± 3.9 years 84 (diagnosis at presentation was not reported, but many of these children probably had PTD). A few other studies report limited prospective data 52, 85, 86.

Age and sex. Corbett et al. 56 found prospective follow-up data after a mean of 5.4 years on 73 of 89 children who had first seen the doctor specifically because of tics. These 89 were part of a larger sample of 180 children with tics. Of the 122 subjects with adequate information recorded about the date of tic onset, at least 44 had experienced tics for more than a year at the initial assessment. Thus many of the 73 children with prospective follow-up data probably had PTD at the baseline visit, but many already had chronic tics. With this caveat, one interesting point is that outcome depended on age of onset; remission was more likely (16 of 26, 62%) in children whose tics had started at age 6–8 years than in children whose tics began at age 2–5 (7 of 29, 24%) or at age 9–15 (6 of 17, 35%; p<.02; Table VIII in 56). In the same study, full remission was significantly more likely the longer the follow-up, so that only 32% of those followed for 2 years or less were tic-free, vs. 65% of those followed for at least 8 years. In other words, many children who do remit completely will do so only after meeting criteria for TS or CTD.

Spontaneous remission was more likely in girls than boys in one follow-up study of mixed tic disorders 87, but a similar study found no relationship of remission to sex 88. The latter study also suggested better prognosis for children whose parents had tics that remitted vs. persisted in adulthood.

Tics and other clinical features. In a clinical sample of 26 children initially diagnosed with TTD, coprolalia during the first year did not differentiate those later diagnosed with Tourette’s disorder from those still diagnosed with TTD at follow-up 1–11 years later [ 16, pp. 373–374]. In the same study, none of the three children who presented with a complex tic remitted, though the group difference was not significant. However, “children were more likely to develop Tourette's disorder if they had three or more vocal symptoms during the first year (8 [of] 17 children); all of the children with less than three vocal symptoms had a final diagnosis of TTD (p = 0.02)” [ 16, p. 374]. Bruun and Budman also reported significantly better prognosis in children who presented without vocal tics 9.

None of the children who remained in remission had a tic below the neck at their first visit, compared with at least 19% of those later diagnosed with TS [ 16, pp. 373–374]. In fact, all four children who had a lower extremity tic at any point in the first year were diagnosed with TS at follow-up. Neither of these results was statistically significant, but the mixed tic disorder follow-up of Corbett et al. similarly found a trend (p<0.10) for fewer remissions in patients with a lower extremity tic at presentation 56.

In the prospective study by Peterson et al., ADHD at baseline was associated with later tic persistence 89. Spencer et al. prospectively monitored the appearance and disappearance of tic disorders over the course of 4 years in boys with or without ADHD who did not have TS at study entry 48. Almost all (~90%) of the boys with tics had a chronic tic disorder, so the data are only partially relevant. Still, over the course of 4 years, the age-adjusted rate of remission was 65% for tic disorders (vs. 20% for ADHD).

One study located 54 children with OCD who had enrolled in OCD treatment studies for which Tourette’s disorder had been an exclusionary criterion, and re-examined them “2–7 years later with a neurological examination and a structured interview to establish the presence or absence of tics and Tourette's disorder” 59. At baseline, 16 of the 54 children had a current tic disorder, and 15 other children had a past history of tics. Twelve of the 31 had a diagnosis of DSM-III-R TTD at baseline. At follow-up, 17 of the 31 had current tics, three others had a past diagnosis of TS/CTD, and for 12 the only lifetime tic diagnosis was TTD. One additional person with no tics at baseline had developed a tic disorder (Tourette’s). Thus in this study of childhood OCD, most children had current (30%) or past (28%) tics at baseline, though none were thought to have TS. At follow-up, 31% had a current, chronic tic disorder, but those with a previous diagnosis of TTD had remained remitted. The study was focused on tics only at follow-up, but bearing that caveat in mind, the results suggest the possibility that OCD at baseline may protect children whose tics have already disappeared from tic recurrence over the next few years.

Unpublished data suggest that among children with tics for less than 6 months at study entry, those who could suppress tics better when asked to do so had more improvement in tic severity at the 12-month anniversary of tic onset 90.

Outcome of PTD: summary

A few indisputable facts are evident from the existing data on outcome of PTD. Some children who start ticcing will go on to have chronic tics, often associated with impaired quality of life. (Still, prognosis even for TS is primarily hopeful: in a group of children with TS, 11.4 ± 1.6 years old, one third had a YGTSS score of 0 when followed up a mean of 7.5 years later; i.e., no evidence of tics over the past week.) At the other extreme, tics will remit completely in many children with recent-onset tics—or, more likely, will cease to be noticed or to affect quality of life.

However, the existing data provide little certainty about outcome even collectively, much less for individuals. The best-quality information we have comes from the few studies limited to patients identified during the first year of ticcing that either followed the patients prospectively or achieved good follow-up rates retrospectively (see Table 1). Overall, these studies suggest that a child with tics who had no tics prior to 1 year ago has a favorable prognosis overall, but has only about a one in three chance of remaining completely tic-free over the next 5–10 years. Many of those who do remit probably remit only after meeting criteria for TS/CTD at some point.

Table 1. Prospective outcome studies of PTD.

Citation N Duration Remission
Shin 1996 82 4 3–18 yr 50%
Shapiro 1988 [ 16, pp. 371–374] 26 1–11 yr 35%
Spencer 2001 77 36 1–55 yr 53%
Bruun and Budman 1997 9 58 2–14 yr 17%
TOTAL 124 -- 32%

Discussion

Overall, the existing data on PTD are relatively limited. Some of the reasons for this state of affairs are inherent in the problem, and others can be attributed to changing knowledge about tic disorders, diagnostic criteria that have changed several times over the past few decades, and an understandable preference for research to focus on patients with severe and persistent symptoms. Nearly every section above included important unanswered questions, but some of the most important include the following. Do most children, or only a minority, experience tics at some time during childhood? Are the causes of tic disappearance different from the causes of tic appearance? Why do tics usually go away—or at least cease to be a clinical problem? And not least, the question we started with: Will my child’s tics go away, or get worse?

Implications for nosology

The conclusion that complete and permanent remission of recent-onset tics may be relatively uncommon is consistent with a number of observations. First, some studies provide direct support for intermittent tics 31, 46, 91. In discussing the DSM-IV-TR criteria, Singer wrote: “Since children are permitted to have recurrent episodes of ‘transient tics’ and recognizing that tics may go unnoticed, this author would suggest that some individuals in this category actually have a CTD” 92. Second, the more comprehensive the assessment strategy, the higher are the rates of tics observed in epidemiological studies. To give one example, in a random population sample with very thorough clinical ascertainment, tics were observed in one sixth of children in mainstream schools 38 (see also Appendix 2). Cubo provides even more direct evidence for this effect [Table 2 in ref. 25]. Therefore, apparent remission of tics is likely to be much more common than complete remission. Third, many adults with tics are unaware of their tics 9, 79, 80, a fact acknowledged explicitly by the working group for DSM-5 as a reason for changing the diagnostic criteria so that any tics more than a year apart could be diagnosed as a chronic tic disorder.

Bruun and Budman suggest that a fluctuating course commonly follows TS: “It is the impression of these authors, rather [than complete, lifelong remission] that the more common course is one of occasional recurrences of mild tics throughout adult life” 9. Shapiro et al. provide prospective data on this point: “27.1% of our [TS] patients had one or more periods of spontaneous remission lasting from less than 1 month to 19 years” [ 16, p. 188]. Similarly, 62% of tic patients hospitalized as children, when followed up between the ages of 15 and 29, reported only occasional short-term tic relapses lasting several days and requiring no treatment; only 14% were considered to have a poor outcome 74. In discussing prognosis of TS/CTD, Singer concluded that “whether tics actually disappear completely is unclear, and results appear to be dependent on the methods used to document the presence of tics.” 93. The data reviewed above suggest that the same conclusion may apply to tics that began only recently.

The conclusion that the most common outcome of PTD is improvement without remission is true only if by remission one means zero tics ever again, consistent with DSM-5. However, this threshold brings with it some uncomfortable conclusions. Consider a patient who first developed winking and sniffing tics 13 months ago. He and his mother report that his tics have been gone for the past 6 months, and he shows no tics during a 45-minute office visit, but some of his old tics are observed when he sits alone for a few minutes. His DSM-5 diagnosis is Tourette syndrome even though for all practical clinical purposes he has remitted. This child is a substantial departure from the iconic (if unrepresentative) Marquess of Dampierre 94. Martino and colleagues address this problem using an idiosyncratic but understandable nomenclature, diagnosing “physiological tics” if the severity does not warrant diagnosing a “disorder.” They conclude that “‘physiological tics’ commonly occur during normal childhood development and reflect a stage of the physiological synaptogenesis within connections between basal ganglia and frontal lobes” [ 15 (pp. 105–107)].

A different solution is to reconsider the DSM-IV-TR choice to remove the “impairment or marked distress” criterion. The experience of Coffey and colleagues supports this view: “Although tics followed a persistent course in the majority of youth with TD [Tourette’s Disorder], they were infrequently associated with impairment. There was a significant reduction in the proportion of youth with TD impairment from baseline to follow-up. These results support the view that TD is a persistent disorder, but suggest a dissociation between tic persistence and tic-associated dysfunction” 95. However, for both biological and societal reasons, psychosocial consequences of illness, like “distress or impairment,” seem unsatisfactory in defining a highly heritable syndrome. Objective measures focused only on tic severity would sidestep these concerns, but valid, objective tic severity measures encompassing a period longer than a single office visit have been difficult to implement. In either case—whether one prefers to measure tics’ severity or their impact—a zero threshold inevitably produces the nosological frustration discussed in the previous paragraph.

Implications for clinical care and research

Prognosis

  • We do not know the cause of or have the ability to predict spontaneous waxing, waning, fluctuation, and temporary or permanent remission of symptoms [ 16, p. 175].

Research to clarify the expected course of PTD would be greatly appreciated by the children and families who seek consultation for recent-onset tics. Firmer group estimates of improvement and remission rates from prospective studies would be welcome, but even more useful would be identification of additional features at presentation that help predict outcome on an individual level.

Treatment

  • Transient tic disorder “is a self-limiting disorder, and active treatment typically is not indicated.” However, given limited follow-up data, “a child with a diagnosis of TTD should be periodically monitored and the diagnosis and treatment revised as necessary.” 2

At the present time, experts generally agree that treatment for PTD is warranted only when symptoms are severe and persistent enough to substantially distress the child or interfere with his or her school experience or social development. However, better prognostic ability would allow the possibility of early intervention to improve the long-term outcome.

Prevention

  • If we can … detect the brain changes years before the behavior starts, then there’s the opportunity to intervene early. And that’s where we do best in medicine. Early intervention, preempting the later stages, is where we’ve had our greatest successes. … At that point, we’ll start to see … the really big public health impact. 96

  • An ounce of prevention is worth a pound of cure. (Benjamin Franklin)

Since Tourette syndrome and persistent (chronic) motor/vocal tic disorder (TS/CTD) are defined as having lasted at least a year from onset to most recent symptoms, one can envision that an effective intervention, supplied within months of the initial onset of tics, could conceivably prevent TS/CTD. No such intervention has been proven, but it is now clear that a behavior therapy approach (Comprehensive Behavioral Interventions for Tics, or CBIT) is reasonably effective and definitely safe for TS/CTD. If CBIT can be shown to improve the outcome of recent-onset tic disorders, it can become an approach to primary prevention of TS/CTD (secondary prevention of tic disorders). At present, factors such as cost and limited availability mean that treating all children with recent-onset tics is impractical, not to mention unnecessarily intrusive for the majority of children whose tics will disappear (or become mild or rare). However, those limitations could be overcome with better prognostic accuracy.

Tics that go away can tell us something important about tics that don’t

  • Francis Bacon … pronounced that it must be of the greatest interest for the physician to study healed cases of incurable diseases 97.

As reviewed above, very little is known about predicting outcome from studies of PTD itself. However, more follow-up data are available after tics have become chronic 22, 79, 80, 95, 98106. These follow-up studies of chronic tic disorders suggest hypotheses that can be tested in prospective follow-up studies of PTD. Leckman 107 lists the following potential prognostic features for TS: visual-motor integration 108, decreased TMS motor inhibition, 109, 110, MRI volumetry 89, and white matter integrity 111. Singer 92 adds: “Proposed predictors of severity and longevity include tic severity, fine motor control, … size of caudate and subgenual volumes 99, 100, but all are controversial 22, 93.” Here, “fine motor control” may refer to Purdue Pegboard performance 112. Additional potential markers for outcome of PTD include baseline ability to suppress tics 68, 113, resting state functional connectivity 114, probabilistic learning 115, ADHD at diagnosis 116, vocal tics 9 or tics below the neck at presentation, or more than two vocal tics in the first year [ 16, pp. 373–374], socioeconomic status (ref. 16, p. 175) and family history of TS/CTD, perhaps especially family history of tics persisting into adulthood 88 or tics in both parents 51.

A careful, prospective study that includes some of the most promising of these potential markers may allow discovery of the cause and pathophysiology of tic improvement, with the potential consequences for clinical care noted in the preceding section. This hope provides one of the key motivations for attempting to identify clinical, neuropsychological, or brain imaging variables that correspond to improvement vs. worsening of recent-onset tic disorders. As we tell parents who enroll their children in our ongoing study of recent-onset tics, “We’d love to find out what happens in the brains of kids whose tics improve spontaneously, and put it in a bottle or find out how to teach it to patients with chronic tics.”

Acknowledgments

We thank the children and parents who have participated in the NewTics study ( www.NewTics.info).

Funding Statement

Research reported in this publication was supported by the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute Of Child Health & Human Development of the National Institutes of Health (NIH) under Award Number U54 HD087011 to the Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities Research Center at Washington University, by NIH grants K24 MH087913, R21 NS091635, K01 MH104592, K12 NS001690, by the McDonnell Center for Systems Neuroscience at Washington University, by a NARSAD Young Investigator Award to DJG from the Brain & Behavior Research Foundation, and by the Tourette Association of America. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health or any of the other funders.

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

[version 1; referees: 3 approved]

Appendices

Appendix 1. Nosological confusion

The literature is complicated by substantial confusion about the meaning of the phrase “transient tic disorder” (the nomenclature used most often in the existing literature). The word “transient” is taken to mean two different things. The Tourette Syndrome Study Group (TSSG) criteria 117 represent a usage in which “transient” means something that was present but has disappeared. The TSSG criteria for Transient Tic Disorder (TTD) require that tics began over a year ago, but lasted no more than 12 consecutive months; i.e., the tics are now gone. Using the TSSG criteria, patients who are ticcing but whose tics started less than a year ago have “diagnosis deferred.” The ICD-10 criteria similarly require that “tics do not persist for longer than 12 months,” so that in the first year since onset one diagnoses “tic disorder, unspecified” 118.

Contrary to a common assumption 24, 27, 119, 120, DSM-IV-TR specified a different meaning of “transient” 121. The DSM-IV-TR criteria for TTD do not require disappearance of the tics, and the accompanying text clearly specifies: “Often, the diagnosis may change over time during the natural history of a Tic Disorder. For example, during the first months, a child may be diagnosed as having a Transient Tic Disorder. After a year, with further tics and longer duration, the diagnosis may become Tourette’s Disorder” 121. This usage—tics are transient until they prove themselves to be chronic—seems natural in the context of other DSM-IV-TR diagnoses such as Major Depressive Disorder that are based on current data but may need to be revised (e.g., if the patient later develops a manic episode, in which case the preceding depression is revealed to have been part of bipolar disorder, or if Alzheimer’s disease is diagnosed in life but autopsy reveals a different dementing illness). To deal with the patients left undiagnosed by this misunderstanding, some epidemiology studies claimed to use DSM-IV-TR criteria, but used “provisional” or “not otherwise specified” to describe patients with tics for less than a year, even though these patients in fact met DSM-IV-TR criteria for TTD.

Due to widespread dissatisfaction with the term Transient Tic Disorder, DSM-5 adopted the term “provisional tic disorder” (PTD) 12, 24. This diagnosis differs from DSM-IV-TR TTD not only in name and in the clarification that the diagnosis is intended for all children whose tics began less than a year ago, but also in two other substantive ways. First, PTD can be diagnosed within the first 4 weeks after tic onset. Second, and more consequentially, DSM-5 requires that any tic present more than 1 year after the first tic be diagnosed as TS or CTD (assuming the tics are not secondary to another illness or to a known toxin), disallowing the diagnosis of PTD for someone with current tics who had tics more than a year ago, regardless of any intervening asymptomatic period.

Appendix 2. Factors that complicate assessment of recent-onset tics.

The interested reader is also referred to insightful discussions of this topic by Cubo 25 and by Robertson and colleagues 26.

Problems Comments and references
Children (and adults) don’t
notice all their tics
A “comparison of tic assessment in adults, based on personal assessments vs. videotape analysis,
showed that 90% still had tics despite frequent claims of their absence 79.” 93
“In clinical practice and in research it is common to observe tics in those who do not report any tic
symptoms 79.” 24
In an epidemiological study, 27 of the 33 TS cases had never been diagnosed clinically with TS, and only
one was being treated with a medication 122.
Parents don’t notice all their
child’s tics
Teachers don’t notice all their
students’ tics
“Tics were coded as present if either a parent or a teacher reported them. … Not all subjects with
preexisting tics were identified by both parents and teachers. Parents reported 18 of the 27 cases of tics,
whereas teachers reported 14; only 5 cases of tics were identified by both parents and teachers.” 49
Teachers notice tics in fewer children than parents do [Table 2 in ref. 25, 34].
“Investigators who have relied strictly on historical data rather than on direct observation of symptoms
have often been misled. For example, multiple recurrent tics were missed by parents in 21% of children
being followed up in a longitudinal study. 51” [ 93, punctuation normalized]
Direct expert examination and structured interviews of relatives of TS patients found over five times as
many cases of TS and almost twice as many cases of CTD as did family history interviews alone 123.
Three of 34 children or younger siblings of probands with TS had tics on exam though their parents had
reported no tics 50.
Different observers notice
tics in different children
In an overall sample of 867 children, parents identified tics in 71 children and teachers in 50 children; 23
were identified by parents and by teachers (κ=0.30, indicating only fair agreement). During a classroom
visit an expert observed tics in 57 of the 867 children 46.
Physicians miss tics in
clinical care
Of 58 children with TS in an epidemiological sample, 73% had had some kind of medical treatment, and
of those, “more than 2 in 5 had received child psychiatric treatment, but only in 1 of 18 cases receiving
such treatment had a diagnosis of tics of Tourette's disorder been considered. No child had received
medication for tics or other neuropsychiatric disorder prior to the study” 124.
A multiple-informant study in a school district estimated the prevalence of DSM-III-R TS as 3.0%, whereas
only 0.05% of children in the area had been diagnosed by physicians 42a.
Observers are subject to
confirmation bias
In a study of 867 children, in whom parents or teachers identified tics in 11.3%, 6.5% (58%) had tics
on observation, but “most of [these] were not noted to have tics on a second observation period during
which the observer was not told which children had screened positive by parents or teachers” 46.
Brief observations miss tics Cubo showed that observers’ sensitivity increased from 33% to 58% as the length of observation
increased from 1 to 3 hours [Table 2 in ref. 25].
More severe tics are
probably noticed more
consistently
Law & Schachar 49 note that in the two children who developed the most noticeable new tics, “symptoms
were reported by both parents and teachers as soon as they developed.” Note however that one of the
children already had other tics.
Most of the tics in the Linazasoro et al. study were mild and had lasted only for a short period of time 46.
Greater tic severity was significantly correlated with shorter time to diagnosis 42.
Sixty (28%) of 214 children who screened positive for a lifetime history of DSM-IV TTD did not want to
come for clinical examination 34.
Tic severity fluctuates
dramatically over time, and
also in response to setting,
stress, and observation
method
Tics fluctuate in severity over time.
Tics are (usually) suppressible.
Tics worsen with stress.
Tics can worsen or improve with environmental cues unrelated to stress (even when people attribute no
importance to the cues 125).
People tend to tic more when unobserved 126.
The first item above is required by the Tourette Syndrome Study Group (TSSG) criteria for TS 117 , and the
next three are mentioned in the Diagnostic Confidence Index, which identifies “classic” features of TS 127.
Effective treatment can hide
tics
In the Snider et al. study, two children had a clinical diagnosis of TS; tics were never seen in the child
taking medication for tics, but were seen on four of eight visits in the unmedicated child 31. In that study,
five children with a clinical diagnosis of motor tics ( i.e., diagnosed as part of regular medical care,
independent of the study) had no tics observed during the 8-month study, and three of these were on
medication for tics.
Tics may be more common
at different times of the year
Snider and colleagues reported tics in 10% of elementary school children when evaluated in November,
4–9% when evaluated in December, January or February, and 3–7% when observed between March and
June 31.
Clinicians commonly see more tic patients at the beginning of a school year.
Different observers differ in
assessment of remission or
change in severity
“In only 7 (25.9%) of the 27 subjects with preexisting tics did parents and teachers agree on the changes;
in 2 (28.6%) of the 7 cases, parents reported a worsening of the tics, whereas teachers reported
improvements” 49.
Over time, children, parents
and teachers get used to
mild tics and worry less
about them
Parents often get used to tics over time and pay less attention to them or are less worried by them.
Retrospective dating of the
first tic is difficult 49, 68
Often there is a long delay from the first appearance of a tic to when someone realizes it is a tic 128.
Several children or parents noticed the current tic but did not realize that earlier movements or noises
were also tics 68.
Normal vagaries of memory.
In a prospective study, nine (43%) of 21 children not diagnosed with tics at baseline, but diagnosed with
tics at 4-year follow-up, reported at follow-up that tics had started before the baseline visit 48.
Nosological questions
about brief remissions and
recurrences
Discussed in various statements and position papers on DSM-5 or ICD11 24, 129132, concluding that “The
criterion that 3 months’ absence of symptoms disqualifies an individual from a diagnosis of TS is not in
keeping with the waxing-and-waning course of this condition” 133.
Nevertheless, having a tic for 2 months each year for 5 years seems quite different from having a tic for
60 months straight; in DSM-IV or DSM-IV-TR the intermittent case is diagnosed with 5 episodes of TTD,
yet in DSM-5 both cases are diagnosed as TS.
Nosological questions about
syndrome and severity
Older studies found tic disorders very rarely, likely in part because of identifying only severe, persistent
cases. For example, a 1980 report on 4,258 school children concluded that childhood tic disorder
affected only 0.23% of children—0.87% if they included single tics 134. However, the authors explicitly
excluded “certain single, transient tics clearly driven by a focal irritation” (our translation).
DSM-IV required marked distress or impairment in a life role to diagnose a tic disorder. This requirement
dropped the rate of tic disorders by over 60% in a large population sample 38 and by over 80% in a school
district study 135.
Classification difficulties with
other complex repetitive
behaviors
Tics vs. compulsions. Most tics and most compulsions can be distinguished clearly. Distinguishing
features include cognitive and affective premonitory phenomena ( i.e., obsessions) before most
compulsions and somatic premonitory phenomena before many tics. However, some repetitive behaviors
satisfy the definitions both of tics and of compulsions, and hence cannot be cleanly separated. For
instance, the Y-BOCS symptom inventory includes touching and tapping 136, 137, while the YGTSS tic
symptom checklist includes symmetry-provoked behaviors and “just right” phenomena 138.
Stereotypies. DSM-5 draws attention to the following differences: compared to tics, stereotypies seen
in autism or in Stereotypic Movement Disorder are involuntary, rhythmic, and stop “with distraction ( e.g.,
name called or touched)”; they also tend to begin earlier in life (before age 3), last many seconds to
minutes, lack a premonitory urge, and consist of a “constant repetitive fixed form and location” [ 12,
under Stereotypic Movement Disorder]. Common “examples include repetitive hand waving/rotating, arm
flapping, and finger wiggling.” Additionally, patients with Stereotypic Movement Disorder often find the
movements rewarding or soothing and do not see them as a bothersome symptom or want them to be
stopped 139; by contrast, seeking treatment appears on a consensus list of typical features of TS 127.
Normal behavior. The YGTSS tic symptom checklist includes nail biting, knuckle popping and toe
tapping, which are very common in children without tics 138.
Trichotillomania and similar disorders may occur more often than chance in tic disorders, but are usually
classified separately.
Non-tic behaviors that may
be interpreted as tics by
non-experts
Eyelid myoclonus.
In 49 of 101 cases in which parents initially endorsed a question about one or more specific tics, a
telephone interview revealed the symptoms not to be tics (false positives) 41. Examples included blinking
or sniffing due to allergic rhinitis, making a face when annoyed, sighing, or vacant mouth opening. The
authors of that report cite a 43% false positive rate in an older study with similar methods 140.
Tics that may not be
recognized as tics by non-
experts
Commonly: blinking, sneezing, coughing, throat clearing, jerking the head as if to throw hair out of the
eyes.
Complex tics are often misconstrued as intentional behavior.
Mol Debes et al. reviewed the histories of 314 children with TS and found that diagnosis of TS occurred a
median of 2.8 years after tic onset, and concluded that the diagnostic delay was primarily due to “a lack
of knowledge about the normal course of TS among professionals and the public” 128.
Confusing diction The word “tic” is also used (at least in English) for unrelated phenomena, including
    • tic douloureux (trigeminal neuralgia)
    • mannerisms (Merriam-Webster gives the example “Constantly playing with her hair is one of her more
      annoying tics” 141).

Appendix 3. Minimum cumulative prevalence of tics

Excluding the relatively rare tic caused by drugs or a systemic illness, any child with a definite tic on even one examination can be diagnosed with DSM-5 PTD. If the tics continue beyond the 1-year anniversary of the first tic, the child still had PTD for the first year 12.

In one careful study, the highest cross-sectional prevalence of motor tics at any one observation was 9.6%, but since a number of children were seen to tic on one visit to the school but not another, 47% of first graders were observed to have a motor tic at some point during the year [Table 1 in ref. 31]. Admittedly, this rate may include some false positives, for instance nose wrinkling or sniffing that could better be explained by allergic rhinitis, since observers did not talk with the children or their parents. On the other hand, false negatives must also have occurred, as observers did not identify tics in several children with a clinical diagnosis of motor tics, and did not record vocal tics at all. A pediatric neurologist had trained the raters and performed reliability testing, and motor tics were counted only if they occurred three or more times in the same visit.

The relevant point for the present discussion comes from the fact that tics were observed in classrooms ranging from kindergarten to sixth grade, and collectively the results lead to an interesting conclusion about lifetime prevalence. For instance, 21% of third graders were also observed to tic. If the year the study was done was typical, then about 47% of the current third grade class would have shown tics if observed two years earlier, when they were in first grade. Even assuming a high true rate of chronic tic disorders (say, 8%), tics must have been new in at least 13% (= 21% − 8%) of the third graders in addition to the 47% who had tics in first grade. Applying this reasoning to the rest of Snider et al.’s data, by sixth grade 79% of students must have had at least one tic, and thus have met criteria for PTD at some point ( Table A1, below). This estimate for lifetime prevalence of PTD does not depend strongly on the assumed prevalence of chronic tic disorders ( Table A2), and is much higher than the cross-sectional prevalence rates reported in epidemiological studies or review articles.

Appendix 3, Table A1. Calculation from the data of Snider et al. 31, assuming 8% as the estimated rate of chronic tic disorders (TS plus chronic motor tic disorder).

grade N no
tics
motor
tic(s)
motor
tic(s) (%)
new vs. last
year (min.)
new vs. 2+
yrs. ago
lifetime
prevalence (min.)
K 83 66 17 20.5% 20.5%
1 st 57 30 27 47.4% 13.4% 39.4% 47.4%
2 nd 85 69 16 18.8% 0.0% 10.8% 47.4%
3 rd 43 34 9 20.9% 1.1% 12.9% 60.3%
4 th 98 69 29 29.6% 4.3% 21.6% 69.0%
5 th 78 57 21 26.9% 0.0% 18.9% 79.2%
6 th 109 93 16 14.7% 0.0% 6.7% 79.2%

Appendix 3, Table A2. Minimum lifetime prevalence of provisional tic disorder assuming various prevalence rates for chronic tic disorders and the results of Snider et al 31.

True rate of
TS+CTD
Minimum lifetime prevalence
of Provisional Tic Disorder by
6th grade
1% 93%
2% 91%
3% 89%
4% 87%
5% 85%
6% 83%
7% 81%
8% 79%
9% 77%
10% 75%
11% 73%
12% 71%

References

  • 1. Dooley JM: Tic disorders in childhood. Semin Pediatr Neurol. 2006;13(4):231–242. 10.1016/j.spen.2006.09.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Kuperman S: Tics and Tourette's syndrome in childhood. Semin Pediatr Neurol. 2003;10(1):35–40. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Bloch MH, Leckman JF: Tic disorders.In: Martin A, Volkmar FR, Lewis M, eds. Lewis's Child and Adolescent Psychiatry: A Comprehensive Textbook, 4th ed. Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins,2007;569–582. [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Zinner SH, Mink JW: Movement disorders I: tics and stereotypies. Pediatr Rev. 2010;31(6):223–233. 10.1542/pir.31-6-223 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Jung HY, Chung SJ, Hwang JM: Tic disorders in children with frequent eye blinking. J AAPOS. 2004;8(2):171–174. 10.1016/j.jaapos.2003.10.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Golden GS: Movement disorders in children: Tourette syndrome. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 1982;3(4):209–216. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Hebebrand J, Klug B, Fimmers R, et al. : Rates for tic disorders and obsessive compulsive symptomatology in families of children and adolescents with Gilles de la Tourette syndrome. J Psychiatr Res. 1997;31(5):519–530. 10.1016/S0022-3956(97)00028-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Miyamoto S: [Tic disorders]. Nihon Rinsho. 1993;51(11):2859–2865. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Bruun RD, Budman CL: The course and prognosis of Tourette syndrome. Neurol Clin. 1997;15(2):291–298. 10.1016/S0733-8619(05)70313-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Kurlan R: Hypothesis II: Tourette's syndrome is part of a clinical spectrum that includes normal brain development. Arch Neurol. 1994;51(11):1145–1150. 10.1001/archneur.1994.00540230083017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Golden GS: Tics and Tourette's: a continuum of symptoms? Ann Neurol. 1978;4(2):145–148. 10.1002/ana.410040208 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. American Psychiatric Association: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association,2013. 10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Fourneret P, Desombre H, Broussolle E: [From tic disorders to Tourette syndrome: current data, comorbidities, and therapeutic approach in children]. Arch Pediatr. 2014;21(6):646–651. 10.1016/j.arcped.2014.03.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Scahill L, Sukhodolsky DG, Williams SK, et al. : Public health significance of tic disorders in children and adolescents. Adv Neurol. 2005;96:240–248. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Martino D, Espay AJ, Fasano A, et al. : Unvoluntary motor behaviors. In: Martino D, Espay AJ, Fasano A, Morgante F, eds. Disorders of Movement: A Guide to Diagnosis and Treatment.1 ed. Berlin: Springer-Verlag.2016;97–153. [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Shapiro AK, Shapiro E, Young JG, et al. : Gilles de la Tourette Syndrome, 2nd ed.New York: Raven Press,1988. [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Sanger TD, Chen D, Fehlings DL, et al. : Definition and classification of hyperkinetic movements in childhood. Mov Disord. 2010;25(11):1538–1549. 10.1002/mds.23088 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Black KJ: Tics. In: Kompoliti K, Verhagen Metman L, Comella C, Goetz C, Goldman J, Kordower J, Shannon K, eds. Encyclopedia of Movement Disorders.Oxford: Elsevier (Academic Press),2010;231–236. 10.1016/B978-0-12-374105-9.00385-3 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Boenheim C: Über den Tic im Kindesalter [On tics in childhood]. Klinische Wochenschrift. 1930;9(43):2005–2011. 10.1007/BF01720562 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Leckman JF, Cohen DJ: Tourette's syndrome -- tics, obsessions, compulsions: Developmental psychopathology and clinical care.New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.,1999. [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Martino D, Leckman JF: Tourette Syndrome.New York: Oxford University Press,2013. Reference Source [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Leckman JF, Zhang H, Vitale A, et al. : Course of tic severity in Tourette syndrome: the first two decades. Pediatrics. 1998;102(1 Pt 1):14–19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Schlaggar BL, Mink JW: Movement disorders in children. Pediatr Rev. 2003;24(2):39–51. 10.1542/pir.24-2-39 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Walkup JT, Ferrão Y, Leckman JF, et al. : Tic disorders: some key issues for DSM-V. Depress Anxiety. 2010;27(6):600–610. 10.1002/da.20711 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Cubo E: Review of prevalence studies of tic disorders: methodological caveats. Tremor Other Hyperkinet Mov (N Y). 2012;2: pii: tre-02-61-349-1. 10.7916/D8445K68 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Robertson MM: The prevalence and epidemiology of Gilles de la Tourette syndrome. Part 2: tentative explanations for differing prevalence figures in GTS, including the possible effects of psychopathology, aetiology, cultural differences, and differing phenotypes. J Psychosom Res. 2008;65(5):473–486. 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2008.03.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Robertson MM: The prevalence and epidemiology of Gilles de la Tourette syndrome. Part 1: the epidemiological and prevalence studies. J Psychosom Res. 2008;65(5):461–472. 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2008.03.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Robertson MM, Eapen V, Cavanna AE: The international prevalence, epidemiology, and clinical phenomenology of Tourette syndrome: a cross-cultural perspective. J Psychosom Res. 2009;67(6):475–483. 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2009.07.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Scharf JM, Miller LL, Gauvin CA, et al. : Population prevalence of Tourette syndrome: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Mov Disord. 2015;30(2):221–228. 10.1002/mds.26089 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Knight T, Steeves T, Day L, et al. : Prevalence of tic disorders: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Pediatr Neurol. 2012;47(2):77–90. 10.1016/j.pediatrneurol.2012.05.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Snider LA, Seligman LD, Ketchen BR, et al. : Tics and problem behaviors in schoolchildren: prevalence, characterization, and associations. Pediatrics. 2002;110(2 Pt 1):331–336. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Stefanoff P, Wolanczyk T, Gawrys A, et al. : Prevalence of tic disorders among schoolchildren in Warsaw, Poland. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2008;17(3):171–178. 10.1007/s00787-007-0651-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33. Gadow KD, Nolan EE, Sprafkin J, et al. : Tics and psychiatric comorbidity in children and adolescents. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2002;44(5):330–338. 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2002.tb00820.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34. Khalifa N, von Knorring AL: Prevalence of tic disorders and Tourette syndrome in a Swedish school population. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2003;45(5):315–319. 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2003.tb00402.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35. Apter A, Pauls DL, Bleich A, et al. : An epidemiologic study of Gilles de la Tourette's syndrome in Israel. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1993;50(9):734–738. 10.1001/archpsyc.1993.01820210068008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36. Comings DE, Himes JA, Comings BG: An epidemiologic study of Tourette's syndrome in a single school district. J Clin Psychiatry. 1990;51(11):463–469. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37. Kurlan R, Whitmore D, Irvine C, et al. : Tourette's syndrome in a special education population: a pilot study involving a single school district. Neurology. 1994;44(4):699–702. 10.1212/WNL.44.4.699 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38. Cubo E, Gabriel y Galán JM, Villaverde VA, et al. : Prevalence of tics in schoolchildren in central Spain: a population-based study. Pediatr Neurol. 2011;45(2):100–108. 10.1016/j.pediatrneurol.2011.03.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Eapen V, Robertson MM, Zeitlin H, et al. : Gilles de la Tourette's syndrome in special education schools: a United Kingdom study. J Neurol. 1997;244(6):378–382. 10.1007/s004150050105 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40. Baron-Cohen S, Scahill VL, Izaguirre J, et al. : The prevalence of Gilles de la Tourette syndrome in children and adolescents with autism: a large scale study. Psychol Med. 1999;29(5):1151–1159. 10.1017/S003329179900896X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41. Nomoto F, Machiyama Y: An epidemiological study of tics. Jpn J Psychiatry Neurol. 1990;44(4):649–655. 10.1111/j.1440-1819.1990.tb01641.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42. Shilon Y, Pollak Y, Benarroch F, et al. : Factors influencing diagnosis delay in children with Tourette syndrome. Eur J Paediatr Neurol. 2008;12(5):398–400. 10.1016/j.ejpn.2007.10.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42a. Mason A, Banerjee S, Eapen V, et al. : The prevalence of Tourette syndrome in a mainstream school population. Dev Med Child Neurol. 1998;40(5):292–296. 10.1111/j.1469-8749.1998.tb15379.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43. Nolan EE, Gadow KD, Sverd J: Observations and ratings of tics in school settings. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 1994;22(5):579–593. 10.1007/BF02168939 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44. Kurlan R, Como PG, Miller B, et al. : The behavioral spectrum of tic disorders: a community-based study. Neurology. 2002;59(3):414–420. 10.1212/WNL.59.3.414 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45. Hornsey H, Banerjee S, Zeitlin H, et al. : The prevalence of Tourette syndrome in 13-14-year-olds in mainstream schools. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2001;42(8):1035–1039. 10.1111/1469-7610.00802 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46. Linazasoro G, Van Blercom N, de Zárate CO: Prevalence of tic disorder in two schools in the Basque country: Results and methodological caveats. Mov Disord. 2006;21(12):2106–2109. 10.1002/mds.21117 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47. Khalifa N, von Knorring AL: Tourette syndrome and other tic disorders in a total population of children: clinical assessment and background. Acta Paediatr. 2005;94(11):1608–1614. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48. Spencer T, Biederman M, Coffey B, et al. : The 4-year course of tic disorders in boys with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1999;56(9):842–847. 10.1001/archpsyc.56.9.842 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49. Law SF, Schachar RJ: Do typical clinical doses of methylphenidate cause tics in children treated for attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder? J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1999;38(8):944–951. 10.1097/00004583-199908000-00009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50. Carter AS, Pauls DL, Leckman JF, et al. : A prospective longitudinal study of Gilles de la Tourette's syndrome. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1994;33(3):377–385. 10.1097/00004583-199403000-00012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51. McMahon WM, Carter AS, Fredine N, et al. : Children at familial risk for Tourette's disorder: Child and parent diagnoses. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2003;121B(1):105–111. 10.1002/ajmg.b.20065 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52. Shapiro E, Shapiro AK: Semiology, nosology and criteria for tic disorders. Rev Neurol (Paris). 1986;142(11):824–832. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53. Chouinard S, Ford B: Adult onset tic disorders. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2000;68(6):738–743. 10.1136/jnnp.68.6.738 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54. Lapouse R, Monk MA: Behavior deviations in a representative sample of children: Variation by sex, age, race, social class and family size. Am J Orthopsychiatry. 1964;34(3):436–446. 10.1111/j.1939-0025.1964.tb02212.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55. Lanzi G, Zambrino CA, Termine C, et al. : Prevalence of tic disorders among primary school students in the city of Pavia, Italy. Arch Dis Child. 2004;89(1):45–47. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56. Corbett JA, Mathews AM, Connell PH, et al. : Tics and Gilles de la Tourette's syndrome: a follow-up study and critical review. Br J Psychiatry. 1969;115(528):1229–1241. 10.1192/bjp.115.528.1229 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57. Zhou KY, Xiao ZH, Chen YZ, et al. : [Clinical features and risk factors of co-morbid tic disorder in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder]. Zhongguo Dang Dai Er Ke Za Zhi. 2014;16(9):892–895. 10.7499/j.issn.1008-8830.2014.09.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58. Grados MA, Riddle MA, Samuels JF, et al. : The familial phenotype of obsessive-compulsive disorder in relation to tic disorders: the Hopkins OCD family study. Biol Psychiatry. 2001;50(8):559–565. 10.1016/S0006-3223(01)01074-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59. Leonard HL, Lenane MC, Swedo SE, et al. : Tics and Tourette's disorder: a 2- to 7-year follow-up of 54 obsessive-compulsive children. Am J Psychiatry. 1992;149(9):1244–1251. 10.1176/ajp.149.9.1244 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60. Zausmer DM, Dewey ME: Tics and heredity. A study of the relatives of child tiqueurs. Br J Psychiatry. 1987;150(5):628–634. 10.1192/bjp.150.5.628 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61. Kurlan R, Behr J, Medved L, et al. : Transient tic disorder and the spectrum of Tourette's syndrome. Arch Neurol. 1988;45(11):1200–1201. 10.1001/archneur.1988.00520350038012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62. Hyde TM, Aaronson BA, Randolph C, et al. : Relationship of birth weight to the phenotypic expression of Gilles de la Tourette's syndrome in monozygotic twins. Neurology. 1992;42(3 Pt 1):652–658. 10.1212/WNL.42.3.652 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63. Comings DE, Comings BG: Tourette's syndrome and attention deficit disorder with hyperactivity: are they genetically related? J Am Acad Child Psychiatry. 1984;23(2):138–146. 10.1097/00004583-198403000-00004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64. Leivonen S, Chudal R, Joelsson P, et al. : Prenatal maternal smoking and Tourette Syndrome: A nationwide register study. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 2016;47(1):75–82. 10.1007/s10578-015-0545-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65. Mathews CA, Scharf JM, Miller LL, et al. : Association between pre- and perinatal exposures and Tourette syndrome or chronic tic disorder in the ALSPAC cohort. Br J Psychiatry. 2014;204(1):40–45. 10.1192/bjp.bp.112.125468 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66. Motlagh MG, Katsovich L, Thompson N, et al. : Severe psychosocial stress and heavy cigarette smoking during pregnancy: an examination of the pre- and perinatal risk factors associated with ADHD and Tourette syndrome. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2010;19(10):755–764. 10.1007/s00787-010-0115-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67. Pringsheim T, Sandor P, Lang A, et al. : Prenatal and perinatal morbidity in children with Tourette syndrome and attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2009;30(2):115–121. 10.1097/DBP.0b013e31819e6a33 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68. Greene DJ, Koller JM, Robichaux-Viehoever A, et al. : Reward enhances tic suppression in children within months of tic disorder onset. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2015;11:65–74. 10.1016/j.dcn.2014.08.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69. Horesh N, Zimmerman S, Steinberg T, et al. : Is onset of Tourette syndrome influenced by life events? J Neural Transm (Vienna). 2008;115(5):787–793. 10.1007/s00702-007-0014-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70. Costello EJ, Angold A, Burns BJ, et al. : The Great Smoky Mountains Study of Youth. Goals, design, methods, and the prevalence of DSM-III-R disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1996;53(12):1129–1136. 10.1001/archpsyc.1996.01830120067012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71. Costello EJ: Personal communication to Black KJ. 1999. [Google Scholar]
  • 72. Remschmidt H, Remschmidt U: [Signs, course and prognosis of tics in children and juveniles (author's transl)]. Klin Padiatr. 1974;186(3):185–199. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73. Abe K, Oda N: Follow-up study of children of childhood tiqueurs. Biol Psychiatry. 1978;13(5):629–630. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74. Stárková L: [Tics in childhood]. Cesk Psychiatr. 1990;86(5):304–310. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75. Eldridge R, Sweet R, Lake R, et al. : Gilles de la Tourette's syndrome: clinical, genetic, psychologic, and biochemical aspects in 21 selected families. Neurology. 1977;27(2):115–124. 10.1212/WNL.27.2.115 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76. Wang HS, Kuo MF: Tourette's syndrome in Taiwan: an epidemiological study of tic disorders in an elementary school at Taipei County. Brain Dev. 2003;25(Suppl 1):S29–S31. 10.1016/S0387-7604(03)90005-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77. Spencer TJ, Biederman J, Faraone S, et al. : Impact of tic disorders on ADHD outcome across the life cycle: findings from a large group of adults with and without ADHD. Am J Psychiatry. 2001;158(4):611–617. 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.4.611 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78. Klawans HL, Barr A: Recurrence of childhood multiple tic in late adult life. Arch Neurol. 1985;42(11):1079–1080. 10.1001/archneur.1985.04060100061023 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79. Pappert EJ, Goetz CG, Louis ED, et al. : Objective assessments of longitudinal outcome in Gilles de la Tourette's syndrome. Neurology. 2003;61(7):936–940. 10.1212/01.WNL.0000086370.10186.7C [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80. Goetz CG, Tanner CM, Stebbins GT, et al. : Adult tics in Gilles de la Tourette's syndrome: description and risk factors. Neurology. 1992;42(4):784–788. 10.1212/WNL.42.4.784 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81. Bruun RD: Gilles de la Tourette's syndrome. An overview of clinical experience. J Am Acad Child Psychiatry. 1984;23(2):126–133. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82. Shin ZH, Jung CH, Kim HC: Follow-up study of the tic disorders. J Korean Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1996; 7:68–76. Reference Source [Google Scholar]
  • 83. Peterson BS, Pine DS, Cohen P, et al. : Prospective, longitudinal study of tic, obsessive-compulsive, and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorders in an epidemiological sample. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2001;40(6):685–695. 10.1097/00004583-200106000-00014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84. Bisker ER, McClelland CM, Brown LW, et al. : The long-term outcomes of ocular tics in a pediatric neuro-ophthalmology practice. J AAPOS. 2014;18(1):31–35. 10.1016/j.jaapos.2013.11.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85. Hong KE: A clinical study of tic disorder in Korea. J Korean Pediatr Soc. 1981;24(3):198–208. Reference Source [Google Scholar]
  • 86. Vrabec TR, Levin AV, Nelson LB: Functional blinking in childhood. Pediatrics. 1989;83(6):967–970. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87. Zausmer DM: The treatment of tics in childhood; a review and a follow-up study. Arch Dis Child. 1954;29(148):537–542. 10.1136/adc.29.148.537 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88. Torup E: A follow-up study of children with tics. Acta Paediatr. 1962;51:261–268. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89. Peterson BS, Staib L, Scahill L, et al. : Regional brain and ventricular volumes in Tourette syndrome. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2001;58(5):427–440. 10.1001/archpsyc.58.5.427 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90. Greene DJ, Koller JM, Schlaggar BL, et al. : “Can you stop that?” Ability to suppress tics is present within months of tic onset, and can predict future clinical outcome.In: Annual meeting, Society for Neuroscience New Orleans,2012; Poster #764.701 Reference Source [Google Scholar]
  • 91. Gadow KD, Sverd J, Sprafkin J, et al. : Long-term methylphenidate therapy in children with comorbid attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and chronic multiple tic disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1999;56(4):330–336. 10.1001/archpsyc.56.4.330 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92. Singer HS: Tourette syndrome and other tic disorders. Handb Clin Neurol. 2011;100:641–657. 10.1016/B978-0-444-52014-2.00046-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93. Singer HS: Discussing outcome in Tourette syndrome. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2006;160(1):103–105. 10.1001/archpedi.160.1.103 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94. Kushner HI: A cursing brain? The histories of Tourette syndrome.Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press,1999. [Google Scholar]
  • 95. Coffey BJ, Biederman J, Geller D, et al. : Reexamining tic persistence and tic-associated impairment in Tourette's Disorder: Findings from a naturalistic follow-up study. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2004;192(11):776–780. 10.1097/01.nmd.0000144696.14555.c4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96. Insel TR: Three promising ideas in psychiatric drug development.In: Reed JC, ed. Medscape One-on-One. [Internet]: WebMD Health Professional Network,2012. Reference Source [Google Scholar]
  • 97. Wagner-Jauregg J: The treatment of dementia paralytica by malaria inoculation (Nobel lecture) [online]. 1927. Reference Source [Google Scholar]
  • 98. de Groot CM, Bornstein RA, Spetie L, et al. : The course of tics in Tourette syndrome: a 5-year follow-up study. Ann Clin Psychiatry. 1994;6(4):227–233. 10.3109/10401239409149009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99. Bloch MH, Leckman JF, Zhu H, et al. : Caudate volumes in childhood predict symptom severity in adults with Tourette syndrome. Neurology. 2005;65(8):1253–1258. 10.1212/01.wnl.0000180957.98702.69 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 100. Bloch MH, Peterson BS, Scahill L, et al. : Adulthood outcome of tic and obsessive-compulsive symptom severity in children with Tourette syndrome. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2006;160(1):65–69. 10.1001/archpedi.160.1.65 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 101. Leckman JF, Bloch MH, King RA, et al. : Phenomenology of tics and natural history of tic disorders. Adv Neurol. 2006;99:1–16. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 102. Palermo SD, Bloch MH, Craiglow B, et al. : Predictors of early adulthood quality of life in children with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2011;46(4):291–297. 10.1007/s00127-010-0194-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 103. Altman G, Staley JD, Wener P: Children with Tourette disorder: a follow-up study in adulthood. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2009;197(5):305–310. 10.1097/NMD.0b013e3181a206b1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 104. Bruun RD, Shapiro AK, Shapiro E, et al. : A follow-up of 78 patients with Gilles de la Tourette's syndrome. Am J Psychiatry. 1976;133(8):944–947. 10.1176/ajp.133.8.944 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 105. Bruun RD, Shapiro AK, Shapiro E: A followup of eighty patients with Tourette's syndrome. Psychopharmacol Bull. 1976;12(2):15–17. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 106. Byler DL, Chan L, Lehman E, et al. : Tourette Syndrome: a general pediatrician's 35-year experience at a single center with follow-up in adulthood. Clin Pediatr (Phila). 2015;54(2):138–144. 10.1177/0009922814550396 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 107. Leckman JF: Phenomenology of tics and natural history of tic disorders. Brain Dev. 2003;25(Suppl 1):S24–S28. 10.1016/S0387-7604(03)90004-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 108. Schultz RT, Carter AS, Gladstone M, et al. : Visual-motor integration functioning in children with Tourette syndrome. Neuropsychology. 1998;12(1):134–145. 10.1037/0894-4105.12.1.134 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 109. Ziemann U, Paulus W, Rothenberger A: Decreased motor inhibition in Tourette's disorder: evidence from transcranial magnetic stimulation. Am J Psychiatry. 1997;154(9):1277–1284. 10.1176/ajp.154.9.1277 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 110. Moll GH, Wischer S, Heinrich H, et al. : Deficient motor control in children with tic disorder: evidence from transcranial magnetic stimulation. Neurosci Lett. 1999;272(1):37–40. 10.1016/S0304-3940(99)00575-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 111. Fredericksen KA, Cutting LE, Kates WR, et al. : Disproportionate increases of white matter in right frontal lobe in Tourette syndrome. Neurology. 2002;58(1):85–89. 10.1212/WNL.58.1.85 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 112. Bloch MH, Sukhodolsky DG, Leckman JF, et al. : Fine-motor skill deficits in childhood predict adulthood tic severity and global psychosocial functioning in Tourette's syndrome. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2006;47(6):551–559. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2005.01561.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 113. Woods DW, Himle MB: Creating tic suppression: comparing the effects of verbal instruction to differential reinforcement. J Appl Behav Anal. 2004;37(3):417–420. 10.1901/jaba.2004.37-417 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 114. Greene DJ, Church JA, Dosenbach NU, et al. : Multivariate pattern classification of pediatric Tourette syndrome using functional connectivity MRI. Dev Sci. 2016. 10.1111/desc.12407 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 115. Marsh R, Alexander GM, Packard MG, et al. : Habit learning in Tourette syndrome: a translational neuroscience approach to a developmental psychopathology. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2004;61(12):1259–1268. 10.1001/archpsyc.61.12.1259 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 116. Woods DW, Himle MB, Miltenberger RG, et al. : Durability, negative impact, and neuropsychological predictors of tic suppression in children with chronic tic disorder. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2008;36(2):237–45. 10.1007/s10802-007-9173-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 117. The Tourette Syndrome Classification Study Group: Definitions and classification of tic disorders. Archives of Neurology. 1993;50(10):1013–1016. 10.1001/archneur.1993.00540100012008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 118. World Health Organization: The ICD-10 Classification of Mental and Behavioural Disorders: Clinical Descriptions and Diagnostic Guidelines. Geneva: World Health Organization.1992. Reference Source [Google Scholar]
  • 119. Leckman JF, Peterson BS, Pauls DL, et al. : Tic disorders. Psychiatr Clin North Am. 1997;20(4):839–861. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 120. Singer HS, Jankovic J, Mink JW, et al. : Movement Disorders in Childhood.Philadelphia, PA: Saunders Elsevier,2010. Reference Source [Google Scholar]
  • 121. American Psychiatric Association: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition. Text Revision. Washington, DC, American Psychiatric Association.2000. Reference Source [Google Scholar]
  • 122. Kraft JT, Dalsgaard S, Obel C, et al. : Prevalence and clinical correlates of tic disorders in a community sample of school-age children. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2012;21(1):5–13. 10.1007/s00787-011-0223-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 123. Pauls DL, Kruger SD, Leckman JF, et al. : The risk of Tourette's syndrome and chronic multiple tics among relatives of Tourette's syndrome patients obtained by direct interview. J Am Acad Child Psychiatry. 1984;23(2):134–137. 10.1097/00004583-198403000-00003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 124. Kadesjo B, Gillberg C: Tourette's disorder: epidemiology and comorbidity in primary school children. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2000;39(5):548–555. 10.1097/00004583-200005000-00007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 125. Woods DW, Walther MR, Bauer CC, et al. : The development of stimulus control over tics: a potential explanation for contextually-based variability in the symptoms of Tourette syndrome. Behav Res Ther. 2009;47(1):41–47. 10.1016/j.brat.2008.10.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 126. Goetz CG, Leurgans S, Chmura TA: Home alone: methods to maximize tic expression for objective videotape assessments in Gilles de la Tourette syndrome. Mov Disord. 2001;16(4):693–697. 10.1002/mds.1159 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 127. Robertson MM, Banerjee S, Kurlan R, et al. : The Tourette Syndrome Diagnostic Confidence Index: development and clinical associations. Neurology. 1999;53(9):2108–2112. 10.1212/WNL.53.9.2108 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 128. Mol Debes NM, Hjalgrim H, Skov L: Limited knowledge of Tourette syndrome causes delay in diagnosis. Neuropediatrics. 2008;39(2):101–105. 10.1055/s-2008-1081457 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 129. The Tourette Syndrome Association: DSM-5 Diagnostic Criteria and Classification of Tourette’s Disorder [online].2015. http://tsa-usa.org/news/DSM-5.htm, Archived by WebCite® at, accessed 21 Oct 2015. Reference Source [Google Scholar]
  • 130. Roessner V, Hoekstra PJ, Rothenberger A: Tourette's disorder and other tic disorders in DSM-5: a comment. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2011;20(2):71–74. 10.1007/s00787-010-0143-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 131. Robertson MM, Eapen V: Tourette's: syndrome, disorder or spectrum? Classificatory challenges and an appraisal of the DSM criteria. Asian J Psychiatr. 2014;11:106–113. 10.1016/j.ajp.2014.05.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 132. Woods DW, Thomsen PH: Tourette and tic disorders in ICD-11: standing at the diagnostic crossroads. Rev Bras Psiquiatr. 2014;36(Suppl 1):51–58. 10.1590/1516-4446-2013-1274 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 133. Freeman RD: Diagnosis and management of Tourette syndrome: Practical aspects. Medscape Psychiatry & Mental Health eJournal. 1997;2(4):431108 Reference Source [Google Scholar]
  • 134. Debray-Ritzen P, Dubois H: [Simple tic disease in children. A report on 93 cases (author's transl)]. Rev Neurol (Paris). 1980;136(1):15–18. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 135. Kurlan R, McDermott MP, Deeley C, et al. : Prevalence of tics in schoolchildren and association with placement in special education. Neurology. 2001;57(8):1383–1388. 10.1212/WNL.57.8.1383 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 136. Goodman WK, Price LH, Rasmussen SA, et al. : The Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale. I. development, use, and reliability. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1989;46(11):1006–1011. 10.1001/archpsyc.1989.01810110048007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 137. Goodman WK, Price LH, Rasmussen SA, et al. : The Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale. II. validity. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1989;46(11):1012–1016. 10.1001/archpsyc.1989.01810110054008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 138. Leckman JF, Riddle MA, Hardin MT, et al. : The Yale Global Tic Severity Scale: Initial testing of a clinician-rated scale of tic severity. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1989;28(4):566–573. 10.1097/00004583-198907000-00015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 139. Black KJ: Chewing, rocking, pacing, echoing: Differential diagnosis and importance of stereotyped movements [v1; not peer reviewed]. F1000Res. 2016;5:387(slides) 10.7490/f1000research.1111462.1 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 140. Macfarlane JW, Allen L, Honzik MP: A Developmental Study of the Behavior Problems of Normal Children Between 21 Months and 14 Years.Berkeley, CA: University of California Press,1954. Reference Source [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 141. Merriam-Webster Incorporated. s.v. "tic". In: Merriam-Webster Online Dictionary: [Internet].2015. http://www.merriam-webster.com/dictionary/tic, Archived by WebCite® at, Accessed: 2016-04-12. Reference Source [Google Scholar]
F1000Res. 2016 May 3. doi: 10.5256/f1000research.9072.r13530

Referee response for version 1

Jeremy Stern 1

The paper is a cogent and very useful exploration of an under-considered topic by critical review which challenges certain established clinical assumptions. The difficulties of estimating the prevalence of PTD and the risk of a newly ascertained case of tics progressing to a chronic disorder are discussed in detail, based on the literature. The inevitable conclusion of a need for (expensive) further prospective study is reached, and is an aspiration shared around the world. The appendices are particularly helpful, explaining problems of terminology, ascertainment of tics and the expected lifetime prevalence of PTD in the case of various prevalences of the other tic disorders.

​I very much agree with my fellow reviewer ( http://f1000research.com/articles/5-696/v1#referee-response-13477) that the clinical term "Tourette" may have outlived its usefulness, can be unhelpful to patients and clinicians and could be improved by now being subsumed into a concept of "tic spectrum disorder", notwithstanding our reverence for its birthplace or, more practically, the freshness of DSM V.

I have read this submission. I believe that I have an appropriate level of expertise to confirm that it is of an acceptable scientific standard.

F1000Res. 2016 Apr 28. doi: 10.5256/f1000research.9072.r13533

Referee response for version 1

Andrea Cavanna 1

This is an interesting article on the nosographic category of provisional tic disorder. The authors provided a useful overview on the development of diagnostic categories for tic disorders which improve over time. The implications for clinical practice are particularly relevant.

I have read this submission. I believe that I have an appropriate level of expertise to confirm that it is of an acceptable scientific standard.

F1000Res. 2016 Apr 29.
Kevin J Black 1

Thank you.

F1000Res. 2016 Apr 20. doi: 10.5256/f1000research.9072.r13477

Referee response for version 1

Andreas Hartmann 1

The authors must be congratulated for this thoughtful and exhaustive review on a difficult topic. I hope that their ongoing initiative ( www.NewTics.info) will provide answers to many of the questions raised in the manuscript.

My comments are few and as follows :

  • The definition of tics varies but a reasonable argument has been made that they are not « abnormal » movements of vocalisations but rather normal or physiologic but occuring in an inappropriate context and/or in an inappropriate fashion. As a net result, one can of course speak of abnormal motor or vocal manifestations but as a movement disorder neurologist, I think it is important to stress that the semiology of tics does not give us clear diagnostic clues , as opposed to dystonia of parkinsonina tremor, for instance. Also, it is worth mentioning that tics can be (temporarily) suppressed, which is not the case in the vast majority of movement disorders. Finally, like it or not, « stereotyped » has been removed from DSM-5.

  • I sympathize with the idea that early intervention, especially using behavioral therapies such as CBIT, may alter disease course, maybe through cerebral plasticity. However, there are no data to support this at present, even in confirmed TS cases. Moreover, CBIT (as opposed to ERP) depends on sensing premonitory urges, which are rarely present before the age of 10, and most PTD cases are likely to fall into this category. Finally, I believe that for patients with PTD, motivation and thus adherence for these kind of treatments will be low, except for severe cases. Thus, the therapeutic angle appears slightly artificial to me.

  • Rather, and this is the main question raised in the manuscript and by parents of children with PTD, is the issue of individual prognosis, and especially conversion of of PTD into TS. The problem, at least in France, is that tics are considered benign, and TS almost malignant. In other words, the terminology we use is paramount. My personal concern is not so much whether one or several tics will persist for more than a year but how damaging they are. This brings us to the talmudic discussion on the utility of DSM criteria and even more on what Tourette syndrome means or not (the authors point out quite rightly that in current clinical pratice, we are usually light years away from the Marquise de Dampierre). If I had a wish, I would get rid of the term « Tourette syndrome » and switch to tic spectrum disorder (or something like that), although I concede this may be a mushy term. However, I don’t expect to see this happen in my lifetime.

I have read this submission. I believe that I have an appropriate level of expertise to confirm that it is of an acceptable scientific standard.

F1000Res. 2016 Apr 29.
Kevin J Black 1

We appreciate Prof. Hartmann's thoughtful comments.


Articles from F1000Research are provided here courtesy of F1000 Research Ltd

RESOURCES