Skip to main content
Elsevier Sponsored Documents logoLink to Elsevier Sponsored Documents
. 2016 May 1;131:81–90. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.09.071

A systematic review of MRI studies examining the relationship between physical fitness and activity and the white matter of the ageing brain

Claire E Sexton a,, Jill F Betts a, Naiara Demnitz a, Helen Dawes b, Klaus P Ebmeier c, Heidi Johansen-Berg a
PMCID: PMC4851455  PMID: 26477656

Abstract

Higher levels of physical fitness or activity (PFA) have been shown to have beneficial effects on cognitive function and grey matter volumes in older adults. However, the relationship between PFA and the brain's white matter (WM) is not yet well established. Here, we aim to provide a comprehensive and systematic review of magnetic resonance imaging studies examining the effects of PFA on the WM of the ageing brain. Twenty-nine studies were included in the review: eleven examined WM volume, fourteen WM lesions, and nine WM microstructure. While many studies found that higher levels of PFA were associated with greater WM volumes, reduced volume or severity of WM lesions, or improved measures of WM microstructure, a number of negative findings have also been published. Meta-analyses of global measures of WM volume and WM lesion volume yielded significant, but small, effect sizes. Overall, we found evidence for cautious support of links between PFA and WM structure, and highlighted key areas for future research including the extent to which the relationship between PFA and WM structure is anatomically specific, the influence of possible confounding factors, and the relationship between PFA, WM and cognition.

Keywords: Ageing, Fitness, Magnetic resonance imaging, Physical activity, Review, White matter

Highlights

  • We review MRI studies of PFA and the WM of the ageing brain.

  • Higher levels of PFA were often associated with improved WM outcomes.

  • Meta-analyses yielded significant, but small, effect sizes.

Introduction

Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) studies have provided key insights into the macro- and micro-structures of the brain's white matter (WM) (Walhovd et al., 2014). For example, T1-weighted MRI studies have quantified WM volumes, with either a single global measure of WM volume assessed, or WM volume analysed on a voxel-wise basis across the whole brain; proton-density, T2, T2* or fluid attenuated inversion recovery studies have examined the volume or rating of WM lesions; and diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) studies have detailed measures of WM microstructure, including fractional anisotropy (FA), mean diffusivity (MD), axial diffusivity (AD) and radial diffusivity (RD).

Such MRI studies have played a prominent role in the characterisation of WM changes with ageing, detailing decreasing WM volumes, increasing volume and severity of WM lesions, and declining WM microstructure with advancing age (Bennett and Madden, 2014, Gunning-Dixon et al., 2009, Sexton et al., 2014). In addition to describing the relationship between age and WM measures, MRI studies have also highlighted substantial between-subject variance. As a result, there is great interest in identifying factors that can help explain such heterogeneity, as well as in interventions that could slow, prevent or even reverse, age-related decline.

Higher levels of physical fitness (PF) and physical activity (PA) have been shown to have beneficial effects on cognitive function and grey matter volumes in older adults (Bherer et al., 2013, Erickson et al., 2014). However, the relationship between physical fitness or activity (PFA) and the brain's WM is not yet well established. Here, we aim to provide a systematic report of cross-sectional and longitudinal MRI studies that have examined the effects of PFA on the WM of the ageing brain. For each aspect of WM structure (WM volume, WM lesions, WM microstructure), we summarise the results to date and perform meta-analyses of global WM measures where possible. We hypothesise that higher levels of PFA would be associated with greater WM volumes, reduced volume or severity of WM lesions, or improved measures of WM microstructure.

Methods

Data sources

Online searches of the databases EMBASE and MEDLINE were performed in August 2015. An example of the search strategy used in the MEDLINE database is shown in the Supplementary Material: Fig. S1. Reference lists of included studies and relevant reviews were manually searched for additional studies.

Study selection

Two authors independently screened the title, abstracts and, where appropriate, full-text of identified citations and any disagreements were resolved by consensus. For studies to be included in the review, the following criteria had to be met:

  • (1)

    Published as a journal article or letter. While this may raise susceptibility to publication bias, restricting the search to published results serves as a guarantee of peer-reviewed quality in included reports.

  • (2)

    Assessed the level of PFA by fitness test, questionnaire or accelerometry, or administered an exercise intervention. Studies assessing mobility or motor performance were excluded, e.g. gait tests and activities of daily living questionnaires.

  • (3)

    Administered an MRI brain scan to examine one or more aspects of WM structure. Composite measures of WM spanning more than one aspect of WM structure (e.g. the single factor outcome of principal component analyses of FA and WML values in Burzynska et al., 2014) were not included, nor were DTI analyses of grey matter structure.

  • (4)

    Examined a direct association between PFA and MRI measures, a difference in MRI measures between groups that differed in PFA, or a difference in PFA measures between groups that differed in WM outcomes.

  • (5)

    Included a sample of healthy adults, with a mean age over the age of 60, who were not selected based upon the presence of medical diagnoses (including hypertension, mild cognitive impairment or dementia), or genotype. Studies that selected participants based upon PFA level, mobility or WM measures were included.

Data extraction and analysis

Two authors independently extracted the following details using a structured data abstraction form: aspect of WM structure examined (WM volume, WM lesions, WM microstructure), anatomical specificity (global or local measure of WM structure), study design (cross-sectional or longitudinal MRI assessment, interval between PFA and MRI assessments, frequency and duration of exercise intervention), participant demographics (sample size, mean age, percentage of female participants), methods (PFA and MRI assessment) and results (statistically significant findings, p < 0.05 unless a different limit was imposed by the authors).

Where possible, results are presented after co-varying for age and sex, but before co-varying for additional factors (e.g. BMI, social class, disease), with results after additional co-varying noted in table footnotes. For studies that examined WM structure locally, regions were grouped according to cerebral lobe (frontal, temporal, parietal, occipital, limbic) or tract (corpus callosum, superior longitudinal fasciculus, etc) and any lobe or tract that contained a minimum of one region that was statistically significant is presented.

Studies with overlapping samples were only excluded when the same aspect of WM structure was examined in both papers. In such cases, preference was first given to the study with the largest sample size (Tian et al., 2014a preferred to Tian et al., 2014b). Where sample size was equal, preference was given to analyses of a global measure of WM volume expressed as a percentage of intracranial volume (ICV), rather than raw values (Tseng et al., 2013b preferred to Tseng et al., 2013a), and voxel-wise analysis was preferred to region-of-interest (ROI) analysis (Liu et al., 2012 preferred to Marks et al., 2011). When studies reported multiple MRI analysis methods in a single paper (e.g. voxel-wise analysis and tract-based spatial statistics (TBSS) (Liu et al., 2012)), results from the primary analysis method is presented in the table, with results from additional methods noted in the table footnotes. Analyses in which the directionality of the relationship between PFA and WM could not be inferred were not included (e.g. differences in the prevalence of moderate WMH score, which could reflect differences in mild or severe WMH score (Frederiksen et al., 2015)). When studies used multiple measures of PFA, results are only presented from one measure (fitness test preferred to accelerometry or questionnaire, accelerometry preferred to questionnaire, kcal/week preferred to frequency of activity). This order was chosen as a result of a recent review concluding that results from cross-sectional studies of PF on grey matter were more consistent than studies of PA (Erickson et al., 2014). Finally, when both group difference and correlational analyses were performed, the primary analyses are reported in the table, with additional results discussed in the text.

Data synthesis

Data for global WM volume and global WM lesion volume were analysed using Comprehensive Meta-Analysis (version 2.2, © July 27 2011, Biostat Inc., Englewood, NJ). Effect size was measured using standardised mean differences (Cohen's) d, calculated from available statistical parameters, and a random-effects model was used to calculate the pooled mean effect size (Borenstein et al., 2010). Heterogeneity was assed using Cochrane's Q and publication bias was considered using Begg and Mazumdar rank correlations (Begg and Mazumdar, 1994). Finally, the influence of participants' characteristics (age, percentage female) and design (interval between PFA and MRI assessment) was analysed using fixed effect regression with Hedges' g.

As only one study analysed global WM microstructure, a meta-analysis of this measure was not performed. Local WM measures require co-ordinate-based or image-based meta-analysis methods (Salimi-Khorshidi et al., 2009), which were considered beyond the scope of this review.

Results

Study selection

Titles and abstracts of three hundred and ninety nine citations were screened, with twenty-nine papers included in the review. A flow diagram of the identification and attrition of studies is provided in the Supplementary Material: Fig. S2.

Global white matter volume

A total of five studies examined the relationship between a global measure of WM volume and PFA, as summarised in Table 1.

Table 1.

Studies of global white matter volume.

Study N Mean age % female Design PFA assessment MRI assessment Significant results
Cross-sectional MRI
 Benedict et al. (2013) 331 75 49.5 T1: PA, MRI Q — no. of activities/week; divided into four groups WM (% ICV) ↑ WM a
 Bugg and Head (2011) 52 69.0 ± 6.7 71.2 T1: MRI, T2: PA; T1–T2: 2.2 y Q — average MET hrs/week over 10 y; divided into two groups WM No significant results
 Burns et al. (2008) 64 72.7 ± 6.3 53.1 T1: PF, MRI FT — VO2 peak WM (% ICV) No significant results
 Gow et al. (2012) 673 70 47.3 T1: PA; T2: MRI; T1–T2: 3.2 y Q — 6-point scale NAWM (% ICV) ↑ WM b
 Tseng et al. (2013b) 20 73.4 ± 4.8 25.0 T1: PA, MRI Q — divided into masters athletes and sedentary WM (% ICV) No significant results c

Abbreviations — FT: fitness test; ICV: intracranial volume; NAWM: normal appearing white matter; Q: questionnaire; T1: time-point 1; T2: time-point 2; T1–T2: interval between time-point 1 and time-point 2; WM: white matter; y: years; ↑ indicates a significant positive relationship between PFA and MRI measures.

a

After co-varying for sex. Results remained significant following additional co-varying for education, serum LDL concentration, fasting plasma glucose, mean arterial blood pressure and abdominal visceral fat volume.

b

% female based on complete sample of 691 participants. After co-varying for age and sex. Results did not remain significant following additional co-varying for IQ, social class and disease.

c

In the same sample, raw global WM was not significantly different between masters athletes and sedentary participants.

All studies employed a cross-sectional MRI design. Although three studies did not report a significant association (Bugg and Head, 2011, Burns et al., 2008, Tseng et al., 2013b), higher levels of PA were associated with greater global WM volumes in the two largest studies (Benedict et al., 2013, Gow et al., 2012).

A meta-analysis of all five studies showed an overall small mean effect size of 0.22 (95% confidence interval (CI) = 0.10 to 0.34, p < 0.001) (Fig. 1). Studies were not significantly heterogeneous (Q4 = 0.956, p = 0.916, I2 = 0). The possibility of publication bias was assessed by calculating Begg and Mazumdar rank correlation and by inspecting a funnel plot (Fig. 2). Both suggest no publication bias: the Begg and Mazumdar rank correlation was not significant (τ = 0.100, two-tailed p = 0.81), and the funnel plot was symmetric (Fig. 2). Attempted modelling with potential confound variables (age, % female, interval between MRI and PFA assessments) was not significant.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Effect sizes (Cohen's d) for global WM volume: higher PFA is associated with greater WM volumes.

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

Funnel plot of standard errors plotted against effect sizes for studies in Fig. 2, in order to identify publication bias.

Local white matter volume

A total of seven studies employed a voxel-wise approach to examine WM volume locally, as summarised in Table 2.

Table 2.

Studies of local white matter volume.

Study N Mean age % female Design PFA assessment MRI assessment Significant results
Cross-sectional MRI
Colcombe et al. (2003) 59 66.5 ± 5.3 55.0 T1: PF, MRI FT — VO2 peak VBM No significant results
Erickson et al. (2007) 54 69.6 (58–80) 100 T1: PF, MRI FT — VO2 peak VBM ↑ frontal, ↑ corpus callosum
Gordon et al. (2008) 40 71.5 ± 4.7 57.5 T1: PF, MRI FT — VO2 max VBM No significant results
Ho et al. (2011) 226 77.9 ± 3.6 57.5 T1: PA, MRI Q — kcal/week over 2 weeks; divided into quintiles TBM ↑ corona radiata, ↑ parietal–occipital a
Honea et al. (2009) 56 73.3 ± 6.2 58.9 T1: PF, MRI FT — VO2 peak VBM No significant results b
Tseng et al. (2013b) 20 73.4 ± 4.8 25.0 T1: PA, MRI Q — divided into masters athletes and sedentary VBM ↑ temporal, ↑ parietal, ↑ occipital



Longitudinal MRI
Colcombe et al. (2006) 59 66.5 (60–79) 55.0 T1: PF, MRI; T2: PF, MRI; T1–T2: 6 m I–E: 60 min aerobic 3 × week C: 60 min stretching 3 × week VBM ↑ frontal, ↑ corpus callosum

Abbreviations — C: control; E: exercise; FT: fitness test; I: intervention; Q: questionnaire; m: months; T1: time-point 1; T2: time-point 2; T1–T2: interval between time-point 1 and time-point 2; TBS: tensor-based morphometry; VBM: voxel-based morphometry; ↑ indicates a significant positive relationship between PFA and MRI measures.

a

After co-varying for age, sex and education. Results were no longer significant following additional co-varying for BMI.

b

VBM was also performed within a limbic ROI, with no significant results.

Of six studies that examined local WM cross-sectionally, three studies did not report any significant results (Colcombe et al., 2003, Gordon et al., 2008, Honea et al., 2009). The largest study of WM volume to date, though, found that higher levels of PA were associated with increased WM volume within the corona radiata and parietal–occipital lobe (Ho et al., 2011), with each increase in PA (categorised in quintiles) associated with ~ 2–2.5% greater average WM volume. In addition, Erickson et al. (2007) reported that higher levels of PF were associated with increased frontal and corpus callosum WM volume, and Tseng et al. (2013b) found higher WM volume in temporal, parietal and occipital regions in a sample of Masters athletes compared with a sedentary control group.

One study to date has examined local WM volume before and after an exercise intervention, with the aerobic exercise group displaying a significant increase in frontal WM and corpus callosum volume compared with the stretching group (Colcombe et al., 2006).

Global white matter lesions

A total of fourteen studies have examined the relationship between global measures of WM lesions and PFA, with results summarised in Table 3.

Table 3.

Studies of global white matter lesions.

Author N Mean age % female Design PFA assessment MRI outcome Significant results
Cross-sectional MRI
 Burzynska et al. (2014) 88 65 ± 4 62.5 T1: PF, MRI FT — VO2 max VOL No significant results a
 Carmelli et al. (1999) 148 72.7 ± 2.1 0 T1: PA, MRI Q VOL No significant results
 Fleischman et al. (2015) 167 80.1 ± 6.7 79.0 T1: PA, MRI A — total daily activity VOL No significant results
 Frederiksen et al. (2015) 282 73.1 ± 5.1 58.2 T1: MRI; T2: PA Q — divided into active and inactive RAT No significant results
 Gow et al. (2012) 676, 685 70 47.3 T1: PA; T2: MRI; T1–T2: 3.2 y Q — 6-point scale VOL, RAT ↓ VOL, ↓ RAT b
 Ho et al. (2011) 226 77.9 ± 3.6 57.5 T1: PA, MRI Q — kcal/week over 2 weeks; divided into quintiles RAT No significant results
 Rosano et al. (2010) 30 81.0 ± 3.6 73.3 T1: PA; T2: PA; T3: PA, MRI; T1–T2: 1 y; T2–T3: 2 y I (T1–T2) and Q (T3) E: T1–T2 > 150 min walking/week, T2–T3 continued activity; C: T1–T2 education, T2–T3 < 20 min activity/week VOL No significant results
 Saczynski et al. (2008) 1787 75.7 ± 5.5 58.2 T1: PA, MRI Q — h MVPA/week; divided into inactive and active RAT (upper quartile) ↓ RAT
 Sen et al. (2012) 715 65 ± 8 54.0 T1: PF, MRI FT — VO2 estimate VOL ↓ VOL c
 Tian et al. (2014a) 276 72.9 ± 2.7 58.7 T1: MRI; T2: PA; T1–T2: ~ 8–11 y Q — kcal/week; divided into three groups VOL (median split) No significant results
 Tseng et al. (2013a) 20 73.4 ± 4.8 25.0 T1: PA, MRI Q — divided into masters athletes and sedentary VOL No significant results
 Willey et al. (2011) 1238 70 ± 9 59.6 T1: PA; T2: MRI; T1–T2: 6 y Q — MET score over 2 weeks; divided into quartiles VOL No significant results
 Wirth et al. (2014) 92 75.2 ± 5.6 63.0 T1: PA, MRI Q — kcal/12 months VOL ↓ VOL d
 Zheng et al. (2012) 287 77.8 ± 4.5 53.7 T1: PA, MRI Q — h/week VOL (median split) No significant results

Abbreviations — C: control; E: exercise; FT: fitness test; I: intervention; Q: questionnaire; RAT: rating; T1: time-point 1; T2: time-point 2; T1–T2: interval between time-point 1 and time-point 2; VOL: volume; y: years; ↓ indicates a significant negative relationship between PFA and MRI measures; ↑ indicates a significant positive relationship between PFA and MRI measures.

a

WM lesion volume was significantly associated with an accelerometry-derived measure of moderate-to-vigorous PA.

b

% female based on complete sample of 691 participants. After co-varying for age and sex. Total volume remained significant, but total rating did not remain significant, following co-varying for age, sex, IQ, social class and disease.

c

After co-varying for age and sex. Results remained significant after additional adjustment for hypertension, diabetes, smoking, cholesterol, BMI, beta-blocker and calcium-channel-blocker treatment.

d

After co-varying for age, sex and education. Results remained significant after additional adjustment for subjective memory functioning, depression or socioeconomic status.

All studies employed a cross-sectional MRI design and primary analyses were not significant for ten studies (Burzynska et al., 2014, Carmelli et al., 1999, Fleischman et al., 2015, Frederiksen et al., 2015, Ho et al., 2011, Rosano et al., 2010, Tian et al., 2014b, Tseng et al., 2013a, Willey et al., 2011, Zheng et al., 2012). However, Saczynski et al. (2008) found that participants in the upper quartile of WML load were more likely to be physically inactive than those in the lower three quartiles; Sen et al. (2012) reported that increased PF was associated with lower total WM lesion volume, and Wirth et al. (2014) found that higher PA was associated with reduced WM lesion volume. Furthermore, Gow et al. (2012) found that a higher level of PA at baseline was associated with decreased WM lesion volume and combined rating of periventricular and deep WM lesions at 3-year follow-up.

A meta-analysis of nine studies that assessed global WM lesion volume continuously showed an overall small mean effect size of − 0.165 (95% confidence interval (CI) = − 0.26 to − 0.07, p = 0.001 (Fig. 3). Studies were not significantly heterogeneous (Q8 = 10.6, p = 0.22, I2 = 25). Begg and Mazumdar rank correlation was not significant with τ = − 0.25 (two-tailed p = 0.35), and a funnel plot also did not indicate significant publication bias (Fig. 4). Attempted modelling with potential confound variables was not significant.

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3

Effect sizes (Cohen's d) for total WM lesion volume: higher PFA associated with reduced WM lesion volume.

Fig. 4.

Fig. 4

Funnel plot of standard errors plotted against effect sizes for studies in Fig. 4, in order to identify publication bias.

Local white matter lesions

Two studies have examined the relationship between local measures of WM lesions and PFA, with results summarised in Table 4.

Table 4.

Studies of local white matter lesions.

Author N Mean age % female Design PFA assessment MRI outcome Significant results
Cross-sectional MRI
Tseng et al. (2013a) 20 73.4 ± 4.8 25.0 T1: PA, MRI Q — divided into masters athletes and sedentary VOL-P, VOL-D ↓ VOL-D



Longitudinal MRI
Podewils et al. (2007) 60 77.3 ± 5.8 61.7 T1: PA, MRI; T2: MRI; T1–T2: 5 y Q — kcal/week over 2 weeks RAT-P, RAT-D ↑ RAT-P, ↑ RAT-D a

Abbreviations — C: control; E: exercise; FT: fitness test; I: intervention; Q: questionnaire; RAT-D: rating — deep white matter; RAT-P: rating — periventricular white matter; T1: time-point 1; T2: time-point 2; T1–T2: interval between time-point 1 and time-point 2; VOL-D: volume — deep white matter; VOL-P: volume — periventricular white matter; y: years; ↓ indicates a significant negative relationship between PFA and MRI measures; ↑ indicates a significant positive relationship between PFA and MRI measures.

a

Model adjusted for age, sex, ethnicity, education, APOE genotype and corresponding baseline WM lesion score. Additional adjustment for other cardiovascular risk factors and follow-up time did not affect the results.

In a cross-sectional study that did not detect a difference in global WM lesion volume between Masters athletes and a sedentary control group, Tseng et al. (2013a) reported an 83% reduction in deep WMH volume.

In contrast, in a longitudinal MRI study, Podewils et al. (2007) found that a higher level of PA at baseline was associated with an increased rate of periventricular and deep WM lesion progression.

Global white matter microstructure

Only one study examined the relationship between PFA and global measures of WM microstructure using DTI, with results summarised in Table 5.

Table 5.

Studies of global white matter microstructure.

Study N Mean age % female Design PFA assessment MRI assessment Measure Significant results
Cross-sectional MRI
 Gow et al. (2012) 691 70 47.3 T1: PA; T2: MRI; T1–T2: 3.2 y Q — 6-point scale Tractography (global — 12 tracts) FA ↑ global a
MD No significant results b
AD No significant results
RD No significant results

Abbreviations — AD: axial diffusivity; FA: fractional anisotropy; MD: mean diffusivity; Q: questionnaire; RD: radial diffusivity; T1: time-point 1; T2: time-point 2; T1–T2: interval between time-point 1 and time-point 2; y: years; ↑ indicates a significant positive relationship between PFA and MRI measures.

a

After co-varying for age and sex. Results were no longer significant following additional co-varying for IQ, social class and disease.

b

After co-varying for age and sex. Results did not change following additional co-varying for IQ, social class and disease.

Gow et al. (2012) reported that higher levels of PA were associated with increased FA at 3-year follow-up, with no significant relationships for diffusivity values.

Local white matter microstructure

A total of eight studies have examined the relationship between PFA and WM microstructure locally using DTI, with results summarised in Table 6.

Table 6.

Studies of local white matter microstructure.

Study N Mean age % female Design PFA assessment MRI assessment Measure Significant results
Cross-sectional MRI
Burzynska et al. (2014) 88 65 ± 4 62.5 T1: PF, MRI FT — VO2 max ROI on TBSS skeleton (temporal, CC, CING, SLF) FA No significant results a
Johnson et al. (2012) 26 64.8 ± 2.8 53.8 T1: PF, MRI FT — composite (VO2 peak, FT time, 1-min HR recovery) TBSS FA ↑ CC
TBSS (FA significant) MD No significant results
TBSS (FA significant) AD No significant results
TBSS (FA significant) RD ↓ CC
Liu et al. (2012) 15 66.2 ± 5.8 46.7 T1: PA, MRI Q — min/week aerobic over 10 y; divided into two groups Voxel-wise FA ↑ SLF, AFb
Marks et al. (2011) 15 66.2 ± 5.8 46.7 T1: PF, MRI FT — VO2 peak ROI (cingulum) MD No significant results
Tian et al. (2014a) 276 72.9 ± 2.7 58.7 T1: MRI; T2: PA; T1–T2: ~ 8–11 y Q — kcal/week; divided into three groups ROI (SLF, UNF) FA No significant results c
Tian et al. (2014b) 164 82.9 ± 2.6 57.1 T1: PF, MRI FT — 400 m walk time ROI (CING) FA No significant results
Tseng et al. (2013a) 20 73.4 ± 4.8 25.0 T1: PA, MRI Q — divided into masters athletes and sedentary TBSS FA ↑ SLF, SCR, IFOF, ILFs
MD ↓ CING, PTR



Longitudinal MRI
Voss et al. (2012) 70 64.9 ± 4.5 64.3 T1: PF, MRI; T2: PF, MRI; T1–T2: 1 y I–E: 40 min walking 3 × week C: 60 min flexibility 3 × week ROI on TBSS skeleton (frontal, temporal, parietal, occipital) FA No significant results
AD No significant results
RD No significant results

Abbreviations — AD: axial diffusivity; AF: arcuate fasciculus; C: control; CC: corpus callosum; CING: cingulum; E: exercise; FA: fractional anisotropy; FT: fitness test; I: intervention; IFOF: inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus; ILF: inferior longitudinal fasciculus; MD: mean diffusivity; PTR: posterior thalamic radiation; Q: questionnaire; RD: radial diffusivity; ROI: region-of-interest; SCR: superior corona radiata; SLF: superior longitudinal fasciculus; T1: time-point 1; T2: time-point 2; T1–T2: interval between time-point 1 and time-point 2; TBSS: tract-based spatial statistics; UNF: uncinate fasciculus; y: years; ↓ indicates a significant negative relationship between PFA and MRI measures. ↑ indicates a significant positive relationship between PFA and MRI measures.

a

Exploratory whole-brain TBSS also showed no significant results. FA within a temporal ROI was associated with an accelerometry-defined measure of light PA.

b

No significant results were detected with voxel-wise TBSS analysis. In a ROI analysis of the cingulum in the same sample, a positive relationship between VO2 peak and FA was reported (Marks et al., 2011).

c

In a sub-sample of 164 participants, 400 m walk time at T1 was not associated with FA within superior longitudinal fasciculus and uncinate fasciculus (Tian et al., 2014b).

Seven studies examined WM microstructure cross-sectionally. Of seven studies that examined FA, three studies reported that higher levels of PFA were associated with increased FA: within the corpus callosum (Johnson et al., 2012), superior longitudinal fasciculus and arcuate fasciculus (Liu et al., 2012), and superior longitudinal fasciculus, superior corona radiate, inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus and inferior longitudinal fasciculus (Tseng et al., 2013a). Of three studies that examined MD, findings were not significant for two studies (Johnson et al., 2012, Marks et al., 2011), while Tseng et al. (2013a) reported decreases in MD in non-overlapping regions to their FA results, within the cingulum and posterior thalamic radiation. Only one study examined AD and RD, with Johnson et al. (2012) finding that reductions in RD accompanied increases in FA.

In an interventional study, Voss et al. (2012) did not find any group-level difference for FA, AD or RD. However, greater percentage change in fitness was associated with significant increases in prefrontal, parietal and temporal FA within the exercise group, with no significant relationships detected within the control group. Furthermore, correlations were significantly different between groups for prefrontal and temporal FA.

Discussion

Summary

Over recent years, MRI has become an increasingly popular research technique in the PFA field, and a growing number of studies are now using MRI to investigate the relationship between PFA and WM measures in healthy older adults. Overall, we view the evidence to date with cautious optimism; while many studies have found that higher levels of PFA are associated with greater WM volumes, reduced volume and severity of WM lesions or improved measures of WM microstructure, a number of negative findings have also been published. Meta-analyses of global measures of WM volume and WM lesion volume further support this standpoint, demonstrating significant, but small, effect sizes.

Anatomy of findings

A key outstanding question identified by this review is the extent to which the relationship between PFA and WM structure is anatomically specific. First, it has been hypothesised that PFA is associated with global measures of WM structure. Despite several negative reports, our meta-analyses found that higher levels of PFA were significantly associated with higher global WM volume and smaller global volume of WM lesions, although effect sizes were small in both cases. Some support for a global effect can also be drawn from the positive relationship observed using DTI between PA levels and global FA (Gow et al., 2012), although more studies of this nature would be necessary for a more confident conclusion.

Second, it is hypothesised that the relationship between PFA and WM structure is localised to specific regions. A recent review of MRI studies that examined the relationship between PFA and grey matter volume reported consistent evidence within the hippocampus and frontal cortex (Erickson et al., 2014). While our review did not identify consistent evidence to support localised WM results within the temporal lobe, it does highlight some complementary results with regard to the frontal lobe. For example, higher PF levels have been associated with greater volumes in prefrontal WM tracts cross-sectionally (Erickson et al., 2007), a 6-month aerobic exercise intervention has been shown to lead to increases in volumes in anterior WM compared with a stretching program (Colcombe et al., 2006), and improvements in PF have been reported to correlate with increases in prefrontal FA following a one-year aerobic exercise intervention (Voss et al., 2012). However, while it is encouraging that positive findings have been reported across different WM measures and study designs, there has not yet been consistent replication of such findings.

While few studies to date have examined WM measures both globally and locally, it is important to note that global and local relationships between PFA and WM are not mutually exclusive. In fact, in a review of age-related changes in cognition, Bennett and Madden (2014) found that there was evidence to support both global and tract-specific changes in WM microstructure. In the case of PFA and WM structure, the evidence, although more sparse, seems to indicate a similar combination of global and localised effects. To further develop this discussion, it would be desirable for future studies to report both global and local changes associated with PFA.

Methodological considerations

Studies included in this review varied in their focus, quality, design and participant demographics — limiting the extent to which results are directly comparable.

A formal quality assessment revealed that, overall, studies were of good quality (Supplementary Material: Table S1). All studies provided a good description of sample characteristics and PFA–WM findings, however, there was variation in the extent to which the methods used to assess PFA and WM outcomes were detailed, reporting of PFA and WM outcome measures for the included participants, sample size (range 15–1787), and reporting of exact p values. In addition, there was variation in the extent to which characteristics of excluded participants were described. Often, limitations reflect the relationship between PFA and WM not being the primary focus of a study, rather than the overall quality of the paper. For the two intervention studies (Colcombe et al., 2006, Voss et al., 2012), risk of selection bias was judged to be unclear, as the methods for random sequence generation and allocation concealment were not specified. Performance bias and detection bias were judged to be low-risk: blinding of participants and trainers is typically not feasible in aerobic exercise interventions, while automated imaging analysis techniques minimise the possibility researcher bias. In addition, in Colcombe et al. (2006) WM analyses were performed by a researcher who was blind to the group assignment of each individual. Attrition bias was also judged to be low-risk/unclear.

With regard to design, although twenty-one of twenty-four studies administered a MRI scan at a single time-point, the order of and interval between MRI and PFA assessments varied from PA assessment occurring 3.2 years prior to MRI scan (Gow et al., 2012) to PA assessment being administered 8–11 years after MRI scan (Tian et al., 2014a). For both global WM volume and global WM lesion volume, though, meta-regressions between effect size and the interval between time-points did not reveal significant associations. Only two studies examined WM measures before and after an aerobic exercise intervention (Colcombe et al., 2006, Voss et al., 2012), of 6-months and 12-months in length, respectively. While these studies reported some encouraging findings, further studies are needed to offer insights into the optimal frequency, intensity, time and type of exercise for WM health.

With regard to participant characteristics, studies included in this review were notably heterogeneous in terms of mean age (range 65 to 83 years) and gender distribution (range 0–100% female), and also varied in terms of the inclusion and exclusion criteria employed.

Advancing age is associated with reductions in WM volume, increasing severity of WM lesion and an accelerated decline in WM microstructure (Bennett and Madden, 2014, Gunning-Dixon et al., 2009); and a key outstanding question concerns the trajectory of the association between WM structure and PFA with age. One possibility is that greater levels of PFA are particularly beneficial in later life. Indeed, in a study of adults aged between 55 and 79 years that did not detect a main effect of PF, it was reported that age-related WM volume loss in frontal, parietal and temporal cortices was reduced as a result of PF (Colcombe et al., 2003). Such findings concur with a meta-analysis of studies examining cognitive function in older adults, which found that adults aged between 55 and 65 years benefitted least from PA interventions, compared with both adults aged between 66 and 70 years and 71 and 80 years (Colcombe and Kramer, 2003). However, in our analyses of global WM volume and global WM lesion volume, meta-regressions with age did not reveal significant associations between effect size and mean age. Furthermore, given it is possible that the brain's ability to adapt and change could actually wane with age, further research into the effect of age on the relationship between PFA and WM structure is warranted.

While beneficial effects of PFA on cognitive function have been reported in both male-only (Schuit et al., 2001, Van Gelder et al., 2004) and female-only (Weuve et al., 2004, Yaffe et al., 2001) populations, a meta-analysis of intervention studies in older adults found that cognitive benefits were significantly greater in studies in which the majority of participants were female (Colcombe and Kramer, 2003). Whether the relationship between PFA and WM structure is different in males and females, though, is not yet clear. For example, in contrast to cognitive findings, in a study of 715 participants, of whom 54% were female, the protective effect of PF on WM lesions was found to be only significant in males (Sen et al., 2012). Also, for both global WM volume and global WM lesion volume, the meta-regression with percentage of female participants in this review was not significant, and more research is needed.

Studies also varied in terms of inclusion and exclusion criteria. For example, some observational and intervention studies limited their sample to participants who displayed low levels of PA at baseline, while studies that administered fitness tests often employed strict criteria regarding vascular risk factors. It is important to note that results from such studies may not necessarily generalise to wider populations. In contrast, cohort studies that administered questionnaire-based measures of PA typically employed less stringent criteria, and such differences in inclusion and exclusion criteria may lead to a systematic bias in findings.

Finally, while the links between PFA and cognition are well documented (Bherer et al., 2013, Colcombe and Kramer, 2003, Smith et al., 2010), it is important to highlight that few studies to date have directly explored whether such links are mediated by the effects of PFA on WM structure. Encouragingly, in a path model, Wirth et al. (2014) demonstrated that current PA had a beneficial effect on global cognitive functioning via the mediation of WM lesions. However, exercise-induced increases in FA were not associated with improvements in backward digit span performance in an interventional study (Voss et al., 2012). Further studies examining domain-specific effects will be of great interest to the field.

Mechanisms of action

A number of pathways may explain the observed relationships between PFA and WM structure. First, PFA may lead directly to improved WM structure. This hypothesis is supported by interventional studies that have shown that a 6-month aerobic exercise intervention leads to increases in volumes compared with a stretching program (Colcombe et al., 2006), and that improvements in PF over the course of a 12-month program correlate with increases in FA (Voss et al., 2012). Such a direct link may be mediated by a number of possible neurobiological mechanisms.

For example, cerebrovascular health and perfusion play an important role in WM health. In addition to periods of hypoxia, damage to the vascular wall, leakage of fluid into the surrounding tissue and disruption of the blood–brain-barrier have all been identified as contributing factors to the formation of WM lesions (Pantoni, 2002, Schmidt et al., 2007). PFA, however, has well-known beneficial effects on the vascular system, including the preservation of arterial elasticity and wall integrity, reduction in arterial stiffness and blood pressure (McDonnell et al., 2013). PFA may thus be protective against potentially damaging cerebrovascular decline. Indeed, MR angiography studies have shown an association between the number and integrity of small blood vessels and aerobic fitness in older adults (Bullitt et al., 2009), as well as many studies reporting increased capillary density with an exercise in animals (Black et al., 1990, Ding et al., 2006, Isaacs et al., 1992, Swain et al., 2003). As such, improved vascular health and cerebral perfusion as a result of PFA may contribute positively to WM health and structure via improved oxygen and nutrient delivery.

The brain's response to exercise involves several neurotrophic factors, including brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF), insulin-like growth factor 1 (IGF-1) and vascular endothelial growth factor (VEGF). The strongest evidence implicates BDNF, which is increased in the expression in the rat brain in response to exercise (Neeper et al., 1996), as well as being positively linked with exercise-induced change in hippocampal volume in humans (Erickson et al., 2012) and improved functional connectivity (Voss et al., 2010). BDNF is especially interesting due to its role in the growth and survival of many neuronal subtypes, as well as in synaptic plasticity and normal axonal pruning (Cao et al., 2007, Singh et al., 2008). In addition to providing neuroprotection against hypoxic–ischaemic insult in an age-dependent fashion (Cheng et al., 1997), BDNF promotes the regeneration of injured axons in the adult rat brain (Mamounas et al., 2000) and has been shown to have a neuroprotective influence on white matter in both rodents and humans (Husson et al., 2005, Weinstock-Guttman et al., 2007). Higher secretion of BDNF in multiple sclerosis patients has been linked with higher WM volume, as well as higher inflammatory activity (Weinstock-Guttman et al., 2007), suggesting protective up-regulation. Increased expression of BDNF in response to PFA may thus have a neuroprotective influence, positively promoting WM structure and integrity. It is notable, though, that many genes that are up-regulated in response to exercise interact with BDNF. IGF-1 is a potent survival factor for neurons and oligodendrocytes, and has also been linked to improved functional connectivity after exercise, along with BDNF and VEGF (Voss et al., 2013). While the evidence is strongest for the effects of BDNF in the exercise response, it is likely that its effects are supported by both IGF-1 and VEGF and it will be important for future studies to include measures of all these factors.

Though not consistently linked, there is also some evidence to suggest an association between exercise and proliferation of oligodendrocyte progenitor cells (OPCs), which continue to generate new oligodendrocytes throughout adulthood, allowing for continued myelination. For example, in a study of voluntary exercise, running increased the number of immature and mature oligodendrocytes in the spinal cord of the mouse (Krityakiarana et al., 2010), and OPC proliferation has been reported to increase in the hippocampus by 30% after 7 days of running (Matsumoto et al., 2011). As such, the extent of axonal myelination may alter in response to exercise, mediated via increased oligodendrocyte proliferation and differentiation.

PFA may also lead to improved WM structure via third variables. For example, PFA is associated with general improvements in health and wellbeing, including positive effects on the cardiovascular system, body composition and mental health, with many of these factors having also been shown to be related to WM structure in ageing populations (Raz and Rodrigue, 2006). Several of the cross-sectional MRI studies included in this review included possible confounding variables as additional covariates in their analyses, in order to examine the degree to which such factors influence the relationship between PFA and WM structure. While, in the majority of analyses, results remained significant after taking such factors into account (Benedict et al., 2013, Gow et al., 2012, Sen et al., 2012, Wirth et al., 2014), notably Ho et al. (2011) reported that PA was no longer statistically correlated with WM volume after BMI was included in their model. In both studies that examined the effects of an aerobic exercise intervention, a control group that participated in stretching and toning classes meant that effects could not be attributed to increased social engagement associated with an exercise program (Colcombe et al., 2006, Voss et al., 2012). Moving forward, it will be important for future studies to systematically examine a range of factors hypothesised to be protective or detrimental to WM structure in the ageing process to examine the relative weight and inter-dependency of factors.

Finally, compromised WM structure may have an impact on an individual's ability to participate in physical activity, and indeed, an increased risk of impaired mobility, gait and balance dysfunction has been identified in individuals with WM changes (Srikanth et al., 2009, Starr, 2003, Wakefield et al., 2010). WM integrity may itself be a propagator of reduced physical activity, and the causal directionality of the two factors may be reversed.

Conclusions

The study of the relationship between PFA and WM structure is still in its infancy. From the studies reviewed here, a promising tendency towards a positive relationship between PFA levels and WM structure emerged. Although encouraging, we remain cautious in our conclusions due to the small size of effects and a number of negative findings. Throughout this review, we have called attention to areas warranting further research. Specifically, the development of this research area would benefit from studies: (1) that further examine the anatomical specificity of the relationship between PFA and WM; (2) that offer insights into the optimal frequency, intensity, time and type of exercise; (3) that address how gender and age mediate the PFA–WM relationship; (4) that explore whether PFA-related effects on WM translate to cognition; and (5) that distinguish between biological mechanisms which may lay behind such effects (e.g. vascular health, cerebral perfusion, and neurotrophic factors). Ultimately, although many questions remain unanswered, PFA remains a promising candidate in the search for factors that can reduce or delay the deteriorating effects of age on WM structure.

Acknowledgments

The research was supported by the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Oxford Biomedical Research Centre based at Oxford University Hospitals NHS Trust and University of Oxford. The views expressed are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the NHS, the NIHR or the Department of Health. HJB is supported by the Wellcome Trust (WT090955AIA)

Footnotes

Appendix A

Supplementary data to this article can be found online at http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.09.071.

Appendix A. Supplementary data

Supplementary material Figure S1: Search criteria.

Figure S2: Identification and attrition of papers.

Table S1: Assessment of the focus and quality of included studies.

mmc1.docx (104KB, docx)

References

  1. Begg C.B., Mazumdar M. Operating characteristics of a rank correlation test for publication bias. Biometrics. 1994;50:1088–1101. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Benedict C., Brooks S.J., Kullberg J., Nordenskjöld R., Burgos J., Le Grevès M., Kilander L., Larsson E.-M., Johansson L., Ahlström H., Lind L., Schiöth H.B. Association between physical activity and brain health in older adults. Neurobiol. Aging. 2013;34:83–90. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2012.04.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Bennett I.J., Madden D.J. Disconnected aging: cerebral white matter integrity and age-related differences in cognition. Neuroscience. 2014;276:187–205. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2013.11.026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bherer L., Erickson K.I., Liu-Ambrose T. A review of the effects of physical activity and exercise on cognitive and brain functions in older adults. J. Aging Res. 2013;2013:657508. doi: 10.1155/2013/657508. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Black J.E., Isaacs K.R., Anderson B.J., Alcantara A.A., Greenough W.T. Learning causes synaptogenesis, whereas motor activity causes angiogenesis, in cerebellar cortex of adult rats. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 1990;87:5568–5572. doi: 10.1073/pnas.87.14.5568. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Borenstein M., Hedges L.V., Higgins J.P.T., Rothstein H.R. A basic introduction to fixed-effect and random-effects models for meta-analysis. Res. Synth. Methods. 2010;1:97–111. doi: 10.1002/jrsm.12. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bugg J.M., Head D. Exercise moderates age-related atrophy of the medial temporal lobe. Neurobiol. Aging. 2011;32:506–514. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2009.03.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bullitt E., Rahman F.N., Smith J.K., Kim E., Zeng D., Katz L.M., Marks B.L. The effect of exercise on the cerebral vasculature of healthy aged subjects as visualized by MR angiography. AJNR Am. J. Neuroradiol. 2009;30:1857–1863. doi: 10.3174/ajnr.A1695. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Burns J.M., Cronk B.B., Anderson H.S., Donnelly J.E., Thomas G.P., Harsha A., Brooks W.M., Swerdlow R.H. Cardiorespiratory fitness and brain atrophy in early Alzheimer disease. Neurology. 2008;71:210–216. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000317094.86209.cb. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Burzynska A.Z., Chaddock-Heyman L., Voss M.W., Wong C.N., Gothe N.P., Olson E.A., Knecht A., Lewis A., Monti J.M., Cooke G.E., Wojcicki T.R., Fanning J., Chung H.D., Awick E., McAuley E., Kramer A.F. Physical activity and cardiorespiratory fitness are beneficial for white matter in low-fit older adults. PLoS One. 2014;9 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0107413. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Cao L., Dhilla A., Mukai J., Blazeski R., Lodovichi C., Mason C.A., Gogos J.A. Genetic modulation of BDNF signaling affects the outcome of axonal competition in vivo. Curr. Biol. 2007;17:911–921. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2007.04.040. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Carmelli D., Swan G.E., Reed T., Wolf P.A., Miller B.L., DeCarli C. Midlife cardiovascular risk factors and brain morphology in identical older male twins. Neurology. 1999;52:1119–1124. doi: 10.1212/wnl.52.6.1119. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Cheng Y., Gidday J.M., Yan Q., Shah A.R., Holtzman D.M. Marked age-dependent neuroprotection by brain-derived neurotrophic factor against neonatal hypoxic–ischemic brain injury. Ann. Neurol. 1997;41:521–529. doi: 10.1002/ana.410410416. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Colcombe S., Kramer A.F. Fitness effects on the cognitive function of older adults: a meta-analytic study. Psychol. Sci. 2003;14:125–130. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.t01-1-01430. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Colcombe S.J., Erickson K.I., Raz N., Webb A.G., Cohen N.J., McAuley E., Kramer A.F. Aerobic fitness reduces brain tissue loss in aging humans. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 2003;58:176–180. doi: 10.1093/gerona/58.2.m176. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Colcombe S.J., Erickson K.I., Scalf P.E., Kim J.S., Prakash R., McAuley E., Elavsky S., Marquez D.X., Hu L., Kramer A.F. Aerobic exercise training increases brain volume in aging humans. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 2006;61:1166–1170. doi: 10.1093/gerona/61.11.1166. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Ding Y.-H., Li J., Zhou Y., Rafols J.A., Clark J.C., Ding Y. Cerebral angiogenesis and expression of angiogenic factors in aging rats after exercise. Curr. Neurovasc. Res. 2006;3:15–23. doi: 10.2174/156720206775541787. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Erickson K.I., Colcombe S.J., Elavsky S., McAuley E., Korol D.L., Scalf P.E., Kramer A.F. Interactive effects of fitness and hormone treatment on brain health in postmenopausal women. Neurobiol. Aging. 2007;28:179–185. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2005.11.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Erickson K.I., Miller D.L., Roecklein K.A. The aging hippocampus: interactions between exercise, depression, and BDNF. Neuroscientist. 2012;18:82–97. doi: 10.1177/1073858410397054. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Erickson K.I., Leckie R.L., Weinstein A.M. Physical activity, fitness, and gray matter volume. Neurobiol. Aging. 2014;35(Suppl. 2):S20–S28. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2014.03.034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Fleischman D.A., Yang J., Arfanakis K., Arvanitakis Z., Leurgans S.E., Turner A.D., Barnes L.L., Bennett D.A., Buchman A.S. Physical activity, motor function, and white matter hyperintensity burden in healthy older adults. Neurology. 2015;84:1294–1300. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000001417. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Frederiksen K.S., Verdelho A., Madureira S., Bäzner H., O'Brien J.T., Fazekas F., Scheltens P., Schmidt R., Wallin A., Wahlund L.-O., Erkinjunttii T., Poggesi A., Pantoni L., Inzitari D., Waldemar G. Physical activity in the elderly is associated with improved executive function and processing speed: the LADIS Study. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry. 2015;30:744–750. doi: 10.1002/gps.4220. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Gordon B.A., Rykhlevskaia E.I., Brumback C.R., Lee Y., Elavsky S., Konopack J.F., McAuley E., Kramer A.F., Colcombe S., Gratton G., Fabiani M. Neuroanatomical correlates of aging, cardiopulmonary fitness level, and education. Psychophysiology. 2008;45:825–838. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2008.00676.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Gow A.J., Bastin M.E., Muñoz Maniega S., Valdés Hernández M.C., Morris Z., Murray C., Royle N.A., Starr J.M., Deary I.J., Wardlaw J.M. Neuroprotective lifestyles and the aging brain: activity, atrophy, and white matter integrity. Neurology. 2012;79:1802–1808. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3182703fd2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Gunning-Dixon F.M., Brickman A.M., Cheng J.C., Alexopoulos G.S. Aging of cerebral white matter: a review of MRI findings. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry. 2009;24:109–117. doi: 10.1002/gps.2087. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Ho A.J., Raji C.A., Becker J.T., Lopez O.L., Kuller L.H., Hua X., Dinov I.D., Stein J.L., Rosano C., Toga A.W., Thompson P.M. The effects of physical activity, education, and body mass index on the aging brain. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2011;32:1371–1382. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Honea R.A., Thomas G.P., Harsha A., Anderson H.S., Donnelly J.E., Brooks W.M., Burns J.M. Cardiorespiratory fitness and preserved medial temporal lobe volume in Alzheimer disease. Alzheimer Dis. Assoc. Disord. 2009;23:188–197. doi: 10.1097/WAD.0b013e31819cb8a2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Husson I., Rangon C.-M., Lelièvre V., Bemelmans A.-P., Sachs P., Mallet J., Kosofsky B.E., Gressens P. BDNF-induced white matter neuroprotection and stage-dependent neuronal survival following a neonatal excitotoxic challenge. Cereb. Cortex. 2005;15:250–261. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhh127. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Isaacs K.R., Anderson B.J., Alcantara A.A., Black J.E., Greenough W.T. Exercise and the brain: angiogenesis in the adult rat cerebellum after vigorous physical activity and motor skill learning. J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab. 1992;12:110–119. doi: 10.1038/jcbfm.1992.14. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Johnson N.F., Kim C., Clasey J.L., Bailey A., Gold B.T. Cardiorespiratory fitness is positively correlated with cerebral white matter integrity in healthy seniors. Neuroimage. 2012;59:1514–1523. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.08.032. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Krityakiarana W., Espinosa-Jeffrey A., Ghiani C.A., Zhao P.M., Topaldjikian N., Gomez-Pinilla F., Yamaguchi M., Kotchabhakdi N., de Vellis J. Voluntary Exercise Increases Oligodendrogenesis in Spinal Cord. 2010;120(4):280–290. doi: 10.3109/00207450903222741. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Liu Z., Farzinfar M., Katz L.M., Zhu H., Goodlett C.B., Gerig G., Styner M., Marks B.L. Automated voxel-wise brain DTI analysis of fitness and aging. Open. Med. Imaging J. 2012;6:80–88. [Google Scholar]
  33. Mamounas L.A., Altar C.A., Blue M.E., Kaplan D.R., Tessarollo L., Lyons W.E. BDNF promotes the regenerative sprouting, but not survival, of injured serotonergic axons in the adult rat brain. J. Neurosci. 2000;20:771–782. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-02-00771.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Marks B.L., Katz L.M., Styner M., Smith J.K. Aerobic fitness and obesity: relationship to cerebral white matter integrity in the brain of active and sedentary older adults. Br. J. Sports Med. 2011;45:1208–1215. doi: 10.1136/bjsm.2009.068114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Matsumoto Y., Tsunekawa Y., Nomura T., Suto F., Matsumata M., Tsuchiya S., Osumi N. Differential proliferation rhythm of neural progenitor and oligodendrocyte precursor cells in the young adult hippocampus. PLoS One. 2011;6 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0027628. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. McDonnell B.J., Maki-Petaja K.M., Munnery M., Yasmin, Wilkinson I.B., Cockcroft J.R., McEniery C.M. Habitual exercise and blood pressure: age dependency and underlying mechanisms. Am. J. Hypertens. 2013;26:334–341. doi: 10.1093/ajh/hps055. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Neeper S.A., Gómez-Pinilla F., Choi J., Cotman C.W. Physical activity increases mRNA for brain-derived neurotrophic factor and nerve growth factor in rat brain. Brain Res. 1996;726:49–56. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Pantoni L. Pathophysiology of age-related cerebral white matter changes. Cerebrovasc. Dis. 2002;13(Suppl. 2):7–10. doi: 10.1159/000049143. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Podewils L.J., Guallar E., Beauchamp N., Lyketsos C.G., Kuller L.H., Scheltens P. Physical activity and white matter lesion progression: assessment using MRI. Neurology. 2007;68:1223–1226. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000259063.50219.3e. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Raz N., Rodrigue K.M. Differential aging of the brain: patterns, cognitive correlates and modifiers. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2006;30:730–748. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2006.07.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Rosano C., Venkatraman V.K., Guralnik J., Newman A.B., Glynn N.W., Launer L., Taylor C.A., Williamson J., Studenski S., Pahor M., Aizenstein H. Psychomotor speed and functional brain MRI 2 years after completing a physical activity treatment. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 2010;65:639–647. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glq038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Saczynski J.S., Jonsdottir M.K., Sigurdsson S., Eiriksdottir G., Jonsson P.V., Garcia M.E., Kjartansson O., van Buchem M.A., Gudnason V., Launer L.J. White matter lesions and cognitive performance: the role of cognitively complex leisure activity. J. Gerontol. Ser. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 2008;63:848–854. doi: 10.1093/gerona/63.8.848. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Salimi-Khorshidi G., Smith S.M., Keltner J.R., Wager T.D., Nichols T.E. Meta-analysis of neuroimaging data: a comparison of image-based and coordinate-based pooling of studies. Neuroimage. 2009;45:810–823. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.12.039. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Schmidt R., Petrovic K., Ropele S., Enzinger C., Fazekas F. Progression of leukoaraiosis and cognition. Stroke. 2007;38:2619–2625. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.107.489112. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Schuit A.J., Feskens E.J., Launer L.J., Kromhout D. Physical activity and cognitive decline, the role of the apolipoprotein e4 allele. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 2001;33:772–777. doi: 10.1097/00005768-200105000-00015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Sen A., Gider P., Cavalieri M., Freudenberger P., Farzi A., Schallert M., Reichmann F., Watzinger N., Zweiker R., Schmidt R., Schmidt H. Association of cardiorespiratory fitness and morphological brain changes in the elderly: results of the Austrian Stroke Prevention Study. Neurodegener. Dis. 2012;10:135–137. doi: 10.1159/000334760. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Sexton C.E., Walhovd K.B., Storsve A.B., Tamnes C.K., Westlye L.T., Johansen-Berg H., Fjell A.M. Accelerated changes in white matter microstructure during aging: a longitudinal diffusion tensor imaging study. J. Neurosci. 2014;34:15425–15436. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0203-14.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Singh K.K., Park K.J., Hong E.J., Kramer B.M., Greenberg M.E., Kaplan D.R., Miller F.D. Developmental axon pruning mediated by BDNF-p75NTR-dependent axon degeneration. Nat. Neurosci. 2008;11:649–658. doi: 10.1038/nn.2114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Smith P.J., Blumenthal J.A., Hoffman B.M., Cooper H., Strauman T.A., Welsh-Bohmer K., Browndyke J.N., Sherwood A. Aerobic exercise and neurocognitive performance: a meta-analytic review of randomized controlled trials. Psychosom. Med. 2010;72:239–252. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e3181d14633. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Srikanth V., Beare R., Blizzard L., Phan T., Stapleton J., Chen J., Callisaya M., Martin K., Reutens D. Cerebral white matter lesions, gait, and the risk of incident falls: a prospective population-based study. Stroke. 2009;40:175–180. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.108.524355. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Starr J.M. Brain white matter lesions detected by magnetic resonance imaging are associated with balance and gait speed. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry. 2003;74:94–98. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.74.1.94. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Swain R., Harris A., Wiener E., Dutka M., Morris H., Theien B., Konda S., Engberg K., Lauterbur P., Greenough W. Prolonged exercise induces angiogenesis and increases cerebral blood volume in primary motor cortex of the rat. Neuroscience. 2003;117:1037–1046. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(02)00664-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Tian Q., Erickson K.I., Simonsick E.M., Aizenstein H.J., Glynn N.W., Boudreau R.M., Newman A.B., Kritchevsky S.B., Yaffe K., Harris T.B., Rosano C. Physical Activity Predicts Microstructural Integrity in Memory-related Networks in Very Old Adults. J. Gerontol. A. Biol. Sci. Med: Sci. 2014;69(10):1284–1290. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glt287. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Tian Q., Simonsick E.M., Erickson K.I., Aizenstein H.J., Glynn N.W., Boudreau R.M., Newman A.B., Kritchevsky S.B., Yaffe K., Harris T., Rosano C. Cardiorespiratory fitness and brain diffusion tensor imaging in adults over 80 years of age. Brain Res. 2014;1588:63–72. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2014.09.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Tseng, Gundapuneedi T., Khan M., Diaz-Arrastia R., Levine B., Lu H., Huang H., Zhang R. White matter integrity in physically fit older adults. Neuroimage. 2013;82:510–516. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.06.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Tseng, Uh J., Rossetti H.C., Munro Cullum C., Diaz-Arrastia R.F., Levine B.D., Lu H., Zhang R. Masters athletes exhibit larger regional brain volume and better cognitive performance than sedentary older adults. J. Magn. Reson. Imaging. 2013;38(5):1169–1176. doi: 10.1002/jmri.24085. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Van Gelder B.M., Tijhuis M.A.R., Kalmijn S., Giampaoli S., Nissinen A., Kromhout D. Physical activity in relation to cognitive decline in elderly men: the FINE study. Neurology. 2004;63:2316–2321. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000147474.29994.35. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Voss M.W., Prakash R.S., Erickson K.I., Basak C., Chaddock L., Kim J.S., Alves H., Heo S., Szabo A.N., White S.M., Wójcicki T.R., Mailey E.L., Gothe N., Olson E.A., McAuley E., Kramer A.F. Plasticity of brain networks in a randomized intervention trial of exercise training in older adults. Front. Aging Neurosci. 2010;2 doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2010.00032. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Voss M.W., Heo S., Prakash R.S., Erickson K.I., Alves H., Chaddock L., Szabo A.N., Mailey E.L., Wójcicki T.R., White S.M., Gothe N., McAuley E., Sutton B.P., Kramer A.F. The influence of aerobic fitness on cerebral white matter integrity and cognitive function in older adults: results of a one-year exercise intervention. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2012;34(11):2972–2985. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22119. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Voss M.W., Erickson K.I., Prakash R.S., Chaddock L., Kim J.S., Alves H., Szabo A., Phillips S.M., Wójcicki T.R., Mailey E.L., Olson E.A., Gothe N., Vieira-Potter V.J., Martin S.A., Pence B.D., Cook M.D., Woods J.A., McAuley E., Kramer A.F. Neurobiological markers of exercise-related brain plasticity in older adults. Brain Behav. Immun. 2013;28:90–99. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2012.10.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Wakefield D.B., Moscufo N., Guttmann C.R., Kuchel G.A., Kaplan R.F., Pearlson G., Wolfson L. White matter hyperintensities predict functional decline in voiding, mobility, and cognition in older adults. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 2010;58:275–281. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2009.02699.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Walhovd K.B., Johansen-Berg H., Káradóttir R.T. Unraveling the secrets of white matter—bridging the gap between cellular, animal and human imaging studies. Neuroscience. 2014;276:2–13. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2014.06.058. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Weinstock-Guttman B., Zivadinov R., Tamaño-Blanco M., Abdelrahman N., Badgett D., Durfee J., Hussein S., Feichter J., Patrick K., Benedict R., Ramanathan M. Immune cell BDNF secretion is associated with white matter volume in multiple sclerosis. J. Neuroimmunol. 2007;188:167–174. doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroim.2007.06.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Weuve J., Kang J.H., Manson J.E., Breteler M.M.B., Ware J.H., Grodstein F. Physical activity, including walking, and cognitive function in older women. JAMA. 2004;292:1454–1461. doi: 10.1001/jama.292.12.1454. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Willey J.Z., Moon Y.P., Paik M.C., Yoshita M., Decarli C., Sacco R.L., Elkind M.S.V., Wright C.B. Lower prevalence of silent brain infarcts in the physically active: the Northern Manhattan Study. Neurology. 2011;76:2112–2118. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e31821f4472. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Wirth M., Haase C.M., Villeneuve S., Vogel J., Jagust W.J. Neuroprotective pathways: lifestyle activity, brain pathology, and cognition in cognitively normal older adults. Neurobiol. Aging. 2014;35:1873–1882. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2014.02.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Yaffe K., Barnes D., Nevitt M., Lui L.Y., Covinsky K. A prospective study of physical activity and cognitive decline in elderly women: women who walk. Arch. Intern. Med. 2001;161:1703–1708. doi: 10.1001/archinte.161.14.1703. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Zheng J.J.J., Delbaere K., Close J.C.T., Sachdev P., Wen W., Brodaty H., Lord S.R. White matter hyperintensities are an independent predictor of physical decline in community-dwelling older people. Gerontology. 2012;58:398–406. doi: 10.1159/000337815. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary material Figure S1: Search criteria.

Figure S2: Identification and attrition of papers.

Table S1: Assessment of the focus and quality of included studies.

mmc1.docx (104KB, docx)

RESOURCES