Abstract
Objective
The present cross-sectional study aimed to: (1) examine associations between measures of psychological well-being, specifically life satisfaction and life engagement, and intima media thickness, a subclinical marker of atherosclerosis; (2) investigate if the interaction of psychological well-being and life events correlated with intima media thickness; and (3) explore these relationships across race.
Methods
A sample of 485 women (38% African American and 62% white; mean (SD) age = 50.2 (2.9)) underwent ultrasonography to assess carotid artery intima media thickness (IMT). The women completed self-report measures of life satisfaction, life engagement, and life events.
Results
Average (SD) IMT was 0.666 (0.10) mm. Life satisfaction showed a significant, independent, inverse relationship with IMT, after controlling for demographic, behavioral, psychological, and cardiovascular covariates (β = −.105, p = .039), such that each 1-point higher life satisfaction score was correlated with a significant 0.008 mm lower level of mean IMT. No significant association was seen between life events and IMT (r = 0.05, p = .32), and life satisfaction did not interact with life events on IMT (β = −.036, p = .46). No significant interaction between life satisfaction and race on IMT was observed (β = .068, p = .37). In contrast to life satisfaction, life engagement was not a significant correlate of IMT (r = −.07, p = .12).
Conclusions
Life satisfaction, a measure of psychological well-being, is an important independent correlate of subclinical atherosclerosis in middle-aged women.
Keywords: psychological well-being, life satisfaction, intima media thickness, atherosclerosis, women, race
INTRODUCTION
Intima media thickness (IMT) of the carotid arteries is a known marker of subclinical atherosclerotic disease, after multiple studies confirmed its association with higher risk for coronary heart disease (1), myocardial infarction (2–7), stroke (2,6), and cardiovascular mortality (5,8). Population-based, prospective studies, including the Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities (ARIC) Study and the Cardiovascular Health Study (CHS), have shown that carotid IMT is an independent predictor of coronary artery disease and cardiovascular events (3,4,6,9). Greater carotid IMT values have also been linked to cardiovascular risk factors, including smoking (1,10–12), male gender (1,13), aging (1,13–15), African American ethnicity (16,17), higher blood pressure (1,10,12,13), elevated low density lipoprotein cholesterol (1,10,12,14), elevated trigylcerides (10), and increased body mass index (BMI) (1,13).
Studies have found significant relationships between psychosocial factors and IMT. Specifically, higher IMT scores have been significantly correlated with greater psychological distress including greater hopelessness (18–20), hostility and cynical distrust (19,21–23), anger suppression (21,22), trait anger (24–26), depressive symptoms (27–29), feelings of discontent (30), sustained anxiety (31), general occupational stress (32), job strain (33,34), high job demand (35), and irritability in adults with low childhood SES (36). Overall, measures of psychosocial distress have been demonstrated to be significantly elevated with increased IMT and in certain studies, have been predictive of progression of IMT over multiple years (18,19,21,25,28,30,31).
While negative psychosocial factors have been shown to be higher in those with greater IMT, constructs capturing psychological well-being have been inversely associated with IMT in a small number of studies. In the Pittsburgh Healthy Women Study of midlife women, those who were more optimistic showed less progression of IMT over 3 years, after controlling for biological, behavioral, and medication covariates (37). Similarly, postmenopausal women in the Pittsburgh Healthy Women Study who reported greater marital satisfaction showed slower 3-year IMT progression than women with low levels of marital satisfaction (38), and a similar inverse correlation between positive marital interactions and IMT was recently shown in a study of healthy middle-aged cohabitating or married partners (39). Relationships between protective psychosocial factors and IMT as a marker of atherosclerosis may be promising but need further exploration.
Differences in the association of psychosocial factors and IMT across race are less established, though some studies have shown differential associations. Anger has been shown to be associated with greater IMT in one multiethnic study of men and women, with the association being more robust in whites (24). In contrast, trait anger has been associated with greater IMT in African American men in a second study (26). In a sample of African American and white women enrolled in the Study of Women’s Health across the Nation (SWAN) Pittsburgh site, higher levels of IMT have been directly associated with chronic stress and unfair treatment among African American women only (40). Studies of psychological well-being and IMT have mostly been conducted on white men and women, though one study of 60 African Americans assessed the impact of a transcendental meditation stress reduction intervention on progression of IMT (41). Participants randomly assigned to the intervention showed a significant decrease in IMT over a 6- to 9-month follow-up period when compared to those assigned to a cardiovascular risk factor health education control group, even after controlling for pre-intervention IMT values and age. This preliminary intervention study highlighted the importance of considering the role of both positive as well as negative psychosocial constructs on progression of IMT in African Americans. Overall, the association between IMT and the interaction of race and psychosocial stressors and well-being factors is preliminary and needs further investigation.
For the purposes of this cross-sectional study, we examined the relationship between psychological well-being and atherosclerosis in middle-aged women enrolled in SWAN Heart. Life satisfaction and life engagement capture components of psychological well-being, with life satisfaction measuring the match between expectations and life experience and life engagement measuring the extent one feels engaged in valued activities (42,43). Given both constructs represent cognitive evaluations of psychological well-being, we hypothesized that higher life satisfaction and life engagement would be associated with lower IMT, a subclinical marker of atherosclerosis. We also hypothesized that the association between our constructs of psychological well-being and IMT was dependent on number of life events. Others have proposed that greater psychological well-being may be a buffer in the face of stressful life events (44,45). It is also possible that with a greater frequency of life events, the relationship between psychological well-being and IMT may change, and this may be most salient for the association of IMT and life satisfaction, which assesses the congruence between current life experience and expectations. Finally, we explored the association of IMT with the interaction of psychological well-being and race because of prior findings in SWAN Heart coupled with the beneficial findings for one psychosocial stress reduction intervention in African Americans (41).
METHODS
Participants
Participants for the current study were recruited from the Chicago, Illinois, and Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania, sites of the Study of Women’s Health Across the Nation (SWAN), a multiethnic, prospective multi-site study of the menopause transition in women between the ages of 42–52 years. A detailed description of the SWAN study design is published elsewhere (46).
At the Chicago (Rush University) and Pittsburgh (University of Pittsburgh) sites, the SWAN Heart Ancillary Study, which began about 4 years after SWAN baseline, was conducted to assess subclinical atherosclerosis during the menopause. The current cross-sectional study only used data collected from women who enrolled in the SWAN Heart Ancillary Study, corresponding with one of their SWAN annual visits 4, 5, 6, or 7. Eligibility at enrollment for the SWAN Heart Ancillary Study included white or African American ethnicity only, no current use of hormone therapy, and no evidence of clinical atherosclerosis including myocardial infarction, angina, intermittent claudication, cerebral ischemia, and/or revascularization. Informed consent was reviewed and signed by all study participants prior to enrollment, and the institutional review boards at both the Chicago and Pittsburgh sites approved the study protocol. Analyses for the current study were performed on all participants in the SWAN Heart Ancillary Study for whom an IMT value and measures of life engagement and life satisfaction were collected.
Procedure
Participants at the Pittsburgh site of SWAN were recruited into the study using random-digit dialing and lists of eligible women from voter registration. At the Chicago site, participants were randomly selected from a complete census of a defined community area. Participants completed psychosocial and sociodemographic measures, carotid ultrasound, and assessment of anthropometric and blood pressure measurements.
Measures
Ultrasound Measurement
During the ancillary study visit, ultrasound of the carotid artery intima media thickness (IMT) was assessed by a Toshiba SSA-270A scanner in Pittsburgh and a Hewlitt Packard 5500 scanner in Chicago. These machines have comparable image quality, and the procedure followed for ultrasound IMT assessment was standardized across both sites. B-mode images of common carotid artery were digitized for measurement of IMT. Specifically, images were taken of the near and far walls of the distal common carotid artery one centimeter proximal to the carotid bulb, the far wall of the carotid bulb, and the internal carotid artery. To measure the average intimal medial thickening, these images were displayed on a workstation monitor, and two lines were electronically drawn along the lumen-intima surface and the media-adventitia interface on a one-centimeter segment of the carotid artery. The computer generated one measurement for each pixel over this area, for a total of about 140 measures. An average of measures from each location was used to calculate an overall measure of IMT (millimeters). Readings of the ultrasound scans were done at the University of Pittsburgh, under the supervision of Dr. Sutton-Tyrrell, and those performing the readings were annually recertified to minimize reader drift. Repeated readings of 20 ultrasound scans were performed to assess reproducibility, and results included an intraclass correlation of 0.98 for IMT values (23).
Psychosocial Measurement
Satisfaction with Life Scale
The Satisfaction with Life Scale (SWLS) is a 5-item scale measuring an individual’s overall satisfaction with life, designed to be used in a non-psychiatric adult population (42). Responses on the SWLS range on a 7-point scale (1=strongly disagree, 7=strongly agree), with all 5 items framed in a positive direction. All items in the SWLS were averaged, with higher scores representing greater overall life satisfaction. Internal consistency of the SWLS was high (Cronbach’s α = .92). Test-retest reliability for the SWLS was .50–.84 in prior studies (42).
Life Engagement Test
The Life Engagement Test (LET) is a 6-item scale measuring one’s present involvement in life through value and importance assigned to one’s activities (43). The LET is an index of current life purpose. Responses range on a 5-point scale (1=strongly disagree, 5=strongly agree), with 3 items framed in a positive direction and 3 items framed in a negative direction. The 3 items framed in a negative direction were reverse coded, then all items were averaged, with higher scores representing greater life engagement. Cronbach’s α for the LET was .87, indicating good internal consistency. In prior studies, test-retest reliability ranged from .61 to .76 (43).
Life Events Scale
The Life Events Scale, a self-report measure of stressful life events, was derived from the Psychiatric Epidemiology Research Interview (47) and modified to include 18 events that were particularly relevant for middle-aged women. Participants were asked to indicate whether the event occurred. A higher score on the Life Events Scale represented a higher number of life events.
Covariates
Covariates for the analyses included relevant demographic, behavioral, psychological, and cardiovascular risk factors of IMT. Sociodemographic factors were assessed through self-reported questionnaires: race (African American, white), education (high school education or less, some college, college degree, or graduate schooling), smoking status (current smoker, not a current smoker), and age. Menopausal status was assessed via self-reported bleeding and categorized as: 1. premenopausal (menstrual period in the past 3 months with no irregularities in the past year), 2. perimenopausal (either menstrual period in the past 3 months with some irregularity over the previous year or no menstrual period in the past 3 months but menstrual bleeding within the past year), and 3. postmenopausal (no menstrual period within the past year). Body mass index was calculated as weight in kilograms divided by the square of height in meters (kg/m2). Systolic (SBP) and diastolic (DBP) blood pressure (mmHg) measurements were calculated as an average of two seated manual readings on the right arm following a 5-minute rest period. Finally, self-reported depressive symptoms were assessed using the well-validated Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale, a summed 20-item measure (range: 0 to 60) with higher scores signifying greater depressive symptoms (48).
Data Analyses
Data analyses were performed on all SWAN Heart Ancillary Study participants who had an IMT value and who completed the life engagement and life satisfaction measures. The study sample was characterized by age, race, education, menopausal status, smoking status, and marital status. Bivariate associations (Spearman correlations) between IMT and the psychological well-being measures were examined. Regression analyses (PROC GLM) were performed to test the association between IMT and psychological well-being. Regression analyses (PROC GLM) were also used the test the association between IMT and the interaction of psychological well-being and life events (e.g., life satisfaction × life events) as well as IMT and the interaction of psychological well-being and race (e.g., life satisfaction × race). In these models, the main effects for each appropriate construct were also included (i.e., psychological well-being (centered), life events (centered), and race (dummy-coded)). Demographic, behavioral, psychological, and cardiovascular risk factors were included as covariates in these models. All analyses were performed using SAS, version 9.4 (SAS Institute, Cary, NC).
RESULTS
Characteristics of the Sample
Descriptive statistics for the total sample as well as by race are presented in Table 1. The overall sample included 485 women, of which 186 (38%) were African American and 299 (62%) were white. The women were on average 50 years old (range: 45 to 58 years), fairly well-educated, mostly non-smokers, and overweight with an average BMI of 29 kg/m2. The majority were married, and over half were perimenopausal.
Table I.
Variable | Total (n = 485) |
African American (n = 186) |
White (n = 299) |
---|---|---|---|
Age [y, M (SD)] | 50.2 (2.9) | 50.3 (2.9) | 50.1 (2.9) |
Education [%] | |||
≤ High School Diploma | 15.6 | 17.5 | 14.4 |
Some College | 31.2 | 38.4 | 26.8 |
College Degree | 21.8 | 19.8 | 23.0 |
Graduate School | 31.4 | 24.3 | 35.8 |
Menopausal Status [%] | |||
Pre | 29.8 | 32.1 | 28.4 |
Peri | 60.0 | 58.7 | 60.9 |
Post | 10.1 | 9.2 | 10.7 |
Smoking Status [%] | |||
Non-Smoker | 85.1 | 84.3 | 85.5 |
Current Smoker | 14.9 | 15.7 | 14.5 |
Marital Status [%] | |||
Single | 9.1 | 9.8 | 8.7 |
Married | 70.0 | 52.7 | 80.6 |
Separated | 3.1 | 4.9 | 2.0 |
Divorced | 15.3 | 29.3 | 6.7 |
Widowed | 2.5 | 3.3 | 2.0 |
y = years; M = mean; SD = standard deviation.
As seen in Table 2, on average, the women reported high levels of life engagement (mean = 4.16, sd = 0.5), though some did report low level of life engagement (range of means = 1.6 to 5.0). They also reported relatively high levels of life satisfaction (mean = 5.10, sd = 1.2), though the range of scores was wide (range of means = 1.2 to 7.0). The scores on the life engagement and life satisfaction measures were normally distributed.
Table II.
Variable | Total | African American | White |
---|---|---|---|
Mean IMT [mm, M (SD)] | 0.666 (0.10) | 0.685*** (0.10) | 0.654 (0.09) |
Life Satisfaction [M (SD)] | 5.10 (1.20) | 4.79 (1.27) | 5.28*** (1.11) |
Life Engagement [M (SD)] | 4.16 (0.54) | 4.16 (0.58) | 4.17 (0.51) |
Life Event Number [M (SD)] | 3.24 (2.39) | 3.84*** (2.51) | 2.91 (2.26) |
BMI [kg/m2, M (SD)] | 29.3 (6.3) | 30.8*** (6.7) | 28.3 (5.8) |
SBP [mmHg, M (SD)] | 119.9 (17.0) | 127.0*** (18.7) | 115.5 (14.1) |
DBP [mmHg, M (SD)] | 76.2 (10.1) | 80.4*** (9.9) | 73.6 (9.3) |
Depressive Symptoms [M (SD)] | 7.28 (8.13) | 7.51 (8.53) | 7.14 (7.89) |
M = mean; SD = standard deviation.
p values were obtained from t tests performed to compare African American and white women.
p < .001.
It was noted that certain variables were significantly different across race, as shown by unadjusted data presented in Table 2. Specifically, life satisfaction was significantly higher for white than for African American women (p < .001), but there was no significant difference in life engagement across race (p = .85). However, African American women reported significantly more life events than white women (p < .001). Also, there was a significant difference in IMT across race (p < .001), with African American women having higher average IMT values than white women. Finally, BMI, SBP, and DBP were all significantly greater in African American women than in white women (all p’s < .001).
Associations between Psychological Well-Being and IMT
Spearman correlations revealed IMT was significantly correlated with life satisfaction (r = −.14, p = .002) across the total sample and for African American women (r = −.17, p = .02) only but not for white women (r = -.06, p = .30). The association between life engagement and IMT was weak and non-significant for the overall sample as well as for African American and white women (r = −.07, r = −.05, and r = −.08, respectively; all p’s > .10). Number of life events significantly correlated with life satisfaction (r = −.19, p < .001) but was not significantly correlated with mean IMT (r = .05, p = .32). Life satisfaction and life engagement had significant, but only modest correlations with depressive symptoms (r = −.37, p < .001; r = −.35, p < .001, respectively), while correlations between life satisfaction and life engagement and other covariates were small (r’s = −.005 to −.10).
Regression analyses were used to determine if psychological well-being was an independent correlate of IMT, after controlling for relevant demographic variables, behavioral and psychological covariates, and established cardiovascular risk factors. In the initial model, age, race, education, and menopausal status were included as covariates. In a subsequent model, behavioral and cardiovascular covariates, including smoking status, BMI, SBP, and DBP, were added to the analysis along with the demographic covariates. In a final model, a psychological measure was added to the list of previously included covariates. Results for all regression models are presented in Table 3.
Table III.
Model 1 (R2 = .09) | Model 2 (R2 = .16) | Model 3 (R2 = .17) | ||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Variable | B | SE(B) | β | p | B | SE(B) | β | p | B | SE(B) | β | p |
Life Satisfaction | −0.008 | 0.004 | −0.095 | 0.041 | −0.010 | 0.004 | −0.124 | 0.009 | −0.008 | 0.004 | −0.105 | 0.039 |
Race | ||||||||||||
African American | 0.022 | 0.009 | 0.111 | 0.016 | 0.007 | 0.010 | 0.034 | 0.50 | 0.008 | 0.010 | 0.039 | 0.44 |
White | Referent | Referent | Referent | |||||||||
Age | 0.007 | 0.002 | 0.217 | <.001 | 0.005 | 0.002 | 0.146 | 0.012 | 0.005 | 0.002 | 0.149 | 0.010 |
Menopausal Status | ||||||||||||
Post | 0.026 | 0.018 | 0.080 | 0.16 | 0.022 | 0.018 | 0.070 | 0.24 | 0.022 | 0.018 | 0.070 | 0.24 |
Peri | 0.014 | 0.011 | 0.071 | 0.22 | 0.016 | 0.011 | 0.082 | 0.17 | 0.016 | 0.011 | 0.085 | 0.15 |
Pre | Referent | Referent | Referent | |||||||||
Education | ||||||||||||
≤ High School | 0.036 | 0.013 | 0.136 | 0.008 | 0.039 | 0.014 | 0.153 | 0.004 | 0.039 | 0.014 | 0.152 | 0.004 |
Some College | 0.020 | 0.011 | 0.099 | 0.068 | 0.012 | 0.012 | 0.061 | 0.28 | 0.013 | 0.012 | 0.065 | 0.25 |
College Degree | −0.002 | 0.012 | −0.007 | 0.90 | 0.009 | 0.013 | 0.037 | 0.50 | 0.009 | 0.013 | 0.039 | 0.48 |
Graduate School | Referent | Referent | Referent | |||||||||
BMI | 0.003 | 0.001 | 0.217 | <.001 | 0.003 | 0.001 | 0.212 | <.001 | ||||
Smoker | ||||||||||||
Current Smoker | 0.028 | 0.013 | 0.105 | 0.028 | 0.029 | 0.013 | 0.111 | 0.021 | ||||
Non-smoker | Referent | Referent | ||||||||||
SBP | 0.001 | 0.0004 | 0.218 | 0.004 | 0.001 | 0.0004 | 0.217 | 0.004 | ||||
DBP | −0.001 | 0.001 | −0.156 | 0.032 | −0.001 | 0.001 | −0.153 | 0.035 | ||||
Depressive Symptoms | 0.001 | 0.001 | 0.056 | 0.26 |
BMI = body mass index; SBP = systolic blood pressure; DBP = diastolic blood pressure; B = unstandardized regression coefficients; SE(B) = standard error of the regression coefficients; β = standardized regression coefficients.
Model 1 included life satisfaction and demographic covariates. Model 2 included life satisfaction, demographic, behavioral, and cardiovascular covariates. Model 3 included life satisfaction, depressive symptoms, demographic, behavioral, and cardiovascular covariates.
The initial regression analysis (Table 3, Model 1) on IMT with life satisfaction and demographic covariates was significant. Life satisfaction was a significant, independent correlated of IMT (p = .041). Other significant covariates were age, having less than or equal to a high school education (referent: graduate school education), and African American race (referent: white). In this model, life satisfaction independently correlated with IMT after controling for all demographic covariates, such that each 1-point higher life satisfaction score was associated with a significant 0.008 mm lower level in mean IMT.
A subsequent regression analysis (Table 3, Model 2) was performed to examine the association between life satisfaction and IMT while adjusting for behavioral and cardiovascular risk factors as well as demographic covariates. In this adjusted model, which was also significant, life satisfaction remained a significant, independent correlate of IMT (p = .009). Specifically, each 1-point higher life satisfaction score was associated with a significant 0.010 mm lower level in mean IMT. Age and education remained significant covariates, though after adjustments were made for BMI, SBP, DBP, and current smoking, race was no longer significantly correlated with IMT. A third model (Table 3, Model 3) examined the association of life satisfaction and IMT adjusting for depressive symptoms along with previously included demographic, behavioral, and cardiovascular risk factors. In this fully-adjusted, significant model, life satisfaction remained a significant, independent correlate of IMT (p = .039), such that each 1-point higher life satisfaction score was associated with a significant 0.008 mm lower level in mean IMT. As in the second model, age, education, smoking, BMI, SBP, and DBP remained significant covariates, while the additional covariate, depressive symptoms, was not a significant correlate of IMT.
In a separate multivariable model, life events did not significantly moderate the association between life satisfaction and IMT, after controlling for demographic, behavioral, and cardiovascular covariates (p = .46). Our second marker of psychological well-being, life engagement was not a significant correlate of IMT (p = .12), and in a multivariable model, the association between IMT and the interaction of life events and life engagement, after controlling for demographic, behavioral, and cardiovascular covariates, was not significant (p = .87).
Regression analyses were performed to test whether the relationship between life satisfaction and IMT varied by race. After controlling for demographic, behavioral, and cardiovascular covariates, no significant interaction was observed between life satisfaction and race on IMT (p = .37).
DISCUSSION
This study aimed to examine the association between carotid IMT and measures of psychological well-being, both directly and through their interaction with life stressors, and explore differences in these associations across race. Support was found for the hypothesis that psychological well-being, measured by life satisfaction, was inversely correlated with IMT. Results demonstrated that level of life satisfaction was a significant inverse correlate of mean IMT values, when adjusting for relevant demographic and behavioral covariates and established risk factors of subclinical atherosclerotic disease. Interestingly, life satisfaction continued to be significantly correlated with IMT, even after inclusion of depressive symptoms, which failed to significantly correlate with IMT. Though the effect size for life satisfaction was small, it remained significantly correlated with IMT along with known cardiovascular risk factors. This finding supports the few prior studies that have reported a relationship between psychosocial well-being and lower IMT (37–39). It is also consistent with findings from the Healthy Women Study, in which an association was found between low life satisfaction and being in the subgroup of women with the highest scores on aortic calcification, another important marker of atherosclerosis (49). Finally, it shows that life satisfaction, as a marker of psychological well-being, is significantly associated with IMT independent of depressive symptoms, a marker of psychological distress. This finding is similar to that seen in the Whitehall Study of 7942 adults, for which positive psychological well-being was associated with decreased risk of CHD and the association was only slightly weakened after adjusting for emotional distress (50).
A similar relationship was not found between life engagement and IMT. Conceptual differences between life satisfaction and life engagement exist, which may help clarify this finding. Life engagement aims at measuring a current sense of purpose in one’s life (43). As a measure of psychological well-being, this construct captures the degree of worth associated with an individual’s current life activities. Meanwhile, life satisfaction captures the quality of one’s overall life experience; therefore, according to Pavot and Diener (42), life satisfaction represents a cognitive evaluation of whether one’s life meets self-set expectations. Individuals whose appraisal of their overall life is more consistent with their own expectations may be more affectively stable and psychologically adaptable and thus, experience greater overall life satisfaction (51). The sense of life balance and general adaptability characteristic of individuals with greater life satisfaction, but not necessarily characteristic of those with greater life engagement, may translate to better health outcomes.
This association between life satisfaction and improved health may occur by means of improved vagal tone, decreased arousal of the sympathetic nervous system and the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis, dampened cardiovascular reactivity, decreased metabolic syndrome, improved lifestyle behaviors, and less dysregulation of immune function (45,52–56). Life satisfaction may also result in stability of mood, given its potential impact on affective states (57,58), and some studies have shown preliminary links between mood and IMT (19,27–31). In the present study, we found that life satisfaction and life engagement had significant, but modest associations with depressive symptoms; therefore, in our sample, psychological well-being is related yet distinct from negative affective. Additional studies examining the interaction of affective states and life satisfaction in their relationship to IMT would be of interest.
This study also aimed to investigate if the frequency of life events moderated the relationship between psychological well-being and IMT. Given life satisfaction is conceptualized as the match between life experiences and expectations, this study hypothesized that higher frequency of life events may temper the relationship between life satisfaction and IMT. The investigation of this relationship was limited, because life events was not a significant correlated of IMT in this study. Our sample of women reported relatively low numbers of life events. In future studies, consideration of the intensity along with number of life stressors may be beneficial to determine if psychosocial stressors such as stressful life events interact with psychological well-being on IMT.
When examining potential differences in the association between IMT and psychological well-being across race, it was revealed that mean IMT was significantly higher in African American women in this sample. African American women had significantly lower life satisfaction and more life events than white women. Despite these mean differences, no significant relationship was found between the interaction of race with life satisfaction and IMT in an adjusted model. The role of race as a moderator of the association between life satisfaction and IMT was no longer significant after cardiovascular risk factors were included in the model. African American race did significantly correlated with IMT in the model including demographic covariates; however, when known cardiovascular risk factors were added to the model, the association between race and IMT attenuated. Given these findings are preliminary and the few other studies that have examined psychological cushioning factors and IMT have enrolled primarily white subjects (37–39), further investigation of the protective nature of life satisfaction on IMT across race is encouraged.
There were a few important limitations to this study. First, the design of the study was cross-sectional. It is preferable to measure progression of IMT over time as a marker of the atherosclerotic disease process. Future studies would benefit from a longitudinal design so that the causal relationships between psychological factors, other relevant risk factors, and IMT can be explored. Second, some characteristics of this sample were not representative of the general population of middle-aged women, and therefore, generalizability of the results is somewhat limited. Third, the addition of more sensitive measures of stressors, which assess level of perceived distress associated with each stressor, would have been ideal. Though likely experiencing some psychosocial distress, our sample of women reported few life events. To truly examine the potential interaction of psychological well-being with psychosocial stressors on IMT, future studies should use measures of stressors that are broadly distributed across the sample and that capture the intensity of the experience (59). Finally, inclusion of a more refined smoking risk factor variable in the model, which assessed smoking history along with current smoking behavior, would be recommended (1).
Despite these limitations, there are important implications of this study. The results for life satisfaction lend support to the assertion that psychological well-being is independently associated with subclinical atherosclerosis and that the role of psychological well-being may be independent of and perhaps more powerful than the role of depressive symptoms. We strongly encourage further study of the associations with and potential longitudinal effects on IMT of life satisfaction in combination with psychological risk factors, such as depressed mood and negative affect. Though the interaction between life satisfaction and life events was not associated with IMT in our sample, our measure of psychosocial stressors had limitations, and additional investigation of this potential interactive effect would be recommended.
The development of additional studies to corroborate and extend upon these findings has important scientific and potential clinical applications. The previously mentioned transcendental meditation stress reduction intervention study (41) of 60 African American men and women concluded that a 0.098mm decrease in IMT displayed by the intervention group was equivalent to an 11% reduction in risk of myocardial infarction, based on the work of Salonen and Salonen ((60), as cited in Ref. (41)), and a 7.7% to 15% reduction in the incidence of stroke, based on the work of O’Leary and colleagues ((6), as cited in Ref. (41)). Interventions promoting psychological well-being may assist in delaying atherosclerosis and cardiovascular events, as shown in the Castillo-Richmond et al. (41) study. There is a growing body of research supporting the relationship between psychological well-being and subclinical cardiovascular disease, and if findings from the current study are consistently replicated and investigated in prospective, longitudinal studies, it may warrant future development of preventive psychological interventions aimed at fostering life satisfaction and a general state of psychological wellness in individuals displaying cardiovascular risk profiles.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
The Study of Women's Health Across the Nation (SWAN) has grant support from the National Institutes of Health (NIH), U.S. Department of Health & Human Services, through the National Institute on Aging (NIA), the National Institute of Nursing Research (NINR) and the NIH Office of Research on Women’s Health (ORWH) (Grants U01NR004061, U01AG012505, U01AG012535, U01AG012531, U01AG012539, U01AG012546, U01AG012553, U01AG012554, U01AG012495). The SWAN Heart Ancillary Study has grant support from the NIH, U.S. Department of Health & Human Services, through the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute (NHLBI) (Grants HL65581 and HL65591). The content of this manuscript is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the NIA, NINR, ORWH or the NIH.
Clinical Centers: University of Michigan, Ann Arbor - Siobán Harlow, PI 2011 – present; MaryFran Sowers, PI 1994-2011; Massachusetts General Hospital, Boston, MA - Joel Finkelstein, PI 1999 – present; Robert Neer, PI 1994 – 1999; Rush University, Rush University Medical Center, Chicago, IL - Howard Kravitz, PI 2009 – present; Lynda Powell, PI 1994 – 2009; University of California, Davis/Kaiser - Ellen Gold, PI; University of California, Los Angeles - Gail Greendale, PI; Albert Einstein College of Medicine, Bronx, NY – Carol Derby, PI 2011 – present, Rachel Wildman, PI 2010 – 2011; Nanette Santoro, PI 2004 – 2010; University of Medicine and Dentistry - New Jersey Medical School, Newark –Gerson Weiss, PI 1995 – 2004; and the University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA - Karen Matthews, PI.
NIH Program Office: National Institute on Aging, Bethesda, MD - Winifred Rossi 2012 - present; Sherry Sherman 1994 – 2012; Marcia Ory 1994 – 2001; National Institute of Nursing Research, Bethesda, MD – Program Officers.
Central Laboratory: University of Michigan, Ann Arbor - Daniel McConnell (Central Ligand Assay Satellite Services).
Coordinating Center: University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA – Maria Mori Brooks, PI 2012 – present; Kim Sutton-Tyrrell, PI 2001 – 2012; New England Research Institutes, Watertown, MA - Sonja McKinlay, PI 1995 – 2001.
Steering Committee: Susan Johnson, Current Chair
Chris Gallagher, Former Chair
We thank the study staff at each site and all the women who participated in SWAN.
Acronyms
- IMT
intima media thickness
- BMI
body mass index
- SWAN
Study of Women’s Health Across the Nation
- SWLS
Satisfaction with Life Scale
- LET
Life Engagement Test
- SBP
systolic blood pressure
- DBP
diastolic blood pressure
Footnotes
Conflicts of Interest: None declared.
Contributor Information
Leila Shahabi, Department of Medicine, University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA.
Kelly Karavolos, Department of Preventive Medicine, Rush University Medical Center, Chicago, IL.
Susan A. Everson-Rose, Department of Medicine, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN.
Tené T. Lewis, Department of Epidemiology, Rollins School of Public Health, Emory University, Atlanta, GA.
Karen A. Matthews, Department of Psychiatry, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA.
Kim Sutton-Tyrrell, Department of Epidemiology, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA.
Lynda H. Powell, Department of Preventive Medicine, Rush University Medical Center, Chicago, IL.
REFERENCES
- 1.Oren A, Vos LE, Uiterwaal CSPM, Grobbee DE, Bots ML. Cardiovascular risk factors and increased carotid intima-media thickness in healthy young adults: the Atherosclerosis Risk in Young Adults (ARYA) Study. Arch Intern Med. 2003;163(15):1787–1792. doi: 10.1001/archinte.163.15.1787. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Bots ML, Hoes AW, Koudstaal PJ, Hofman A, Grobbee DE. Common carotid intima-media thickness and risk of stroke and myocardial infarction: the Rotterdam Study. Circulation. 1997;96(5):1432–1437. doi: 10.1161/01.cir.96.5.1432. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Burke GL, Evans GW, Riley WA, Sharrett AR, Howard G, Barnes RW, Rosamond W, Crow RS, Rautaharju PM, Heiss G. Arterial wall thickness is associated with prevalent cardiovascular disease in middle-aged adults : the Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities (ARIC) Study. Stroke. 1995;26(3):386–391. doi: 10.1161/01.str.26.3.386. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Chambless LE, Heiss G, Folsom AR, Rosamond W, Szklo M, Sharrett AR, Clegg LX. Association of coronary heart disease incidence with carotid arterial wall thickness and major risk factors: the Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities (ARIC) Study, 1987–1993. Am J Epidemiol. 1997;146(6):483–494. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a009302. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Hodis HN, Mack WJ, LaBree L, Selzer RH, Liu CR, Liu CH, Azen SP. The role of carotid arterial intima-media thickness in predicting clinical coronary events. Ann Intern Med. 1998;128(4):262–269. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-128-4-199802150-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.O'Leary DH, Polak JF, Kronmal RA, Manolio TA, Burke GL, Wolfson SK., Jr Carotid-artery intima and media thickness as a risk factor for myocardial infarction and stroke in older adults. N Engl J Med. 1999;340(1):14–22. doi: 10.1056/NEJM199901073400103. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Salonen JT, Salonen R. Ultrasonographically assessed carotid morphology and the risk of coronary heart disease. Arterioscler Thromb Vasc Biol. 1991;11(5):1245–1249. doi: 10.1161/01.atv.11.5.1245. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Murakami S, Otsuka K, Hotta N, Yamanaka G, Kubo Y, Matsuoka O, Yamanaka T, Shinagawa M, Nunoda S, Nishimura Y, Shibata K, Takasugi E, Nishinaga M, Ishine M, Wada T, Okumiya K, Matsubayashi K, Yano S, Ichihara K, Cornelissen G, Halberg F. Common carotid intima-media thickness is predictive of all-cause and cardiovascular mortality in elderly community-dwelling people: Longitudinal Investigation for the Longevity and Aging in Hokkaido County (LILAC) study. Biomed Pharmacother. 2005;59(Supplement 1):S49–S53. doi: 10.1016/s0753-3322(05)80010-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.O'Leary DH, Polak JF, Kronmal RA, Savage PJ, Borhani NO, Kittner SJ, Tracy R, Gardin JM, Price TR, Furberg CD. Thickening of the carotid wall: a marker for atherosclerosis in the elderly? Stroke. 1996;27(2):224–231. doi: 10.1161/01.str.27.2.224. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Heiss G, Sharrett AR, Barnes R, Chambless LE, Szklo M, Alzola C. Carotid atherosclerosis measured by B-mode ultrasound in populations: associations with Cardiovascular Risk Factors in the ARIC Study. Am J Epidemiol. 1991;134(3):250–256. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a116078. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Howard G, Burke GL, Szklo M, Tell GS, Eckfeldt J, Evans G, Heiss G. Active and passive smoking are associated with increased carotid wall thickness. The Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities Study. Arch Intern Med. 1994;154(11):1277–1282. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Kuller L, Borthani N, Furberg C, Gardin J, Manolio T, O'Leary D, Psaty B, Robbins J. Prevalence of subclinical atherosclerosis and cardiovascular disease and association with risk factors in the Cardiovascular Health Study. Am J Epidemiol. 1994;139(12):1164–1179. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a116963. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Gariepy J, Salomon J, Denarie N, Laskri F, Megnien JL, Levenson J, Simon A. Sex and topographic differences in associations between large-artery wall thickness and coronary risk profile in a French working cohort : the AXA Study. Arterioscler Thromb Vasc Biol. 1998;18(4):584–590. doi: 10.1161/01.atv.18.4.584. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Gariepy J, Simon A, Massonneau M, Linhart A, Levenson J. Wall thickening of carotid and femoral arteries in male subjects with isolated hypercholesterolemia. Atherosclerosis. 1995;113(2):141–151. doi: 10.1016/0021-9150(94)05436-m. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Homma S, Hirose N, Ishida H, Ishii T, Araki G, Halsey JH., Jr Carotid plaque and intima-media thickness assessed by B-mode ultrasonography in subjects ranging from young adults to centenarians. Stroke. 2001;32(4):830–835. doi: 10.1161/01.str.32.4.830. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.D'Agostino RB, Burke G, O'Leary D, Rewers M, Selby J, Savage PJ, Saad MF, Bergman RN, Howard G, Wagenknecht L, Haffner SM. Ethnic differences in carotid wall thickness: the Insulin Resistance Atherosclerosis Study. Stroke. 1996;27(10):1744–1749. doi: 10.1161/01.str.27.10.1744. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Arnett DK, Tyroler HA, Burke G, Hutchinson R, Howard G, Heiss G. Hypertension and subclinical carotid artery atherosclerosis in blacks and whites: the Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities Study. Arch Intern Med. 1996;156(17):1983–1989. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Everson SA, Kaplan GA, Goldberg DE, Salonen R, Salonen JT. Hopelessness and 4-year progression of carotid atherosclerosis: the Kuopio Ischemic Heart Disease Risk Factor Study. Arterioscler Thromb Vasc Biol. 1997;17(8):1490–1495. doi: 10.1161/01.atv.17.8.1490. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Pollitt RA, Daniel M, Kaufman JS, Lynch JW, Salonen JT, Kaplan GA. Mediation and modification of the association between hopelessness, hostility, and progression of carotid atherosclerosis. J Behav Med. 2005;28(1):53–64. doi: 10.1007/s10865-005-2563-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Whipple MO, Lewis TT, Suton-Tyrrell K, Matthews KA, Barinas-Mitchell E, Powell LH, Everson-Rose SA. Hopelessness, depressive symptoms, and carotid atherosclerosis in women: the Study of Women's Health Across the Nation (SWAN) Heart Study. Stroke. 2009;40:3166–3172. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.109.554519. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Julkunen J, Salonen R, Kaplan G, Chesney M, Salonen J. Hostility and the progression of carotid atherosclerosis. Psychosom Med. 1994;56(6):519–525. doi: 10.1097/00006842-199411000-00007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Matthews KA, Owens JF, Kuller LH, Sutton-Tyrrell K, Jansen-McWilliams L. Are hostility and anxiety associated with carotid atherosclerosis in healthy postmenopausal women? Psychosom Med. 1998;60(5):633–638. doi: 10.1097/00006842-199809000-00021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Everson-Rose SA, Lewis TT, Karavolos K, Matthews KA, Sutton-Tyrrell K, Powell LH. Cynical hostility and carotid atherosclerosis in African American and white women: the Study of Women's Health Across the Nation (SWAN) Heart Study. Am Heart J. 2006;152(5):982.e7–982.e13. doi: 10.1016/j.ahj.2006.08.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Ohira T, Diez Roux AV, Polak JF, Homma S, Iso H, Wasserman BA. Associations of anger, anxiety, and depressive symptoms with carotid arterial wall thickness: the Multi-Ethnic Study of Atherosclerosis. Psychosom Med. 2012;74:517–525. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e31824f6267. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Raikkonen K, Matthews KA, Sutton-Tyrrell K, Kuller LH. Trait anger and the metabolic syndrome predict progression of carotid atherosclerosis in healthy middle-aged women. Psychosom Med. 2004;66(6):903–908. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000143638.31297.11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Williams JE, Couper DJ, Din-Dzietham R, Nieto J, Folsom AR. Race-Gender differences in the association of trait anger with subclinical carotid artery atherosclerosis: the Atherosclerosis Risk in the Communities Study. Am J Epidemiol. 2007;165(11):1296–1304. doi: 10.1093/aje/kwm001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Elovainio M, Keltikangas-Jarvinen L, Kivimaki M, Pulkki L, Puttonen S, Heponiemi T, Juonala M, Viikari J, Raitakari OT. Depressive symptoms and carotid artery intima-media thickness in young adults: the Cardiovascular Risk in Young Finns Study. Psychosom Med. 2005;67(4):561–567. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000170340.74035.23. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Stewart JC, Janicki DL, Muldoon MF, Sutton-Tyrrell K, Kamarck TW. Negative emotions and 3-year progression of subclinical atherosclerosis. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2007;64:225–233. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.2.225. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Tiemeier H, van Dijck W, Hofman A, Witteman JCM, Stijnen T, Breteler MMB. Relationship between atherosclerosis and late-life depression: the Rotterdam Study. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2004;61:369–376. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.61.4.369. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Agewall S, Wikstrand J, Dahlof C, Fagerberg B. Negative feelings (discontent) predict progress of intima-media thickness of the common carotid artery in treated hypertensive men at high cardiovascular risk. Am J Hypertens. 1996;9(6):545–550. doi: 10.1016/0895-7061(95)00355-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Paterniti S, Zureik M, Ducimetiere P, Touboul PJ, Feve JM, Alperovitch A. Sustained anxiety and 4-year progression of carotid atherosclerosis. Arterioscler Thromb Vasc Biol. 2001;21(1):136–141. doi: 10.1161/01.atv.21.1.136. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Jedryka-Góral A, Pasierski T, Zabek J, Widerszal-Bazyl M, Radkiewicz P, Szulczyk GA, Wojciechowska B, Bugajska J. Risk factors for atherosclerosis in healthy employees--a multidisciplinary approach. Eur J Intern Med. 2006;17(4):247–253. doi: 10.1016/j.ejim.2005.12.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Hintsanen M, Kivimaki M, Elovainio M, Pulkki-Raback L, Keskivaara P, Juonala M, Raitakari OT, Keltikangas-Jarvinen L. Job strain and early atherosclerosis: the Cardiovascular Risk in Young Finns Study. Psychosom Med. 2005;67(5):740–747. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000181271.04169.93. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Lynch JW, Krause N, Kaplan GA, Salonen R, Salonen JT. Workplace demands, economic reward, and progression of carotid atherosclerosis. Circulation. 1997;96:302–307. doi: 10.1161/01.cir.96.1.302. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Rosvall M, Ostergren PO, Hedblad B, Isacsson SO, Janzon L, Berglund G. Work-related psychosocial factors and carotid atherosclerosis. Int J Epidemiol. 2002;31(6):1169–1178. doi: 10.1093/ije/31.6.1169. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Merjonen P, Pulkki-Raback L, Puttonen S, Keskivaara P, Juonala M, Telama R, Viikari J, Raitakari OT, Keltikangas-Jarvinen L. Anger is associated with subclinical atherosclerosis in low SES but not in higher SES men and women. The Cardiovascular Risk in Young Finns Study. J Behav Med. 2008;31(1):35–44. doi: 10.1007/s10865-007-9131-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Matthews KA, Raikkonen K, Sutton-Tyrrell K, Kuller LH. Optimistic attitudes protect against progression of carotid atherosclerosis in healthy middle-aged women. Psychosom Med. 2004;66(5):640–644. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000139999.99756.a5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Gallo LC, Troxel WM, Kuller LH, Sutton-Tyrrell K, Edmundowicz D, Matthews K. Marital status, marital quality, and atherosclerotic burden in postmenopausal women. Psychosom Med. 2003;65:952–962. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000097350.95305.fe. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Joseph NT, Kamarck TW, Muldoon MF, Manuck SB. Daily marital interaction quality and carotid artery intima-media thickness in healthy middle-aged adults. Psychosom Med. 2014;76(5):347–354. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0000000000000071. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Troxel WM, Matthews KA, Bromberger JT, Sutton-Tyrrell K. Chronic stress burden, discrimination, and subclinical carotid artery disease in African American and Caucasian women. Health Psychol. 2003;22(3):300–309. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.22.3.300. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Castillo-Richmond A, Schneider RH, Alexander CN, Cook R, Myers H, Nidich S, Haney C, Rainforth M, Salerno J. Effects of stress reduction on carotid atherosclerosis in hypertensive African Americans. Stroke. 2000;31(3):568–573. doi: 10.1161/01.str.31.3.568. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Pavot W, Diener E. Review of the satisfaction with life scale. Psychol Assess. 1993;5(2):164–172. [Google Scholar]
- 43.Scheier MF, Wrosch C, Baum A, Cohen S, Martire LM, Matthews KA, Schulz R, Zdaniuk B. The Life Engagement Test: assessing purpose in life. J Behav Med. 2006;29(3):291–298. doi: 10.1007/s10865-005-9044-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Ryff CD, Singer BH, Dienberg Love G. Positive health: connecting well-being with biology. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2004;359(1449):1383–1394. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2004.1521. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Rozanski A, Kubzansky LD. Psychologic functioning and physical health: a paradigm of flexibility. Psychosom Med. 2005;67(Supplement 1):S47–S53. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000164253.69550.49. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Sowers M, Crawford SL, Sternfeld B, Morganstein D, Gold EB, Greendale GA, Evans D, Neer R, Matthews K, Sherman S, Lo A, Weiss G, Kelsey J. SWAN: a multicenter, multiethnic, community-based cohort study of women and the menopausal transition. In: Lobo R, Marcus R, Kelsey J, editors. Menopause: Biology and pathobiology. New York: Academic Press; 2000. pp. 175–188. [Google Scholar]
- 47.Dohrenwend B, Krasnoff L, Askenasy A, Dohrenwend B. Exemplification of a method for scaling life events: the PERI Life Events Scale. J Health Soc Behav. 1978;19:205–229. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Radloff LS. The CES-D scale: a self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Appl Psychol Meas. 1977;1:385–401. [Google Scholar]
- 49.Mathews KA, Owens JF, Edmundowicz D, Lee L, Kuller LH. Positive and negative attributes and risk for coronary and aortic calcification in healthy women. Psychosom Med. 2006;68:355–361. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000221274.21709.d0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Boehm JK, Peterson C, Kivimaki M, Kubzansky L. A prospective study of positive psychological well-being and coronary heart disease. Health Psychol. 2011;30(3):259–267. doi: 10.1037/a0023124. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Diener E. Subjective well-being: the science of happiness and a proposal for a national index. Am Psychol. 2000;55(1):34–43. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Kok BE, Fredrickson BL. Upward spirals of the heart: autonomic flexibility, as indexed by vagal tone, reciprocally and prospectively predicts positive emotions and social connectedness. Biol Psychol. 2010;85:432–436. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.09.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Rozanski A, Blumenthal JA, Davidson KW, Saab PG, Kubzansky L. The epidemiology, pathophysiology, and management of psychosocial risk factors in cardiac practice: the emerging field of behavioral cardiology. J Am Coll Cardiol. 2005;45(5):637–651. doi: 10.1016/j.jacc.2004.12.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Futterman AD, Kemeny ME, Shapiro D, Fahey JL. Immunological and physiological changes associated with induced positive and negative mood. Psychosom Med. 1994;56:499–511. doi: 10.1097/00006842-199411000-00005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Boylan JM, Ryff CD. Psychological well-being and metabolic syndrome: findings from the Midlife in the United States National Sample. Psychosom Med. 2015;77(5):548–558. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0000000000000192. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Brydon L, Edwards S, Jia H, Mohamed-Ali V, Zachary I, Martin JF, Steptoe A. Psychological stress activates interleukin-1[beta] gene expression in human mononuclear cells. Brain Behav Immun. 2005;19(6):540–546. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2004.12.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Ryan RM, Deci EL. On happiness and human potentials: a review of research on hedonic and eudaimonic well-being. Annu Rev Psychol. 2001;52:141–166. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.141. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Taylor SE, Kemeny ME, Reed GM, Bower JE, Gruenewald TL. Psychological resources, positive illusions, and health. Am Psychol. 2000;55(1):99–109. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.55.1.99. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Lazarus RS, Folkman S. Stress, appraisal, and coping. New York: Springer Publishing Company; 1984. [Google Scholar]
- 60.Salonen JT, Salonen R. Quantitative imaging, risk factors, prevalence, and change: Chairman's discussion of session 2: ultrasound B-mode imaging in observational studies of atherosclerotic progression. Circulation. 1993;87(3S):II56–II65. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]