Abstract
To migrate, animals rely on endogenous, genetically inherited programmes, or socially transmitted information about routes and behaviours, or a combination of the two. In long-lived animals with extended parental care, as in bats, migration tends to be socially transmitted rather than endogenous. For a young bat to learn migration via social transmission, they would need to follow an experienced individual, most likely one roosting nearby. Therefore, we predicted that bats travelling together originate from the same place. It is also likely that young bats would follow their mothers or other kin, so we predicted that bats travelling together are more closely related to each other than bats not travelling together. To test our predictions, we used microsatellite genotypes and stable isotope values of δ13C, δ15N and δ2H to analyse the relatedness and geographical origins of migrating hoary bats (Lasiurus cinereus/Aeorestes cinereus (Baird et al. 2015 J. Mammal. 96, 1255–1274 (doi:10.1093/jmammal/gyv135)); n = 133) and silver-haired bats (Lasionycteris noctivagans; n = 87) killed at wind turbines over two consecutive autumn migrations. Contrary to our predictions, there was no evidence that related dyads of hoary bats or silver-haired bats were killed on the same night more frequently than expected by chance, or that the number of days between the fatalities of dyad members was influenced by relatedness or latitude of origin. Our data suggest that these bats do not socially transmit migration routes and behaviours among close kin.
Keywords: hoary bat, Lasiurus cinereus, Lasionycteris noctivagans, migration, relatedness, silver-haired bat
1. Introduction
How animals find their way during migration has long fascinated us and inspired centuries of research. Migratory animals rely on endogenous, genetically inherited programmes or socially transmitted information about routes and behaviours, or a combination of the two [1–4]. The degree to which each source of information is used depends on numerous factors, including the age, experience, personality and sociality of an individual [5]. Thus, social transmission of migratory information may be more likely to occur in animals that live in groups that contain a mix of experienced leaders and naive followers than in solitary animals or in a group containing only experienced or only naive individuals.
Navigational abilities of experienced individuals are superior to those of inexperienced individuals [6,7]. In fact, following an experienced leader may be necessary for successful migration, as shown in the ultralight-led migrations of various species of birds [8,9]. Often, the leaders are parents guiding their offspring during their first migration, as in mammals such as some whales [10–12] and ungulates [13–15]. However, the experienced individual may not always be a parent [16].
Animals that learn components of migration from social transmission show increased flexibility and adaptability of migratory behaviour and a decreased use of suboptimal routes relative to those relying on genetic programmes [17]. For example, in whooping cranes (Grus americana), social learning reduces suboptimal migration strategies; inexperienced birds that followed older birds were significantly better at accurately arriving at their over-wintering location compared with inexperienced birds that flew only with other inexperienced birds [18].
The degree to which social transmission of migratory information occurs is correlated with life history. In long-lived birds with extended parental care, such as cranes, geese, swans, storks and bustards, migration tends to be socially transmitted, while it is under endogenous control in the majority of small songbirds [3,19]. Long lifespans facilitate the transmission of historic traditions, in African elephants (Loxodonta africana) for example [20,21].
Social transmission of migratory information and the use of optimal migration routes may be particularly important for animals with slow life histories because they have low reproductive rates, increasing the importance of successful migrations of juveniles. Migration is a risky behaviour, with most of the annual mortality occurring during migration [22–24], particularly in first-time migrants [25–27].
Bats have slow life histories and long lives [28], and we thus predict that social transmission of migratory behaviour is common in bats. However, we know little about their migratory behaviours. Bats are certainly capable of social transmission of behaviour [29–35]. Mothers and offspring are also capable of recognizing each other acoustically, even in large maternity colonies [36–38], which suggests that bonds occur between mother and offspring. There is also evidence of extended mother–pup associations [39–41]. Given that maternal investment is pronounced in bats, we hypothesized that migratory bats transmit information about migratory routes and behaviours to juveniles, as occurs in other animals with similar levels of parental investment.
Although highly gregarious species of bat likely migrate as a group (e.g. Mexican free-tailed bats (Tadarida brasiliensis) [42] and straw-coloured fruit bats (Eidolon helvum) [43]), it is not known how many species of bats flock while migrating. There are historical accounts of flocks of bats migrating during the day [44–47]. One of those flocks consisted of at least three sizes of bats, but the flock did not behave like flocks in the classic sense (i.e. with coordinated movements), but rather, appeared to be a group of individuals moving through the same place at the same time [45]. Echolocation activity and bat fatalities at wind energy facilities also appear in waves [48], but it is unclear whether this represents flocks in the classic sense, or whether multiple individuals simply take advantage of the same favourable environmental conditions to migrate (e.g. in low wind speeds) [48]. Regardless, flying in a group would allow for social transmission of migratory behaviours among both con- and heterospecifics [32].
Social transmission is most likely to occur from mother to offspring, but perhaps also from other conspecifics. For a young bat to learn migration via social transmission, it would need to follow an experienced individual, most likely one roosting nearby. Therefore, we predicted that bats travelling together on the same night originate from the same place. Stable isotopes can be used to determine the geographical origins of bat fur grown on the summering grounds, because fur is metabolically inert once grown and the stable isotope ratios in fur therefore reflect the environmental conditions (i.e. isoscape) where it was grown [49–51].
It is likely that young bats follow their mothers or other close relatives, so we predicted that bats travelling together on the same night are more closely related to each other than bats travelling on different nights. This pattern should be particularly apparent in adult females and juveniles because they roost together in family groups, whereas males roost independently. Given that some species of migratory bats are solitary (e.g. hoary bats; Lasiurus cinereus/Aeorestes cinereus [52]) and some are colonial (e.g. silver-haired bats; Lasionycteris noctivagans), we hypothesized that the degree to which migration is socially transmitted differs among species, being more prevalent in colonial than in solitary bats. If so, then we predicted that the similarity in geographical origins and the degree of relatedness of individuals travelling on the same night would be greater in colonial bats than in solitary ones, if colony members are closely related, as in some species such as big brown bats (Eptesicus fuscus) [53]. However, if colony members are not closely related, as in brown long-eared bats (Plecotus auritus) [54], then we might expect the degree of relatedness between migrating dyads to be less than we see in solitary bats, because young bats may have the opportunity to follow unrelated group members. To test our predictions, we used recently developed multilocus microsatellite genotypes [55] and stable isotope values of carbon (δ13C), nitrogen (δ15N) and hydrogen (δ2H) to analyse the temporal relatedness and geographical origins of migrating hoary bats and silver-haired bats killed at a wind energy facility in southwestern Alberta, Canada over two consecutive autumn migrations.
1.1. Study species
Hoary bats roost solitarily in tree foliage throughout much of North and South America [56]. Although we still do not understand the movement patterns or seasonal distributions of hoary bats, it seems they winter in Mexico and the southern United States, then migrate long distances north and east in the spring [51,57,58]. In summer, hoary bats in Canada are found throughout the prairies, aspen parkland and the southern boreal forests [50,59]. Reproductive females exhibit some degree of year-to-year site fidelity [60]. There may be some sexual segregation of hoary bats during summer, with females potentially migrating further than males [57,58,61] and males roosting separately from mother–pup family units [60], but stable isotope data show no strong evidence for sexual segregation at our site in southwestern Alberta [50]. However, the timing of fatalities suggests differential migration: adult males arrived one–two weeks before the adult females and subadults, which we first detected on the same night in both years [48]. Mating may occur in autumn during migration or on the wintering grounds, with one to four, but usually two pups born the following spring/early summer [56,62]. Pups are volant at about four weeks of age, but not weaned until about seven weeks of age [60].
Known wintering grounds of silver-haired bats are in the US Pacific Northwest, southwestern states and middle latitudes of the eastern United States [58,63]. The limited data on their migratory patterns suggest that in spring, silver-haired bats from the east migrate long distances north and east and western silver-haired bats migrate northward [58,64]. In summer, silver-haired bats in Canada are common throughout forested areas, where males roost singly and reproductive females form small colonies in tree cavities [65–67]. There is some sexual segregation during summer, with females potentially migrating farther than males [58], but stable isotope analysis of the fur of fatalities at turbines in southern Alberta showed no evidence of sexual segregation [50]. The timing of fatalities does not suggest differential migration, because adult males, adult females and subadults arrived together [48]. Mating appears to occur in autumn during migration and one or two, but usually two, pups are born the following spring/early summer [62,65]. Lactation lasts about five weeks and volancy appears to occur at about 4 weeks [65] (E. F. Baerwald and B. J. Klüg-Baerwald 2010, unpublished data).
2. Methods
2.1. Sample collection
We collected hair and skin samples from bat carcasses found under wind turbines in southwestern Alberta, Canada (49° 35′04′′ N, 113° 47′48′′ W). We searched for bat carcasses from 15 July to 30 September 2006 and 2007. We searched 10 randomly chosen turbines every day and the remaining 29 turbines once a week (see [68] for details). Searcher efficiency was very high (97% at turbines searched daily and 78% at those searched weekly [68]), thus bats killed by any of the 39 turbines had a high probability of being recovered within a week. For each carcass, we recorded species, age [69] and sex (when possible), and the degree of decomposition. In 2007, we verified age classifications during necropsies by noting the presence or absence of a thymus gland, which is present in subadults but absent in adults [70,71]. Details regarding fatalities can be found in [48].
We collected skin tissue from the wingtips of freshly killed bat carcasses (i.e. those killed the previous night at any of the 39 turbines) using dissecting scissors. Tissue was stored in 70–90% ethanol and kept in a −20°C freezer until DNA extraction. We collected hair from between the scapulae of bats killed the previous night, placed samples into plastic microcentrifuge tubes and stored them in a freezer until analysis.
2.2. DNA extraction, amplification and genotyping
Development of primers and microsatellite analysis were conducted at the University of Georgia's Savannah River Ecology Laboratory (see [55] for details on primer development). DNA was extracted using Qiagen DNEasy blood and tissue kit. PCR amplifications were performed in a 12.5 µl volume (10 mM Tris pH 8.4, 50 mM KCl, 25.0 µg ml−1 BSA, 0.4 µM unlabelled primer, 0.04 µM tag-labelled primer, 0.36 µM universal dye-labelled primer, 3.0 mM MgCl2, 0.8 mM dNTPs, 0.5 units AmpliTaq Gold® Polymerase (Applied Biosystems) and 20 ng DNA template) using an Applied Biosystems GeneAmp 9700. Touchdown thermal cycling programmes [72] encompassing a 10°C span of annealing temperatures ranging between 65 and 55°C (TD65) were used for all loci. Touchdown cycling parameters consisted of an initial denaturation step of 5 min at 95°C followed by 20 cycles of 95°C for 30 s, highest annealing temperature (decreased 0.5°C per cycle) for 30 s, and 72°C for 30 s; and 20 cycles of 95°C for 30 s, lowest annealing temperature for 30 s, and 72°C for 30 s and a final extension at 72°C for 5 min. PCR products were run on an ABI-3130xl sequencer and sized with Naurox size-standard prepared as described in [73], except that unlabelled primers started with GTTT. Results were analysed using Genemapper v.~3.7 (Applied Biosystems).
One hundred and thirty-three hoary bats were genotyped at 19 microsatellite loci and 87 silver-haired bats were genotyped at 18 microsatellite loci. We calculated the number of alleles (Na), observed (Ho) and expected heterozygosity (He) and linkage disequilibrium (LD) using Arlequin v.~3.5.1.3 [74]. We used Micro-Checker v.~2.2.3 [75] to search for loci with the following genotyping errors: dropout of large alleles, stuttering and null alleles. We did not remove null alleles from further analysis, because the software programs we used are equipped to handle them.
2.3. Stable isotope analysis
Stable carbon (δ13C), nitrogen (δ15N) and deuterium (δ2H) analyses were conducted at the Saskatchewan Isotope Laboratory in the Department of Geological Sciences, University of Saskatchewan, Saskatoon, Saskatchewan, Canada (see [50] for details).
2.4. Analysis of relatedness
We examined relatedness of individual bats in multiple ways. First, we used the full-likelihood method of Colony v.~2.0.5.8 [76] to putatively distinguish dyads as unrelated, half-sibs, full-sibs or parent–offspring, from the genotypes of potential offspring and the potential mothers and fathers. We considered all young-of-the-year (from both 2006 and 2007) to be potential offspring and, given that bats have overlapping generations, we also considered adults from 2007 to be potential offspring of 2006 and 2007 adults (i.e. a 2007 adult could be the offspring of an older 2006 or 2007 adult). We considered all adult females and males to be potential mothers or fathers, respectively, each with a 0.5 probability of parenting one of the candidate offspring. To be classified as belonging to one of the relatedness categories, dyads had to have a greater than or equal to 95% probability of belonging to that category. We used the genotyping error estimated by Colony, assumed polygamy for both sexes, re-calculated allele frequencies and used no sibship prior (i.e. we made no assumptions about the average paternal or maternal sibship size).
We used Coancestry v. 1.0.1.5 [77] to calculate pairwise relatedness of all possible dyads. Coancestry calculates seven measures of relatedness simultaneously, but only the two maximum-likelihood methods (TrioML and DyadML) allow one to incorporate genotyping errors, which, if not accounted for, can affect the precision of the relatedness estimates. We used the genotyping errors calculated by Colony during the first analysis and compared the results of TrioML and DyadML to determine which model to use. For both species, the two models were highly correlated (r2 > 0.90), but the relatedness values were slightly higher in DyadML, so we used these values. However, the estimates from maximum-likelihood methods are not normally distributed, because they are bounded between zero and one, and thus result in loss of statistical power. Therefore, we used the package Related v. 1.0 in R 3.2.3 [78] to compare relatedness estimators and included the Wang estimator [79] in future Coancestry-based analyses because it performed the best (based on having the highest Pearson's correlation value greater than 0.90 for both species [78]). A relatedness of 0.25 is indicative of half-siblings, but to minimize the likelihood of making a type I error, we considered a dyad to be related if its relatedness was greater than or equal to 0.20.
2.5. Statistical analyses
To investigate whether related individuals were killed on the same night more frequently than expected by chance, we compared related and unrelated dyads killed on the same night to related and unrelated dyads killed on different nights using two different methods. For the putative relatedness categories of Colony, we used two-tailed Fisher's exact tests and for the relatedness values of Coancestry, we used two-tailed t-tests. We restricted the analysis to within-year dyads (i.e. we did not include dyads that contained individuals from different years). We used an α-value of 0.05. We present mean ± standard error where applicable.
We also examined whether the time between individuals within a dyad (in days) was influenced by their relatedness or the similarity in their geographical origins by using generalized linear models (GLMs) in JMP 10 (SAS Institute, Cary, NC). We used the normally distributed Wang relatedness value from the Coancestry analysis as a predictor variable. As a proxy for geographical origins, we used the absolute difference in the stable isotope values of δ13C, δ15N and δ2H between members of each dyad that had stable isotope values available for both individuals (hoary bats, n = 91 individuals and 2063 dyads; silver-haired bats, n = 59 individuals and 1711 dyads) as the other predictor variables. We included the interactions between relatedness and each of the isotope variables, but removed the interactions from the model if they were not significant. For the GLM, we used the log link function for Poisson distributions, accounted for overdispersion, and used an α-value of 0.05.
3. Results
3.1. Genetic diversity
For hoary bats, the number of alleles per locus ranged from three to 66 (electronic supplementary material, table S1). Average observed heterozygosity was 0.73 ± 0.23. Ten loci contained null alleles (electronic supplementary material, table S1). Forty-four of 171 comparisons (25.73%) showed evidence of significant LD. In silver-haired bats, the number of alleles per locus ranged from six to 20 (electronic supplementary material, table S2). Average observed heterozygosity was 0.67 ± 0.17. Eight loci contained null alleles (electronic supplementary material, table S2). Thirty-seven of 153 comparisons (24.18%) showed evidence of significant LD.
3.2. Stable isotope analysis
Details regarding overall patterns and relationships of the stable isotopes can be found in [50].
3.3. Statistical analyses
For hoary bats, Colony identified 134 related dyads, 83 of which occurred within the same year. The majority of putatively related dyads (97%) were half-sibling dyads, but there were two full-sibling dyads, one mother–offspring dyad and one father–offspring dyad. Neither the full-sibling dyads nor the parent–offspring dyads involved individuals killed on the same night. There was no evidence that a greater number of related dyads of hoary bats were killed on the same night more frequently than expected by chance (table 1).
Table 1.
related dyads | unrelated dyads | |
---|---|---|
dyads killed on the same night | 8 | 457 |
dyads killed on different nights | 75 | 3888 |
Using the DyadML estimator, Coancestry identified 101 related dyads of hoary bats, 64 of which occurred within the same year, and using the Wang estimator, it identified 67 related dyads, 31 of which occurred within the same year. There was no evidence that mean relatedness of dyads of hoary bats killed on the same night was greater than that of dyads killed on different nights, regardless of the estimator used to determine related dyads (DyadML mean relatedness same night = 0.03 ± 0.002, mean relatedness different night = 0.03 ± 0.0008, two-tailed t-test, t4352 = −1.18, p = 0.24; Wang mean relatedness same night = −0.02 ± 0.001, mean relatedness different night = −0.03 ± 0.003, two-tailed t-test, t4352 = −1.9, p = 0.06).
In the GLM that assessed the influence of relatedness and geographical origin on the days between individuals in dyads of hoary bats, the model explained a significant proportion of the variation (model χ2/d.f.5 = 27.05, p < 0.001; Pearson's χ2/d.f.2061 = 12 768.07, p < 0.001; overdispersion = 6.20). The number of days between the fatalities of dyad members increased as the difference in their δ13C values increased, and as the difference in their δ15N values decreased, and was influenced by the interaction between dyad relatedness and the difference in their δ2H values (table 2); the number of days between fatalities was influenced by the similarity of their δ2H values, but when relatedness increased, this relationship decreased (table 2).
Table 2.
parameter | estimate | s.e. | χ2 | p-value |
---|---|---|---|---|
intercept | 2.136 | 0.047 | 1660.15 | <0.001 |
Wang relatedness | −0.706 | 0.270 | 7.04 | 0.008 |
difference in δ13C | 0.045 | 0.019 | 5.46 | 0.02 |
difference in δ15N | −0.049 | 0.024 | 4.11 | 0.04 |
difference in δ2H | −0.003 | 0.001 | 8.27 | 0.004 |
Wang relatedness × difference in δ2H | −0.028 | 0.013 | 4.84 | 0.03 |
For silver-haired bats, Colony identified 96 related dyads, 58 of which occurred within the same year. The majority of putatively related dyads (98%) were half-sibling dyads, but there was one full-sibling dyad and one father–offspring dyad. Neither the full-sibling dyad nor the father–offspring dyad involved individuals killed on the same night. There was no evidence that a greater number of related dyads of silver-haired bats were killed on the same night more frequently than expected by chance (table 3).
Table 3.
related dyads | unrelated dyads | |
---|---|---|
dyads killed on the same night | 9 | 147 |
dyads killed on different nights | 49 | 1644 |
Using the DyadML estimator, Coancestry identified 155 related dyads of silver-haired bats, 113 of which occurred within the same year, and using the Wang estimator, it identified 53 related dyads, 40 of which occurred within the same year. There was no evidence that mean relatedness of dyads of silver-haired bats killed on the same night was greater than that of dyads killed on different nights, regardless of the estimator used to determine related dyads (DyadML mean relatedness same night = 0.04 ± 0.005, mean relatedness different night = 0.04 ± 0.001, two-tailed t-test, t2853 = 0.22, p = 0.83; Wang mean relatedness same night = −0.05 ± 0.009, mean relatedness different night = −0.05 ± 0.002, two-tailed t-test, t2853 = −0.74, p = 0.46).
In the GLM that assessed the influence of relatedness and geographical origin on the days between individuals in dyads of silver-haired bats, the model explained a significant proportion of the variation (model χ2/d.f.4 = 19.7, p < 0.001; Pearson's χ2/d.f.842 = 7891.40, p < 0.001; overdispersion = 9.72). The number of days between the fatalities of dyad members increased as the difference in their δ15N values increased but was not influenced by any other predictor variable (table 4).
Table 4.
parameter | estimate | s.e. | χ2 | p-value |
---|---|---|---|---|
intercept | 2.38 | 0.072 | 835.43 | <0.001 |
Wang relatedness | −0.074 | 0.279 | 0.07 | 0.79 |
difference in δ13C | −0.019 | 0.035 | 0.29 | 0.58 |
difference in δ15N | 0.106 | 0.024 | 18.91 | <0.001 |
difference in δ2H | −0.002 | 0.002 | 1.65 | 0.20 |
4. Discussion
Contrary to our predictions, we found no conclusive evidence that either hoary bats or silver-haired bats migrate in family groups. In both species, the majority of dyads were not related and although a few related dyads were killed on the same night, 85–93% of the related dyads were killed on different nights. None of the putative parent–offspring (n = 1) or full-sibling dyads (n = 3) were killed on the same night. However, siblings from the same litter may have multiple paternity and be half-siblings rather than full-siblings, as seen in other species of bat that have twins [80–82], thus obscuring the closeness of the family connections. Thus, it appears that young bats on their first migrations are not following their mothers or travelling with their sibling(s). Given that sexual segregation occurs on the summering grounds for both species, it is also highly unlikely that juvenile bats are following their fathers. In silver-haired bats, young bats may follow non-related colony members, but hoary bats do not live in colonies, so do not have other group members to follow.
If inexperienced bats are not following kin or colony members, then they may follow other conspecifics. However, our hypothesis that bats migrating on the same night originate from a similar location was also not fully supported. In general, δ2H decreases with increasing latitude and elevation and this pattern is linked to the δ2H in precipitation [83,84]. δ13C and δ15N values also decrease with increasing latitude and elevation, but this is linked to climate and habitat shifts that occur with increasing latitude, such as shifts to cooler, drier conditions and from predominantly C4 to C3 plants [85–90]. Therefore, it appears that bats travelling on the same night originate from similar habitats, as indicated by the significant effect of δ13C and δ15N for hoary bats and δ15N for silver-haired bats, but not necessarily from similar latitudes, as a significant effect of δ2H would have indicated [50]. Instead, the negative interaction between relatedness of hoary bats and δ2H indicates that the correlation of δ2H with the number of days between dyad members is diminished as relatedness increases. This may suggest a subtle impact of relatedness on timing, but we cannot be certain given this dataset. Our stable isotope results may well suggest that individuals travelling together on the same night are not doing so in a coordinated manner, but rather, responding to similar cues in similar habitats and moving accordingly.
In addition to our data indicating that neither silver-haired bats nor hoary bats are migrating in family groups, there is evidence from other species of bats that mothers may embark on autumn movements before their young do [91–93]. This is surprising given the slow life histories of bats and the high mortality commonly associated with the first migration of juvenile animals [25]. Although mortality rates of juvenile bats during their first migration are not known, migratory bats in the genera Perimyotis, Nyctalus, Lasiurus and Lasionycteris commonly have twins, which is unusual among bats [28]. This, and the fact that juvenile hoary bats and silver-haired bats of both sexes are ready to mate during their first autumn, which is also unusual among bats [62], suggests that mortality of first-year individuals is relatively high. Survival estimates of migratory Leisler's bat (Nyctalus leisleri) are relatively high for adults (annual survival estimates for adult females vary from 0.45 to 0.61 and from 0.55 to 0.91 for adult males [94,95]), but are lower for first-year individuals, with annual survival estimates of 0.45 for juvenile females [94].
Because information obtained socially is sometimes easier to obtain, but less reliable than information obtained asocially (i.e. through trial-and-error), there is often a mixture within a group in the number of individuals acquiring information asocially and those acquiring information socially [96,97]. Information should be obtained socially when the costs of independently acquiring that information outweigh the benefits (‘copy when asocial learning is costly’ [96]) or when the independently acquired information results in too much uncertainty (‘copy when uncertain’ [96–99]). We assumed that both of these criteria were fulfilled and that the benefit of increasing the probability of a successful first migration of offspring would outweigh any costs a mother incurred from travelling with the pups. However, there are multiple costs and benefits that affect the decision to lead, and also to follow.
The costs and benefits of social transmission of migratory information can be divided into two categories, those associated with being in a group and those associated with the teaching or learning of behaviours. Benefits of being in a group may include increased predator detection and social thermoregulation, whereas costs may include increased conspicuousness and competition for resources [100,101]. Because these costs and benefits are related to being in a group, they should be shared among group members (e.g. among leaders and followers), although not necessarily equally. It may be that for reproductive female hoary bats, the costs of travelling in a group outweigh any benefits and they leave their offspring, becoming solitary again during migration, thus precluding social transmission of migratory behaviours from mother to offspring.
If the social transmission of migratory information is purposeful (i.e. migratory behaviours are taught), then it is likely that the costs are higher for the leaders/demonstrators than for the followers/observers. In fact, one of the main characteristics of teaching behaviour is that the~demonstrator incurs a cost, or at least not an immediate benefit, from altering their behaviour in the presence of a naive observer [102]. Costs may be measured in time, energy and mortality [103]. We know virtually nothing about the importance of migratory timing to bats, but if timing of departure and arrival and/or minimization of travel time is important, and travelling in a group with naive individuals negatively affects timing for experienced individuals, then experienced individuals may forego social transmission. For example, diets of adult and juvenile bats frequently differ [104–106], as found in the specific individual bats used in our study [107]. If the dietary differences result in longer foraging times for juveniles than for adults, and this slows migratory movement, this delay may be overly costly for adults.
We also know little about the mating behaviour of hoary or silver-haired bats, but as in other temperate-zone bats, they likely mate in autumn, during migration [56,62,65]. Mating may occur along migration routes, potentially with males intercepting females at lekking sites [108], although there is limited evidence to support this. Juveniles of both species may be ready to mate during their first migration, although there is no evidence they successfully do so [62]. Regardless, if migrating with juveniles negatively affects mating opportunities for the mothers, perhaps by affecting timing, they may forego social transmission.
Bats may employ a ‘fly-and-forage strategy’, foraging periodically during migration flights rather than foraging solely at emergence in the evening, as seen in Nathusius' pipistrelle (Pipistrellus nathusii) [109]. If attention to other individuals' echolocation is required to maintain contact with group members, and if this involves a trade-off between attention and foraging, the costs of attention may be too high, thus favouring solitary migration. It may be particularly challenging to maintain contact among small-bodied individuals travelling through the vast aerosphere, especially at night. Although bats may use echolocation [110,111] and passerines may use flight calls to communicate with group members [112,113], the range of these vocalizations may be insufficient to ensure group cohesion (but see [114]). Perhaps this constraint partially explains why migration in the majority of nocturnally migrating songbirds is under endogenous control rather than learned through social transmission [3,19].
If migratory behaviours in bats are the result of a genetic programme rather than social transmission, how do individuals find their way? The literature on orientation and navigation systems of animals during migration is vast and rich, particularly for birds [115–118]. Bats are capable of perceiving stars [119] and using post-sunset glow [120,121], the Earth's magnetic field [121–123] and geographical landmarks and linear features [124–127] for orientation and the creation of large-scale navigational maps [128]. Although bats are highly specialized for echolocation, it seems unlikely that they use echolocation to navigate over long distances [129]. They and other echolocating animals, such as porpoises [130], use echolocation for spatial orientation at small/local scales [131] and not for long-distance movements.
We hypothesized that the benefits of social transmission, such as increased flexibility and decreased use of suboptimal routes [17], would outweigh any costs associated with travelling in a group or teaching, but our findings suggest otherwise. This leads to many interesting questions and areas for further research. Why do bats not rely on social transmission of migratory behaviours as in other migratory mammals? How do bats, particularly juvenile bats, find their way to their over-wintering grounds? Do bats travel in groups of unrelated individuals, and if so, how do they maintain cohesion? Is social transmission more likely in the nomadic migrations of nectarivores such as the grey-headed flying-fox (Pteropus poliocephalus) that follow the flowering of plants [132,133]? What is the mortality rate of juvenile bats during their first migration? How do these mortality rates differ with social structure, roosting ecology and migration distance? In short, much more work is needed on the use and relative importance of social transmission and endogenous programmes to bats before we can fully understand their migration biology.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgements
We thank the many field and laboratory assistants who helped us collect and analyse data. We also thank the Saskatchewan Isotope Laboratory, specifically W. Patterson and D. Besic, for their assistance in stable isotope analysis and The Savannah River Ecology Laboratory, specifically R. Beasley, for their assistance in genotyping. B. Baerwald, P. Cryan, S. Rogers and A. Russell provided comments that improved the manuscript.
Authors' contributions
E.F.B. conceived of the study, designed the study, collected the tissue samples, analysed the data, and drafted the manuscript; R.M.R.B. coordinated the study, participated in the design of the study, assisted with data analysis and helped draft the manuscript. All authors gave final approval for publication.
Competing interests
We declare we have no competing interests.
Funding
Funding for the overall research project was provided by grants from the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada, TransAlta Wind, Bat Conservation International, the North American Bat Conservation Partnership, the University of Calgary's Institute for Sustainable Energy Environment and Economy, Alberta Innovates–Technology Futures, and the Alberta Conservation Association. This specific project was made possible by 236 generous people who donated to our crowdfunding campaign.
References
- 1.Dodson JJ. 1988. The nature and role of learning in the orientation and migratory behavior of fishes. Environ. Biol. Fishes 23, 161–182. (doi:10.1007/BF00004908) [Google Scholar]
- 2.Berthold P. 1991. Genetic control of migratory behaviour in birds. Trends Ecol. Evol. 6, 254–257. (doi:10.1016/0169-5347(91)90072-6) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Pulido F. 2007. The genetics and evolution of avian migration. Bioscience 57, 165–174. (doi:10.1641/B570211) [Google Scholar]
- 4.Liedvogel M, Åkesson S, Bensch S. 2011. The genetics of migration on the move. Trends Ecol. Evol. 26, 561–569. (doi:10.1016/j.tree.2011.07.009) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Németh Z, Moore FR. 2014. Information acquisition during migration: a social perspective. The Auk 131, 186–194. (doi:10.1642/AUK-13-195.1) [Google Scholar]
- 6.Thorup K, Bisson I-A, Bowlin MS, Holland RA, Wingfield JC, Ramenofsky M, Wikelski M. 2007. Evidence for a navigational map streching across the continental U.S. in a migratory songbird. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 104, 18 115–18 119. (doi:10.1073/pnas.0704734104) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Chernetsov N, Berthold P, Querner U. 2004. Migratory orientation of first-year white storks (Ciconia ciconia): inherited information and social interactions. J. Exp. Biol. 207, 937–943. (doi:10.1242/jeb.00853) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Sladen WJL, Lishman WA, Ellis DH, Shire GG, Rininger DL. 2002. Teaching migration routes to Canada geese and trumpeter swans using ultralight aircraft, 1990–2001. Waterbirds: Int. J. Waterbird Biol. 25, 132–137. (doi:10.2307/1522344) [Google Scholar]
- 9.Ellis DH, Sladen WJL, Lishman WA, Clegg KR, Duff JW, Gee GF, Lewis JC. 2003. Motorized migrations: the future or mere fantasy? Bioscience 53, 260–264. (doi:10.1641/0006-3568(2003)053[0260:mmtfom]2.0.co;2) [Google Scholar]
- 10.Martin AR, Katona SK, Matilla D, Hembree D, Waters TD. 1984. Migration of humpback whales between the Caribbean and Iceland. J. Mammal. 65, 330–333. (doi:10.2307/1381174) [Google Scholar]
- 11.Corkeron PJ, Connor RC. 1999. Why do baleen whales migrate? Mar. Mamm. Sci. 15, 1228–1245. (doi:10.1111/j.1748-7692.1999.tb00887.x) [Google Scholar]
- 12.Valenzuela LO, Sironi M, Rowntree VJ, Seger J. 2009. Isotopic and genetic evidence for culturally inherited site fidelity to feeding grounds in southern right whales (Eubalaena australis). Mol. Ecol. 18, 782–791. (doi:10.1111/j.1365-294X.2008.04069.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.McCullough DR. 1985. Long range movements of large terrestrial mammals. Contrib. Mar. Sci. 68. [Google Scholar]
- 14.Nelson ME. 1998. Development of migratory behavior in northern white-tailed deer. Can. J. Zool. 76, 426–432. (doi:10.1139/z97-207) [Google Scholar]
- 15.Sawyer H, Lindzey F, McWhirter D. 2005. Mule deer and pronghorn migration in western Wyoming. Wildl. Soc. Bull. 33, 1266–1273. (doi:10.2193/0091-7648(2005)33[1266:MDAPMI]2.0.CO;2) [Google Scholar]
- 16.Palacín C, Alonso JC, Alonso JA, Magaña M, Martín CA. 2011. Cultural transmission and flexibility of partial migration patterns in a long-lived bird, the great bustard Otis tarda. J. Avian Biol. 42, 301–308. (doi:10.1111/j.1600-048X.2011.05395.x) [Google Scholar]
- 17.Sutherland WJ. 1998. Evidence for flexibility and constraint in migration systems. J. Biol. 29, 441–446. (doi:10.2307/3677163) [Google Scholar]
- 18.Mueller T, O'Hara RB, Converse SJ, Urbanek RP, Fagan WF. 2013. Social learning of migratory performance. Science 341, 999–1002. (doi:10.1126/science.1237139) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Berthold P. 2001. Bird migration. A general survey, 2nd edn Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 20.McComb K, Moss C, Durant SM, Baker L, Sayialel S. 2001. Matriarchs as repositories of social knowledge in African elephants. Science 292, 491–494. (doi:10.1126/science.1057895) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Archie EA, Moss CJ, Alberts SC. 2006. The ties that bind: genetic relatedness predicts the fission and fusion of social groups in wild African elephants. Proc. R. Soc. B 273, 513–522. (doi:10.1098/rspb.2005.3361) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Klaassen RH, Hake M, Strandberg R, Koks BJ, Trierweiler C, Exo KM, Bairlein F, Alerstam T. 2014. When and where does mortality occur in migratory birds? Direct evidence from long-term satellite tracking of raptors. J. Anim. Ecol. 83, 176–184. (doi:10.1111/1365-2656.12135) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Nicholson MC, Bowyer RT, Kie JG. 1997. Habitat selection and survival of mule deer: tradeoffs associated with migration. J. Mammal. 78, 483–504. (doi:10.2307/1382900) [Google Scholar]
- 24.Sillett TS, Holmes RT. 2002. Variation in survivorship of a migratory songbird throughout its annual cycle. J. Anim. Ecol. 71, 296–308. (doi:10.1046/j.1365-2656.2002.00599.x) [Google Scholar]
- 25.Strandberg R, Klaassen RHG, Hake M, Alerstam T. 2009. How hazardous is the Sahara desert crossing for migratory birds? Indications from satellite tracking of raptors. Biol. Lett. 6, 297–300. (doi:10.1098/rsbl.2009.0785) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Owen M, Black JM. 1989. Factors affecting the survival of barnacle geese on migration from the breeding grounds. J. Anim. Ecol. 58, 603–617. (doi:10.2307/4851) [Google Scholar]
- 27.Menu S, Gauthier G, Reed A, Holberton RL. 2005. Survival of young greater snowgeese (Chen caerulescens atlantica) during fall migration. The Auk. 122, 479–496. (doi:10.1642/0004-8038(2005)122[0479:SOYGSG]2.0.CO;2) [Google Scholar]
- 28.Barclay RMR, Harder LD. 2003. Life histories of bats: life in the slow lane. In Bat ecology (eds Kunz TH, Fenton MB), pp. 209–253. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. [Google Scholar]
- 29.Wilkinson GS. 1992. Information transfer at evening bat colonies. Anim. Behav. 44, 501–518. (doi:10.1016/0003-3472(92)90059-I) [Google Scholar]
- 30.Ratcliffe JM, ter Hofstede HM. 2005. Roosts as information centres: social learning of food preferences in bats. Biol. Lett. 1, 72–74. (doi:10.1098/rsbl.2004.0252) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Page RA, Ryan MJ. 2006. Social transmission of novel foraging behavior in bats: frog calls and their referents. Curr. Biol. 16, 1201–1205. (doi:10.1016/j.cub.2006.04.038) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Clarin TM, Borissov I, Page RA, Ratcliffe JM, Siemers BM. 2014. Social learning within and across species: information transfer in mouse-eared bats. Can. J. Zool. 92, 129–139. (doi:10.1139/cjz-2013-0211) [Google Scholar]
- 33.Gaudet CL, Fenton MB. 1984. Observational learning in three species of insectivorous bats (Chiroptera). Anim. Behav. 32, 385–388. (doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(84)80273-0) [Google Scholar]
- 34.Wright GS, Wilkinson GS, Moss CF. 2011. Social learning of a novel foraging task by big brown bats, Eptesicus fuscus. Anim. Behav. 82, 1075–1083. (doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.07.044) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Bunkley JP, Barber JR. 2014. An observation of apparent teaching behavior in the pallid bat, Antrozous pallidus. West N. Am. Nat. 74, 249–252. (doi:10.3398/064.074.0213) [Google Scholar]
- 36.Balcombe JP, McCracken GF. 1992. Vocal recognition in Mexican free-tailed bats: do pups recognize mothers? Anim. Behav. 43, 79–87. (doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80073-9) [Google Scholar]
- 37.Knörnschild M, Feifel M, Kalko EK. 2013. Mother–offspring recognition in the bat Carollia perspicillata. Anim. Behav. 86, 941–948. (doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.08.011) [Google Scholar]
- 38.Jin L, Yang S, Kimball RT, Xie L, Yue X, Luo B, Sun K, Feng J. 2015. Do pups recognize maternal calls in pomona leaf-nosed bats, Hipposideros pomona? Anim. Behav. 100, 200–207. (doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2014.12.006) [Google Scholar]
- 39.Brigham RM, Brigham AC. 1989. Evidence for association between a mother bat and its young during and after foraging. Am. Midl. Nat. 121, 205–207. (doi:10.2307/2425674) [Google Scholar]
- 40.Rossiter SJ, Jones G, Ransome RD, Barratt EM. 2002. Relatedness structure and kin-biased foraging in the greater horshoe bat (Rhinolophus ferrumequinum). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 51, 510–518. (doi:10.1007/s00265-002-0467-1) [Google Scholar]
- 41.Geipel I, Kalko EK, Wallmeyer K, Knörnschild M. 2013. Postweaning maternal food provisioning in a bat with a complex hunting strategy. Anim. Behav. 85, 1435–1441. (doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.03.040) [Google Scholar]
- 42.Cockrum EL. 1969. Migration in the guano bats, Tadarida brasiliensis. Miscellaneous Publ. The University of Kansas Museum of Natural History. 51, 303–336. [Google Scholar]
- 43.Thomas DW. 1983. The annual migrations of three species of west African fruit bats (Chiroptera: Pteropodidae). Can. J. Zool. 61, 2266–2272. (doi:10.1139/z83-299) [Google Scholar]
- 44.Mearns EA. 1898. A study of the vertebrate fauna of the Hudson Highlands, with observations on the Mollusca, Crustacea, Lepidoptera, and the flora of the region. Bull. Am. Mus. Nat. Hist. 10, 303–352. [Google Scholar]
- 45.Howell AH. 1908. Notes on the diurnal migrations of bats. Proc. Biol. Soc. Washington. 21, 35–38. [Google Scholar]
- 46.Allen GM. 1939. Bats. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 47.Hall ER. 1946. Mammals of Nevada. Berkley, CA: University of California Press. [Google Scholar]
- 48.Baerwald EF, Barclay RMR. 2011. Patterns of activity and fatality of migratory bats at a wind energy facility in Alberta, Canada. J. Wildl. Manag. 75, 1103–1114. (doi:10.1002/jwmg.147) [Google Scholar]
- 49.Popa-Lisseanu AG, et al. 2012. A triple-isotope approach to predict the breeding origins of European bats. PLoS ONE 7, e30388 (doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0030388) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Baerwald EF, Patterson WP, Barclay RMR. 2014. Origins and migratory patterns of bats killed by wind turbines in southern Alberta: evidence from stable isotopes. Ecosphere 5, 118.. (doi:10.1890/ES13-00380.1) [Google Scholar]
- 51.Cryan PM, Stricker CA, Wunder MB. 2014. Continental-scale, seasonal movements of a heterothermic migratory tree bat. Ecol. Appl. 24, 602–616. (doi:10.1890/13-0752.1) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Baird AB, Braun JK, Mares MA, Morales JC, Patton JC, Tran CQ, Bickham JW. 2015. Molecular systematic revision of tree bats (Lasiurini): doubling the native mammals of the Hawaiian Islands. J. Mammal. 96, 1255–1274. (doi:10.1093/jmammal/gyv135) [Google Scholar]
- 53.Metheny JD, Kalcounis-Rueppell MC, Bondo KJ, Brigham RM. 2008. A genetic analysis of group movement in an isolated population of tree-roosting bats. Proc. R. Soc. B 275, 2265–2272. (doi:10.1098/rspb.2008.0532) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Burland T, Barratt E, Nichols R, Racey P. 2001. Mating patterns, relatedness and the basis of natal philopatry in the brown long-eared bat, Plecotus auritus. Mol. Ecol. 10, 1309–1321. (doi:10.1046/j.1365-294X.2001.01273.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Baerwald EF, Beasley RR, Lance SL, Jones KL, Barclay RMR. In press Development of novel polymorphic microsatellite markers for two bat species affected by wind turbines, hoary bats (Lasiurus cinereus) and silver-haired bats (Lasionycteris noctivagans). Conserv. Genet. Resour. (doi:10.1007/s12686-016-0522-2) [Google Scholar]
- 56.Shump KA, Shump AU. 1982. Lasiurus cinereus. Mammal. Species 185, 1–5. (doi:10.2307/3503878) [Google Scholar]
- 57.Findley JS, Jones C. 1964. Seasonal distribution of the hoary bat. J. Mammal. 45, 461–470. (doi:10.2307/1377421) [Google Scholar]
- 58.Cryan PM. 2003. Seasonal distribution of migratory tree bats (Lasiurus and Lasionycteris) in North America. J. Mammal. 84, 579–593. (doi:10.1644/1545-1542(2003)084<0579:SDOMTB>2.0.CO;2) [Google Scholar]
- 59.Buehler DM, Piersma T. 2008. Travelling on a budget: predictions and ecological evidence for bottlenecks in the annual cycle of long-distance migrants. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 363, 247–266. (doi:10.1098/rstb.2007.2138) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Koehler CE, Barclay RMR. 2000. Post-natal growth and breeding biology of the hoary bat (Lasiurus cinereus). J. Mammal. 81, 234–244. (doi:10.1644/1545-1542(2000)081<0234:PNGABB>2.0.CO;2) [Google Scholar]
- 61.Cryan PM, Bogan MA, Rye RO, Landis GP, Kester CL. 2004. Stable hydrogen isotope analysis of bat hair as evidence for seasonal molt and long-distance migration. J. Mammal. 85, 995–1001. (doi:10.1644/BRG-202) [Google Scholar]
- 62.Cryan PM, Jameson JW, Baerwald EF, Willis CK, Barclay RM, Snider EA, Crichton EG. 2012. Evidence of late-summer mating readiness and early sexual maturation in migratory tree-roosting bats found dead at wind turbines. PLoS ONE 7, e47586 (doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0047586) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Izor RJ. 1979. Winter range of the silver-haired bat. J. Mammal. 60, 638–641. (doi:10.2307/1380113) [Google Scholar]
- 64.Fraser E, Longstaffe F. 2014. Stable isotope investigation of the latitudinal migratory movements of silver-haired bats in eastern North America. North American Symp. on Bat Research, Albany, NY, USA, 22–24 October 2014.
- 65.Kunz TH. 1982. Lasionycteris noctivagans. Mammal. Species 172, 1–5. (doi:10.2307/3504029) [Google Scholar]
- 66.Mattson TA, Buskirk SW, Stanton NL. 1996. Roost sites of the silver-haired bat (Lasionycyeris noctivagans) in the Black Hills, South Dakota. West N. Am. Nat. 56, 247–253. [Google Scholar]
- 67.Betts BJ. 1998. Roosts used by maternity colonies of silver-haired bats in northeastern Oregon. J. Mammal. 79, 643–650. (doi:10.2307/1382994) [Google Scholar]
- 68.Baerwald EF, Barclay RMR. 2009. Geographic variation in activity and fatality of migratory bats at wind energy facilities. J. Mammal. 90, 1341–1349. (doi:10.1644/09-MAMM-S-104R.1) [Google Scholar]
- 69.Anthony ELP. 1988. Age determination in bats. In Ecological and behavioural methods for the study of bats (ed. Kunz TH.), pp. 47–58. Washington, DC: Smithsonian Institute. [Google Scholar]
- 70.Kallen F. 1977. The cardiovascular system of bats: structure and function. Biol. Bats. 3, 289–483. [Google Scholar]
- 71.Chakraborty A, Chakravarty A. 1984. Thymus in different ages of a bat, (Pteropus giganteus). Curr. Sci. 531, 39–40. [Google Scholar]
- 72.Don R, Cox P, Wainwright B, Baker K, Mattick J. 1991. ‘Touchdown'PCR to circumvent spurious priming during gene amplification. Nucleic Acids Res. 19, 4008 (doi:10.1093/nar/19.14.4008) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.DeWoody JA, Schupp J, Kenefic L, Busch J, Murfitt L, Keim P. 2004. It ROX! Biotechniques 37, 348–352. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Excoffier L, Lischer HE. 2010. Arlequin suite ver 3.5: a new series of programs to perform population genetics analyses under Linux and Windows. Mol. Ecol. Resour. 10, 564–567. (doi:10.1111/j.1755-0998.2010.02847.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Van Oosterhout C, Hutchinson WF, Wills DP, Shipley P. 2004. Micro-checker: software for identifying and correcting genotyping errors in microsatellite data. Mol. Ecol. Notes 4, 535–538. (doi:10.1111/j.1471-8286.2004.00684.x) [Google Scholar]
- 76.Jones OR, Wang J. 2010. COLONY: a program for parentage and sibship inference from multilocus genotype data. Mol. Ecol. Resour. 10, 551–555. (doi:10.1111/j.1755-0998.2009.02787.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Wang J. 2011. COANCESTRY: a program for simulating, estimating and analysing relatedness and inbreeding coefficients. Mol. Ecol. Resour. 11, 141–145. (doi:10.1111/j.1755-0998.2010.02885.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Pew J, Muir PH, Wang J, Frasier TR. 2015. related: an R package for analysing pairwise relatedness from codominant molecular markers. Mol. Ecol. Resour. 15, 557–561. (doi:10.1111/1755-0998.12323) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Wang J. 2002. An estimator for pairwise relatedness using molecular markers. Genetics 160, 1203–1215. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Vonhof MJ, Barber D, Fenton MB, Strobeck C. 2006. A tale of two siblings: multiple paternity in big brown bats (Eptesicus fuscus) demonstrated using microsatellite markers. Mol. Ecol. 15, 241–247. (doi:10.1111/j.1365-294X.2005.02801.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Mayer F. 1995. Multiple paternity and sperm competition in the noctule bat (Nyctalus noctule) revealed by DNA fingerprinting. Bat Res. News 36, 88. [Google Scholar]
- 82.Fox S, Spencer H, O'Brien GM. 2008. Analysis of twinning in flying-foxes (Megachiroptera) reveals superfoetation and multiple-paternity. Acta Chiropterol. 10, 271–278. (doi:10.3161/150811008X414845) [Google Scholar]
- 83.Poage MA, Chamberlain CP. 2001. Empirical relationships between elevation and the stable isotope composition of precipitation and surface waters: considerations for studies of paleoelevation change. Am. J. Sci. 301, 1–15. (doi:10.2475/ajs.301.1.1) [Google Scholar]
- 84.Bowen GJ, Revenaugh J. 2003. Interpolating the isotopic composition of modern meteoric precipitation. Water Resour. Res. 39, 1299 (doi:10.1029/2003wr002086) [Google Scholar]
- 85.Körner C, Farquhar GD, Roksandic Z. 1988. A global survey of carbon isotope discrimination in plants from high altitude. Oecologia 74, 623–632. (doi:10.1007/bf00380063) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Hobson KA, Wassenaar LI, Mila B, Lovette I, Dingle C, Smith TB. 2003. Stable isotopes as indicators of altitudinal distributions and movements in an Ecuadorean hummingbird community. Oecologia 136, 302–308. (doi:10.1007/s00442-003-1271-y) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Suits NS, Denning AS, Berry JA, Still CJ, Kaduk J, Miller JB, Baker IT. 2005. Simulation of carbon isotope discrimination of the terrestrial biosphere. Glob. Biogeochem. Cycles 19, pGB1017. (doi:10.1029/2003gb002141) [Google Scholar]
- 88.Handley LL, Austin AT, Stewart GR, Robinson D, Scrimgeour CM, Raven JA, Heaton THE, Schmidt S. 1999. The 15N natural abundance (δ15N) of ecosystem samples reflects measures of water availability. Funct. Plant Biol. 26, 185–199. (doi:10.1071/PP98146) [Google Scholar]
- 89.Hobson KA. 1999. Stable-carbon and nitrogen isotope ratios of songbird feathers grown in two terrestrial biomes: implications for evaluating trophic relationships and breeding origins. The Condor 101, 799–805. (doi:10.2307/1370067) [Google Scholar]
- 90.Amundson R, Austin AT, Schuur EAG, Yoo K, Matzek V, Kendall C, Uebersax A, Brenner D, Baisden WT. 2003. Global patterns of the isotopic composition of soil and plant nitrogen. Glob. Biogeochem. Cycles 17, 1031–1041. (doi:10.1029/2002gb001903) [Google Scholar]
- 91.Strelkov PP. 1969. Migratory and stationary bats (Chiroptera) of the European parts of the Soviet Union. Acta Zool. Cracov. 14, 393–439. [Google Scholar]
- 92.Rodrigues L, Palmeirim JM. 2007. Migratory behaviour of the Schreiber's bat: when, where and why do cave bats migrate in a Mediterranean region? J. Zool. 274, 116–125. (doi:10.1111/j.1469-7998.2007.00361.x) [Google Scholar]
- 93.Steffens R, Zöphel U, Brockmann D. 2007. 40th anniversary Bat Marking Centre Dresden—evaluation of methods and overview of results. Dresden, Germany: Sächsisches Landesamt für Umwelt und Geologie.
- 94.Schorcht W, Bontadina F, Schaub M. 2009. Variation of adult survival drives population dynamics in a migrating forest bat. J. Anim. Ecol. 78, 1182–1190. (doi:10.1111/j.1365-2656.2009.01577.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Giavi S, Moretti M, Bontadina F, Zambelli N, Schaub M. 2014. Seasonal survival probabilities suggest low migration mortality in migrating bats. PLoS ONE 9, e85628 (doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0085628) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Laland KN. 2004. Social learning strategies. Anim. Learn. Behav. 32, 4–14. (doi:10.3758/BF03196002) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Galef BG, Laland KN. 2005. Social learning in animals: empirical studies and theoretical models. Bioscience 55, 489–499. (doi:10.1641/0006-3568(2005)055[0489:SLIAES]2.0.CO;2) [Google Scholar]
- 98.Boyd R, Richerson PJ. 1985. Culture and the evolutionary process. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. [Google Scholar]
- 99.Boyd R, Richerson PJ. 1988. An evolutionary model of social learning: the effects of spatial and temporal variation. Soc. Learn. 29–48. [Google Scholar]
- 100.Kerth G. 2008. Causes and consequences of sociality in bats. Bioscience 58, 737–746. (doi:10.1641/B580810) [Google Scholar]
- 101.Krause J, Ruxton GD. 2002. Living in groups. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- 102.Caro TM, Hauser MD. 1992. Is there teaching in nonhuman animals? Q. Rev. Biol. 67, 151–174. (doi:10.1086/417553) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103.Alerstam T, Hedenstrom A, Akesson S. 2003. Long-distance migration: evolution and determinants. Oikos 103, 247–260. (doi:10.1034/j.1600-0706.2003.12559.x) [Google Scholar]
- 104.Rolseth SL, Koehler CE, Barclay RMR. 1994. Differences in the diets of juvenile and adult hoary bats, Lasiurus cinereus. J. Mammal. 75, 394–398. (doi:10.2307/1382558) [Google Scholar]
- 105.Adams RA. 1996. Size-specific resource use in juvenile little brown bats, Myotis lucifugus (Chiroptera: Vespertilionidae): is there an ontogenetic shift? Can. J. Zool. 74, 1204–1210. (doi:10.1139/z96-133) [Google Scholar]
- 106.Hamilton IM, Barclay RMR. 1998. Diets of juvenile, yearling, and adult big brown bats (Eptesicus fuscus) in Southeastern Alberta. J. Mammal. 79, 764–771. (doi:10.2307/1383087) [Google Scholar]
- 107.Reimer JP, Baerwald EF, Barclay RMR. 2010. Diet of hoary (Lasiurus cinereus) and silver-haired (Lasionycteris noctivagans) bats while migrating through southwestern Alberta in late summer and autumn. Am. Midl. Nat. 164, 230–237. (doi:10.1674/0003-0031-164.2.230) [Google Scholar]
- 108.Cryan PM. 2008. Mating behavior as a possible cause of bat fatalities at wind turbines. J. Wildl. Manag. 72, 845–849. (doi:10.2193/2007-371) [Google Scholar]
- 109.Šuba J, Petersons G, Rydell J. 2012. Fly-and-forage strategy in the bat Pipistrellus nathusii during autumn migration. Acta Chiropterol. 14, 379–385. (doi:10.3161/150811012X661693) [Google Scholar]
- 110.Jones G, Siemers BM. 2011. The communicative potential of bat echolocation pulses. J. Comp. Physiol. A 197, 447–457. (doi:10.1007/s00359-010-0565-x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 111.Bohn KM, Smotherman M. 2015. Audio-vocal feedback in bats and new roles for echolocation calls in social communication. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 137, 2249–2250. (doi:10.1121/1.4920204) [Google Scholar]
- 112.Hamilton WJ. 1962. Evidence concerning the function of nocturnal call notes of migratory birds. Condor 64, 390–401. (doi:10.2307/1365547) [Google Scholar]
- 113.Farnsworth A. 2005. Flight calls and their value for future ornithological studies and conservation research. The Auk 122, 733–746. (doi:10.1642/0004-8038(2005)122[0733:FCATVF]2.0.CO;2) [Google Scholar]
- 114.Cvikel N, Egert Berg K, Levin E, Hurme E, Borissov I, Boonman A, Amichai E, Yovel Y. 2015. Bats aggregate to improve prey search but might be impaired when their density becomes too high. Curr. Biol. 25, 206–211. (doi:10.1016/j.cub.2014.11.010) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 115.Griffin DR. 1952. Bird navigation. Biol. Rev. Camb. Phil. Soc. 27, 359–400. (doi:10.1111/j.1469-185X.1952.tb01509.x) [Google Scholar]
- 116.Able KP. 2001. The concepts and terminology of bird navigation. J. Avian Biol. 32, 174–183. (doi:10.1034/j.1600-048X.2001.320211.x) [Google Scholar]
- 117.Wiltschko R, Wiltschko W. 2003. Avian navigation: from historical to modern concepts. Anim. Behav. 65, 257–272. (doi:10.1006/anbe.2003.2054) [Google Scholar]
- 118.Wiltschko R, Wiltschko W. 2009. Avian navigation. The Auk 126, 717–743. (doi:10.1525/auk.2009.11009) [Google Scholar]
- 119.Childs SB, Buchler ER. 1981. Perception of simulated stars by Eptesicus fuscus (Vespertilionidae): a potential navigation mechanism. Anim. Behav. 29, 1029–1035. (doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(81)80056-5) [Google Scholar]
- 120.Buchler ER, Childs SB. 1982. Use of the post-sunset glow as an orientation cue by big brown bats (Eptesicus fuscus). J. Mammal. 63, 243–247. (doi:10.2307/1380633) [Google Scholar]
- 121.Holland RA, Borissov I, Siemers BM. 2010. A nocturnal mammal, the greater mouse-eared bat, calibrates a magnetic compass by the sun. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 107, 6941–6945. (doi:10.1073/pnas.0912477107) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 122.Holland RA, Thorup K, Vonhof MJ, Cochran WW, Wikelski M. 2006. Bat orientation using Earth's magnetic field. Nature 444, 702 (doi:10.1038/444702a) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 123.Holland RA, Kirschvink JL, Doak TG, Wikelski M. 2008. Bats use magnetite to detect the Earth's magnetic field. PLoS ONE 3, e1676 (doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0001676) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 124.Barclay RMR. 1984. Observations on the migration, ecology, and behaviour of bats at Delta Marsh, Manitoba. Can. Field-Nat. 98, 331–336. [Google Scholar]
- 125.Timm RM. 1989. Migration and molt patterns of red bats, Lasiurus borealis (Chiroptera: Vespertilioni- dae), in Illinois. Bull. Chicago Acad. Sci. 14, 1–7. [Google Scholar]
- 126.Ahlen I. 1997. Migratory behaviour of bats at south Swedish coasts. Z. Saugetierkunde 62, 375–380. [Google Scholar]
- 127.Serra-Cobo J, Lopez-Roig M, Marques-Bonet T, Lahuerta E. 2000. Rivers as possible landmarks in the orientation flight of Miniopterus schreibersii. Acta Theriol. (Warsz.) 45, 347–352. (doi:10.4098/AT.arch.00-34) [Google Scholar]
- 128.Tsoar A, Nathan R, Bartan Y, Vyssotski A, Dell'Omo G, Ulanovsky N. 2011. Large-scale navigational map in a mammal. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 108, E718–E724. (doi:10.1073/pnas.1107365108) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 129.Griffin DR. 1970. Migrations and homing of bats. In Biology of bats (ed. Wimsatt WA.), pp. 233–264. New York, NY: Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
- 130.Verfuß UK, Miller LA, Schnitzler H-U. 2005. Spatial orientation in echolocating harbour porpoises (Phocoena phocoena). J. Exp. Biol. 208, 3385–3394. (doi:10.1242/jeb.01786) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 131.Schnitzler H-U, Moss CF, Denzinger A. 2003. From spatial orientation to food acquisition in echolocating bats. Trends Ecol. Evol. 18, 386–394. (doi:10.1016/S0169-5347(03)00185-X) [Google Scholar]
- 132.Eby P. 1991. Seasonal movements of grey-headed flying-foxes, Pteropus poliocephalus (Chiroptera: Pteropodidae), from two maternity camps in northern New South Wales. Wildl. Res. 18, 547–559. (doi:10.1071/WR9910547) [Google Scholar]
- 133.Fleming TH, Eby P. 2003. Ecology of bat migration. In Bat ecology (eds Kunz TH, Fenton MB), pp. 156–208. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.