Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2017 Jul 1.
Published in final edited form as: Behav Brain Res. 2016 Mar 16;307:1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2016.03.001

Studies of mice with cyclic AMP-dependent protein kinase (PKA) defects reveal the critical role of PKA's catalytic subunits in anxiety

George Briassoulis 1,2, Margaret F Keil 1, Bilal Naved 1, Sophie Liu 1, Matthew F Starost 3, Maria Nesterova 1, Nirmal Gokarn 1, Anna Batistatos 1, T John Wu 3, Constantine A Stratakis 1
PMCID: PMC4853257  NIHMSID: NIHMS774369  PMID: 26992826

Abstract

Cyclic adenosine mono-phosphate-dependent protein kinase (PKA) is critically involved in the regulation of behavioral responses. Previous studies showed that PKA's main regulatory subunit, R1α, is involved in anxiety-like behaviors. The purpose of this study was to determine how the catalytic subunit, Cα, might affect R1α's function and determine its effects on anxiety-related behaviors.

The marble bury (MB) and elevated plus maze (EPM) tests were used to assess anxiety-like behavior and the hotplate test to assess nociception in wild type (WT) mouse, a Prkar1a heterozygote (Prkar1a+/-) mouse with haploinsufficiency for the regulatory subunit (R1α), a Prkaca heterozygote (Prkaca+/-) mouse with haploinsufficiency for the catalytic subunit (Cα), and a double heterozygote mouse (Prkar1a+/-/Prkaca+/-) with haploinsufficiency for both R1α and Cα. We then examined specific brain nuclei involved in anxiety.

Results of MB test showed a genotype effect, with increased anxiety-like behavior in Prkar1a+/- and Prkar1a+/-/Prkaca+/- compared to WT mice. In the EPM, Prkar1a+/- spent significantly less time in the open arms, while Prkaca+/- and Prkar1a+/-/Prkaca+/- mice displayed less exploratory behavior compared to WT mice. The loss of one Prkar1a allele was associated with a significant increase in PKA activity in the basolateral (BLA) and central (CeA) amygdala and ventromedial hypothalamus (VMH) in both Prkar1a+/- and Prkar1a+/- /Prkaca+/- mice.

Alterations of PKA activity induced by haploinsufficiency of its main regulatory or most important catalytic subunits result in anxiety-like behaviors. The BLA, CeA, and VMH are implicated in mediating these PKA effects in brain.

Keywords: anxiety, amygdala, protein kinase A, knockout mice

Introduction

In brain, genetic manipulations of cAMP-dependent protein kinase A (PKA) suggest its critical role in a wide spectrum of neurobiological and psychiatric disorders. The PKA is a multi-substrate serine/threonine protein kinase responsible for modulating a variety of cellular processes [1-4]. It is a heterotetramer composed of two N-terminally dimerized regulatory (R) subunits bound to two catalytic (C) subunits [5, 6]. PKA activation involves simple subunit dissociation upon binding of cAMP to the R subunits; thereby freeing catalytically active C subunits, which in turn phosphorylate protein substrates that contain appropriate consensus phosphorylation motifs [7]. One target of PKA is the cAMP responsive element (CRE) binding protein (CREB). Phospho-CREB in turn binds to CREs to mediate gene expression that has been linked to numerous behavioral reactions including those to stressors [8-10]. The response generated by cAMP can be terminated by the hydrolysis of cAMP into 5′AMP by phosphodiesterases (PDEs). Various studies indicate that increased cAMP signaling is associated with an anxiety-like phenotype and suggest that elevated cAMP activity is associated with abnormal reactivity to novel environments [11, 12], stress-coping responses [13, 14], and an overall increased anxiety [15-18]. Thus, therapeutic manipulation of PKA activation, such as decreasing phosphorylation of cAMP–CREB might lead to new treatment strategies for anxiety, addiction, or psychiatric disorders [3, 19-21].

Our laboratory has studied the consequences of R and C subunit dysregulation [22]. The Prkar1a and Prkaca genes encode the type 1A regulatory (R1α) and type A catalytic (Cα) subunits of PKA [23]. Regulatory subunits form a homodimer that binds two catalytic subunits (Cα, Cβ, or Cγ coded by the PRKACA, PRKACB, and PRKACG genes, respectively) in the PKA tetramer (R2C2) [8]. When the R and C subunits form a complex, the cAMP-catalytic activity is suppressed. As shown in knockout (KO) mouse studies, these four genes function in a tissue and cell-type specific manner to regulate the activity of the catalytic subunits [5]

Recently, we reported that loss of one Prkar1a allele in mice (Prkar1a+/-) led to an augmentation of anxiety-like behaviors in association with an increase in PKA activity in both the basolateral (BLA) and central amygdala (CeA) [24]. These regions of the amygdala coordinate fear- and anxiety-like behavioral responses [24-27] as well as external cues, responding to threats via direct and indirect projections to the paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus (PVH) and brainstem regions [27-29]. PKA activity is increased in Prkar1a+/-mice because of a deficiency in overall Cα inhibition, although the net effect varies widely in different tissues [25-30].

We reasoned that the introduction of half-null alleles of Cα into the Prkar1a+/- mice might abrogate the excess Cα activity caused by R1α haploinsufficiency. Thus, we crossed Prkar1a+/- and Prkaca+/- mice to generate double heterozygous Prkar1a+/-/Prkaca+/- mice and expected a reversal of the anxiety phenotype that we observed previously [24, 27] in Prkar1a+/-mice, consistent with what has been reported in PKA activity regulation [25, 26]. The data perhaps not unexpectedly support attenuation but not elimination of the anxiety phenotype noted in Prkar1a+/- knockout mice (given the variable expression of other PKA subunits in brain) and highlight the importance of even modest changes in total PKA activity in the regulation of anxiety behaviors and the fact that Cα is not the sole determinant of PKA's cAMP signaling effects.

Materials and Methods

Animals

All mice were housed three to four per standard barrier cages on a ventilated rack in a room with a constant temperature (∼22+/- 1° C) with same-sex littermates with ad libitum access to food and water and maintained on a 12:12 light schedule (lights on at 0600h). All animals were adults at the time of testing (2-7 months old at time of behavioral testing; 6- 10 months at time of biochemical testing). Throughout the entire experimental period, the mice were handled daily and weighed weekly to acclimate to the investigator. All animal procedures were conducted in accordance with the standards approved by the NIH Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals. All animal protocols received prior approval at the NIH. All behavioral testing was performed, as previously reported [31-33], between the hours of 1300-1700 h. One behavioral test per day was performed, with a span of at least two days between tests. The order of behavioral tests was randomly assigned [27]. Two scorers performed behavioral testing and obtained scoring of all results in a blinded fashion (without knowledge of the genotypes of the mice under observation).

Generation of Prkar1a+/−/Prkaca+/− double heterozygous mice

Prkar1a heterozygous mice (Prkar1a+/−) with one null allele of Prkar1aΔ2 were previously generated in our laboratory [28]. Prkaca heterozygous mice (Prkaca+/), which have a neomycin-resistance cassette to replace exons 6–8 of the Prkaca gene [29], were purchased from Mutant Mouse Regional Resource Centers (MMRRC) (strain name: B6; 129×1-Prkacatm1Gsm/Mmnc). We used heterozygotes Prkaca mice because the majority of Cα knockouts are either embryonic lethal or not viable and die in the immediate postnatal period [29, 34]. In addition, using heterozygotes for both Prkar1a+/- and Prkaca+/-, and also double heterozygotes, we ensured equivalent genotyped groups, more appropriate for multiple comparisons. Prkar1a+/- and Prkaca+/- mice were crossed to generate the Prkar1a+/-/Prkaca+/- double heterozygous mice, which were maintained on a mixed C57BL/6 129Sv/B6 hybrid background (REF). All mice were genotyped using tail DNA. Control (wild-type, WT) mice were used from the same litter and were matched for age. Genotyping was conducted by polymerase chain reaction (PCR) using primers previously validated [30]. Three primers (5′-AGCTAGCTTGGCTGGACGTA-3′, 5′-AAGCAGGCGAGCTATTAGTTTAT-3′ and 5′-CATCCATCTCCTATCCCCTTT-3′) were used for Prkar1a genotyping: the WT allele generated a 250 base pair (bp) fragment and the null allele generated an180 bp product. Primers and conditions for the PCR reactions are available upon request [28, 35]. Genotyping of prkaca was done with two pairs of primers: the first pair (5′- CTGACCTTTGAGTATCTGCAC-3′ and 5′- GTCCCACACAAGGTCCAAGTA-3′) was used to detect the WT allele by amplification of the intron between exons 6 and 7 with a product of 250 bp; the second pair was used to detect the knockout allele (5′-AGACTACTGCTCTATCACTGA-3′ and 5′- GTGGTTTGTCCAAACTCATCAATGT-3′) by amplification of a 270-nucleotide fragment of the region between the 3′-end of the neomycin resistance gene and a portion of the intron just 3′ to exon 8. The wild-type allele generated a 6.0-kb fragment, and the knockout allele generated a 2.7-kb fragment.

Mouse phenotyping, numbers, behavioral tests, necropsies

Adrenal tumors or corticosterone overproduction were not identified in Prkar1a+/- or Prkar1a+/-/Prkaca+/- mice by the age they were used in this study; likewise, thyroid neoplasms, schwannomas and other tumors were observed only in older Prkar1a+/- mice, as reported previously [28]. No mice with tumors were used in this study. In addition, standard assessment of neurological function [24] revealed no deficits in mice enrolled in the study. A total of 55 control (wild-type, WT), 40 Prkar1a+/-, 29 Prkaca+/-, and 33 Prkar1a+/-/Prkaca+/- mice (all littermates) were handled for the purposes of this project.

Measurement of anxiety-like behavior and nociception

Heterozygous (Prkaca+/-, Prkar1a+/-), double heterozygous (Prkar1a+/-/Prkaca+/-), and WT mice (both males and females) were tested with various behavioral assays in order to characterize the phenotype. The marble bury test [31] and elevated plus maze (EPM) were performed as described previously [31-33]. The hotplate test was performed as described previously to assess nociception [36].

Marble bury test

Marble bury test was performed as previously described [31, 37, 38]. Mice were transported in their home cages to the testing room two hours prior to the experiments, to acclimate to the new environment. Rodent sawdust bedding was placed in standard mouse cages (38×22×16cm) and eight dark colored marbles were placed on top of the bedding in two evenly spaced rows and the cage was closed with a lid. No food or water was present during the 30-min test period. Lights were turned off in the room for 30-min and then the number of marbles buried 2/3 or greater was recorded.

Elevated plus maze

Elevated plus maze (EPM) testing was performed as described previously [24, 27, 32, 33]. Mice were transported in their home cages to the testing room two hours prior to acclimate prior to testing. The EPM consists of two open arms (30 × 5 cm) and two enclosed arms (30 × 5cm), with end and side- walls (15cm height), and a center platform (5 × 5 cm). The maze was raised to a height of 38 cm above the counter and illuminated (100 lux) from above. The mouse was placed in the center area of the EPM, facing an open arm, and allowed to explore the maze for 5 minutes. Tests were video recorded and analyzed by ANY-maze© software (Stoelting Co., Wood Dale, IL, USA). Arm entry was defined as all four paws in an arm or center area. After 5 minutes, the mouse was removed from the EPM, the number of boli recorded, the maze cleaned with 70% ethanol and allowed to dry prior to testing the next mouse. In addition, hand scoring was performed to validate time and entries into arms, as well as record risk assessment behavior (calculated by dividing number of protected stretch attend postures by total closed arm time) and exploratory behavior (head dips). Measures scored included: open and closed arm time, open and closed arm entries, open to total time ratio (open arm time/open arm time + closed arm time), number of head dips, and number of protected stretch attend postures (defined as two hind feet remaining in closed arm while the mouse elongated its head and shoulders, followed by retraction), and risk assessment ratio (number of protected stretch attend postures/ amount of closed arm time) [39]. The number of closed arm entries is used as a measure of locomotor activity. A video recording device and automated scoring software (ANY-MAZE©) allow standardization and objectivity for behavior in the EPM.

Hotplate test of nociception

This test was performed as described previously [24, 36]. Mice were transported in their home cages to the testing room one to two hours prior to the experiment. The mouse was placed on a hotplate inside a clear plastic cylinder, with the temperature of the hotplate set to 50 degrees Celsius. Latency to lick the hind-paw was recorded. If no response was observed after 45 seconds, the mouse was removed from the hotplate, to avoid any tissue injury.

PKA assays

WT, Prkar1a+/-, Prkaca+/-, and Prkar1a+/-/Prkaca+/- littermates were moved to the testing room 2 h prior to euthanization by CO2 inhalation. Mice were 6- 10 months age at time of tissue procurement, after a minimum of two months from last behavioral testing. The brains were removed and immediately frozen in liquid nitrogen and stored at -80°C until processing. Punch biopsies (0.5 mm diameter stainless steel punch) of specific tissues were obtained from 250 μm sections from the following brain regions [40] : BLA, CeA, (corresponding to Figs. 64-70, Bregma -0.955 to -1.555mm); ventromedial (VMH) and paraventricular hypothalamus (PVH) (corresponding to Figs.64 and 70, Bregma -0.955 to -1.555mm). Olfactory bulb (Olf), eyes, prefrontal cortex (PFC), cerebellum (Cb), hippocampus (Hipp), and thalamus (Thal) were dissected in entirety. All dissected tissues were stored in cryotubes at -80 C prior to homogenization and PKA activity assay. The mouse brain atlas of Allen [41] was used to guide the dissections.

PKA enzymatic activity was measured following our previously described protocol [24, 27, 35, 42]. The assays were carried out in a total volume of 50 μL for 15 min at 37°C in the reaction mixture containing 1 mol/L Tris-HCl (pH 7.5), 1 mol/L DTT, 1 mol/L MgCl2, 60 μmol/L Kemptide (a phosphate acceptor peptide; Leu-Arg-Arg-Ala-Ser-Leu-Gly), 20 μmol/L [γ-32P] ATP (25 Ci/mmol), with or without 5 μmol/L cAMP and 10 μL of the cell extracts. After incubation, the reaction mixtures were spotted onto 0.23-mm phosphocellulose discs and washed thrice in 0.5% phosphoric acid. Filters were air dried and counted by liquid scintillation counter. Basal levels of PKA activity represent the non-stimulated PKA activity. Total PKA activity reflects the PKA activity after the addition of cAMP. PKA values were normalized by protein content of each sample.

Immunohistochemistry

Perfused brains were sent to Histoserv Inc. (Germantown, MD) for immunohistochemistry (IHC) processing. Briefly, brains were dehydrated and cleared with xylene and infiltrated with paraffin. Coronal slices of 10 μm thickness were mounted on slides and processed for IHC with the following steps: deparaffinization with xylene, distilled water wash, pre-treatment of slides at 70°C for 40 min, hydrogen peroxidase treatment to remove endogenous peroxidase activity, bovine serum albumin blocking, tris buffered saline-tween (TBST) wash, incubation with the phosphodiesterase 4 (PDE4) antibody (1:100; Cat #ab14628, abCam, Cambridge, MA). TBST wash, secondary antibody (Biotinylated Anti-Rabbit IgG; #BA1000, Vector, Burlingame, CA), TBST wash, Streptavidin-HRP, tris buffered saline-tween wash, develop slides with DAB (3,3′-Diaminobenzidine; Sigma, St Louis, MO), dehydrate in graded ethanol, treated with xylene to remove ethanol, and coverslipped. This anti-PDE4 antibody detects all known PDE4 A and D variants.

The Allen brain atlas of the mouse was used to identify corresponding nuclei location for all PDE4 antibody stained slides. Slides were coded and analyzed by two independent experimenters were conducted blinded to genotype. Three to four slides per brain (3 brains per genotype) containing the amygdala and hippocampus were assessed at approximately −1.8 to -3.30 Bregma. For each section, slides were photographed at 5× magnification with a Leica microscope/camera (Leica DMRX; Olympus DP72) and files created by CellSens software.

Statistical Analyses

Data were analyzed for effect of genotype and sex by ANOVA and Bonferroni posthoc comparisons among the four groups using SPSS statistical software. A paired samples t-test was done for the paired (basal and total PKA activity) experiments. Significance was determined at p<0.05; all values are reported as means +/- SEM. Behavioral measures in the EPM were analyzed by a multifactorial ANOVA, with between subjects' factors of sex and genotype.

Results

Measurement of anxiety-like behaviors

Marble bury (MB) test

Prkar1a+/-, Prkaca+/, and Prkar1a+/-/Prkaca+/- were compared to WT littermates. ANOVA analysis showed a significant difference (F 5.6, df3, p<0.001), with increased marble burying for Prkar1a+/- (6.3 ± 0.3) and Prkar1a+/-/Prkaca+/- (5.9+ 0.3) compared to WT (4.5± 0.3, p<0.001, p<0.03, respectively) (Figure 1). Prkaca+/- mice showed no difference in marble burying behavior (5.2+ 0.3) compared to WT mice. No sex difference was observed within each genotype (N = 11-24 per sex/genotype).

Figure 1. Marble bury test.

Figure 1

Comparison of wild type, Prkar1a+/- and Prkaca+/- heterozygotes, and Prkar1a+/-/Prkaca+/- double heterozygous for number of marbles 2/3 or more buried after 30 min; Bonferroni significance: *, p<0.05 vs. WT (data combined for gender by genotype).

Elevated Plus Maze (EPM)

ANOVA showed differences among groups in the center of EPM (F 2.6, df 3, p<0.05), closed arm time (F 2.7, df 3, p<0.05), open arm time (F 3.3, df 3, p<0.03), and open to total time ratio (F 2.9, df 3, p<0.04). Bonferroni post hoc analysis showed that Prkaca+/- spent significantly more time in the center (63.75 ± 10.7 vs. 28 ± 8.2 sec; p<0.05) and less in the closed arms (220.5±13.4 vs. 259.4.5 ±5.8 sec; p<0.05) than Prkar1a+/- mice (Figure 2a and 2d), while Prkar1a+/-/Prkaca+/- and WT mice had similar amount of center time. Prkar1a+/-spent less time (6.8 ± 1.7 sec) in the open arm than WT mice (21.4 ± 3.9 sec, p<0.02) (Figure 2b), scoring half open to total time ratio (0.03 ± 0.01, F 2.9, df 3, p<0.04) compared to WT (0.09 ± 0.02), Prkaca+/- (1.04 ± 0.05) or Prkar1a+/-/Prkaca+/- mice (0.08 ± 0.02). No other differences between genotypes Prkaca+/- (18.3± 6.8 sec) or Prkar1a+/- /Prkaca+/- (16.4 ± 4.2 sec) were detected. Similarly, no difference was found between heterozygote groups and WT for risk assessment. Interestingly, Prkaca+/- and Prkar1a+/- /Prkaca+/- mice displayed significantly less exploratory behavior compared to WT mice (total head dips: 3.7±0.9, 4.2±0.9, respectively vs. 7.3 ± 0.6; F 4.9, df 3, p< 0.03) (n= 49 to 57 per group) (Figure 2c). The number of closed arm entries was used as an indicator of locomotor activity and no genotype differences were found (#closed arm entries: WT 8 ± 0.6; Prkar1a+/- 7.5±0.7; Prkaca+/- 8 ±1.3; Prkar1a+/- /Prkaca+/- 7.9 ± 1.3, p<0.9), which suggests that locomotion did not influence the measures of anxiety-like behavior. No genotype effect was found for percentage of open arm entries (% open/closed arm entries: WT 17%; Prkar1a+/- 13%; Prkaca+/- 6%; Prkar1a+/-/Prkaca+/- 22%, p<0.74). No sex effects on various EPM measures of unconditioned response to novelty were found (data not shown).

Figure 2. Elevated plus maze.

Figure 2

a) Closed arm time and b) Open arm time. Comparison of wild type, Prkar1a+/- and Prkar1a+/-heterozygotes, and Prkar1a+/-/Prkaca+/- double heterozygous for amount of time spent (seconds) in closed arm of elevated plus maze during 5 min test period. *, p<0.05. c) Total head dips. Comparison of wild type, Prkar1a+/- and Prkaca+/-heterozygotes, and Prkar1a+/-/Prkaca+/- double heterozygous for the total number of head dips in closed and open arms and center of elevated plus maze during 5 min test period. *, p<0.05 vs. WT d) Center time. Comparison of wild type, Prkar1a+/- and Prkar1a+/-heterozygotes, and Prkar1a+/-/Prkaca+/- double heterozygous for amount of time spent (seconds) in center of elevated plus maze during 5 min test period. *, p<0.05 vs. Prkar1a+/- (data combined for gender by genotype).

Measurement of nociception

Since the amygdala is known to have a key role in the emotional –affective dimension of pain, the inclusion of a nociceptive assessment was important to include in the behavioral phenotype of the Prkar1a, Prkaca knock-out mice. To investigate the potential role of the R1α subunit in nociception we examined the response in the hot-plate test. ANOVA analysis of latency to lick response in the hotplate test showed the expected sex differences noted for Prkar1a+/- and WT, with a longer latency in females compared to males (males: WT 28.8 ±1.6, Prkar1a+/- 24.9 ±1 seconds vs. females: WT 31.7 ±1.2, Prkar1a+/- 31.4 ±1.7 seconds; p<0.05; n= 20-21 per group); but no significant gender effect for Prkaca+/- or Prkar1a+/-/Prkaca+/- mice (Figure 3). The finding of expected sex differences in Prkar1a+/- and WT, but not Prkaca+/- or Prkar1a+/-/Prkaca+/- mice, suggests that the alteration in PKA signaling is not a ubiquitous effect.

Figure 3. Hotplate test.

Figure 3

Comparison of latency to paw lick (sec) in male and female mice of all 4 genotypes. *, p<0.05.

PKA activity

To investigate possible anatomical sites associated with changes in anxiety-like behavior we determined the PKA activity in the brain of WT, Prkar1a+/-, Prkaca+/-, and Prkar1a+/-/Prkaca+/- mice. Only BLA, CeA, and VMH PKA activity differed among genotypes (Table 1). Bonferroni post-hoc tests revealed differences among the following brain regions: BLA and CeA basal PKA activity was higher in the Prkar1a+/- mice compared to WT (p<0.03) and Prkaca+/- mice (p<0.01) (Figure 4a,c); BLA and CeA total PKA activity was higher in Prkar1a+/- compared to WT mice (p<0.02). (Figure 4b,d). VMH basal PKA activity was significantly higher in Prkar1a+/- compared to WT (p<0.01) and Prkaca+/- mice (p<0.04). VMH and thalamus total PKA activity was significantly higher in Prkar1a+/- compared to WT mice (p<0.01) (Figures 4e,f). No genotype differences in basal or cAMP-stimulated PKA activity were found in PVH, hippocampus, PFC, cerebellum, olfactory bulb, or eyes (Table 1). However, all four genotypes, without exception, showed an increase in their cAMP-stimulation of kinase activity (p<0.0001) compared to basal PKA activity.

Table 1.

Comparison of basal and cAMP-stimulated PKA activity among Prkar1a+/-/Prkaca+/-, Prkaca+/-, Prkar1a+/-, and WT in a control (non-stressed) situation in various brain areas (BLA = basolateral amygdala; CeA = central amygdala; VMH = ventral medial hypothalamus; PVH = paraventricular nucleus; Thal = Thalamus; Hipp = hippocampus; PFC = prefrontal cortex; Cb= cerebellum; Olf= olfactory bulb).

Ar
ea
Basal PKA activity
(- cAMP Normalized PKA/1 mcg)
(mean±SEM)
N
(mi
ce)
AN
OVA
F
(df)
p-
value
(group
differe
nces)
Total PKA activity
(+ cAMP Normalized PKA/1 mcg)
(mean±SEM)
N
(mi
ce)
AN
OVA
F
(df)
p-
value
(group
differe
nces)
WT Prkar
1a+/-
Prka
ca+/-
Prkar
1a+/-
/Prka
ca+/-
WT Prkar1a
+/-
Prkac
a+/-
Prkar1
a+/-
/Prkac
a+/-
BLA 284±108 1275±306* 337±95 585±121 27 5.84(3) <0.004 2892±681 12060±3219** 5544±1747 6855±1600 27 3.64 (3) <0.028
CeA 385±154 2575±806* 293±60 1089±287 27 5.69 (3) <0.005 3700±975 19486±6102** 4354±847 13887±4340 27 4.27 (3) <0.015
VMH 536±87 1732±558# 489±93 717±131 55 3.9 (3) <0.01 3968±646 15402±4999## 4404±1163 8274±2313 55 4.07 (3) 0.01
PVH 296±94 1330±835 734±280 1362±622 21 1.04 (3) 0.3 2373±1037 7494±2153 4530±560 8387±1274 33 1.22 (3) 0.4
Thal 374±180 951±365 216±66 1005±338 35 1.6 (3) 0.2 3095±1335 7548±2306## 1737±668 12134±4081 35 3.4 (3) 0.03
Hipp 110±53 450±318 99±21 266±61 21 1.38 (3) 0.28 829±406 1566±1491 706±258 2212±377 21 1.86 (3) 0.17
PFC 92±16 202±70 112±50 145±112 45 1.76 (3) 0.17 743±135 1654±691 1132±386 1395±416 45 1.55 (3) 0.22
Cb 310±87 208±64 90±38 126±39 45 1.3 (3) 0.29 716±159 861±139 364±159 534±186 45 1.8 (3) 0.16
Olf 272±37 300±56 150±29 255±46 62 1.9 (3) 0.14 2117±208 3010±486 1749±335 2685±381 62 2.5 (3) 0.07
Eyes 267±29 232±45 235±40 326±172 41 0.4 (3) 0.77 2195±258 1907±278 3483±510 2410±1373 41 2.4 (3) 0.08

Bonferroni post-hoc tests:

*

Basal PKA activity was higher in the Prkar1a+/- mice compared to WT (p<0.03) and Prkar1a+/- mice (p<0.01);

**

Total PKA activity was higher in Prkar1a+/-compared to WT mice (p<0.02);

#

VMH basal PKA activity was higher in Prkar1a+/- compared to WT (p<0.01) and Prkar1a+/- mice (p<0.04);

##

VMH and thalamus total PKA activity was significantly higher in Prkar1a+/- compared to WT mice (p<0.01)

Figure 4. PKA Activity (basal and total).

Figure 4

Figure 4

Comparison of PKA activity among all four genotypes in the basolateral amygdala (BLA) (a, b), central amygdala (CeA) (c, d), and ventromedial hypothalamus (VMH) (e, f). Basal (left column (figures a, c and e)) and total (right column (figures b, d and f)) PKA activity. Bonferroni significance: *Prkar1a+/- vs. WT, Prkaca+/- (p<0.01); ** Prkar1a+/- vs. WT (p<0.01).

PDE4 expression

To investigate possible compensatory mechanisms of PKA-catalytic subunits we examined PDE4 expression in the hippocampus and amygdala, which comprise the two independent memory systems of the temporal lobe that have a crucial role during emotional arousal. Photomicrographs were qualitatively analyzed by two observers (blinded) and scored (-, +, ++, +++, ++++). Photomicrographs demonstrate qualitative differences in expression of PDE4 in brain areas across genotypes (Figure 5).

Figure 5. PDE4 expression.

Figure 5

Photomicrographs of PDE4 expression in brain areas among all four genotypes (WT, Prkar1a+/- (R1α), Prkaca+/- (C α), Prkar1a+/-/Prkaca+/- (DH) in the hippocampus (CA1, CA2, CA3, Dentate gyrus (DG)), amygdala, and cortex. Scale bar at the lower right corner = 100 μm.

In the dorsal dentate gyrus (DG) PDE4 expression is attenuated in Prkar1a+/- compared to WT. PDE4 expression in Prkaca+/- and Prkar1a+/-/Prkaca+/- is diminished compared to WT and less than the expression in Prkar1a+/-. PDE4 expression in the CA-subfields of the hippocampus shows a similar pattern as the DG, with less expression in Prkar1a+/-, Prkaca+/-, and Prkar1a+/-/Prkaca+/- compared to WT. The differences in PDE4 expression in the basolateral amygdala between genotypes is less pronounced; however a pattern of attenuated expression is noted in Prkar1a+/-, Prkaca+/-, and Prkar1a+/-/Prkaca+/- compared to WT. There are minor observed differences in PDE4 expression in the cortex between genotypes; a pattern of attenuated expression is noted in Prkar1a+/- compared to WT and Prkaca+/-, and Prkar1a+/-/Prkaca+/- (Table 2).

Table 2.

Comparison of PDE4 expression among WT, Prkar1a+/-, Prkaca+/-, and Prkar1a+/-/Prkaca+/-various brain areas (Dentate gyrus of hippocampus, CA-subfields of hippocampus (CA1, CA2, CA3), BLA = basolateral amygdala; LA = lateral amygdala, hippocampus; cortex).

WT R1α R1α/ Cα

Dentate gyrus ++++ ++ + +

CA-subfield hippocampus (CA1, CA2, CA3) +++ ++ + +

BLA +++ - ++ ++
LA +++ +/- ++ +

Cortex +++ + ++ ++

Discussion

The results of the present study suggest first and not surprisingly, that the relative availability of the R1α and Cα subunit in various parts of the brain is important for regulating the PKA activity-related effects on anxiety-like behaviors. Second and interestingly, by halving the amount of the main PKA catalytic subunit (Prkaca+/-) in the previously described Prkar1a+/- mice, the anxiety phenotype of the latter was attenuated but not eliminated. Third, our data support the importance of the BLA, CeA, and DG in mediating PKA effects on anxiety-related behaviors.

Results of the MB test, a test of unconditioned defensive behavior (that involves approach-avoidance behavior) showed a clear effect of the genotype: Prkar1a+/-and Prkar1a+/-/Prkaca+/- mice buried significantly more marbles than WT or Prkaca+/- mice, consistent with higher anxiety-like behavior. Interestingly, the behavioral response of Prkaca+/- mice was similar to the WT mice, which suggests that the finding of increased anxiety-like behavior noted in the Prkar1a+/- and Prkar1a+/-/Prkaca+/- mice is most likely related to attenuated inhibition from knockdown of the regulatory subunit. Recent studies propose that marble bury behavior in mice may reflect repetitive and perseverative behavior that is sensitive to anxiolytic and 5-HT1A compounds that are regulated by cAMP/PKA signaling [43, 44].

However, Prkar1a+/-/Prkaca+/-mice did not demonstrate increased anxiety-like behavior in the spatiotemporal measures of the EPM, suggesting that alterations in catalytic subunit activity independently affect neural pathways associated with various aspects of anxiety behavior [24, 45]. Although Prkar1a+/- mice spent less time in the open arms of the maze compared to WT and Prkaca+/- littermates, no difference was found in open arm time between Prkaca+/- or Prkar1a+/-/Prkaca+/- and WT mice, implying that Cα subunit activity is an important modulator of anxiety behavior. This is further supported by the finding that both genotypes with down-regulated Cα exhibited less exploratory behavior (total head dips), compared to WT mice [46] since exploratory behavior loads as a distinct dimension from anxiety in factor analysis of spatiotemporal measures of the EPM [45]. Prkaca+/- mice spent significantly more time in the center area and less time in the closed arms than Prkar1a+/- mice (a factor related to decision making behavior in factor analysis of EPM behavior), which also illustrates the behaviorally selective effects of down-regulation of the catalytic subunit in the EPM.

It is interesting there was no apparent effect of the genotypes in risk assessment in the EPM [46-48], which implies that arousal/emotional reactivity behavior [49, 50] was similar among the four genotypes. Recent studies report that risk assessment behavior is a highly sensitive index of anxiety, based on ethological and pharmacological manipulations while head dipping is as an index of exploratory behavior (head dip over side arm of the maze). Although exposure to the open arm area functioned as a stressor that was sufficient to elicit increased anxiety-like (avoidance) behavior in Prkar1a+/- mice, the similarities in risk assessment in all four groups suggest that alteration in PKA activity discriminated between anxiety and sedation by independently affecting neural pathways associated with various aspects of anxiety behaviors [51]. Importantly, these behaviors were observed in the absence of changes to total arm entries, suggesting that the observed changes in spatiotemporal- related measures in the EPM were behaviorally selective [52-54].

Studies on the neural mechanisms of anxiety and threat processing propose two mechanisms: trait anxiety significantly increases prefrontal cortex-amydala connectivity during threat processing; or that trait anxiety may be the result of failed top-down inhibition of the prefrontal cortex resulting in attenuated inhibition of the amygdala aversive response [55-59]. In this study there was no genotype difference in PKA activity in the PFC, an area known to have extensive reciprocal connections with the amygdala suggesting that the alteration of PKA activity in the amygdala of the of Prkar1a+/- mice has a role in the inhibition of approach in situations of potential threat (results of MB test) and unconditioned response to novelty (i.e. EPM).

Measurement of PKA activity in various brain areas revealed increased PKA activity in the amygdala in Prkar1a+/- compared to Prkaca+/- or WT, and in part compared to Prkar1a+/-/Prkaca+/- mice. The alteration in PKA activity in these transgenic mice was not a ubiquitous effect, since PKA activity was found to be similar between heterozygotes and WT mice in other brain (orbitofrontal cortex, hippocampus, cerebellum, paraventricular hypothalamus) and neural sensory (olfactory bulb, eyes) areas. The data presented here also suggest that the effect of down regulation of the catalytic subunit of PKA is localized to the amygdala, since Prkaca+/- and Prkar1a+/-/Prkaca+/- mice did not differ from WT mice in PKA activity in the BLA and spatiotemporal measures of the EPM. However, Prkar1a+/-/Prkaca+/- and Prkar1a+/- mice showed increased anxiety-like behavior in the MB test, which also correlated with higher CeA- PKA activity compared to WT and Prkaca+/- mice. These data indicate that the partial restoration of the amygdala PKA activity is associated with a parallel increase in the anxietylike behavior in the Prkar1a+/-/Prkaca+/- mice, further supporting the previously established role of the amygdala in the processing of sensory information related to anxiety and emotion as well as regulation of arousal level [19, 24]. In addition, the anxiety-like behavior noted in the open arm time in the EPM of Prkar1a+/- but not the Prkar1a+/-/Prkaca+/- mice (compared to the WT) further supports the role of the catalytic subunit activity in ameliorating the anxiety-like behavior in the open arm.

Prior studies provide evidence that the CeA, which is the major output and the anatomical link between the amygdala complex and motor regions in the brain stem, is responsible for exploratory behavior in the EPM [60]. The BLA, strongly involved in emotional processing and the first to process an aversive stimulus [61], mediates anxiety possibly through hypothalamic modulation of glucocorticoid release [62, 63]. Importantly, John et al. [51], reported that the intra-BLA infusion of an anxiolytic increased the time spent in the open arms as well as the number of open arm entries. The higher PKA activity of Prkar1a+/- mice in the VMH and the overall higher PKA activity in both Prkar1a+/- and Prkar1a+/-/Prkaca+/- mice may account for the differences in exploratory or marble burying behavior of the two genotypes bearing one (alone or combined) Prkar1a allele loss in our study. These findings also support the hypothesis of discrete regions of the hypothalamus in mediating anxiogenic effects through the major catalytic subunit of PKA [49].

In parallel studies of generalized anxiety disorder patients, the BLA and CeA connectivity patterns with the midbrain, thalamus, and cerebellum measured during functional magnetic resonance imaging were significantly less distinct than in healthy controls, and increased gray matter volume was noted primarily in the CeA [59, 64]. Similarly, adolescents with generalized anxiety disorder, but not controls, showed greater activation to fearful faces than to happy faces in a distributed network including the amygdala, ventral prefrontal cortex, and anterior cingulate cortex [65]. These results are in accordance with findings of this study showing a significantly higher PKA activity difference between amygdala and other brain areas (thalamus, cerebellum, olfactory bulb or eyes) in the Prkar1a+/- and even Prkar1a+/-/Prkaca+/- mice as compared to the WT and Prkaca+/- mice. Recent imaging studies on aversion highlighted the recruitment of thalamus, midbrain and orbitofrontal regions that remained independent of sensory modality [66], a finding that supports our results of indifferent eye and olfactory PKA activity between genotypes.

Compensatory mechanisms in remaining PKA subunits and PDE4 may also play a role in areas not showing any differences in PKA activity between Prkar1a+/- and Prkar1a+//Prkaca+/- and WT or Prkaca+/- mice. In some brain areas, such as in the PVH, Prkaca+/- mice appeared to have more PKA activity as compared to the WT mice, probably indicating a compensatory mechanism. Prior studies report that compensatory increases in Cβ levels occurred in brain of Cα knockout mice [67]. Also, it has previously been shown that, although accounting for just 5–10% of total PKA activity in mouse cells, Cβ compensates not only states of Cα deficiency [29] but also in the presence of strain-specific genetic modifiers [68] and dysregulated PKA activity [26]. Similarly, RI compensation has been shown in the hippocampus and cerebral cortex of RII/ mice, resulting in a PKA holoenzyme with a significantly increased basal activity, probably caused by a lower threshold of activation associated with the RI holoenzyme [69, 70].

The dentate gyrus and hippocampus have a significant role in affective processing via extensive connections from the amygdala and hypothalamus and have been implicated in anxiety disorders. The dorsal axis of the hippocampus has an important role in spatial processing and the ventral axis is important in anxiety and olfactory processes. PDE4 is highly expressed in brain regions involved with the regulation of memory and anxiety. PDE4 activity is enhanced through PKA phosphorylation, leading to an abrupt termination of the signal [71], and has a crucial role in the regulation of it's intracellular concentration. An increase in PDE4 activity may be a compensatory mechanism to accelerate cAMP degradation.

Our data show that the pattern of PDE4 expression in neural areas involved with emotional arousal and anxiety is not ubiquitous across genotypes similar to the variance in PKA activity noted, and is consistent with a lack of compensatory mechanism. In the dentate gyrus and CA-subfields of the hippocampus and to a lesser extent in the amygdala and cortex the pattern of PDE4 expression parallels the differences in behavioral and PKA activity noted among genotypes suggesting that compensatory mechanisms in PKA-catalytic or regulatory subunits may mediate various aspects of anxiety behavior. The increase in PKA activity noted in various neural areas parallels the attenuated PDE4 expression across genotypes. Since PDE4 accelerates cAMP degradation, the attenuated expression noted across genotypes is consistent with a lack of a compensatory mechanism. These findings are consistent with what is known about PDE4 expression and the regulation of anxiety and depression [72-75] and provide additional support that the relative availability of the R1α and Cα subunit in various parts of the brain is important for regulating the PKA activity-related effects on anxiety-like behaviors.

It appears that PRKAR1A inhibition in humans and Prkar1a-down-regulation in mice is associated with increased risk for psychopathology, consistent with the importance of this protein in cAMP/PKA signaling in brain function. We recently reported an increased incidence of psychiatric disorders in adults and children with PRKAR1A mutations. The most frequent psychiatric diagnosis in adults were anxiety, depression, and bipolar disorder (in that order), while for children learning difficulties, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, anxiety, and depression (in that order) [76]. The amygdala and prefrontal cortex are key structures in the pathophysiology of anxiety disorders, and a putative target for anxiolytic treatments [77, 78]. Liu et al [79] recently reported the essential role of protein kinase C in anxiety-related behaviors. Using a mouse model they showed that activation of glutamatergic kainate-type receptors led to anxiety-related behaviors and glutamergic synapse up-regulation through protein kinase C located in the dendritic spines of the prelimbic cortical excitatory neurons, which suggests that PKC inhibitors may have a role in pharmacologic management of anxiety. Our results suggest that PKA activity in the amygdala regulates the anxiety response, and a better understanding of the downstream targets of increased PKA activity may identify novel therapeutic targets to treat anxiety.

Highlights.

  • Alterations of total PKA activity in the amygdala regulate the anxiety response.

  • Both R1α and Cα subunit activity regulate PKA signaling in the amygdala and anxiety.

  • Cα activity is not the sole determinant of PKA's cAMP signaling effects.

Acknowledgments

We thank Dr. Kitman Tsang, Ms. Florencia Vasta, Ms. Emily Sun, and Ms. Melissa Tang for their assistance with this project. This work was supported by NICHD, NIH intramural project Z01-HD-000642-04 to Dr. C.A. Stratakis. This study was supported in part by intramural graduate student grant from the Uniformed Services University of the Health Sciences (MFK) and Office of Naval Research (TJW) and, in part, by the Department of Pediatrics, Faculty of Medicine, University of Crete, Heraklion, Crete, Greece (GB).

Footnotes

Conflict of Interest: NICHD, USUHS, or DOD had no further role in the study design; in the collection, analysis, and interpretation of the data; in the writing of the report, or in the decision to submit the paper for publication.

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

References

  • 1.Clegg CH, Abrahamsen MS, Degen JL, Morris DR, McKnight GS. Cyclic AMP-dependent protein kinase controls basal gene activity and steroidogenesis in Y1 adrenal tumor cells. Biochemistry. 1992;31:3720–6. doi: 10.1021/bi00129a023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Taylor SS, Keshwani MM, Steichen JM, Kornev AP. Evolution of the eukaryotic protein kinases as dynamic molecular switches. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2012;367:2517–28. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2012.0054. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Arnsten AF, Ramos BP, Birnbaum SG, Taylor JR. Protein kinase A as a therapeutic target for memory disorders: rationale and challenges. Trends Mol Med. 2005;11:121–8. doi: 10.1016/j.molmed.2005.01.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Beer B, Chasin M, Clody DE, Vogel JR. Cyclic adenosine monophosphate phosphodiesterase in brain: effect on anxiety. Science. 1972;176:428–30. doi: 10.1126/science.176.4033.428. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Amieux PS, McKnight GS. The essential role of RI alpha in the maintenance of regulated PKA activity. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2002;968:75–95. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2002.tb04328.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Oyen O, Myklebust F, Scott JD, Cadd GG, McKnight GS, Hansson V, et al. Subunits of cyclic adenosine 3′,5′-monophosphate-dependent protein kinase show differential and distinct expression patterns during germ cell differentiation: alternative polyadenylation in germ cells gives rise to unique smaller-sized mRNA species. Biol Reprod. 1990;43:46–54. doi: 10.1095/biolreprod43.1.46. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Taylor SS, Buechler JA, Yonemoto W. cAMP-dependent protein kinase: framework for a diverse family of regulatory enzymes. Annu Rev Biochem. 1990;59:971–1005. doi: 10.1146/annurev.bi.59.070190.004543. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Skalhegg BS, Tasken K. Specificity in the cAMP/PKA signaling pathway. Differential expression, regulation, and subcellular localization of subunits of PKA. Front Biosci. 2000;5:D678–93. doi: 10.2741/skalhegg. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Adamec R, Strasser K, Blundell J, Burton P, McKay DW. Protein synthesis and the mechanisms of lasting change in anxiety induced by severe stress. Behav Brain Res. 2006;167:270–86. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2005.09.019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Arnsten AF. Stress signalling pathways that impair prefrontal cortex structure and function. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2009;10:410–22. doi: 10.1038/nrn2648. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Plagge A, Isles AR, Gordon E, Humby T, Dean W, Gritsch S, et al. Imprinted Nesp55 influences behavioral reactivity to novel environments. Mol Cell Biol. 2005;25:3019–26. doi: 10.1128/MCB.25.8.3019-3026.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Barrot M, Olivier JD, Perrotti LI, DiLeone RJ, Berton O, Eisch AJ, et al. CREB activity in the nucleus accumbens shell controls gating of behavioral responses to emotional stimuli. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2002;99:11435–40. doi: 10.1073/pnas.172091899. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Kim KS, Han PL. Mice lacking adenylyl cyclase-5 cope badly with repeated restraint stress. J Neurosci Res. 2009;87:2983–93. doi: 10.1002/jnr.22119. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Kelly MP, Cheung YF, Favilla C, Siegel SJ, Kanes SJ, Houslay MD, et al. Constitutive activation of the G-protein subunit Galphas within forebrain neurons causes PKA-dependent alterations in fear conditioning and cortical Arc mRNA expression. Learn Mem. 2008;15:75–83. doi: 10.1101/lm.723708. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Zhang HT, Huang Y, Masood A, Stolinski LR, Li Y, Zhang L, et al. Anxiogenic-like behavioral phenotype of mice deficient in phosphodiesterase 4B (PDE4B) Neuropsychopharmacology. 2008;33:1611–23. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301537. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Favilla C, Abel T, Kelly MP. Chronic Galphas signaling in the striatum increases anxiety-related behaviors independent of developmental effects. J Neurosci. 2008;28:13952–6. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4986-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Hansen RT, 3rd, Conti M, Zhang HT. Mice deficient in phosphodiesterase-4A display anxiogenic-like behavior. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2014;231:2941–54. doi: 10.1007/s00213-014-3480-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Wood SJ, Toth M. Molecular pathways of anxiety revealed by knockout mice. Mol Neurobiol. 2001;23:101–19. doi: 10.1385/MN:23:2-3:101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Pandey SC, Roy A, Zhang H. The decreased phosphorylation of cyclic adenosine monophosphate (cAMP) response element binding (CREB) protein in the central amygdala acts as a molecular substrate for anxiety related to ethanol withdrawal in rats. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2003;27:396–409. doi: 10.1097/01.ALC.0000056616.81971.49. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Zhang J, Huang XY, Ye ML, Luo CX, Wu HY, Hu Y, et al. Neuronal nitric oxide synthase alteration accounts for the role of 5-HT1A receptor in modulating anxiety-related behaviors. J Neurosci. 2010;30:2433–41. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5880-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Azam MA, Tripuraneni NS. Selective Phosphodiesterase 4B Inhibitors: A Review. Sci Pharm. 2014;82:453–81. doi: 10.3797/scipharm.1404-08. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Kirschner LS, Carney JA, Pack SD, Taymans SE, Giatzakis C, Cho YS, et al. Mutations of the gene encoding the protein kinase A type I-alpha regulatory subunit in patients with the Carney complex. Nat Genet. 2000;26:89–92. doi: 10.1038/79238. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Bossis I, Stratakis CA. Minireview: PRKAR1A: normal and abnormal functions. Endocrinology. 2004;145:5452–8. doi: 10.1210/en.2004-0900. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Keil MF, Briassoulis G, Gokarn N, Nesterova M, Wu TJ, Stratakis CA. Anxiety phenotype in mice that overexpress protein kinase A. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2012;37:836–43. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2011.09.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.McKnight GS, Clegg CH, Uhler MD, Chrivia JC, Cadd GG, Correll LA, et al. Analysis of the cAMP-dependent protein kinase system using molecular genetic approaches. Recent Prog Horm Res. 1988;44:307–35. doi: 10.1016/b978-0-12-571144-9.50014-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Yin Z, Pringle DR, Jones GN, Kelly KM, Kirschner LS. Differential role of PKA catalytic subunits in mediating phenotypes caused by knockout of the Carney complex gene Prkar1a. Mol Endocrinol. 2011;25:1786–93. doi: 10.1210/me.2011-1008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Keil MF, Briassoulis G, Nesterova M, Miraftab N, Gokarn N, Wu TJ, et al. Threat bias in mice with inactivating mutations of Prkar1a. Neuroscience. 2013;241:206–14. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2013.03.027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Kirschner LS, Kusewitt DF, Matyakhina L, Towns WH, 2nd, Carney JA, Westphal H, et al. A mouse model for the Carney complex tumor syndrome develops neoplasia in cyclic AMP-responsive tissues. Cancer Res. 2005;65:4506–14. doi: 10.1158/0008-5472.CAN-05-0580. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Skalhegg BS, Huang Y, Su T, Idzerda RL, McKnight GS, Burton KA. Mutation of the Calpha subunit of PKA leads to growth retardation and sperm dysfunction. Mol Endocrinol. 2002;16:630–9. doi: 10.1210/mend.16.3.0793. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Griffin KJ, Kirschner LS, Matyakhina L, Stergiopoulos S, Robinson-White A, Lenherr S, et al. Down-regulation of regulatory subunit type 1A of protein kinase A leads to endocrine and other tumors. Cancer Res. 2004;64:8811–5. doi: 10.1158/0008-5472.CAN-04-3620. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Broekkamp CL, Rijk HW, Joly-Gelouin D, Lloyd KL. Major tranquillizers can be distinguished from minor tranquillizers on the basis of effects on marble burying and swim-induced grooming in mice. Eur J Pharmacol. 1986;126:223–9. doi: 10.1016/0014-2999(86)90051-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Lister RG. The use of a plus-maze to measure anxiety in the mouse. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1987;92:180–5. doi: 10.1007/BF00177912. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Pellow S, Chopin P, File SE, Briley M. Validation of open:closed arm entries in an elevated plus-maze as a measure of anxiety in the rat. J Neurosci Methods. 1985;14:149–67. doi: 10.1016/0165-0270(85)90031-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Huang Y, Roelink H, McKnight GS. Protein kinase A deficiency causes axially localized neural tube defects in mice. J Biol Chem. 2002;277:19889–96. doi: 10.1074/jbc.M111412200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Nesterova MV, Sashchenko LP, Vasiliev VY, Severin ES. A cyclic adenosine 3′,5′-monophosphate-dependent histone kinase from pig brain. Purification and some properties of the enzyme. Biochim Biophys Acta. 1975;377:271–81. doi: 10.1016/0005-2744(75)90309-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Ballou LR, Botting RM, Goorha S, Zhang J, Vane JR. Nociception in cyclooxygenase isozyme-deficient mice. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2000;97:10272–6. doi: 10.1073/pnas.180319297. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Treit D, Pinel JP, Fibiger HC. Conditioned defensive burying: a new paradigm for the study of anxiolytic agents. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1981;15:619–26. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(81)90219-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Njung'e K, Handley SL. Evaluation of marble-burying behavior as a model of anxiety. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1991;38:63–7. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(91)90590-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Rodgers RJ, Dalvi A. Anxiety, defence and the elevated plus-maze. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 1997;21:801–10. doi: 10.1016/s0149-7634(96)00058-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Palkovits M. Punch sampling biopsy technique. Methods Enzymol. 1983;103:368–76. doi: 10.1016/s0076-6879(83)03025-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Dong HW. Allen reference atlas: a digital color brain atlas of the C57Black/6J male mouse. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley; 2008. [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Nesterova M, Bossis I, Wen F, Horvath A, Matyakhina L, Stratakis CA. An immortalized human cell line bearing a PRKAR1A-inactivating mutation: effects of overexpression of the wild-type Allele and other protein kinase A subunits. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 2008;93:565–71. doi: 10.1210/jc.2007-1902. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Thomas A, Burant A, Bui N, Graham D, Yuva-Paylor LA, Paylor R. Marble burying reflects a repetitive and perseverative behavior more than novelty-induced anxiety. Psychopharmacology. 2009;204:361–73. doi: 10.1007/s00213-009-1466-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Lopez-Rubalcava C, Fernandez-Guasti A, Urba-Holmgren R. Age-dependent differences in the rat's conditioned defensive burying behavior: effect of 5-HT1A compounds. Developmental psychobiology. 1996;29:157–69. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-2302(199603)29:2<157::AID-DEV5>3.0.CO;2-R. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Rodgers RJ, Johnson NJ. Factor analysis of spatiotemporal and ethological measures in the murine elevated plus-maze test of anxiety. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1995;52:297–303. doi: 10.1016/0091-3057(95)00138-m. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Cole JC, Burroughs GJ, Laverty CR, Sheriff NC, Sparham EA, Rodgers RJ. Anxiolytic-like effects of yohimbine in the murine plus-maze: strain independence and evidence against alpha 2-adrenoceptor mediation. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 1995;118:425–36. doi: 10.1007/BF02245943. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Carobrez AP, Bertoglio LJ. Ethological and temporal analyses of anxiety-like behavior: the elevated plus-maze model 20 years on. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2005;29:1193–205. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.04.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Espejo EF. Selective dopamine depletion within the medial prefrontal cortex induces anxiogenic-like effects in rats placed on the elevated plus maze. Brain Res. 1997;762:281–4. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(97)00593-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Currie PJ, Khelemsky R, Rigsbee EM, Dono LM, Coiro CD, Chapman CD, et al. Ghrelin is an orexigenic peptide and elicits anxiety-like behaviors following administration into discrete regions of the hypothalamus. Behav Brain Res. 2012;226:96–105. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2011.08.037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Roy V, Chapillon P. Further evidences that risk assessment and object exploration behaviours are useful to evaluate emotional reactivity in rodents. Behav Brain Res. 2004;154:439–48. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2004.03.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.John CS, Currie PJ. N-arachidonoyl-serotonin in the basolateral amygdala increases anxiolytic behavior in the elevated plus maze. Behav Brain Res. 2012;233:382–8. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2012.05.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Adamec RE, Burton P, Shallow T, Budgell J. Unilateral block of NMDA receptors in the amygdala prevents predator stress-induced lasting increases in anxiety-like behavior and unconditioned startle--effective hemisphere depends on the behavior. Physiol Behav. 1999;65:739–51. doi: 10.1016/s0031-9384(98)00225-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Adamec RE, Blundell J, Collins A. Neural plasticity and stress induced changes in defense in the rat. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2001;25:721–44. doi: 10.1016/s0149-7634(01)00053-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Lapiz-Bluhm MD, Bondi CO, Doyen J, Rodriguez GA, Bedard-Arana T, Morilak DA. Behavioural assays to model cognitive and affective dimensions of depression and anxiety in rats. J Neuroendocrinol. 2008;20:1115–37. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2826.2008.01772.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Bishop SJ. Neural mechanisms underlying selective attention to threat. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2008;1129:141–52. doi: 10.1196/annals.1417.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Etkin A. Neurobiology of anxiety: from neural circuits to novel solutions? Depress Anxiety. 2012;29:355–8. doi: 10.1002/da.21957. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Robinson OJ, Charney DR, Overstreet C, Vytal K, Grillon C. The adaptive threat bias in anxiety: amygdala-dorsomedial prefrontal cortex coupling and aversive amplification. Neuroimage. 2012;60:523–9. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.11.096. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Mogg K, Bradley BP, Williams R, Mathews A. Subliminal processing of emotional information in anxiety and depression. J Abnorm Psychol. 1993;102:304–11. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.102.2.304. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Rauch SL, Shin LM, Phelps EA. Neurocircuitry models of posttraumatic stress disorder and extinction: human neuroimaging research--past, present, and future. Biol Psychiatry. 2006;60:376–82. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.06.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Moreira CM, Masson S, Carvalho MC, Brandao ML. Exploratory behaviour of rats in the elevated plus-maze is differentially sensitive to inactivation of the basolateral and central amygdaloid nuclei. Brain Res Bull. 2007;71:466–74. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2006.10.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Fendt M, Fanselow MS. The neuroanatomical and neurochemical basis of conditioned fear. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 1999;23:743–60. doi: 10.1016/s0149-7634(99)00016-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Ganon-Elazar E, Akirav I. Cannabinoids prevent the development of behavioral and endocrine alterations in a rat model of intense stress. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2012;37:456–66. doi: 10.1038/npp.2011.204. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Hill MN, McLaughlin RJ, Morrish AC, Viau V, Floresco SB, Hillard CJ, et al. Suppression of amygdalar endocannabinoid signaling by stress contributes to activation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2009;34:2733–45. doi: 10.1038/npp.2009.114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Etkin A, Prater KE, Schatzberg AF, Menon V, Greicius MD. Disrupted amygdalar subregion functional connectivity and evidence of a compensatory network in generalized anxiety disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2009;66:1361–72. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.McClure EB, Monk CS, Nelson EE, Parrish JM, Adler A, Blair RJ, et al. Abnormal attention modulation of fear circuit function in pediatric generalized anxiety disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2007;64:97–106. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.1.97. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Hayes DJ, Northoff G. Identifying a network of brain regions involved in aversion-related processing: a cross-species translational investigation. Front Integr Neurosci. 2011;5:49. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2011.00049. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Howe DG, Wiley JC, McKnight GS. Molecular and behavioral effects of a null mutation in all PKA C beta isoforms. Mol Cell Neurosci. 2002;20:515–24. doi: 10.1006/mcne.2002.1119. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Liu S, Saloustros E, Mertz EL, Tsang K, Starost MF, Salpea P, et al. Haploinsufficiency for either one of the type-II regulatory subunits of protein kinase A improves the bone phenotype of Prkar1a+/- mice. Hum Mol Genet. 2015 doi: 10.1093/hmg/ddv320. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Uhler MD, McKnight GS. Expression of cDNAs for two isoforms of the catalytic subunit of cAMP-dependent protein kinase. J Biol Chem. 1987;262:15202–7. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Burton KA, Johnson BD, Hausken ZE, Westenbroek RE, Idzerda RL, Scheuer T, et al. Type II regulatory subunits are not required for the anchoring-dependent modulation of Ca2+ channel activity by cAMP-dependent protein kinase. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1997;94:11067–72. doi: 10.1073/pnas.94.20.11067. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Maurice DH, Ke H, Ahmad F, Wang Y, Chung J, Manganiello VC. Advances in targeting cyclic nucleotide phosphodiesterases. Nat Rev Drug Discov. 2014;13:290–314. doi: 10.1038/nrd4228. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Cherry JA, Davis RL. Cyclic AMP phosphodiesterases are localized in regions of the mouse brain associated with reinforcement, movement, and affect. J Comp Neurol. 1999;407:287–301. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Cherry JA, Thompson BE, Pho V. Diazepam and rolipram differentially inhibit cyclic AMP-specific phosphodiesterases PDE4A1 and PDE4B3 in the mouse. Biochim Biophys Acta. 2001;1518:27–35. doi: 10.1016/s0167-4781(01)00164-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Perez-Torres S, Miro X, Palacios JM, Cortes R, Puigdomenech P, Mengod G. Phosphodiesterase type 4 isozymes expression in human brain examined by in situ hybridization histochemistry and[3H]rolipram binding autoradiography. Comparison with monkey and rat brain. J Chem Neuroanat. 2000;20:349–74. doi: 10.1016/s0891-0618(00)00097-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Dlaboga D, Hajjhussein H, O'Donnell JM. Regulation of phosphodiesterase-4 (PDE4) expression in mouse brain by repeated antidepressant treatment: comparison with rolipram. Brain Res. 2006;1096:104–12. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.04.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Keil MF, Lyssikatos C, Shaikh M, Belyavskaya E, Elliott B, Batista D, et al., editors. European Congress of Endocrinology. Wroclaw, Poland: European Society of Endocrinology; 2014. May, Effects of PRKAR1A mutations in behavior and brain function. [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Faria V, Appel L, Ahs F, Linnman C, Pissiota A, Frans O, et al. Amygdala subregions tied to SSRI and placebo response in patients with social anxiety disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2012;37:2222–32. doi: 10.1038/npp.2012.72. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.McNaughton N, Gray JA. Anxiolytic action on the behavioural inhibition system implies multiple types of arousal contribute to anxiety. J Affect Disord. 2000;61:161–76. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0327(00)00344-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Liu B, Feng J, Wang JH. Protein kinase C is essential for kainate-induced anxiety-related behavior and glutamatergic synapse upregulation in prelimbic cortex. CNS neuroscience & therapeutics. 2014;20:982–90. doi: 10.1111/cns.12313. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES