Abstract
Background
The authors tested three possible pathways linking prenatal maternal depressive symptoms to adolescent depressive symptoms. These pathways went through childhood Irritability Symptoms, Anxiety/Depressive Symptoms or Conduct Problems.
Method
Data were collected from 3,963 mother–child pairs participating in the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. Measures include maternal depressive symptoms (pre‐ and postnatal); toddler temperament (2 years); childhood (7–13 years) irritability symptoms, anxiety/depressive symptoms, conduct problems, and adolescent depressive symptoms (16 years).
Results
Irritability Symptoms: This pathway linked sequentially – prenatal maternal depressive symptoms, toddler temperament (high perceived intensity and low perceived adaptability), childhood irritability symptoms, and adolescent depressive symptoms. Anxiety/Depressive symptoms: This pathway linked sequentially – prenatal maternal depressive symptoms, toddler temperament (negative perceived mood), childhood anxiety/depressive symptoms, and adolescent depressive symptoms. Childhood conduct problems were not associated with adolescent depressive symptoms, above and beyond irritability symptoms and anxiety/depressive symptoms.
Conclusions
Results suggest evidence for two distinct developmental pathways to adolescent depressive symptoms that involve specific early and midchildhood features.
Keywords: Psychopathology, oppositional defiant disorder, depression, development, mother–child relationships
Introduction
Depression contributes significantly to the global burden of disease and affects people in all communities with an onset that typically occurs in adolescence (Andrade et al., 2003; Kessler et al., 2005; Patel, Flisher, Hetrick, & McGorry, 2007). Indeed, those with adolescent‐onset depression often go on to have recurrent episodes and significant impairment (e.g. Hammen, Brennan, and Keenan‐Miller, 2008). As a result, research has sought to identify early family risk factors and child characteristics that can predict adolescent depressive symptoms, to enable early identification and mobilize preventative intervention measures that focus on early risk factors (e.g. Garber, 2006).
In this research, we tested for three distinct pathways defined by correlated but distinct child characteristics, linking a common family risk factor — prenatal maternal depressive symptoms — to an equifinal outcome of adolescent depressive symptoms. The first pathway that we tested goes through childhood Irritability Symptoms; the second, through childhood Anxiety/Depressive symptoms; and the third through childhood Conduct Problems.
Oppositional defiance in youth is a highly prevalent psychiatric condition with strong associations with a wide range of adult psychiatric illness, including both emotional (e.g. depression) and externalizing disorders (e.g. conduct disorder, and callous‐unemotional traits) (Angold, Costello, & Erkanli, 1999; Loeber, Green, Keenan, & Lahey, 1995; Maughan, Rowe, Messer, Goodman, & Meltzer, 2004). Partly due to the fact that Oppositional Defiant Disorder (ODD) predicts to such a wide range of adjustment difficulties in children, the DSM 5 (American Psychiatric Association, 2013) has suggested a distinction among irritable, headstrong, and hurtful ODD dimensions, as these dimensions appear to associate with distinct outcomes. Importantly, studies suggest that the ODD subdimension of irritability (i.e. has temper outbursts; touchy or easily annoyed; angry or resentful) predicts adolescent and young adult depressive symptoms (Leibenluft, Cohen, Gorrindo, Brook, & Pine, 2006; Leibenluft & Stoddard, 2013; Stringaris & Goodman, 2009a; Whelan, Stringaris, Maughan, & Barker, 2013). In addition, previous studies show predictive associations between adolescent depressive symptoms and other child characteristics such as anxiety/depressive symptoms (e.g. has many worries or often seems worried; often unhappy, depressed or tearful) and conduct problems (e.g. often fights with other children or bullies them, often lies or cheats) (Barker, Oliver, & Maughan, 2010; Goodman, 2001; Lahey, Loeber, Burke, & Rathouz, 2002; Stringaris, Lewis, & Maughan, 2014). Of importance, irritability symptoms are associated with child anxiety/depressive symptoms and conduct problems (Dougherty et al., 2013; Krieger et al., 2013; Stringaris & Goodman, 2009b) and at present, we cannot be certain whether the association between irritability symptoms and adolescent depressive symptoms is better accounted for by these other more well‐established pathways of anxiety/depressive symptoms and conduct problems.
With regard to early family risk factors, maternal depressive symptoms (pre‐ and postnatal) are robust and well researched risks for offspring depressive symptoms in adolescence (Pawlby, Hay, Sharp, Waters, & O'Keane, 2009; Pearson et al., 2013) and may act as a common antecedent of the three pathways outlined above (i.e. Irritability Symptoms; Anxiety/Depressive Symptoms; and Conduct Problems; Cents et al., 2013; Leis, Heron, Stuart, & Mendelson, 2014; Mars et al., 2012). Moreover, pre‐ and postnatal maternal depressive symptoms are associated with difficult (i.e. negative perceived mood, low perceived adaptability, and high perceived intensity/reactivity) early child temperament (Bruder‐Costello et al., 2007), which in turn, is associated with childhood anxiety/depressive symptoms and conduct problems (Barker & Maughan, 2009). Recently, Stringaris, Maughan, and Goodman (2010), reported that early temperamental dysregulation (emotionality and activity) predicted ODD diagnoses; however, the unique contribution of irritability symptoms was not examined. Yet, as irritability symptoms, anxiety/depressive symptoms, and conduct problems are highly comorbid, they may also share temperamental features.
Understanding whether there is a unique contribution of irritability symptoms toward adolescent depressive symptoms above anxiety/depressive symptoms and conduct problems may help refine risk to outcome associations and evidence‐based interventions. In addition, as little is known about the unique or shared temperamental antecedents of irritability symptoms, anxiety/depressive symptoms, and conduct problems, we explored associations of these child characteristics and toddler temperament (high perceived intensity, low perceived adaptability, and negative perceived mood). The three pathways were tested within an autoregressive cross‐lag model that allows us to test three possible equifinal pathways from the common family risk of maternal depressive symptoms toward adolescent depressive symptoms (e.g. Barker, Jaffee, Uher, & Maughan, 2011).
Methods
Sample
The Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC) was established to understand how genetic and environmental characteristics influence health and development in parents and children. All pregnant women residing in a defined area in the South West of England, with an expected date of delivery between 1st April 1991 and 31st December 1992, were eligible and 13,761 women (contributing 13,867 pregnancies) were recruited. These women have been followed over the last 19–22 years (Fraser et al., 2013). When compared with 1991 National Census Data, the ALSPAC sample was found to be similar to the UK population as a whole (Boyd et al., 2013). Ethical approval for the study was obtained from the ALSPAC Law and Ethics Committee and the Local Research Ethics Committees. More detailed information on ALSPAC is available from the website: http://www.bris.ac.uk/alspac/.
Measures
Mothers completed questionnaires about their impressions of their own – and their children's – psychosocial wellbeing, at multiple time points during pregnancy, and their child's toddlerhood and childhood. The children reported their impressions of their own depressive symptoms at age 16.
Maternal depressive symptoms at 18 and 32 weeks prenatally and in the postnatal period at 8 weeks, 8 months, 21 months were assessed by asking mothers to complete the Edinburgh Postnatal Depression symptoms Scale Questionnaire (EPDS), a widely used 10‐item self‐report questionnaire that has been shown to be valid in and outside the postnatal period (Cox, Holden, & Sagovsky, 1987; Murray & Carothers, 1990). The EPDS is a 10 item self‐report questionnaire of symptoms experienced in the last 7 days and it has been used to identify pregnant women and mothers at risk of depressive symptoms. EPDS is a reliable measure of maternal depressive symptoms: prenatal (α = .78) and postnatal (α = .82).
Toddler temperament measures of Negative Perceived Mood, Low Perceived Adaptability and High Perceived Intensity at 24 months were used. Mothers completed each question using a 6‐point scale response, from ‘almost never’ to ‘almost always’ for each measure which are all Carey Infant Temperament sub‐scales (Carey & McDevitt, 1978). The ‘Negative perceived mood’ subscale is a measure of general tone of affect (i.e. positive or negative). Example items are, ‘he/she is fussy on waking up and going to sleep (frowns, cries)’, ‘he/she cries when left to play alone’. The ‘Low perceived adaptability’ subscale is a measure of responses to novel or altered situations. Examples of items are, ‘he/she resists changes in feeding schedule (1 hr or more) even after two tries, ‘he/she is still wary or frightened of strangers after 15 min’. The ‘High perceived intensity’ subscale is a measure of the level of energy with which an emotional response is made. Examples of items are, ‘he/she displays much feeling (vigorous laughing or crying) during nappy change or dressing, ‘he/she reacts strongly to strangers: laughing or crying’.
Irritability Symptoms at ages 8, 10, and 13 (mother and teacher reports) was derived from the Development and Well‐Being Assessment (DAWBA), a well‐validated measure, developed for the British Child Mental Health surveys (Meltzer, Gatward, Goodman, & Ford, 2000). In addition to generating binary (yes/no) diagnostic indicators, DAWBA algorithms have been developed to generate six‐level ordered‐categorical measures of the probability of disorder for each of the individual items underlying the diagnoses, ranging from <0.1% to >70% (Goodman, Heiervang, Collishaw, & Goodman, 2011). Evaluated in two large‐scale national samples, these DAWBA ‘bands’ functioned well as ordered‐categorical measures, showed dose–response associations with mental health service contacts, and showed very similar associations with potential risk factors as clinician‐rated diagnoses (Goodman et al., 2011).
The DAWBA asks 9 separate symptoms of ODD. Each question was introduced with the stem: ‘over the last 6 months, and as compared with other children the same age, has s/he often….’ followed by the specific clause. Following the lead of Stringaris and Goodman (2009a), and subsequently the DSM‐5 (American Psychiatric Association, 2013), irritability symptoms were defined by the following three symptoms: ‘has temper outbursts’, ‘has been touchy or easily annoyed’, and ‘has been angry or resentful’ where age 8–13 years (α = .71). Anxiety/Depressive symptoms at ages 7, 10, and 12 years were measured by mother reports on the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire, a widely used screening instrument with well‐established reliability and validity (Goodman, 1997, 2001; Van Widenfelt, Goedhart, Treffers, & Goodman, 2003) and anxiety/depressive symptoms has the following five items: ‘often complains of headaches, stomach aches or sickness’, ‘has many worries or often seems worried’, ‘often unhappy, depressed or tearful’,’is nervous or clingy in new situations, easily loses confidence’ and ‘has many fears, is easily scared’. Items were coded as a 3‐point scale (‘not true’, ‘somewhat true’, and ‘certainly true’) where age 7–12 years (α = .71). It should be noted that at age 7 the item ‘has many fears, is easily scared’ was not available in the dataset, but was included at ages 10 and 12.
Conduct Problems at ages 7, 10 and 12 years were measured by mother reports on the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire, a widely used screening instrument with well‐established reliability and validity (Goodman, 1997, 2001), with the following four items: ‘is generally obedient, usually does what adults request’ (reverse coded), ‘often fights with other children or bullies them’, ‘often lies or cheats, ‘steals from home, school, or elsewhere’. Items were coded as a 3‐ point scale (‘not true,’ ‘somewhat true,’ and ‘certainly true’) where age 7–12 years (α = .72). It should be noted that SDQ was found to be at least as efficient at detecting externalizing problems as the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL; Goodman & Scott, 1999); and associates with ICD‐10 diagnoses of CD and ODD (Goodman, Renfrew, & Mullick, 2000). However, the temper outburst item that is typically the final measure for the SDQ for conduct problems was removed in this study to avoid item overlap between this item and the irritability temper tantrum/outburst item.
Depressive symptoms at age 16 were derived from the adolescent‐reported Mood and Feelings Questionnaire Short Form (SMFQ) (Messer et al., 1995). The SMFQ is a 13‐item self‐report questionnaire of symptoms experienced in the previous 2 weeks (that codes symptoms on a 3‐point scale: ‘true’, ‘sometimes true’, ‘not true’) with a range of 0–26 (α = .91). This scale has been found to have high reliability and validity, and the short form is made up of items that best discriminated depressed and nondepressed children in field trials using structured psychiatric interviews (Costello & Angold, 1988).
Attrition and missing data
Participants with data for depressive symptoms at 16 years were selected for the analysis (n = 3,963). In a multiple regression model, we tested the extent to which risk factors common to irritability symptoms, anxiety/depressive symptoms, and conduct problems (see Tremblay, 2010) associated with noninclusion in this study. Partner status (OR = 2.45; 95% CI = 2.00–2.99), low SES (OR = 1.68; 95% CI = 1.47–1.91), teen pregnancy (OR = 2.81; 95% CI = 2.53–3.12), and maternal education (OR = 2.57; 95% CI = 2.53–3.12). We note that inclusion of these variables in the analysis – in conjunction with missing data replacement by full‐information maximum likelihood – can help to minimize bias and maximize recoverability of ‘true’ scores (Little & Rubin, 2002).
Analysis
Using an autoregressive cross‐lag model (ARCL), we tested three possible equifinal pathways from the common family risk of maternal depressive symptoms toward adolescent depressive symptoms (Figure 1). In this modeling approach, each variable in the model is regressed on all of the variables that precede it in time in order to examine developmental continuity and inter‐relationships across the three hypothesized pathways. The Irritability Symptoms pathway predicts associations among pre‐and postnatal maternal depressive symptoms, temperament, irritability symptoms and adolescent depressive symptoms; the Anxiety/Depressive Symptoms pathway predicts associations among pre‐ and postnatal maternal depressive symptoms, temperament, anxiety/depressive symptoms and adolescent depressive symptoms; the Conduct Problems pathway predicts associations between pre‐ and postnatal maternal depressive symptoms, temperament, conduct problems and adolescent depressive symptoms.
Results
Descriptive statistics
All study variables were significantly positively correlated (Table 1). For example, negative perceived mood, low perceived adaptability and high perceived intensity were all highly correlated. Moreover, childhood irritability symptoms, anxiety/depressive symptoms and conduct problems were highly correlated. Adolescent depressive symptoms were significantly associated with maternal depressive symptoms, toddler temperaments and child irritability symptoms, anxiety/depressive symptoms and conduct problems. We note that the highest correlation was between pre‐ and postnatal maternal depressive symptoms (r = .64), which indicated that these measures shared 41% common variance (i.e. 0.64*0.64 = 0.41).
Table 1.
1. | 2. | 3. | 4. | 5. | 6. | 7. | 8. | 9. | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Prenatal Matdep | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – |
Postnatal Matdep | 0.64a | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – |
Mood | 0.21a | 0.22a | – | – | – | – | – | – | – |
Adapt | 0.17a | 0.18a | 0.58a | – | – | – | – | – | – |
Intens | 0.14a | 0.15a | 0.37a | 0.40a | – | – | – | – | – |
A/D | 0.05a | 0.06a | 0.24a | 0.17a | 0.12a | – | – | – | – |
Irrit | 0.05a | 0.05a | 0.16a | 0.20a | 0.18a | 0.49a | – | – | – |
CP | 0.05a | 0.06a | 0.18a | 0.25a | 0.17a | 0.35a | 0.61a | – | – |
Dep16 | 0.09a | 0.08a | 0.06a | 0.05a | 0.04a | 0.19a | 0.16a | 0.08a | – |
p < .01.
Matdep prenatal = Prenatal maternal depressive symptoms; Matdep postnatal = Postnatal maternal depressive symptoms; Mood = Negative perceived mood; Adapt = Low perceived adaptability; Intens = High perceived intensity; Irrit = Irritability symptoms; A/D = anxiety/depressive symptoms; CP= Conduct problems; Dep16 = Adolescent depressive symptoms.
Examining three pathways to adolescent depressive symptoms
The ARCL model showed acceptable fit on three indices (χ 2 (121) = 471.978, p < .001; CFI: 0.964; TLI: 0.953; RMSEA: 0.027). R 2 values (the proportion of variance explained based on each variable's predictors) are reported for adolescent depressive symptoms (R 2 = .06); irritability (R 2 = .08); anxiety/depressive symptoms (R 2 = .12) and conduct problems (R 2 = .09).
Figure 1 shows the significant path coefficients in the ARCL model. To begin with, we note that prenatal maternal depressive symptoms were associated with postnatal maternal depressive symptoms (β = .64). In addition, we highlight four main results. First, for the Irritability Symptoms pathway: postnatal maternal depressive symptoms was associated with low perceived adaptability (β = .11) and high perceived intensity (β = .11) in toddlerhood, which were associated with irritability symptoms at 8–13 years (β = .12; β = .11; respectively), which, in turn, was associated with adolescent depressive symptoms (β = .11). Second, for the Anxiety/Depressive Symptoms pathway: postnatal maternal depressive symptoms were associated with toddler negative perceived mood (β = .15), which was associated with anxiety/depressive symptoms at 7–12 years (β = .16), which, in turn, was associated with adolescent depressive symptoms (β = .15). Third, for the Conduct Problems pathway: postnatal maternal depressive symptoms were associated with low perceived adaptability (β = .11) and high perceived intensity (β = .11), which were associated with conduct problems at 7–12 years (β = .19; β = .06; respectively). Conduct problems at 7–12 years did not predict adolescent depressive symptoms, above and beyond the other variables in the ARCL model.
Fourth, in addition, direct associations were found between prenatal depressive symptoms and anxiety/depressive symptoms at 7–12 years and depressive symptoms at 16 years (β = .11; β = .05, respectively). Direct associations were also found between postnatal depressive symptoms and irritability (β = .14); anxiety/depressive symptoms (β = .17); and conduct problems (β = .12).
Discussion
The present epidemiological study examined three distinct pathways linking a common antecedent – maternal depressive symptoms – to a shared equifinal outcome of adolescent depressive symptoms. The overall findings provide evidence for two distinct co‐occurring pathways from maternal to adolescent depressive symptoms: an Irritability Symptoms pathway and an Anxiety/Depressive Symptoms pathway; however a third Conduct Problems pathway was not found.
With regard to irritability symptoms (i.e. temper outbursts, being easily annoyed and angry or resentful), unlike previous studies (e.g. Stringaris et al., 2010; Whelan et al., 2013), this study examined irritability symptoms while controlling for co‐occurring anxiety/depressive symptoms and conduct problems. Study findings delineate the specific contribution of childhood irritability symptoms to adolescent depressive symptoms, alongside its earlier temperamental features. More specifically, we found a pathway from pre‐ and postnatal maternal depressive symptoms, to temperamental low perceived adaptability and high perceived intensity in toddlerhood, to childhood irritability symptoms, and ultimately to adolescent depressive symptoms. With regard to temperamental low perceived adaptability, studies have found that irritable children perform poorly in tasks of cognitive flexibility thereby demonstrating deficits (Adleman et al., 2011; Leibenluft, 2011) – that is, low perceived adaptability. It may therefore be that irritable children display early temperamental signs of cognitive and behavioral inflexibility (i.e. low perceived adaptability) which manifest as prodromal signs of irritability symptoms, which, if recognized early, may offer a treatment target. In addition, the association that we found between toddler high perceived intensity and childhood irritability symptoms may be explained by the fact that biological systems relevant to the regulation of arousal are functionally immature during pregnancy and birth and mature gradually during the toddler years (Glover, 2011); postnatal maternal depressive symptoms may impact adversely upon the development of these systems. During infancy, the child is dependent on parenting (Jaffee, 2007) to support the achievement of developmental milestones such as cognitive maturation and early social and emotional competence (Shonkoff, Boyce, & McEwen, 2009). However, the presence of depressive symptoms may compromise a mother's ability to provide the sensitive care needed to foster the development of the toddler's self‐regulatory capabilities (Barker, 2013; Feldman et al., 2009; Goodman & Gotlib, 1999).
Childhood anxiety/depressive symptoms – worrying, being unhappy and tearful and fearful –were found to uniquely contribute to adolescent depressive symptoms above and beyond childhood irritability symptoms and conduct problems, thereby providing a second pathway to depressive symptoms at 16 years. This second pathway linked pre‐ and postnatal maternal depressive symptoms with toddler temperamental negative perceived mood, childhood anxiety/depressive symptoms, and adolescent depressive symptoms. The associations between pre‐ and postnatal maternal depressive symptoms and child anxiety/depressive symptoms may be explained by two distinct but related processes. With regard to prenatal depressive symptoms, our findings appear congruent with research suggesting that depressive symptoms can lead to an intra‐uterine environment not conducive to healthy fetal development (Weinstock, 2008), thereby increasing risk for abnormal child development, including –but not specific to – childhood anxiety/depressive symptoms (Barker et al., 2011; Glover, 2011). Additionally, as noted above with respect to irritability symptoms, postnatal depressive symptoms may negatively alter a mother's ability to provide attentive and sensitive care needed to foster the development of the toddler's self‐regulatory capabilities (Barker, 2013; Feldman et al., 2009; Goodman & Gotlib, 1999). This in turn could increase the risk of a toddler developing temperamental negative perceived mood and childhood anxiety/depressive symptoms, ultimately increasing the risk for the onset of adolescent depressive symptoms. Future research may want to examine more closely how specific symptoms of postnatal maternal depressive symptoms (e.g. as measured by the EPDS: anxious or worried vs. low laughter, humor) may align more as a risk for anxiety/depressive symptoms or irritability.
Third, conduct problems were not found to associate with adolescent depressive symptoms when childhood irritability symptoms and anxiety/depressive symptoms were controlled. In this study, at the bivariate level, the correlation between conduct problems and depressive symptoms was significant, albeit half the magnitude of the association between irritability symptoms and adolescent depressive symptoms. However, in the autoregressive cross‐lagged model, this association became nonsignificant. Possibly, conduct problems associate with adolescent depressive symptoms (e.g. Barker et al., 2010; Lahey et al., 2002) via irritability symptoms. Indeed, a recent study (Stringaris et al., 2014) highlighted that irritability symptoms shared genetic associations with childhood depressive symptoms and conduct problem symptoms.
Strengths of this study include large sample size, longitudinal focus, and inclusion of cross‐informant predictions (i.e. mother and teacher reports of risks, child reports of adolescent depressive symptoms). However, the present results should be interpreted in light of a number of limitations. First, this study, as with previous studies, is correlational and not causative. Second, it should be noted that we relied on self‐reports of mothers on a range of study variables including their own depressive symptoms and the child temperaments and irritability, anxiety/depressive symptoms and conduct problems. An important limitation is that the study almost exclusively (exception being adolescent depressive symptoms) relied on mothers’ impressions of their own – and their child's – psychosocial wellbeing (e.g. hence ‘perceived’ temperament) Indeed, although studies suggest that depressed mothers can be as accurate as other informants about their children's behavior (Richters, 1992), ALSPAC does not have the capability of confirming or disconfirming potential bias associated with maternal depressive symptoms by comparing mother reports of their children to independent, validated criterion raters. Third, we do not have information on whether mothers in this study received treatment for depression. As treatment induced reduction in maternal depressive symptoms associates in improved adjustment in offspring (Pilowsky et al., 2008; Weissman et al., 2006), the present results may underestimate the effect of maternal depression on child wellbeing. Fourth, younger and more socially disadvantaged mothers were more likely to be lost to follow‐up. As these predictors of attrition also predict child psychopathology, our sample is likely to under‐represent the most severely affected children. Of note, an ALSPAC cohort study (Wolke et al., 2009) has shown that attrition affects the prevalence of DSM‐IV disruptive behavior disorders (which includes ODD), however, associations between risks and outcomes remained present, although conservative of the likely true effects. Fifth, although the ALSPAC sample represents a broad spectrum of SES backgrounds, it includes relatively low rates of ethnic minorities. The present results will need replication with more ethnically diverse and high risk samples. Sixth, this study did not test for indirect effects between study variables and as such future studies may wish to examine indirect effects. If indirect effects were found they would suggest that successful intervention on maternal depressive symptoms could lead to lower adolescent depressive symptoms through higher perceived toddler temperamental adaptability and lower perceived intensity, and lower irritability symptoms.
In conclusion, we found that irritability symptoms contributed independently to adolescent depressive symptoms when co‐occurring anxiety/depressive symptoms and conduct problems were controlled. Moreover, common risk factors of pre‐ and postnatal maternal depressive symptoms were associated with two equifinal pathways to adolescent depressive symptoms, based on temperamental features and child characteristics. First, we found, an Irritability Symptoms pathway linked with toddler temperamental low perceived adaptability and high perceived intensity. Second, we also found an Anxiety/Depressive Symptoms pathway linked with toddler temperamental negative perceived mood. Thus, this study supports the existence of distinct developmental pathways to adolescent depressive symptoms while pinpointing important targets and windows of opportunity for prevention. We suggest that interventions addressing childhood irritability symptoms, as well as maternal depressive symptoms, toddler temperamental low perceived adaptability and high perceived intensity; and those that target childhood anxiety/depressive symptoms alongside toddler temperamental negative perceived mood may be the most efficient manner to prevent the onset of adolescent depressive symptoms.
Key points.
Studies suggest that the ODD subdimension of irritability prospectively associates with adolescent and young adult depressive symptoms
This study supports the existence of a distinct Irritability Symptoms developmental pathway to adolescent depressive symptoms above and beyond co‐occurring anxiety/depressive symptoms and conduct problems.
Common risk factors – maternal depressive symptoms were associated with two equifinal pathways to adolescent depressive symptoms, based on temperamental features and child characteristics. An Irritability Symptoms pathway linked with toddler temperamental low adaptability and high intensity; and an Anxiety/Depressive Symptoms pathway linked with toddler temperamental negative mood.
We suggest that interventions addressing childhood irritability symptoms, as well as childhood anxiety/depressive symptoms alongside toddler temperamental negative mood may be the most efficient manner to prevent the onset of adolescent depressive symptoms.
Acknowledgements
The authors are extremely grateful to all the families who took part in this study, the midwives for their help in recruiting them, and the whole ALSPAC team, which includes interviewers, computer and laboratory technicians, clerical workers, research scientists, volunteers, managers, receptionists, and nurses. The UK Medical Research Council (grant ref: 74883), the Wellcome Trust (grant ref: 0754567) and the University of Bristol provide core support for ALSPAC.
Research reported in this publication was supported by the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health & Human Development of the National Institutes of Health under Award Number R01HD068437 to E. D. B; Y. M. W. is supported by an ESRC studentship (ES/J500021/1 W85058B). E. L. is supported by the National Institute of Mental Health Division of Intramural Research Programs; A. S. is funded by the Wellcome Trust, the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) and the Department of Health UK and receives royalties from Cambridge University Press for his book The Maudsley Reader in Phenomenological Psychiatry. All authors declare they have no potential or competing conflicts of interest with relation to this work.
Y.M.W. had full access to all of the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health.
Conflicts of interest statement: See Acknowledgements for disclosures.
Footnotes
Correction note: This article was first published online on the 9th of February 2015, under a subscription publication license. The article has since been made OnlineOpen, and the copyright line and license statement was therefore updated in May 2015.
References
- Adleman, N.E. , Kayser, R. , Dickstein, D. , Blair, R.J.R. , Pine, D. , & Leibenluft, E. (2011). Neural correlates of reversal learning in severe mood dysregulation and pediatric bipolar disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 50, 1173–1185. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th edn). Washington, DC: Author. [Google Scholar]
- Andrade, L. , Caraveo‐anduaga, J.J. , Berglund, P. , Bijl, R.V. , Graaf, R.D. , Vollebergh, W. , … & Wittchen, H.U. (2003). The epidemiology of major depressive episodes: Results from the International Consortium of Psychiatric Epidemiology (ICPE) Surveys. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 12, 3–21. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Angold, A. , Costello, E.J. , & Erkanli, A. (1999). Comorbidity. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40, 57–87. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barker, E.D. (2013). The duration and timing of maternal depression symptoms as a moderator of the relationship between dependent interpersonal stress, contextual risk and early child dysregulation. Psychological Medicine, 43, 1587–1596. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barker, E.D. , Jaffee, S.R. , Uher, R. , & Maughan, B. (2011). The contribution of prenatal and postnatal maternal anxiety and depression symptoms to child maladjustment. Depression and Anxiety, 28, 696–702. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barker, E.D. , & Maughan, B. (2009). Differentiating early‐onset persistent versus childhood‐limited conduct problem youth. American Journal of Psychiatry, 166, 900–908. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barker, E.D. , Oliver, B.R. , & Maughan, B. (2010). Co‐occurring problems of early onset persistent, childhood limited, and adolescent onset conduct problem youth. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51, 1217–1226. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boyd, A. , Golding, J. , Macleod, J. , Lawlor, D.A. , Fraser, A. , Henderson, J. , … & Smith, G.D. (2013). Cohort profile: The ‘Children of the 90s’—the index offspring of the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children. International Journal of Epidemiology, 42, 111–127. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bruder‐Costello, B. , Warner, V. , Talati, A. , Nomura, Y. , Bruder, G. , & Weissman, M. (2007). Temperament among offspring at high and low risk for depression symptoms. Psychiatry Research, 153, 145–151. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carey, W.B. , & McDevitt, S.C. (1978). Revision of the infant temperament questionnaire. Pediatrics, 61, 735–739. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cents, R.A.M. , Diamantopoulou, S. , Hudziak, J.J. , Jaddoe, V.W.V. , Hofman, A. , Verhulst, F.C. , … & Tiemeier, H. (2013). Trajectories of maternal depressive symptoms predict child problem behaviour: The Generation R Study. Psychological Medicine, 43, 13–25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd edn). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. [Google Scholar]
- Costello, E.J. , & Angold, A. (1988). Scales to assess child and adolescent depression symptoms: Checklists, screens, and nets. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 27, 726–737. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cox, J.L. , Holden, J.M. , & Sagovsky, R. (1987). Detection of postnatal depression. Development of the 10‐item Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 150, 782–786. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dougherty, L.R. , Smith, V.C. , Bufferd, S.J. , Stringaris, A. , Leibenluft, E. , Carlson, G.A. , & Klein, D.N. (2013). Preschool irritability: Longitudinal associations with psychiatric disorders at age 6 and parental psychopathology. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 52, 1304–1313. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Feldman, R. , Granat, A. , Pariente, C. , Kanety, H. , Kuint, J. , & Gilboa‐Schechtman, E. (2009). Maternal depression symptoms and anxiety across the postpartum year and infant social engagement, fear regulation, and stress reactivity. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 48, 919–927. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fraser, A. , Macdonald‐Wallis, C. , Tilling, K. , Boyd, A. , Golding, J. , Smith, G.D. , … & Lawlor, D.A. (2013). Cohort profile: The Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children: ALSPAC mothers cohort. International Journal of Epidemiology, 42, 97–110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Garber, J. (2006). Depression symptoms in children and adolescents: Linking risk research and prevention. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 31, 104–125. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Glover, V. (2011). Annual research review: Prenatal stress and the origins of psychopathology: An evolutionary perspective. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52, 356–367. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goodman, R. (1997). The strengths and difficulties questionnaire: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38, 581–586. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goodman, R. (2001). Psychometric properties of the strengths and difficulties questionnaire. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 40, 1337–1345. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goodman, S.H. , & Gotlib, I.H. (1999). Risk for psychopathology in the children of depressed mothers: A developmental model for understanding mechanisms of transmission. Psychological Review, 106, 458. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goodman, A. , Heiervang, E. , Collishaw, S. , & Goodman, R. (2011). The “DAWBA bands” as an ordered‐categorical measure of child mental health: Description and validation in British and Norwegian samples. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 46, 521–532. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goodman, R. , Renfrew, D. , & Mullick, M. (2000). Predicting type of psychiatric disorder from Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) scores in child mental health clinics in London and Dhaka. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 9, 129–134. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goodman, R. , & Scott, S. (1999). Comparing the strengths and difficulties questionnaire and the child behavior checklist: Is small beautiful? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 27, 17–24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hammen, C. , Brennan, P.A. , & Keenan‐Miller, D. (2008). Patterns of adolescent depression to age 20: The role of maternal depression and youth interpersonal dysfunction. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36, 1189–1198. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jaffee, S.R. (2007). Sensitive, stimulating caregiving predicts cognitive and behavioral resilience in neurodevelopmentally at‐risk infants. Development and Psychopathology, 19, 631–647. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kessler, R.C. , Berglund, P. , Demler, O. , Jin, R. , Merikangas, K.R. , & Walters, E.E. (2005). Lifetime prevalence and age‐of‐onset distributions of DSM‐IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62, 593–602. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krieger, F.V. , Polanczyk, G.V. , Goodman, R. , Rohde, L.A. , Graeff‐Martins, A.S. , Salum, G. , … & Stringaris, A. (2013). Dimensions of oppositionality in a brazilian community sample: Testing the DSM‐5 proposal and etiological links. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 52, 389–400. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lahey, B.B. , Loeber, R. , Burke, J. , & Rathouz, P.J. (2002). Adolescent outcomes of childhood conduct disorder among clinic‐referred boys: Predictors of improvement. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 30, 333–348. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leibenluft, E. (2011). Severe mood dysregulation, irritability, and the diagnostic boundaries of bipolar disorder in youths. American Journal of Psychiatry, 168, 129–142. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leibenluft, E. , Cohen, P. , Gorrindo, T. , Brook, J.S. , & Pine, D.S. (2006). Chronic versus episodic irritability in youth: A community‐based, longitudinal study of clinical and diagnostic associations. Journal of Child & Adolescent Psychopharmacology, 16, 456–466. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leibenluft, E. , & Stoddard, J. (2013). The developmental psychopathology of irritability. Development and Psycho‐pathology, 25, 1473–1487. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Leis, J.A. , Heron, J. , Stuart, E.A. , & Mendelson, T. (2014). Associations between maternal mental health and child emotional and behavioral problems: Does prenatal mental health matter? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 42, 161–171. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Little, R.J. , & Rubin, D.B. (2002). Statistical analysis with missing data. Hoboken, NJ: J Wiley & Sons. [Google Scholar]
- Loeber, R. , Green, S.M. , Keenan, K. , & Lahey, B.B. (1995). Which boys will fare worse? Early predictors of the onset of conduct disorder in a six‐year longitudinal study. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 34, 499–509. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mars, B. , Collishaw, S. , Smith, D. , Thapar, A. , Potter, R. , Sellers, R. , … & Thapar, A. (2012). Offspring of parents with recurrent depression symptoms: Which features of parent depression symptoms index risk for offspring psychopathology? Journal of Affective Disorders, 136, 44–53. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maughan, B. , Rowe, R. , Messer, J. , Goodman, R. , & Meltzer, H. (2004). Conduct disorder and oppositional defiant disorder in a national sample: Developmental epidemiology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 609–621. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meltzer, H. , Gatward, R. , Goodman, R. , & Ford, T. (2000). Mental health of children and adolescents in Great Britain. London: TSO. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Messer, S.C. , Angold, A. , Costello, E.J. , Loeber, R. , Van Kammen, W. , & Stouthamer‐Loeber, M. (1995). Development of a short questionnaire for use in epidemiological studies of depression symptoms in children and adolescents: Factor composition and structure across development. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 5, 251–262. [Google Scholar]
- Murray, L. , & Carothers, A.D. (1990). The validation of the Edinburgh Post‐natal Depression Scale on a community sample. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 157, 288–290. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Patel, V. , Flisher, A.J. , Hetrick, S. , & McGorry, P. (2007). Mental health of young people: A global public‐health challenge. The Lancet, 369, 1302–1313. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pawlby, S. , Hay, D.F. , Sharp, D. , Waters, C.S. , & O'Keane, V. (2009). Antenatal depression symptoms predicts depression symptoms in adolescent offspring: Prospective longitudinal community‐based study. Journal of Affective Disorders, 113, 236–243. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pearson, R.M. , Evans, J. , Kounali, D. , Lewis, G. , Heron, J. , Ramchandani, P.G. , … & Stein, A. (2013). Maternal depression symptoms during pregnancy and the postnatal period: Risks and possible mechanisms for offspring depression symptoms at age 18 years. JAMA Psychiatry, 70, 1312–1319. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pilowsky, D. , Wickramaratne, P. , Talati, A. , Tang, M. , Hughes, C. , Garber, J. , … & Weissman, M. (2008). Children of depressed mothers 1 year after the initiation of maternal treatment: Findings from the STAR* D‐Child Study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 165, 1136–1147. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Richters, J.E. (1992). Depressed mothers as informants about their children: A critical review of the evidence for distortion. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 485. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shonkoff, J.P. , Boyce, W.T. , & McEwen, B.S. (2009). Neuroscience, molecular biology, and the childhood roots of health disparities: Building a new framework for health promotion and disease prevention. JAMA, 301, 2252–2259. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stringaris, A. , & Goodman, R. (2009a). Longitudinal outcome of youth oppositionality: Irritable, headstrong, and hurtful behaviors have distinctive predictions. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 48, 404–412. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stringaris, A. , & Goodman, R. (2009b). Three dimensions of oppositionality in youth. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50, 216–223. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stringaris, A. , Lewis, G. , & Maughan, B. (2014). Developmental pathways from childhood conduct problems to early adult depression: Findings from the ALSPAC cohort. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 205, 17–23. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stringaris, A. , Maughan, B. , & Goodman, R. (2010). What's in a disruptive disorder? Temperamental antecedents of oppositional defiant disorder: Findings from the Avon longitudinal study. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 49, 474–483. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tremblay, R.E. (2010). Developmental origins of disruptive behaviour problems: The ‘original sin’hypothesis, epigenetics and their consequences for prevention. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51, 341–367. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Van Widenfelt, B.M. , Goedhart, A.W. , Treffers, P.D. , & Goodman, R. (2003). Dutch version of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ). European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 12, 281–289. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weinstock, M. (2008). The long‐term behavioural consequences of prenatal stress. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 32, 1073–1086. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weissman, M.M. , Pilowsky, D.J. , Wickramaratne, P.J. , Talati, A. , Wisniewski, S.R. , Fava, M. , … & Rush, A.J. (2006). Remissions in maternal depression and child psychopathology: A STAR* D‐child report. JAMA, 295, 1389–1398. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whelan, Y.M. , Stringaris, A. , Maughan, B. , & Barker, E.D. (2013). Developmental continuity of oppositional defiant disorder subdimensions at ages 8, 10, and 13 Years and their distinct psychiatric outcomes at age 16 years. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 52, 961–969. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolke, D. , Waylen, A. , Samara, M. , Steer, C. , Goodman, R. , Ford, T. , & Lamberts, K. (2009). Selective drop‐out in longitudinal studies and non‐biased prediction of behaviour disorders. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 195, 249–256. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]