Skip to main content
Wiley Open Access Collection logoLink to Wiley Open Access Collection
. 2015 Oct 21;31(6):666–675. doi: 10.1002/gps.4377

The Test Your Memory cognitive screening tool: sociodemographic and cardiometabolic risk correlates in a population‐based study of older British men

Efstathios Papachristou 1,, Sheena E Ramsay 1, Olia Papacosta 1, Lucy T Lennon 1, Steve Iliffe 1, Peter H Whincup 2, S Goya Wannamethee 1
PMCID: PMC4855642  PMID: 26489874

Abstract

Objective

This study aimed to examine the association of Test Your Memory (TYM)‐defined cognitive impairment groups with known sociodemographic and cardiometabolic correlates of cognitive impairment in a population‐based study of older adults.

Methods

Participants were members of the British Regional Heart Study, a cohort across 24 British towns initiated in 1978–1980. Data stemmed from 1570 British men examined in 2010–2012, aged 71–92 years. Sociodemographic and cardiometabolic factors were compared between participants defined as having TYM scores in the normal cognitive ageing, mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and severe cognitive impairment (SCI) groups, defined as ≥46 (45 if ≥80 years of age), ≥33 and <33, respectively.

Results

Among 1570 men, 636 (41%) were classified in the MCI and 133 (8%) in the SCI groups. Compared with participants in the normal cognitive ageing category, individuals with SCI were characterized primarily by lower socio‐economic position (odds ratio (OR) = 6.15, 95% confidence interval (CI) 4.00–9.46), slower average walking speed (OR = 3.36, 95% CI 2.21–5.10), mobility problems (OR = 4.61, 95% CI 3.04–6.97), poorer self‐reported overall health (OR = 2.63, 95% CI 1.79–3.87), obesity (OR = 2.59, 95% CI 1.72–3.91) and impaired lung function (OR = 2.25, 95% CI 1.47–3.45). A similar albeit slightly weaker pattern was observed for participants with MCI.

Conclusion

Sociodemographic and lifestyle factors as well as adiposity measures, lung function and poor overall health are associated with cognitive impairments in late life. The correlates of cognitive abilities in the MCI and SCI groups, as defined by the TYM, resemble the risk profile for MCI and Alzheimer's disease outlined in current epidemiological models. © 2016 The Authors. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry Published by John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.

Keywords: cognition, TYM, ageing, cognitive impairments

Introduction

Current global estimates suggest that 25–35 million people are currently affected by severe cognitive impairments (SCI) (Qiu et al., 2009; World Health Organization, 2012), the most common form being Alzheimer's disease (ad) (Plassman et al., 2007). Established correlates for AD or even milder declines in cognitive abilities in late life include low physical activity (Winchester et al., 2013), impaired motor (Mirelman et al., 2014) and lung function (Bozek and Jarzab, 2011), and smoking (Anstey et al., 2007; Peters et al., 2008) as well as a positive history of cardiovascular diseases (CVD) (de la Torre, 2004) and/or diabetes (Tolppanen et al., 2013). Patients with cognitive impairments are also more likely to report poorer overall health (Montlahuc et al., 2011), sleep disturbances (Ownby et al., 2014) and higher levels of functional dependence (Aguero‐Torres et al., 1998). However, it remains unclear whether AD is associated with late‐life hypertension (Power et al., 2011), hypercholesterolaemia (Polidori et al., 2012) and obesity (Anstey et al., 2011).

The past four decades have seen growing interest in developing easy‐to‐administer cognitive screening tools with clinical utility for primary and secondary care settings (Larner, 2013; Zygouris and Tsolaki, 2014). One of the most promising tools is the Test Your Memory (TYM) cognitive test (Brown et al., 2009), which has sound psychometric properties (Brown et al., 2009; Hancock and Larner, 2011), remarkable cross‐cultural validity (Hanyu et al., 2011; Abd‐Al‐Atty et al., 2012; Szczesniak et al., 2013; Ferrero‐Arias and Turrion‐Rojo, 2014; Iatraki et al., 2014; Muñoz‐Neira et al., 2014; Postel‐Vinay et al., 2014) and good concurrent validity with established tests, such as the Mini‐mental state examination, and the Addenbrooke's cognitive examination test battery (Brown et al., 2009; van Schalkwyk et al., 2012; Koekkoek et al., 2013). However, studies using the TYM were conducted among small clinical samples and did not report on the sociodemographic and cardiometabolic correlates of TYM‐defined cognitive groups.

In this study, we investigated whether the established risk pattern for late‐life mild cognitive impairments (MCI) and SCI is the same in a general population sample of British men aged 71–92 years assessed with the TYM. In particular, we sought (a) to characterize the cognitive profiles defined by the TYM in terms of their sociodemographic and cardiometabolic correlates and (b) to examine whether the sociodemographic and cardiometabolic profile of TYM‐defined cognitive impairments is similar to the ones reported in studies employing different classification criteria to define groups with MCI or SCI. Data included sociodemographic, lifestyle and overall health characteristics, health service use, adiposity measures, cardiovascular risk factors or diseases, diabetes and blood markers. It was hypothesized that these risk markers would be significantly associated with differing abilities of cognitive functioning as defined by the TYM, in line with the epidemiological models proposed for patients with MCI and SCI.

Methods

The British Regional Heart Study (BRHS) is a prospective study including a socially and geographically representative sample of 7735 men aged 40–59 years recruited from 24 towns representative of all major British regions. The BRHS commenced in 1978–1980 and did not cover the study of women (Walker et al., 2004). In 2010–2012, all surviving men (n = 3137) aged 71–92 years were invited to attend a 30‐year re‐examination (Figure 1). It was attended by 1722 BRHS participants (55% response rate). Ethical approval was provided by all relevant local research ethics committees. All men provided written informed consent to the investigations, which were carried out in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Recruitment and retention flow chart for the 30‐year replication of the British Regional Heart Study (2010–2012).

Physical examination of subjects involved anthropometric (waist circumference) and physiological (blood pressure, forced expiratory volume (FEV1) in 1 s) measurements as well as the calculation of body mass index (BMI). Physical performance assessments included a walking test, a chair rise test (time taken, in seconds, to sit down and stand up five times from a chair with the arms folded across the chest) and grip strength. The details of these assessments have been described elsewhere (Ramsay et al., 2014).

Cognitive skills were assessed using the TYM (Brown et al., 2009). The TYM is a 10‐task self‐assessment test that covers a broad range of cognitive domains including orientation, copying, semantic knowledge, calculation, fluency, similarities, naming, visuospatial abilities, anterograde memory and executive functioning (Brown et al., 2009). The last item is scored according to whether the TYM was completed with help from others or not (major = 1 to none = 5). Because participants completed the TYM in a controlled setting without assistance, the maximum score of 5 was given to all participants for this item. Total TYM scores range between 0 and 50 with higher scores indicating superior cognitive performance. Upon calculating the total TYM scores, participants were divided into three categories: a normal cognitive ageing group, a group with MCI and one with SCI. Cut‐offs for the respective categories were based on the original TYM scores (Brown et al., 2009; Royal College of Psychiatrists (RPsych)). Specifically, scores below 33 were considered consistent with SCI, while scores between 33 and 45 (if older than 80 years of age) or 46 (if younger than 80 years of age) were considered to be indicative of MCI.

Socio‐economic position was defined based on the longest‐held occupation of subjects at study entry (aged 40–59 years) in accordance to the Registrar Generals' Social Class Classification and has been described elsewhere (Wannamethee et al., 1996).

Blood measurements

Assessments of high‐density lipoprotein cholesterol, triglycerides and glucose have been described in a previous study (Ramsay et al., 2014). Insulin resistance was estimated according to the homeostasis model assessment as the product of fasting glucose (mmol/l) and insulin (μU/ml) divided by the constant 22.5 (Ferrara and Goldberg, 2001).

Lifestyle factors

Subjects were asked detailed questions about their smoking and drinking habits as well as their pattern of physical activity. These variables have been described in a previous study (Ramsay et al., 2014). Sleep quality was assessed using a self‐reported measure of the quality of sleep ranging from 1 ‘excellent’ to 4 ‘poor’ as well as a self‐reported measure of the average hours of the daytime and night‐time sleep. To assess social interactions, a modified version of the social engagement scale developed for the Nottingham Activity and Ageing Study (Harwood et al., 2000) was used; participants were asked whether they spend any time (a) with family, friends and neighbours, (b) with friends/relatives on the telephone, (c) in paid work, (d) in voluntary work, (e) in a pub or club, (f) attending religious services, (g) playing cards, games or bingo, (h) reading or (i) attending class or course of study. The sum of these questions ranged from 0 to 9 and scores ≤ 3 indicated low social interactions.

Overall health, disabilities and health service use

History of cardiovascular disorders, that is, myocardial infarction, heart failure, stroke, angina and diabetes, was collected using information from the participants' general practitioner record reviews prior to their physical examination date. Participants were classified as having a disability if they reported difficulties in carrying out any of the following activities as a result of a long‐term health problem: going up or down stairs, bending down, straightening up, keeping balance, going out of the house or walking 400 yards. Mobility problems were defined as not being able to walk more than 200 yards, not being able to walk up and down a flight of 12 stairs without resting or not being able to bend down and pick up a shoe from the floor. Overall health was assessed using the number of general practitioner consultations in the last year, as well as using a self‐report health scale ranging from 0 (worst imaginable health state) to 100 (best imaginable health state). Depressive symptomatology was assessed using the four‐item Geriatric Depression Scale (D'Ath et al., 1994), a short instrument consisting of four questions based on the early work of Yesavage et al. (1982), which has been shown to have good sensitivity and specificity rates for the detection of depression in ill geriatric patients (Shah et al., 1997). Scores for this scale range from 0 to 4 with scores >2 being indicative of depression.

Cut‐offs used for cardiometabolic characteristics

The definitions used to identify patients with hypertension as well as the cut‐offs for low levels of high‐density lipoprotein cholesterol and high levels of triglycerides have been described elsewhere (Ramsay et al., 2014). Impaired fasting glucose was taken as >6.1 and <7 mmol/l. Low FEV1 was defined as being in the lowest quintile of FEV1.

Statistical analysis

Statistical analysis was conducted in four stages. First, the internal consistency of the items comprising the TYM was examined using Cronbach's alpha. Next, the proportion (%) of participants classified in each respective group (normal cognitive ageing, MCI or SCI) was computed. Third, the proportion (%) of participants who scored the maximum possible points in each TYM item across groups was computed to identify the affected cognitive domains. Finally, multiple logistic regressions were performed to estimate age‐adjusted odds ratios (ORs) and 95% confidence intervals (CI) for sociodemographic and cardiometabolic characteristics according to categories of cognitive impairment using ‘normal cognitive ageing’ as the reference group. The p‐value for linear trend was computed for all regression models and compared with a significance level adjusted for multiple comparisons (=0.002). Additional regression models were run for analyses examining the relationships between blood measures and cognitive functioning upon adjustment for adiposity measures because excess weight has been associated with insulin resistance and hypercholesterolaemia (Bagi, 2009). All analyses were carried out using stata/ic 13 (Stata Corp, College Station, TX, USA).

Results

The analyses for this study were restricted to 1570 participants who completed the TYM. Items comprising the TYM presented with satisfactory internal consistency (Cronbach's α = 0.72).

Of the 1570 men aged 71–92 years, 801 (51%) had cognitive skills in the normal cognitive ageing range (M = 47.73, SD = 1.49), while 636 (41%) had MCI (M = 41.21, SD = 3.13) and 133 (8%) SCI (M = 27.43, SD = 5.33). Results of a one‐way analysis of variance suggested that the mean differences in the TYM scores were statistically significant between groups (F(2, 1567) = 3469.18, p < 0.001; p‐values of all pairwise comparisons <0.001). Table 1 summarizes the proportions (%) of participants who scored the maximum possible points for items assessing different cognitive domains. The results suggest that for all items comprising the TYM, higher rates of participants classified in the normal cognitive ageing group were able to obtain maximum scores (proportions of people scoring maximum points ranged from 55% to 78%), followed by those with MCI (22–39%) and SCI (0–6%).

Table 1.

Overall cognitive performance and score per cognitive domain across cognitive performance groups defined using the TYM in a population‐based study of 1570 older British men aged 71–92 years

Normal cognitive ageing (n = 801, 51%) Mild cognitive impairment (n = 636, 41%) Severe cognitive impairments (n = 133, 8%) Total sample (n = 1570)
Overall cognitive performance
Total TYM score (M ± SD)* 47.73 ± 1.49 41.21 ± 3.13 27.43 ± 5.33 43.37 ± 6.38
Affected cognitive domains
Orientation n (%) with highest score (=10) 698 (58%) 438 (36%) 66 (5%) 1202 (77%)
Copying n (%) with highest score (=2) 778 (55%) 556 (39%) 77 (5%) 1411 (90%)
Semantic knowledge n (%) with highest score (=3) 574 (67%) 241 (28%) 37 (4%) 852 (54%)
Calculation n (%) with highest score (=4) 630 (58%) 399 (37%) 55 (5%) 1084 (69%)
Fluency n (%) with highest score (=4) 661 (69%) 276 (29%) 24 (3%) 961 (61%)
Similarities n (%) with highest score (=4) 653 (65%) 318 (32%) 29 (3%) 1000 (64%)
Naming n (%) with highest score (=5) 719 (57%) 497 (39%) 48 (4%) 1264 (81%)
Visuospatial 1 n (%) with highest score (=3) 719 (62%) 418 (36%) 19 (2%) 1156 (74%)
Visuospatial 2 n (%) with highest score (=4) 712 (63%) 390 (35%) 29 (3%) 1131 (72%)
Anterograde memory n (%) with highest score (=6) 504 (78%) 142 (22%) 1 (0%) 647 (41%)

TYM; Test Your Memory.

*

p < 0.001.

Table 2 presents the age‐adjusted sociodemographic characteristics and lifestyle factors of the three cognitive function groups. Compared with participants in the normal cognitive ageing group, those with SCI were more likely to be of manual social class, physically inactive and ex‐smokers. In addition, they were more prone to report worse sleep quality, fewer hours of night‐time and more hours of daytime sleep as well as limited social interactions. With the exception of the duration of night‐time sleep, the relationships were also significant, albeit slightly weaker, for participants with MCI. p‐values for linear trend across ordered categories for these risk factors were statistically significant and survived adjustments for multiple comparisons, suggesting that risk exposure was systematically higher for participants in different cognitive groups (all p‐values <0.002).

Table 2.

Sociodemographic and lifestyle factors across cognitive performance groups defined using the TYM in a population‐based study of 1570 older British men aged 71–92 years

Normal cognitive ageing (n = 801, 51%) Mild cognitive impairment (n = 636, 41%) Severe cognitive impairments (n = 133, 8%) p‐value (linear trend)
Age
(M ± SD)
Odds ratio (95% CI)
78.09 ± 4.39
1.00
78.32 ± 4.69
1.01 (0.99–1.08)
78.88 ± 4.76
1.04 (1.00–1.08)
0.16
Manual social class
n (%)
Odds ratio (95% CI)
272 (35%)
1.00
334 (54%)
2.20 (1.77–2.74)**
100 (76%)
6.15 (4.00–9.45)**
<0.001
Physical inactivity
n (%)
Odds ratio (95% CI)
274 (36%)
1.00
247 (42%)
1.29 (1.03–1.61)**
61 (50%)
1.77 (1.20–2.61)**
0.001
Smoking, never smoked
n (%)
Odds ratio (95% CI)
344 (43%)
1.00
209 (33%)
0.66 (0.53–0.82)**
41 (31%)
0.61 (0.41–0.90)*
<0.001
Alcohol consumption, moderate/heavy drinker
n (%)
Odds ratio (95% CI)
25 (2%)
1.00
10 (2%)
0.85 (0.38–1.92)
3 (2%)
1.29 (0.37–4.53)
0.99
Fair or poor sleep quality
n (%)
Odds ratio (95% CI)
273 (35%)
1.00
237 (38%)
1.17 (0.94–1.45)
68 (53%)
2.09 (1.44–3.05)*
<0.001
Hours of night‐time sleep,
Lower quintile (<6.0 h)
n (%)
Odds ratio (95% CI)
277 (35%)
1.00
249 (40%)
1.24 (1.00–1.54)
67 (52%)
1.95 (1.34–2.84)**
<0.001
Hours of daytime sleep,
Top quintile (≥1.5 h)
n (%)
Odds ratio (95% CI)
48 (7%)
1.00
61 (12%)
1.74 (1.17–2.59)*
27 (26%)
4.46 (2.63–7.58)**
<0.001
Limited social interactions
n (%) with 0–3 of 9 social interactions
Odds ratio (95% CI)
78 (15%)
1.00
86 (25%)
1.86 (1.32–2.62)**
18 (28%)
2.11 (1.17–3.83)**
<0.001

All odds ratios are age‐adjusted.

TYM, Test Your Memory; CI, confidence interval.

*

p < 0.05.

**

p < 0.01.

p for linear trend across ordered categories.

Table 3 presents the age‐adjusted associations between cognitive groups and history of CVD or diabetes, overall health, health service use, physical performance and disabilities. The results suggest that participants with SCI were more likely to have a positive history of stroke or diabetes or to report poorer overall health. Moreover, their average walking speed was slower; they performed poorly in the chair rise test, and they presented with decreased mobility and problems with keeping balance, as well as with other disabilities (all p‐values <0.001). Similarly, in comparison with participants in the normal cognitive ageing group, those with MCI were more likely to have a positive history of stroke and to report poorer overall health, have slower average walking speed and require longer times to complete the chair rise test. They were also more prone to have problems with keeping their balance and to have mobility problems and other disabilities. p‐values (trend across ordered groups) adjusted for multiple comparisons suggested that history of stroke or CVDs was marginally associated with cognitive groups (for both comparisons p = 0.003); trend p‐values for all other associations, including poor overall health, gait speed, problems with keeping balance, disabilities and mobility problems survived adjustments for multiple comparisons (<0.002)

Table 3.

History of CVD, service use, overall health, disabilities and physical performance across cognitive performance groups defined using the TYM in a population‐based study of 1570 older British men aged 71–92 years

Normal cognitive ageing (n = 801, 51%) Mild cognitive impairment (n = 636, 41%) Severe cognitive impairments (n = 133, 8%) p‐value (linear trend)
History of CVD or diabetes
Myocardial infarction
n (%)
Odds ratio (95% CI)
69 (9%)
1.00
59 (9%)
1.09 (0.75–1.56)
16 (12%)
1.43 (0.80–2.55)
0.25
Heart failure
n (%)
Odds ratio (95% CI)
27 (3%)
1.00
34 (5%)
1.60 (0.95–2.69)
8 (6%)
1.74 (0.77–3.94)
0.05
Stroke
n (%)
Odds ratio (95% CI)
32 (4%)
1.00
44 (7%)
1.80 (1.13–2.87)*
12 (9%)
2.41 (1.21–4.81)*
0.003
Diabetes
n (%)
Odds ratio (95% CI)
91 (11%)
1.00
89 (14%)
1.29 (0.94–1.76)
23 (17%)
1.69 (1.03–2.80)*
0.03
Any CVD
n (%) (MI, HF or stroke)
Odds ratio (95% CI)
117 (15%)
1.00
118 (19%)
1.33 (1.00–1.76)*
32 (24%)
1.82 (1.17–2.84)**
0.003
Service use and overall health
Number of GP consultations in last year
Top quintile (≥6)
n (%)
Odds ratio (95% CI)
136 (18%)
1.00
116 (20%)
1.17 (0.88–1.54)
29 (24%)
1.44 (0.92–2.29)
0.08
Poor overall health
n (%)
Odds ratio (95% CI)
187 (24%)
1.00
188 (30%)
1.38 (1.09–1.75)**
59 (46%)
2.63 (1.79–3.87)**
<0.001
Depressive symptomatology
n (%) with score >2
Odds ratio (95% CI)
47 (6%)
1.00
38 (7%)
1.11 (0.72–1.74)
8 (7%)
1.17 (0.54–2.55)
0.56
Physical performance and disabilities
Walking speed (3 m)
Top quintile (≥4.12 s)
n (%)
Odds ratio (95% CI)
116 (15%)
1.00
157 (25%)
1.93 (1.47–2.54)**
49 (37%)
3.34 (2.20–5.07)**
<0.001
Sit/stand five times
Top quintile (≥17.54 s)
n (%)
Odds ratio (95% CI)
154 (19%)
1.00
178 (28%)
1.62 (1.26–2.08)**
35 (26%)
1.41 (0.91–2.17)
0.001
Grip strength
Lower quintile (<23)
n (%)
Odds ratio (95% CI)
149 (19%)
1.00
139 (23%)
1.21 (0.93–1.57)
26 (20%)
0.97 (0.61–1.56)
0.30
Problem keeping balance
n (%)
Odds ratio (95% CI)
87 (14%)
1.00
87 (19%)
1.43 (1.02–1.98)*
28 (29%)
2.47 (1.49–4.10)**
<0.001
Any disability
n (%)
Odds ratio (95% CI)
222 (34%)
1.00
221 (44%)
1.49 (1.17–1.90)**
64 (55%)
2.30 (1.53–3.43)**
<0.001
Mobility problems
n (%)
Odds ratio (95% CI)
116 (15%)
1.00
143 (24%)
1.81 (1.38–2.39)**
55 (45%)
4.61 (3.04–6.97)**
<0.001

All odds ratios are age‐adjusted.

CVD, cardiovascular diseases; GP, general practitioner; MI, myocardial infarction; HF, heart failure; TYM, Test Your Memory; CI, confidence interval.

*

p < 0.05.

**

p < 0.01.

p for linear trend across ordered categories.

Finally, Table 4 presents the age‐adjusted comparisons of blood markers, blood pressure, adiposity measures and lung function in the three cognitive function groups. Initial results showed that low high‐density lipoprotein and high insulin resistance were associated with a higher probability of being in the SCI group. However, after adjusting for BMI, the ORs associated with these blood markers were no longer statistically significant (OR = 1.38, 95% CI 0.82–2.33; and OR = 1.39, 95% CI 0.85–2.27, respectively). In contrast, obesity (BMI > 30 kg/m2) and high waist circumference (>102 cm) were significantly associated with SCI. Additionally, participants with SCI were more likely to have lower FEV1 than those in the normal cognitive ageing group. High blood pressure and other blood markers were not significantly associated with cognitive function group. Compared with participants in the normal cognitive ageing group, those with MCI did not differ significantly in any of the examined cardiometabolic risk factors or adiposity measures. p‐values (trend) adjusted for multiple comparisons suggested that a BMI > 30, waist circumference > 102 and membership in the bottom quintile of FEV1 showed a statistically significant trend across the different TYM‐defined cognitive groups (all p‐values < 0.002).

Table 4.

Cardiometabolic risk factors, adiposity measures and lung function across cognitive performance groups defined using the TYM in a population‐based study of 1570 older British men aged 71–92 years

Normal cognitive ageing (n = 801, 51%) Mild cognitive impairment (n = 636, 41%) Severe cognitive impairments (n = 133, 8%) p‐value (linear trend)
Cardiometabolic risk factors
Low HDL (<1.04 mmol/l)
n (%)
Odds ratio (95% CI)
92 (12%)
1.00
93 (15%)
1.31 (0.96–1.79)
24 (19%)
1.76 (1.07–2.89)*
0.01
High LDL (>4 mmol/l)
n (%)
Odds ratio (95% CI)
65 (9%)
1.00
52 (9%)
1.01 (0.69–1.47)
8 (7%)
0.76 (0.35–1.62)
0.60
High triglycerides (≥2.3 mmol/l)
n (%)
Odds ratio (95% CI)
53 (7%)
1.00
53 (9%)
1.30 (0.87–1.93)
11 (9%)
1.40 (0.71–2.77)
0.23
Total cholesterol (≥5 mmol/l)
n (%)
Odds ratio (95% CI)
306 (40%)
1.00
216 (36%)
0.82 (0.66–1.03)
38 (31%)
0.67 (0.44–1.01)
0.02
Impaired total fasting glucose (<6.1 or >7.0 mmol/l)
n (%)
Odds ratio (95% CI)
73 (10%)
1.00
69 (12%)
1.18 (0.83–1.68)
11 (10%)
0.90 (0.46–1.76)
0.70
Insulin resistance, top quintile (≥3.68 mmol/l × μU/ml)
n (%)
Odds ratio (95% CI)
126 (18%)
1.00
116 (20%)
1.17 (0.88–1.55)
34 (30%)
1.97 (1.26–3.08)**
0.01
High blood pressure
n (%)
Odds ratio (95% CI)
545 (68%)
1.00
429 (68%)
0.97 (0.78–1.22)
92 (69%)
1.04 (0.70–1.55)
0.97
Adiposity measures and lung function
BMI ≥ 30 kg/m2
n (%)
Odds ratio (95% CI)
137 (17%)
1.00
124 (20%)
1.19 (0.91–1.56)
44 (34%)
2.59 (1.72–3.91)**
<0.001
High waist circumference (>102 cm)
n (%)
Odds ratio (95% CI)
288 (36%)
1.00
259 (41%)
1.23 (1.00–1.53)
69 (52%)
1.98 (1.36–2.87)**
0.001
FEV1
Bottom quintile (≤1.93 l)
n (%)
Odds ratio (95% CI)
139 (18%)
1.00
132 (22%)
1.25 (0.95–1.64)
42 (33%)
2.24 (1.46–3.43)**
<0.001

All odds ratios are age‐adjusted.

HDL, high‐density lipoprotein; LDL, low‐density lipoprotein; TYM, Test Your Memory; CI, confidence interval.

*

p < 0.05.

**

p < 0.01.

p for linear trend across ordered categories.

Discussion

This study in a representative sample of older British men shows that numerous sociodemographic and cardiometabolic factors are significantly associated with MCI and SCI, as assessed using the TYM. The strongest associations were observed for factors that have also been associated with MCI and AD in different studies and included low occupation‐based socio‐economic status (Karp et al., 2004), physical inactivity (Winchester et al., 2013), motor (Mirelman et al., 2014) and lung dysfunction (Bozek and Jarzab, 2011), smoking (Anstey et al., 2007; Peters et al., 2008), positive history of CVD (de la Torre, 2004) or diabetes (Tolppanen et al., 2013), poor overall health (Montlahuc et al., 2011), sleep disturbances (Ownby et al., 2014) and higher levels of functional dependence related to abilities to perform everyday activities (Aguero‐Torres et al., 1998; Femminella et al., 2014). These correlates showed a significant ordered trend across cognitive groups suggesting a systematically increased likelihood of presenting with MCI and SCI.

Comparison with other studies

While some of the significant correlates of cognitive functioning assessed with the TYM were consistent with findings from the available literature, late‐life hypertension (Power et al., 2011) and hypercholesterolaemia (Polidori et al., 2012), which have shown contradictory results with respect to incident AD, did not emerge as significant characteristics in this study. In addition, obesity was one of the strongest correlates of SCI although a recent meta‐analysis suggests that primarily midlife rather than late‐life obesity is associated with cognitive impairments (Anstey et al., 2011). However, the cross‐sectional design of this study did not allow for the examination of the prospective relationships between these measures. Moreover, lung function and adiposity measures were associated with SCI but not MCI and might therefore serve as target factors in future research on the differing manifestations and progress of MCI and AD. Two studies by Scarmeas et al. (2009a, 2009b) in prospective cohort samples of elders have also shown that BMI differs significantly only between individuals with intact cognitive skills and SCI, but not MCI (Scarmeas et al., 2009a, 2009b). With respect to lung function, reduced FEV1 rates in midlife were shown to be predictive of MCI in a recent study (Vidal et al., 2013), while declines in pulmonary function over an 8‐year period of time were not. The biological mechanisms underlying these relationships are yet to be disentangled.

Strengths and limitations

This study is unique in terms of using the TYM in a general population sample to assess cognitive abilities in the elderly population. Previous studies using the TYM were based on clinical or smaller probabilistic samples and focused mainly on the discriminant abilities of the TYM rather than the characteristics associated with the cognitive groups assessed. Moreover, for this study, we assessed numerous sociodemographic and cardiometabolic factors, and therefore, it is an excellent inventory of cardiovascular and related factors associated with cognitive impairments.

The items of the TYM showed satisfactory internal consistency (72%) in this general population sample. While reliability rates between 78% and 98% have been reported for the TYM in clinical samples (Abd‐Al‐Atty et al., 2012; Muñoz‐Neira et al., 2014), Charter (2003) has noted that with low sample sizes, alpha coefficients can be rather unstable (Charter, 2003); the reported reliability of 0.72 might therefore represent a more accurate estimate.

The BRHS is a highly representative sample of the older male UK population, which has been successful in keeping attrition rates at very low levels. However, the issue of survivor bias cannot be overlooked in cohorts of ageing populations; subjects with AD are more likely to have died earlier (Weuve et al., 2014). An additional limitation of the BRHS is that it comprises only men of predominantly White European origin; therefore, the results cannot necessarily be generalized to women and different ethnic groups.

For this study, total TYM scores were calculated, which may have obscured findings on atypical, for example non‐amnestic, presentations of SCI. In fact, the TYM includes items assessing visuospatial tasks, which contribute 7/50 points and could help in distinguishing between amnestic and non‐amnestic (atypical) manifestations of MCI or SCI. However, non‐amnestic AD is less frequent than amnestic AD (Chertkow et al., 2013). Additionally, the TYM is not a diagnostic tool, and therefore, it is likely that study members with SCI did not meet diagnostic criteria for AD or suffered from other forms of dementia.

It is also likely that different cut‐offs of the TYM to define cognitive groups could be more sensitive for use in general population samples. Yet, when we applied the stricter cut‐offs proposed by Hancock and Larner (2011) to define cognitive groups in an exploratory manner, the results were very similar to the ones described for this study. In addition, these cut‐offs are less inclusive and could lead to less robust estimates for the relationship estimates between cognitive impairments and the correlates examined. As this is the first study to administer the TYM to a general population sample, we could not calibrate new cut‐offs for use in general population samples in the absence of an additional cognitive screening instrument and/or information on relevant clinical outcomes, for example AD. It is also acknowledged that assigning 5 points to every participant for the last task of the TYM could be a source of bias in terms of the classification obtained. Nonetheless, a sensitivity analysis to examine the classification without assigning 5 points for this question to the study members extracted a classification almost identical to the one reported in the study. Finally, the results presented are based on cross‐sectional assessments, and therefore, only associative but not causative aetiological mechanisms can be inferred, primarily because of possible temporal biases (reverse causality).

Implications

The proportion of participants classified as having MCI (41%) or SCI (8%) using the TYM in this sample is in line with the prevalence rates for MCI and AD reported in previous studies (Bischkopf et al., 2002; World Health Organization, 2012). Available literature reports that the respective prevalence rates for mild forms of cognitive dysfunction and AD range between 2% and 56% (Bischkopf et al., 2002) and 2% and 8.5%, respectively, for those aged 60 years and over (World Health Organization, 2012).

Moreover, previous studies, which report similar cardiometabolic risk profiles for cognitive impairments, have used different cognitive screening tools or classification criteria to ascertain classifications of AD or MCI, for example the Mini‐mental state examination, the criteria of the joint‐working group of the National Institute of Neurological and Communicative Disorders and Stroke and the Alzheimer's Disease and Related Disorders Association (McKhann et al., 1984) and the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (Third Edition Revised) (APA, 1987). Therefore, future research needs to compare the TYM against established cognitive screening instruments and diagnostic tools to validate its potential as a highly reliable cognitive test for use in general population samples.

Conclusion

This is the first study to utilize the TYM within a population‐based sample. It provides further evidence that cognitive impairment among older adults is associated with a range of sociodemographic and cardiometabolic factors, particularly low occupation‐based socio‐economic status, physical inactivity, motor and lung dysfunction, smoking, positive history of CVD or diabetes, poor overall health, sleep disturbances and higher levels of functional dependence related to abilities to perform everyday activities. The cardiometabolic and sociodemographic correlates of TYM‐defined cognitive groups are almost identical to those extracted using established screening and diagnostic tools. Targeting these risk factors that are modifiable would increase the scope for primary and secondary interventions aiming to reduce the adverse effects of dementia in the older population.

Conflict of interest

None declared.

Key points.

  • This is the first study to administer the Test your Memory (TYM) cognitive screening instrument and to examine the sociodemographic and cardiometabolic correlates of TYM‐defined cognitive function groups in a general population sample.

  • The strongest correlates of severe cognitive impairments among older adults are low occupation‐based socio‐economic status, physical inactivity, motor and lung dysfunction, smoking, positive history of cardiovascular diseases or diabetes, poor overall health, sleep disturbances and higher levels of functional dependence related to abilities to perform everyday activities.

  • The cardiometabolic and sociodemographic correlates of TYM‐defined cognitive groups in this representative sample of older British men are almost identical to those outlined in current epidemiological models using established cognitive screening tests.

Sponsor's Role

None.

Acknowledgements

The British Regional Heart Study is supported by a British Heart Foundation Programme Grant (RG/13/16/30528) and a British Heart Foundation Project Grant for the 30‐year re‐examination (PG09/024). E. P is funded by the NIHR (UCL Hospital Biomedical Research Centre). S. R is funded by a UK MRC Fellowship (G1002391).

Papachristou, E. , Ramsay, S. E. , Papacosta, O. , Lennon, L. T. , Iliffe, S. , Whincup, P. H. , and Goya Wannamethee, S. (2016) The Test Your Memory cognitive screening tool: sociodemographic and cardiometabolic risk correlates in a population‐based study of older British men. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry, 31: 666–675. doi: 10.1002/gps.4377.

References

  1. Abd‐Al‐Atty MF, Abou‐Hashem RM, Abd Elaziz KM, et al 2012. Test Your Memory test, Arabic version: is it practical in a different culture? J Am Geriatr Soc 60: 596–597. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Aguero‐Torres H, Fratiglioni L, Guo Z, et al. 1998. Dementia is the major cause of functional dependence in the elderly: 3‐year follow‐up data from a population‐based study. Am J Public Health 88: 1452–1456. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Anstey KJ, Cherbuin N, Budge M, et al 2011. Body mass index in midlife and late‐life as a risk factor for dementia: a meta‐analysis of prospective studies. Obes Rev 12: e426–437. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Anstey KJ, Von Sanden C, Salim A, et al 2007. Smoking as a risk factor for dementia and cognitive decline: a meta‐analysis of prospective studies. Am J Epidemiol 166: 367–378. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Apa . 1987. American Psychiatric Association Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (3rd., rev.) Washington, DC: APA. [Google Scholar]
  6. Bagi Z. 2009. Mechanisms of coronary microvascular adaptation to obesity. Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 297: R556–R567. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bischkopf J, Busse A, Angermeyer MC. 2002. Mild cognitive impairment—a review of prevalence, incidence and outcome according to current approaches. Acta Psychiatr Scand 106: 403–414. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Bozek A, Jarzab J. 2011. Improved activity and mental function related to proper antiasthmatic treatment in elderly patients with Alzheimer's disease. Allergy Asthma Proc 32: 341–345. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Brown J, Pengas G, Dawson K, et al 2009. Self administered cognitive screening test (TYM) for detection of Alzheimer's disease: cross sectional study. BMJ 338: b2030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Charter RA. 2003. Study samples are too small to produce sufficiently precise reliability coefficients. J Gen Psychol 130: 117–129. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Chertkow H, Feldman HH, Jacova C, et al 2013. Definitions of dementia and predementia states in Alzheimer's disease and vascular cognitive impairment: consensus from the Canadian conference on diagnosis of dementia. Alzheimers Res Ther 5: S2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. D'ath P, Katona P, Mullan E, et al 1994. Screening, detection and management of depression in elderly primary care attenders. I: the acceptability and performance of the 15 item Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS15) and the development of short versions. Fam Pract 11: 260–266. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. De La Torre JC. 2004. Is Alzheimer's disease a neurodegenerative or a vascular disorder? Data, dogma, and dialectics. Lancet Neurol 3: 184–190. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Femminella GD, Rengo G, Komici K, et al 2014. Autonomic dysfunction in Alzheimer's disease: tools for assessment and review of the literature. J Alzheimers Dis 42: 369–377. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Ferrara CM, Goldberg AP. 2001. Limited value of the homeostasis model assessment to predict insulin resistance in older men with impaired glucose tolerance. Diabetes Care 24: 245–249. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Ferrero‐Arias J, Turrion‐Rojo MA. 2014. Validation of a Spanish version of the Test Your Memory. Neurologia . DOI:10.1016/j.nrl.2013.12.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Hancock P, Larner AJ. 2011. Test Your Memory test: diagnostic utility in a memory clinic population. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 26: 976–980. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Hanyu H, Maezono M, Sakurai H, et al 2011. Japanese version of the Test Your Memory as a screening test in a Japanese memory clinic. Psychiatry Res 190: 145–148. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Harwood RH, Pound P, Ebrahim S. 2000. Determinants of social engagement in older men. Psychol Health Med 5: 75–85. [Google Scholar]
  20. Iatraki E, Simos PG, Lionis C, et al 2014. Cultural adaptation, standardization and clinical validity of the test your memory dementia screening instrument in Greek. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 37: 163–180. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Karp A, Kåreholt I, Qiu C, et al 2004. Relation of education and occupation‐based socioeconomic status to incident Alzheimer's disease. Am J Epidemiol 159: 175–183. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Koekkoek PS, Rutten GEHM, Van Den Berg E, et al 2013. The “Test Your Memory” test performs better than the MMSE in a population without known cognitive dysfunction. J Neurol Sci 328: 92–97. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Larner AJ. 2013. Conclusion: place of cognitive screening instruments: test characteristics and suspected diagnosis In Cognitive Screening Instruments, Larner AJ. (ed.). Springer: London; 219–238. [Google Scholar]
  24. Mckhann G, Drachman D, Folstein M, et al 1984. Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease: report of the NINCDS‐ADRDA Work Group under the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer's Disease. Neurology 34: 939–944. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Mirelman A, Weiss A, Buchman AS, et al 2014. Association between performance on Timed Up and Go subtasks and mild cognitive impairment: further insights into the links between cognitive and motor function. J Am Geriatr Soc 62: 673–678. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Montlahuc C, Soumare A, Dufouil C, et al 2011. Self‐rated health and risk of incident dementia: a community‐based elderly cohort, the 3C study. Neurology 77: 1457–1464. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Muñoz‐Neira C, Henríquez Chaparro F, Delgado C, et al 2014. Test Your Memory—spanish version (TYM‐S): a validation study of a self‐administered cognitive screening test. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 29: 730–740. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Ownby RL, Peruyera G, Acevedo A, et al 2014. Subtypes of sleep problems in patients with Alzheimer disease. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry 22: 148–156. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Peters R, Poulter R, Warner J, et al 2008. Smoking, dementia and cognitive decline in the elderly, a systematic review. BMC Geriatr 8: 36. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Plassman BL, Langa KM, Fisher GG, et al 2007. Prevalence of dementia in the United States: the aging, demographics, and memory study. Neuroepidemiology 29: 125–132. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Polidori MC, Pientka L, Mecocci P. 2012. A review of the major vascular risk factors related to Alzheimer's disease. J Alzheimers Dis 32: 521–530. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Postel‐Vinay N, Hanon O, Clerson P, et al 2014. Validation of the Test Your Memory (F‐TYM test) in a French memory clinic population. Clin Neuropsychol 28: 1–14. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Power MC, Weuve J, Gagne JJ, et al 2011. The association between blood pressure and incident Alzheimer disease: a systematic review and meta‐analysis. Epidemiology 22: 646–659. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Qiu C, Kivipelto M, Von Strauss E. 2009. Epidemiology of Alzheimer's disease: occurrence, determinants, and strategies toward intervention. Dialogues Clin Neurosci 11: 111–128. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Ramsay SE, Arianayagam DS, Whincup PH, et al 2014. Cardiovascular risk profile and frailty in a population‐based study of older British men. Heart 101: 616–622. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Royal College of Psychiatrists (Rpsych) . 2013. Interpreting the TYM [Online]. Available: http://www.rcpsych.ac.uk/pdf/TYM_Interpreting.pdf Accessed 26 July 2015.
  37. Scarmeas N, Luchsinger JA, Schupf N, et al 2009a. Physical activity, diet, and risk of Alzheimer disease. JAMA 302: 627–637. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Scarmeas N, Stern Y, Mayeux R, et al. 2009b. Mediterranean diet and mild cognitive impairment. Arch Neurol 66: 216–225. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Shah A, Herbert R, Lewis S, et al 1997. Screening for depression among acutely ill geriatric inpatients with a short geriatric depression scale. Age Ageing 26: 217–221. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Szczesniak D, Wojtynska R, Rymaszewska J. 2013. Test Your Memory (TYM) as a screening instrument in clinical practice – the Polish validation study. Aging Ment Health 17: 863–868. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Tolppanen AM, Lavikainen P, Solomon A, et al 2013. History of medically treated diabetes and risk of Alzheimer disease in a nationwide case‐control study. Diabetes Care 36: 2015–2019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Van Schalkwyk G, Botha H, Seedat S. 2012. Comparison of 2 dementia screeners, the Test Your Memory test and the Mini‐Mental State Examination, in a primary care setting. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol 25: 85–88. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Vidal J‐S, Aspelund T, Jonsdottir MK, et al 2013. Pulmonary function impairment may be an early risk factor for late‐life cognitive impairment. J Am Geriatr Soc 61: 79–83. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Walker M, Whincup PH, Shaper AG. 2004. The British Regional Heart Study 1975–2004. Int J Epidemiol 33: 1185–1192. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Wannamethee SG, Whincup PH, Shaper G, et al 1996. Influence of fathers' social class on cardiovascular disease in middle‐aged men. The Lancet 348: 1259–1263. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Weuve J, Hebert LE, Scherr PA, et al 2014. Deaths in the United States among persons with Alzheimer's disease (2010–2050). Alzheimers Dement 10: e40–e46. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Winchester J, Dick MB, Gillen D, et al 2013. Walking stabilizes cognitive functioning in Alzheimer's disease (AD) across one year. Arch Gerontol Geriatr 56: 96–103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. World Health Organization . 2012. Alzheimer's Disease International. Dementia: a public health priority [Online]. Geneva (CH).
  49. Yesavage JA, Brink TL, Rose TL, et al 1982. Development and validation of a geriatric depression screening scale: a preliminary report. J Psychiatr Res 17: 37–49. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Zygouris S, Tsolaki M. 2014. Computerized cognitive testing for older adults: a review. Am J Alzheimers Dis Other Demen 30: 1–16. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry are provided here courtesy of Wiley

RESOURCES