Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2017 Feb 1.
Published in final edited form as: Mindfulness (N Y). 2015 Apr 12;7(1):5–33. doi: 10.1007/s12671-015-0395-6

Assessing Fidelity of Implementation (FOI) for School-Based Mindfulness and Yoga Interventions: A Systematic Review

Laura Feagans Gould 1, Jacinda K Dariotis 2, Mark T Greenberg 3, Tamar Mendelson 4
PMCID: PMC4856056  NIHMSID: NIHMS680452  PMID: 27158278

Abstract

As school-based mindfulness and yoga programs gain popularity, the systematic study of fidelity of program implementation (FOI) is critical to provide a more robust understanding of the core components of mindfulness and yoga interventions, their potential to improve specified teacher and student outcomes, and our ability to implement these programs consistently and effectively. This paper reviews the current state of the science with respect to inclusion and reporting of FOI in peer-reviewed studies examining the effects of school-based mindfulness and/or yoga programs targeting students and/or teachers implemented in grades kindergarten through twelve (K-12) in North America. Electronic searches in PsychInfo and Web of Science from their inception through May 2014, in addition to hand searches of relevant review articles, identified 312 publications, 48 of which met inclusion criteria. Findings indicated a relative paucity of rigorous FOI. Fewer than 10% of studies outlined potential core program components or referenced a formal theory of action, and fewer than 20% assessed any aspect of FOI beyond participant dosage. The emerging nature of the evidence base provides a critical window of opportunity to grapple with key issues relevant to FOI of mindfulness-based and yoga programs, including identifying essential elements of these programs that should be faithfully implemented and how we might develop rigorous measures to accurately capture them. Consideration of these questions and suggested next steps are intended to help advance the emerging field of school-based mindfulness and yoga interventions.

Keywords: mindfulness, yoga, fidelity, implementation, review, school-based intervention

Introduction

In the current climate of enthusiasm for school-based mindfulness and yoga programs, research efforts have aimed primarily to evaluate program impacts on teacher and student outcomes. Indeed, a number of reviews and meta-analyses have now summarized the growing evidence base for effects of school-based mindfulness and yoga (Davidson & Mind and Life Education Research Network 2012; Meiklejohn et al. 2012; Serwacki & Cook-Cottone 2012). Assessing intervention outcomes is critical to testing program efficacy and gaining support and funding for these programs (Greenberg & Harris 2012; Weare 2013). Outcomes assessment alone, however, is not sufficient to build a rigorous evidence base for school-based contemplative practices. The systematic study of fidelity of program implementation (FOI) is needed to provide a more robust understanding of the core components of mindfulness and yoga interventions for youth, their potential to improve specified teacher and student outcomes, and our ability to implement these programs consistently and effectively across time and in diverse school settings (Davidson & Mind and Life Education Research Network 2012; Greenberg & Harris 2012).

FOI is a multi-dimensional construct that refers to the degree to which intervention delivery adheres to the intervention developers’ model (Dane & Schneider 1998). Whereas traditional intervention outcomes research focuses on program effects (the dependent variables), the study of FOI refines our understanding of the core elements that constitute a given program (the independent variable) and their relationship to program outcomes. In order to study FOI, researchers and program developers must first identify the key constituent parts of an intervention and articulate how these components are anticipated to create desired outcomes. They must then develop reliable and valid measures of FOI and establish measurable criteria for implementation integrity. These criteria can be used in subsequent research to examine empirically whether variation in the implementation of core components is systematically related to particular outcomes across replication trials (Feagans Gould et al. 2014).

Why is Fidelity of Implementation Important?

Assessing fidelity of implementation is important to our understanding of whether and how school-based mindfulness programs work for several reasons. First, what actually gets implemented in real-world settings, like schools, may vary from study to study, even within the same program. Therefore evidence-based practice needs a means of evaluating whether a program is being implemented as intended (Carroll et al. 2007). Evidence indicates marked variation in implementation fidelity both across and within youth psychosocial prevention and promotion programs focused on mental and physical health (Durlak & Dupre 2008). It is highly likely that similar variability in implementation fidelity exists for mindfulness-based programs. Such variation will become more apparent as an increasing number of mindfulness-based programs are implemented and larger studies are conducted.

Second, the degree to which programs are implemented with fidelity in real-world settings directly informs the conclusions we can make about the effectiveness of a single program or school-based mindfulness programs more generally (Carroll et al. 2007). Durlak and Dupre reviewed over 500 studies of promotion and prevention programs for youth and adolescents, including 5 meta-analyses, and concluded that, “Achieving good implementation not only increases the chances of program success in statistical terms, but also can lead to much stronger benefits for participants” (2008, p. 334). Indeed the magnitude of mean effect sizes was at least two to three times higher when programs were carefully implemented and free from serious implementation problems, particularly when fidelity or dosage were assessed. This is consistent with an emerging body of evidence that suggests program fidelity leads to better outcomes and program outcomes are sensitive to variations in implementation fidelity (Kutash, Cross, Madias, Duchonowski, & Green 2012). In addition, assessing fidelity of implementation guards against making what is known as a Type III error - the incorrect conclusion that a program itself is not effective, when in fact poor outcomes are the result of shortcomings in implementation (e.g., the instructor did not have time to cover all the curriculum components) (Domitrovich & Greenberg 2000).

Third, assessing program fidelity can help move us toward an understanding of how programs work and the “active ingredients,” or drivers, of program effects. Although mindfulness and yoga programs all include contemplative practices that focus on anchoring attention in the present moment, programs vary widely in the specific forms of mindfulness practice they teach, in program duration and dosage, and in the types and characteristics of school-based populations they target (e.g., students and/or teachers, developmental stage or grade level, socioeconomic status) (Greenberg & Harris 2012; Meiklejohn et al. 2012). Programs are likely to produce different levels of impact based on program features and characteristics of the target population. Particular practices (e.g., breath work) or program components (e.g., assigned homework) also may be differentially effective in producing outcomes. Thus, FOI measures are critical for developing our understanding of which mindfulness practices or program components are most effective, for whom, and under which conditions.

Finally, assessing FOI can help facilitate program improvement and refinement. FOI findings can identify which aspects of a program are contributing to its efficacy and which aspects are not, potentially informing changes in intervention content. For instance, if practice of guided mindfulness reflections is found to predict particularly robust intervention gains, program developers may wish to increase the frequency with which this skill is practiced throughout the program. FOI findings can also inform decisions about which program aspects may require modification to overcome implementation challenges and facilitate delivery as intended. For instance, if program instructors consistently have difficulty fully covering curriculum material, program developers may decide that the curriculum needs to be pared down or that more intervention sessions are needed.

How do we study FOI?

Approaches for assessing and analyzing FOI vary based on program and research goals, as well as the nature of the program and context of implementation. Conceptualizations of FOI span many disciplines including mental health, prevention research, education, criminal justice, public health and policy (Burkel et al. 2011; Century, Rudnick, & Freeman 2010; Caroll et al. 2007; Durlak & Dupre 2008; O’Donnell 2008; Fixsen, Blase, Naoom, & Wallace 2009). Although differences across these frameworks merit attention, their core aspects are fairly similar. This review references a general framework based on the Plan Do Study Act (PDSA) cycle (Deming 1986) and consistent with leading FOI conceptualizations (Century et al. 2010), which our team has discussed elsewhere in the context of our school-based mindfulness research (Feagans Gould et al. 2014). We have termed this framework the CORE cycle, as it involves the following steps: (C) Conceptualize core components; (O) Operationalize and measure; (R) Run analyses and review; and (E) Enhance and refine. Thus, we conceptualize the study of FOI as a four-step, iterative process that leads, over time, to a more refined theory of change, greater knowledge about the effective or core components of a program, and more rigorous measures of implementation integrity (see Figure 1). Below we briefly outline the four steps and their relevance to the study of FOI in school-based mindfulness and yoga programs.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

The CORE Process Model for Assessing Fidelity of Implementation (FOI)

Step 1: Define program core components and their relation to hypothesized outcomes

The aim of this step is to answer the question: What should be implemented? To judge whether a program is implemented faithfully, we must first identify the core components, also referred to as critical components (see Ruiz-Primo 2005; Century et al. 2012) that comprise the program. Core components are -- “the most essential and indispensable components of an intervention practice or program” (Fixen et al. 2005 p.24)—and the backbone of program fidelity. Core components have been conceptualized as being of two types: structural components (the content or activities to be delivered, say, as part of a manual) and process components (the manner in which content should be implemented, for instance, the modeling of compassionate responses by program instructors) (Century et al. 2012). Identification of core components transforms an intervention from a “black box” to a set of elements that can be measured and assessed.

Development of a programmatic logic model--sometimes called a theory of change—is also critical and goes hand in hand with specification of core program components. A logic model guides measurement and analysis by specifying how each program core component, or combinations of components, should lead to hypothesized outcomes. For instance, program developers may predict that focused attention training through awareness of the breath will enhance capacities for self-regulation, leading to downstream improvements in students’ behavior and performance in class. A logic model generally draws on relevant theoretical perspectives and empirical findings from the literature, e.g., an evidence base supporting links between self-regulation and classroom behavior. Ideally, there is logic not only behind specification of the hypothesized core components but also to support other aspects of a program like the sequencing of intervention activities and program dosing.

Step 2: Operationalize and measure the FOI of core program components

The aim of this step is to answer the question: How will we know if a program is implemented with fidelity? Once a program’s core components have been articulated, an objective assessment system is needed to monitor fidelity of implementation to these core components (Durlak 1998; Domitrovich & Greenberg 2000). There are four commonly agreed-upon dimensions of fidelity (Dane & Schneider 1998; Dusenbury, Brannigan, Falco, & Hansen 2003): 1) Adherence - the extent to which the core components were implemented as designed; 2) Dosage - the amount of the intervention received by participants; 3) Quality - the extent to which an instructor delivered program content as intended; and 4) Responsiveness - the extent to which participants were engaged in the program. Assessing multiple dimensions of fidelity is preferable, not only because it offers a more well-rounded assessment of these various aspects of FOI, but also because evidence shows that each has the potential to be a critical dimension fostering participant outcomes (Durlak & Dupre 2008).

Measures of each dimension of FOI should be reliable and valid, using the same standards applied to intervention outcome measures (Domitrovich & Greenberg 2000; O’Donnell 2008). Collecting FOI data from multiple reporters is desirable, particularly using “objective” measures, such as observational coding of intervention sessions by coders because these are typically more highly correlated with program outcomes than instructor-reported data, which are prone to bias (Dane & Schneider 1998). Along with developing reliable and valid measures, a priori criteria for what constitutes implementation “as intended” or “not as intended” must be defined and operationalized in terms of the measures being used. For example, it is helpful to create a cut-off for the number of sessions a participant must attend or the extent of training a teacher must receive in order to qualify as a sufficient “dose.” Another way to operationalize as intended is to create categories of “low,” “medium,” and ‘high” dosage. The important point is that these criteria are defined a priori within a single study, so that they are theoretically informed. Across replication trials, however, specific cut-offs can be empirically informed by results.

Two final aspects of rigorously assessing FOI are the monitoring of control/comparison conditions and reporting adaptations made to the program during implementation (Durlak & Dupre 2008). Monitoring control/comparison conditions involves describing the nature and amount of services received by members of comparison conditions because it is often incorrectly assumed that controls do not receive any services, but this is almost never the case in school-based studies (Durlak 1985). In order to fully understand control-comparison condition differences, FOI data can be collected to inform differential uptake of the IV and therefore a more accurate picture of the unique value of an intervention. In addition, collecting data on what meaningful adaptations were made to program delivery is important as sometimes such adaptations have been found to have adverse effects on outcomes, and other times adaptations based on context or the specific characteristics of recipients have been found to improve impacts (Durlak & Dupre 2008).

Developing and refining valid and reliable measures for each of the four dimensions of FOI is a challenging process that takes time and may also require additional resources, such as recording of intervention sessions and training independent coders. Given the iterative nature of the process, FOI measures for a given intervention have potential to improve following initial formative work, as the program components are increasingly refined and as implementation issues are better understood.

Step 3: Analyze FOI data and report findings

This step may address a variety of questions, including: Was the program implemented as intended? If variation in implementation exists, to what extent are outcomes affected? It is important for researchers to report the level of FOI in studies on school-based mindfulness and yoga programs to document the implementation quality associated with particular outcomes and to identify potential variation in program implementation across intervention instructors and/or sites. If variation exists, researchers should gauge whether FOI was so low that participants did not in fact receive what would be considered a minimally effective dose of the program. If there is sufficient variation in FOI, evaluators can also categorize intervention groups, classrooms, or schools by levels of FOI to test whether variation is related to outcomes. When such analyses are performed they help us answer important questions like “what is the dosage or frequency needed to produce certain level of outcomes?”

Step 4: Enhance and refine the logic model and FOI measures based on findings from FOI data

This step aims to address the question, What did we learn about a program and FOI measures? Researchers should ideally use FOI data to reflect on their hypothesized core components and logic model. Rigorous measurement and analysis of FOI can facilitate the iterative learning cycle of program development. FOI analyses within a given study can refine understanding of why and under what conditions a program works. Across programs, such analyses can move the field towards identification of best practices or common active ingredients, a key next step in the growing new field of school-based mindfulness and yoga interventions research.

Aims of the current review

Given the importance of FOI for building a robust and informative evidence-base, we reviewed the current state of the science with respect to inclusion and reporting of FOI in studies on school-based mindfulness and yoga interventions. We focused on the extent to which: 1) hypothesized program core components and logic models are specified in the literature; 2) FOI is being rigorously assessed and reported; and 3) the relationship between FOI and program outcomes is being reported. We hope this paper will offer useful suggestions for school-based mindfulness researchers beginning to tackle the challenges of FOI measurement and analysis. Synthesizing FOI data across studies also provides an opportunity to reflect on the commonalities across specified core components and logic models and the utility of particular FOI measures. Consideration of these questions is intended to help advance the emerging field of school-based mindfulness and yoga interventions.

Method

Information sources and searches

To identify potentially relevant articles, we searched two databases, PsycInfo and Web of Science, from their inception to May 2014 using combinations of the terms mindfulness, mindful, yoga, meditation, school, education, program, students, and teachers. We also searched reference lists in relevant review articles.

Study selection

To be selected for inclusion, a study was required to meet the following criteria: 1) assessment of a program for students and/or teachers whose primary content was mindfulness-based practices or yoga-based movement, 2) program delivery in a school setting--either during or after the school day—in grades kindergarten through twelfth grade (K-12), 3) program delivery in the United States or Canada, 4) Experimental, Quasi-experimental, or single group study designs with a sample size of greater than five participants (consistent with Meiklejohn et al. 2012), 5) publication in a peer-reviewed journal or book chapter, and 6) publication in English. We chose to focus on mindfulness and yoga-based programs because these are the most widely-used forms of contemplative practices secularly implemented and studied in school settings (Greenberg & Harris 2012). Our focus on grades K-12 was motivated by the focus of this special issue on school-based mindfulness programs for youth. We chose to limit our review to programs delivered in North America as we anticipated that these programs and school settings would be most comparable and thus most amenable to this initial attempt at synthesis of FOI measurement. Questions regarding whether or not a study met eligibility criteria were discussed among two or more co-authors until consensus was reached. In three instances when it was unclear if a study met inclusion criteria based on the full text of an article, the lead author contacted the corresponding author to provide additional details.

Data abstraction

The lead author abstracted the following data from each study included in the review within the following broad domains:

Program and study characteristics included primary program focus, program approach, program session length, frequency, duration, and format, grade-level of school setting, when and where a program was implemented within the school setting, study design, sample size, and number of schools and classrooms in which a program was implemented. These variables capture the potential variation in program focus and implementation methods as well as the kinds of studies conducted to date. Primary program focus refers to whether the intervention content consisted mostly of “Meditation,” “Yoga;” or “Combined Meditation and Yoga.” In order for a program to be categorized as “primarily meditation,” the primary program practices and components, as described in the article, included forms of meditation such as open-monitoring, focused attention, and/or loving kindness/compassion practices (see Ricard, Lutz, & Davidson 2014; Roeser & Pinela 2014 for further discussion of forms of meditation). For a program to be “primarily yoga,” the predominate program practices and components, as described in the article, included yoga –based physical movements (e.g. asanas) and embodied practices. For programs categorized as “combined meditation and yoga,” the program focus was relatively equally distributed across meditation and yoga practices and components. Program approach is based on the major approaches outlined by Meiklejohn and colleagues (2012) to characterize school-based mindfulness programs as directly targeting students, indirectly delivering to teachers or delivering program components to both students and teachers.

Theoretical rationale underpinning core program components included whether a study articulated the core or potentially essential program components and theoretical underpinnings for the program being evaluated. For this domain, we extracted the language used to describe the main program components and any rationale for these components, coded whether key or core program components were articulated (as opposed to simply describing components of the program without any reference to their centrality to program theory), as well as whether or not a logic model was included.

FOI rigor and reporting categorized whether a study assessed each of the four dimensions of FOI (i.e. adherence, quality, dosage, and responsiveness), what measures were used to assess each dimension, if multiple measures were used to assess a single dimension, if reliability or validity of measures were assessed, if any a priori criteria for “high” or “low” levels of FOI were set, if FOI was monitored in the control/comparison condition, and if and what adaptions made during implementation were reported. We also recorded whether and how levels of FOI were reported and whether there was any variation in FOI across different instances of program delivery in the study.

FOI associations with outcomes categorized whether a study assessed the association of FOI aspects with outcomes and, if yes, briefly summarized the findings.

Results

Our literature search identified 312 citations, from which 60 articles were retrieved and 48 judged to meet study criteria and retained (see Figure 2). Additional details about the programs and studies included as well as select categories of data extracted are included in Appendix A.

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

Flow Diagram of Relevant Article Identification and Selection

Appendix A.

List of Programs and Studies Reviewed and Select Categories of Data Extracted

Program Study Delivery
Approach
Study Design Session Delivery FOI Dimensions Assessed and Measures Used FOI Cut-Offs
Established
FOI Reported Linked to
Outcomes
Program Name Core Components
(CC) Articulated
& Logic Model
(LM) Included
Citation Program targets
Students,
Teachers, or Both
Design (RCT; QED,
Single Group); Level
of Assign (School or
Class); & # IUs
(intervention units)
Total # Sessions
Delivered, Weeks, and
Length
Program Adherence Program Quality Participant Dosage Participant Responsiveness Were a priori cut-
offs established?
Was any aspect of FOI reported? If so, what? Was relationship
between FOI and
participant
outcomes
assessed?
Learning to
BREATHE
CC - Yes

LM -No
Metz et. al. (2013) Teachers Design: QED pre-
post, with instruction-
as-usual comparison
Level: School
IU: 1 school
Total #: 6 thematic
lessons broken up and
delivered over 18
sessions
Weeks 16
Session Length 15–25
minutes at beginning of
class
Measures. teacher logs
(unclear # of items; at each
lesson, however very few
completed) & observations by
program staff (unclear number
of items - checklist; 5% of all
sessions)
Measures. teacher logs
(unclear number of items) &
observations by program staff
(teacher enthusiasm and
preparedness)
Unclear if Assessed Measures. Observations
(qualitative)
Not Reported Adherence/Quality/Responsiveness: Descriptive statement:
"Observations indicated lesson adherence, teacher,
enthusiasm and preparedness and high student engagement"

No teacher logs reported
No
Broderick & Metz
(2009)
Students Design: QED pre-
post, with portion of
junior class as
comparison
Level: Classroom
IU: 1 school - 7
sections of health
class
Total #: 6 lessons per
group (7 groups)
Weeks: @ 5 (could be
as few as 3)
Session Length: 32–43
minutes
Not Reported Not Reported Measures. How oftern
practiced mindfulness
outside of class
(qualitative and then
catgorized as 4 or more
days/week, once a month
to 3 days a week, and
none)
Not Reported Not Reported Dosage. 65% of students reported practicing some
mindfulness techniques otuside of class

For those practicing 4 or more days per week outside of class, compared to those who only practiced in class, overall
somatic complaints were reduced & specific somatic
complaints of dizziness and feeling over-tired increased.
Yes
Inner Kids
Program
CC- No

LM - No
Flook et al. (2010) Students Design: RCT with
active reading period
control
Level: Student with
block randomization
stratified by
classroom, gender,
and age
IU: 32 students
Total #: 16 sessions
Weeks: 8
Session Length: 30
minutes
Not Reported Not Reported Not Reported Not Reported Not Reported Adherence. Number of sessions delivered No
Mindfulness
Education (ME)
Program
CC-Yes

LM - No
Schonert-Reichl and
Lawlor (2010)
Teachers Design: QED, pre-
post

Level Classroom

IU: 6 Classrooms
Total # 9–10 lessons +
(daily mindfulness
exercises 3 times a day
for up to 3 minutes)

Weeks: 9–10 (final
week optional)

Session Length: 40–50
minutes
Measures. Teacher Daily
Diary track daily
implementation of core
exercises; extent to which
implemented program lessons
each week, and # of ways
integrated into classroom
curriculum and practices
Not Reported Not Reported

(although classify the
adherence measures to
the left as "dosage")
Not Reported Not Reported Adherence. Mean and range across lessons: teachers reported
implementing components of lessons 75% of time, indicating
a moderate to high level of average implementation

Average proportion of program core mindful exercises
(breathing practices) completed by week. Included a table for
this. Range of implementation of core exercises was 73%–
100% with an average of 87% across 9 weeks.

100% of teachers reported that they implemented extension
activities within classrooms (not clear what this means)
No
Holistic Life
Foundation
CC- No

LM - Yes
Mendelson et al.
(2010)
Students Design: RCT w/ wait-
list control (not
active)

Level: School

IU: 2 elementary
schools
Total #: 48 sessions

Weeks: 12

Session Length: 45
minutes
Not Reported Not Reported Measures. Student
Attendance (but don't
outline how assessed)
Not Reported Not Reported Dosage. Percent of students at each school who attended at
least 75% of sessions. 73.5% at one school and 40% of
students at another. Teacher focus group data indicated that
some teachers prevented students from attending as form of
punishment for poor in-class behavior
No
Feagans Gould et al.
(2012)
Students Design: RCT w/ wait-
list control (not
active)

Level: School

IU: 2 elementary
schools
Total #: 48 sessions

Weeks: 12

Session Length: 45
minutes
Not Reported Not Reported Not Reported Not Reported Not Reported Not Reported No
CARE:
Cultivating
Awareness &
Resilience in
Education
CC- Yes

LM - Yes
Jennings et al. (2013) Teachers Design: RCT

Level: Teacher

IU 27 teachers
Total #: 5 full-day
sessions, plus 2
coaching calls, plus
local group support
activities

Weeks: approx. 12
weeks

Session Length:
Varied: Full-day
sessions (6 hours);
Coaching calls (20–30
minutes)
Measures. Facilitators Record
sheet completed at end of each
session by facilitator and trained
observer. Don't say anything
specific about number of items
whether quant or qual or how
assessed at all.
Not Reported Not Reported Not Reported Not Reported Adherence. Desriptive statement: "Because the facilitators
were working directly from the materials they created, the
program was delivered with a high degree of fidelity (100%)"

However, do not report what measures were comprised of
or how compiled and cross-validated to get at 100% fidelity
No
Jennings et al. (2011) Teachers   Study 1
Design: Single group
pre-post

Level: Teacher

IU: 31

  Study 2
Design: RCT

Level: Classroom
(student teacher/
mentor teacher pairs)

IU: 21
Total #: 4 or 5 full-day
sessions, plus 2
coaching calls

Weeks: approx. 5
weeks

Session Length:
Varied: Full-day
sessions; Coaching calls
(20–30 minutes)
Not Reported Not Reported Not Reported Not Reported Not Reported Not Reported No
mMBSR
(modified
mindfulness-
based stress
reduction)
CC- No

LM - No
Flook et al. (2013) Teachers Design: RCT with
wait-list control - 4
schools total

Level:
Classroom/teacher

IU: 10
teachers/classrooms
Total #: 9 sessions (26
hours total)

Weeks: 8

Session Length: 2.5
hours per week for 8
weeks plus a day-long
immersion for 6 hours
Not Reported Not Reported Measures. Weekly
practice logs in which
participants recorded
Mindfulness practice
compliance (or number of
minutes per day spent
engaging in formal (e.g.
sitting meditation) and
informal (e.g. brief
moments of mindfulness)
mindfulness practice.
Not Reported Not Reported Dosage. Reported the average minutes per day in formal and
informal practice as well as frequency of mindfulness
practice.
Specifically, participants reported spending on average 21.7
min (SD=13.8) per day in formal practice and 7.5 min
(SD=4.7) per day in informal practice. During 8-week
course, participats reported engaging in formal practice
83.7% of days (M=46.9; SD=7.1) and informal practice
88.7% of days (M=49.7; SD=4.4).
No
MM
(Mindfulness
Meditation)
CC- No

LM - No
Beauchemin et al.
2008
Both Students &
Teachers
Design: Single
group, pre-post

Level: Classroom

IU 4 classes (2
teachers & 34
students)
Total # approx. 27

Weeks: 5

Session Length 5–10
minutes at beginning of
class period (# of class
periods per day not
specified) plus two 20
minute instructional
sessions.
Not Reported Not Reported Not Reported Not Reported Not Reported Not Reported No
Cultivating
Emotional
Balance
CC- No

LM - No
Kemeny et al. (2012) Teachers Design: RCT, pre-
post, 5-month follow-
up

Level: Teacher

IU: 41 teachers
Total #: 4 sessions

Weeks 8

Session Length: 4 All-
Day & 4 Evening
sessions (total of 42
hours of training);
recommended 25
min/day home practice
Not Reported Not Reported Measures. Weekly online
self-reported logs to assess
# of minutes of meditation
practiced each day.
Created varaible: total
days mediated 20 min or
more across 8-week
period.
Not Reported No Dosage. The greater the number of days individuals reported
practing meditation (20 min/day or more), the lower their trait
anxiety and the higher their mindfulness at posttraining, but
these did not occur with other self-report measures.

Greater meditation practice associated with diminished blood
pressure reactivity during lab task, compared with those who
practiced less, & decreased Diastolic Blood Pressure during
speech and math portions of Trier Social Stress Test at follow-
up as well as decreased Respiratory Sinus Arrhythmia in
response to the math task at follow-up

Greater mediation practice was not associated with
compassionate responding or social behavior on marital task.
Yes
MIL: Moving
into Learning
CC- Yes

LM - No
Klatt et al. (2013) Students Design: Single
group, pre-post &
follow-up

Level: Classroom

IU: 2 classrooms (41
students)
Total #: 8 weekly
sessions; 32 daily
sessions

Weeks: 8

Session Length: 45
minutes for weekly
sessions; 15 minutes for
daily sessions
Not Reported Not Reported Not Reported Not Reported No No No
Inner Resilience
Program (IRP)
CC- No

LM - No
Lantieri et al. (2011) Both Students &
Teachers
Design: RCT

Level: Classroom
across many schools
(unsure #)

IU: 29 teachers &
471 students in their
classrooms
Total #: 27 weekly yoga
sessions; 9 monthly
NTIL sessions; 1
weekend-long retreat

Weeks 27–36 weeks
across components

Session Length:
Weekly yoga (75
minutes); Monthly NTIL
meetings (2.5 hours
each); 2-day weekend
residential retreat
Not Reported Not Reported Not Reported Not Reported No No No
SMART-in-
Education
(Stress
Management &
Relaxation
Techniques)
Program
CC- Yes

LM - Yes
Roeser et al. 2013 Teachers Design: RCT, with 3-
month follow-up

Level: Teacher

IU: 54 Teachers
Total #: 11

Weeks: 8

Session Length:
Doesn't say, but total of
36 contact hours across
11 sessions
Not Reported

(although assert adherence
was controlled for because
program developer
implemented at all sites)
Measures. Evaluation survey
participants filled out at end
of program. Instructor
domain-specific expertise,
genuineness, effectiveness at
presenting material, and
trustworthiness on 5-pt.
Likert scale.
Measures. Facilitator-
reported attendance at
weekly sessions as well as
teacher completion of
program.
Daily mindfulness practice
journal. Teachers self-
reported minutes of daily
practice
Not Reported Yes.

Program completer
had to attend at
least 8 of the 11
sessions.

Suggested 15/min
a day of home
practice.
Dosage. Those who didn't drop out attended 92% of sessions.
Absences ranged from 0–4 with 87% of participants
completed the program by attending 8 or more of the 11
sessions.

Amount of home practice examined for the 60% of
participants who returned daily practice journals. Teachers
reported avg. of 16 min. of practice/day (Canadian sample)
and 15 min. of practice/day (U.S. sample). This showed
compliance w/ 15-min a day home practice

Quality. On average, participants "strongly agreed" that
instructor demonstrated good knowledge of the subject matter
(expert knowledge, M=4.98, SD=.14); was a "good role
model for what was being taught" (genuineness, M= 4.94,
SD=.24), was "effective in presentation of material"
(effectiveness, M=4.83; SD=.38), and participants
"developed a faith in their ability to trust & learn from the
instructor" (trustworthiness, M=4.88, SD=.48). Instructions
for home practice very clear and useful.
No
CC- No

LM - No
Benn et. al. (2012) Teachers Design: RCT with 2-
month follow-up

Level:
Teacher/Parent

IU: 31 participants
(12 parents and 19
educators)
Total #: 11 sessions (2
times per week for a
total of 36 hours)

Weeks: 5

Session Length: 2.5
hours ( 9 sessions) & 6
hours (2 sessions)
Measures. Research assistant
observed sessions and provided
qualitative feedback on program
fidelity (instructor adherence to
format, content, and process of
delivery) during weekly
research meetings.
Measures. Participant
responses to open-ended
questions on individual
session evaluations and
ratings of overall instructor
quality at the conclusion of
the program.
Measures. Program
completion and
attendance. Unclear what
determines program
completion.
Participant-reported
estimates of frequency of
home practice.
Not Reported No Dosage. Results showed that all but 1 participant competed
the MT program and all attended most of the sessions (M=9.9
sessions, range 7–11 sessions).

Quality. Participants indicated high levels of satisfaction with
the program in terms of quality of instruction, content, and
structure. They rated the level of instruction as either a 4 or 5
on a 5-point scale.

Adherence. Qualitative reports by RAs suggest high-quality
instructor adherence to the format, content, and process of
curriculum delivery.

Participants reported an average of 10 minutes of formal
mindfulness home practice per day.
No
MBSR adapted
for urban youth
CC- No

LM - No
Sibinga et al. (2013) Students Design: RCT, with 3-
month follow-up,
active control - health
education program

Level: Student

IU: 1 school (22
students)
Total #: 12 sessions

Weeks: 12

Session Length: 50
minutes
Not Reported Not Reported Not Reported Not Reported No No No
Adapted MBSR
Program
CC- No

LM - No
Frank et al. (2013) Teachers Design QED, pre-
post

Level: School

IU 1 School - 18
instructors,
specialists, and
administrators
Total # 8

Weeks: 8

Session Length: 2
hours (weekly sessions)
plus 25–30 min of daily
practice (at home) 6
days/wk.
Not Reported Not Reported Measures. Daily logs - no
other details on what
those are.
Not Reported No Dosage. MBSR participants practiced mindfulness meditation
outside class on average 4 times per week (M=3.9; SD=1.5)
for a mean of 22.6 minutes (SD=4.6) per practice period over
the 8 week course.
No
Transformative
Life Skills (TLS)
CC- No

LM - No
Frank et al. (2014) Teachers Design Single
group, pre-post

Level: Student

IU: 49 students
Total # 48 lessons

Weeks: Approximately
12

Session Length: 30
minutes (3–4 days a
week)
Measures. Instructor-reported
lesson component completion
checklist at the end of each
lesson. Say supervision of
instructors by program
developers, observation, and
review of these checklists were
used to monitor fidelity
throughout implementation.
Measures. Instructor-
reported reflection on quality
of lesson implementation.
Not Reported Measures. Instructor-reported
overall level of student
engagement (as a whole not
per student) at the end of each
lesson.
No Overall fidelity: All lessons were implemented with greater
than 80% fidelity (not sure how calculated - if refers to
adherence or adherence & quality)
No
Mindfulness
Meditation
(MM) Program
CC- No

LM - No
Wisner (2013) Students Design Single group

Level: Student

IU: 35 Students in a
single alternative
high school (total
enrollment of school
was 36 students)
Total #: 29 sessions

Weeks 8

Session Length: Varied
(1 30-min intro session,
plus 30-min sessions 2
times per week; plus 10
minute sits 2 times per
week in weeks 3–8)
Not Reported Not Reported Measures. Participant
reported use of practice
CD and home practice.
Not Reported No Dosage. Ten out of 35 (approx. 30%) students reported using
practice CD at home, with most of these students using the
CD once, twice, or three times. One student used CD on
regular basis. Five out 35 students (approx. 15%) reported
that they practicd meditation at home without the CD and 2
students reported using meditation on a regular basis while 3
students reported trying meditation on two or three
occassions.
No
Wisner et al. (2013) Students Design: Single
group, pre-post

Level: student

IU: 28 students from
1 alternative high
school (78% of
student body)
Total #: 29 sessions

Weeks: 8

Session Length: Varied
(1 30-min intro session,
plus 30-min sessions 2
times per week; plus 10
minute sits 2 times per
week in weeks 3–8)
Not Reported Not Reported Not Reported Not Reported No No No
RISE Program CC- No

LM - No
Winzelberg & Luskin
(1999)
Teachers Design: RCT, with 8-
week follow-up

Level: Teacher

IU: Unclear - but
probably 8 teachers
in training (6 couldn't
attend training so
assigned to wait-list
control; remaining 15
randomized to exp.
Or control)
Total #: 4

Weeks: 4

Session Length 45-
minutes
Not Reported Not Reported Measures. Self-report
questionnaire administered
at follow-up (8 weeks
after). Asked frequency
with which practiced
techniques in an average
week during the program
and at the time of follow-
up (both for mediation and
the 3 corollary techniques)
Not Reported No Dosage. During program, participants reported practicing
meditation an average of 3 times/week. At follow-up, 1/2 of
the participants reported they were no longer practicing the
meditation, but most were still practicing the corollary
techniiques. They reported remimbering to "slow down" and
"do one thing at a time" several times a week. Overall, use of
all techniques decreased from an average of 13.4 at post-test
to 9.1 at follow-up.

Includes a Table of treatment group practice frequency per
week over the course of the program.
No
Mindful Schools
(K-5
Curriculum)
CC- No

LM - No
Black & Fernando
(2013)
Students (with
small Teachers
component)
Design: RCT (no
control) either MS or
MS + 7 additional
sessions

Level Classroom

IU: 17 classrooms
total (409 students)
Total #: 15 (MS) or 22
(MS +); brief (2 min)
practices on non-session
school days

Weeks: 5 (MS) or 12
(MS +)

Session Length: 15
min., 3 times/week
(once weekly for
additional 7 weeks MS
+). 2-min short practices
on all other school days
Not Reported Not Reported Not Reported Not Reported No No No
Liehr and Diaz
(2010)
Students Design: RCT w/
Health Education
control

Level: Student

IU: 9 students
Total #: 10

Weeks 2

Session Length: 15
minutes of MS
curriculum plus 20
minutes of time to "shift
from previous activities
and document
presence."
Not Reported Not Reported Not Reported Not Reported No No No
Attention
Academy
Program (AAP)
CC- No

LM - No
Napoli et al. (2005) Students Design: RCT

Level: Student

IU: 114 students
(across 9 classrooms)
Total # 12

Weeks: 24

Session Length: 45
minutes
Not Reported Not Reported Measures. Student
attendance in both
experimental and control
conditions.
Not Reported Yes.

Program completer
had to attend 12
sessions. Control
participants had to
attend 12 control
sessions.
Dosage/Completers. Thirty-four students (approx. 15%)
missed more than one training/control group session and were
excluded from analysis. A total of 194 students completed the
program (94 experimental and 97 control).
No
Standardized
Meditation
Program
CC- No

LM - No
Anderson et al.
(1999)
Teachers Design: RCT, with
follow-up (4-weeks
post)

Level: Teacher

IU 45 teachers
Total #: 6 (5 weekly
and 1 follow-up) plus
40-minutes a day of
mediation practice.

Weeks: 5 weeks

Session Length: 1.5
hours for weekly
sessions, 2, 20-minute
daily mediations, and 1
hour for follow-up
session
Not Reported Not Reported Measures. Participant
teachers completed a
questionnaire during each
of the 6 sessions that asked
them to estimate how
many times they had
meditated during the week.
Not Reported No Dosage. 60% of participating teachers reported meditating at
least 6 times/wk and 40% reported 2–5 times/week
No
Mindfulness
Workbook
(Seymour N.B.
Mack's Top
Secret Detective
Manual)
CC- No

LM - No
Reid & Miller (2009) Students (with
teacher delivering
workbook - called
"inspector
connectors")
Design: Single
group, pre-post

Level: Academic
Summer program

IU 24 students and 4
teachers (leading 2
groups of 12 kids
each)
Total #:Varies (24–30
sessions recommended)

Weeks 6

Session Length: Not
reported (and may vary
based on teacher leeway
to use workbook as
deem appropriate)
Not Reported Not Reported Not Reported Not Reported No No No
Transcendental
Meditation (TM)
CC- No

LM - No
Nidich et. al. (1986) Unclear Design: Single
group, pre-post

Level: Student

IU: 75 students (37
incoming students
and 38 continuing
students)
Total #: Unclear

Weeks: Unclear

Session Length: a few
minutes in morning and
few minutes in afternoon
Not Reported Not Reported Not Reported Not Reported No No No
Gelderloos et al.
(1987)
Unclear Design: QED, single
time point design
with Montessori
school as comparison

Level: School

IU 1 School (48
students)
Total # Unclear

Weeks Unclear

Session Length: a few
minutes in morning and
few minutes in afternoon
Not Reported Not Reported Not Reported Not Reported No No No
CC- No

LM - No
Rosaen & Benn
(2006)
Students Design: Single
group, qualitative
assessment

Level Student

IU: 10 students
Total #: Unclear - every
school day for 12
months

Weeks: approx. 52

Session Length: 10
minutes (twice a day
each school day)
Not Reported Not Reported Not Reported Not Reported No No No
Transcendental
Meditation (TM)
CC- No

LM - No
Barnes et al. (2001) Students Design: RCT, with
active, HE control
condition

Level: student

IU: 15 students
Total #: approx. 40 in-
school sessions plus
approx. 72 at-home
sessions

Weeks approx. 8

Session Length: 15
minutes
Not Reported Not Reported Measures. Attendance at
school sessions for both
experimental and control
group.
Self-reported compliance
with TM home practice.
Unclear number of items
or how asked.
Not Reported No Dosage. Average attendance of the TM group was 67.8%
while average attendance for control group was 68.2%.

Percentage of students attending at least 60% of sessions was
80% for TM group and 58% for control group.

Average self-reported compliance with TM practice at home
was 76.6%
No
Elder et al. (2011) Students Design: QED, pre-
post

Level: Student

IU: 68 students
Total # Not specified

Weeks: approximately 16

Session Length: Varies
- An hour for the initial
set of sessions and then
personal practice 10–15
minutes morning and
afternoon every school
day
Not Reported Not Reported Not Reported Not Reported No No No
Breathing
Awareness
Mediation
(BAM)
CC- No

LM - No
Gregoski et al. (2011) Students Design: RCT, with
active LST (Life
Skills Training) and
HE (Health
Education) Control
conditions

Level: School (to
treatment group &
Classroom (one
teacher per semester
randomly assigned to
teach intervention.

IU: 53 students
Total #: 108 (Weekly
health class plus home
practice each weekday
and twice daily on
weekends).

Weeks: approx. 12

Session Length: 10
minutes each
Not Reported Measures. Instructor
thoroughness and enthusiasm
assessed weekly by single
rater using Likert scale
ratings (0–4 scale). 1 item for
thoroughness and 1 item for
enthusiasm.

Also rated Control and LST
instructors on these.

(fairly certain enthusiasm is
for instructor - small
possibility it could be about
students - not fully clear
from write -up)
Measures. Attendance &
Self-reported compliance
of home practice.

Also measured Control
and LST conditions on
attendance
Measures. Class attentiveness
assessed weekly by single
rater using Likert scale ratings
(0–4 scale). 1 item for
attentiveness.

Also rated Control and LST
instructors on this.
No Dosage. For BAM group - Average in-school attendance was
79% of total sessions. For all conditions - statistical
differences observed for attendance between two schools
(77% vs. 90%, p=.01). These differences were primarily due
to bomb threats and fire alarm activations. Attendance was
not statistically different by treatment group (p=.52) and the
group by school interaction was non-significant (p=.46)
Self-reported home compliance for home practice was 86.6%
+/− 7.4%

Quality & Responsiveness.
All instructors were rated as competent in implementing the
various components throughout the intervention: average of
ratings (on scale of 0–4) were 3.34 +/− 0.26 for thoroughness;
3.28 +/− 0.32 for class attentiveness; & 3.31 +/− 0.27 for
enthusiasm. No sign. differences between treatment groups,
schools, teachers, or interactions of these factors observed for
any components (all p's > .05)
No
Barnes et al. (2008) Students Design: RCT

Level: School

IU: 20 students in 1
high school
Total #: Unclear

Weeks: approx. 12

Session Length: 10
minutes
Not Reported Not Reported Measures. Attendance Not Reported No Dosage. Self-reported home compliance for home practice
was 86.6% +/− 7.4%

Examined sodium handling excluding subjects with less than
70% attendance and adjusting for baseline values of
attendance (BAM, n=11; Control, n=28), overnight urinary
sodium excretion rate decreased from pre-to post-intervention
in the BAM group but increased in the control group (−1.6+/−
1.1 vs 1.5+/− 0.7 mEq/hr, p < .03) as did overnight urine
sodium content (−1.1+/− 0.7 vs 8+/− 0.4g, p < .03).
Yes
Barnes et al. (2004) Students Design: RCT with
active, HE control

Level: Classroom

IU: 34 students in 2
classrooms in same
school
Total # approx. 60 in-
school sessions and 84
at-home practice
sessions; 12 instructor
sessions

Weeks approx. 12

Session Length: 10-
minutes for practice
time in-school and
home; 20-minutes/week
with instructor
discussing
Not Reported Not Reported Measures.
Teacher/instructor
recorded daily attendance
of students at sessions and
individual meditation
practice at home.
Attendance and home
practice (which was 20-
minute daily walks) also
collected for control
group.
Not Reported No Dosage. The average attendance of the meditation group was
88.5% and the control group 86%.

The average self-reported compliance with meditation
practice at home was 86%.
No
Meditation
Practice (no
formal name)
CC- No

LM - No
Linden (1973) Students Design: RCT with
two control
conditions (guidance
group and no interv)

Level: student

IU 30 students in 1
elementary school
Total #: 36 (twice a
week)

Weeks: 18

Session Length 20–25
minutes
Not Reported Not Reported Not Reported Measures. Clinical
observations as qualitative
evidence that the independent
variable "took."
No Responsiveness. Descriptive Statement: "Many of the
Subjects were unaccepting of the instructions at first or
seemed to fear being judged "silly" if they accepted
them…gradually the groups' nomr shifted from curiosity and
hesitancy to approval and anticipation of the instructions. As
the sessions continued, the subjects seemed to do the
exercises more readily. It is likely that in addition to the
experimenter's demand, the subjects sensed that their
neighbors really were engaged in something they wished to
continue doing undisturbed. Had the new group norm not
become operative, the effectiveness of the mediation practice
would have been nil or severely limited."
No
Youth
Empowerment
Seminar (YES!)
CC- No

LM - No
Ghahremani et al
(2013)
Students Design: QED, pre-
post

Level: Classroom

IU: 327 students in 3
schools (# classrooms
not reported)
Total # 20 lessons

Weeks: 4

Session Length: 60
minutes
Not Reported Not Reported Not Reported Not Reported No No No
Tai Chi
curriculum,
augmented by
MBSR
CC- No

LM - No
Wall (2005) Students Design Single
group, qualitative

Level Student

IU 14 students
Total #: 5 (once per
week)

Weeks: 5

Session Length: 60
minutes
Not Reported Not Reported Not Reported Not Reported No No No
Mindfulness-
Based Youth
Suicide
Prevention
Intervention in a
Native American
Community
CC- No

LM - No
Le & Gobert (2013) Students Design Single
group, pre-post

Level: Student

IU: 8 students
Total #: 36 sessions

Weeks: 9

Session Length: 55
minutes
Not Reported Not Reported Not Reported Measures. Facilitators
reported weekly (via open-
ended personal reflection) on
what group dynamic was and
what contributed to the
dynamics, what activities
worked and why, what
experiences, events or
participants stood out, what
helped me to be effective and
to connect with youth.
No No No
Mindfulness-
based
Intervention for
Chronically Ill
Youth
CC- No

LM - No
Lagor et al. (2013) Students Design: Single
group, pre-post

Level: 1 school

IU: 15 students
(entire student
population)
Total # 6

Weeks 6

Session Length: 50
minutes
Measures. Clinical notes and
records kept
Not Reported Measures. Attendance
records kept at sessions.
Outside practice assessed
via semi-structured
interviews with
participants after
intervention.
Qualitatively mentioned in
discussion section. Unclear
source of data.
Yes.

Treatment
completers were
defined as those
who attended at
least 4 of 6 (66%)
of clinical sessions.
Analyses were
only conducted on
treatment
completers.
Dosage/Completers. 13 out of 15 participants were
"Treatment completers" - those defined as attending at least 4
of 6 sessions. Average attendance rate was 85%.

Adherence/Adaptation. First clinical session delayed by 1
week. Slight adjustments made to session content based on 50-
minute sessions (as opposed to curriculum which outlined 60
min sessions), Adaptations: To maximize continuity between
sessions and catch up students who missed, each session
began with a review of the previous sessions material.
No
Yoga Ed
(modified
version)
CC- No

LM - No
Khalsa et al. (2012) Students Design: RCT

Level: Class

IU: 4 classes (74
students total)
Total #: ranged from 23–
32 sessions

Weeks: 11

Session Length: 30–40
minutes long,
Not Reported Not Reported Measures. Participant
attendance at sessions.
Not Reported No Dosage. Reported the number of participants attending at
least 1 yoga session (73 out of 74 participants); average
number of sessions attended for all participants (M=20.5
;SD=7.7) as well as for those with approx. 2 sessions per
week (M=18.0; SD=5.1) and those with approx. 3 sessions
per week (M=23.7 (SD=9.2); & percentage of available
sessions attended (80% at the beginning of the yoga program
and declined to just under 70% by the end).

Adaptation. Reported the number of sessions cancelled due to
school events - 6 different days.
Yes
Yoga Ed CC- No

LM - No
Steiner et al. (2013) Students Design Single
group, pre-post

Level Student

IU: 37 Students
Total #: approx. 28

Weeks: approx. 14

Session Length: 60
minutes
Measures. Instructor-report of
time spent on each of the 4 main
components of curriculum at
each session.
Not Reported Measures. Session
attendance forms in which
instructors tracked
participant attendance
(including excuses for
absences)
Measures. Instructor-reported
"group dynamics" and
individual participant
engagement for each of the 4
curricular components using
categories:"engagement,"
"medium engagement," or
"need for redirection."
No Dosage. On average, students attendended 90% of sessions.

Responsiveness. Students were engaged for the majority (78%) of poses.

Adherence. "Fidelity was ensured because of experienced
yoga instructors following Yoga Ed curriculum, as well as
instructor-rated adherence." No supporting data or methods
to back up statement.
No
Kripalu-based
Yoga Program
CC- Yes

LM - No
Noggle et al. (2012) Students Design: RCT, active
control (PE as usual)

Level: Student

IU: 36 students
within 3 PE classes
Total #: 28 sessions

Weeks 10 weeks

Session Length: 30–40
minutes
Not Reported

(assert assessed adherence,
but does not fit this definition)
Not Reported Measures. Participant
attendance at sessions.
Yoga Evaluation
Questionnaire (YEQ)
asked if students used yoga
skills at school or home on
a 10-cm. visual analgue
scale on which mark
degree of agreement from
"not at all" to "very much
so". Not sure how many
items.
Not Reported No Dosage. Central tendencies of attendance rates for
experimental condition (Mean =58%; =/- 26% SD; Median =
64%, and Mode = 75%). Range of attendance (0% - 93%).
Attendance less than 25% of sessions for 7 of 36 students.

Qualitatively report range of answers for outside use.
Specifically, when asked whether yoga was helpful or
whether they used any yoga skills at school and home,
responses were scattered more evenly across scale (data not
shown) indicating perhaps not all students who liked yoga
were applying it outside of class.

Examined correlation between attendance rates and all
outcome measures and NONE were correlated
Yes
Conboy et al. (2013) Students Design: RCT, active
control (PE as usual)

Level: Student

IU: approx. 56 -
because say selected
half to include in this
study
Total #: 32 sessions

Weeks 12 weeks

Session Length: 30
minutes
Not Reported Not Reported Not Reported Not Reported No No No
Get Ready to
Learn (GRTL)
CC- No

LM - No
Koenig et al. (2012) Students Design QED, pre-
post

Level: Classroom

IU: 4 classrooms (24
students)
Total # 80 sessions

Weeks: 16

Session Length: 15–20
minutes
Measures. FOI assessed using
checklist and videotaped
sessions. Checklist included 16-
pt. scale in five categories: 1)
classroom-environment, 2)
classroom organization and
setup, 3) program
implementation by the teacher,
4) DVD routine and student
support, and 5) GRTL program
conclusion. A score of 12–16
points indicates good program
implementation. Researchers
used checklists and video tapes
to reach 80% agreement on
categories. Once reliable,
observed classrooms directly in
4 intervention classrooms
(unclear how many times).
Unclear.

The categories outlined as
part of overall FOI could be
conceptualized as "quality"
but don't talk about it as
such.
Not Reported Not Reported Yes.

A score of 12–16
points on FOI
checklist indicated
"good program
implementation."
Overall Fidelity/Adherence. Classroom observations were
rated for fidelity, and all classes scored within the "good"
implementation range. Unclear which dimensions used to
construct Overall Fidelity.

Reliability. Raters achieved 100% agreement on two
independent samples.
No
Yoga Fitness for
Kids (Gaiam,
2003)
CC- No

LM - No
Peck et al. (2005) Students Design: QED,
multiple baseline,
intervention, and
follow-up periods
with convenience
comparison group

Level: Grade-level

IU: 10 students (3 in
Grade 1, 3 in Grade
2, and 3 in Grade 3)
Total #: 6 (2 X per
week)

Weeks 3

Session Length: 30
minutes
Measures. Treatment Integrity
Checklist completed by data
collector. Intervention
components checked off if
completed as intended. These
included 2 adherence items: 1)
all participants dressed
appropriately, and 2) researcher
played yoga videotape and
participants followed along with
deep breaths, physical postures,
and relaxation exercises.
Not Reported Measures. Attendance for
group recorded via
Treatment Integrity
Checklist completed by
data collector. Single Item:
all participants in grade
level group were present at
session.
Not Reported No Dosage/Adherence. It was determined that all elements of the
intervention were implemented with 100% accuracy. This is
what is termed "Treatment Integrity" which is composed of
the 3 items (2 adherence and 1 dosage) but, don't say how
determined or calculated this.
No
Yoga Program CC- No

LM - No
Hagins, Haden, Daly
(2013)
Students Design: RCT, with
PE control

Level Student

IU: 15 students
Total #: approx. 30-45
(says 3 X per week in
one place and 2 X per
week in another)

Weeks: 15

Session Length 50
minutes
Not Reported Not Reported Measures. Student
attendance at each session.
Assessed in both Yoga and
PE Control groups.
Measures. "Child
Engagement Index" created in
which Yoga & PE instructors
completed index on each child
twice within the trial period
(approx. 5 weeks and 10
weeks). 3-point scale
anchored by the terms
"minimal", "moderate", and
"maximum" engagement -
narrative text describing each.
Put form online via hyperlink
as supplemental material for
readers.
No Dosage. Mean attendance for yoga group was 26.87 classes
(SD=4.85); Mean attendance for PE group was 22.8 classes
(SD=7.36).

Responsiveness. Yoga Group: At Time 1 (5 weeks in) 10
students were maximally engaged, 4 were moderately
engaged, and 0 were minimally engaged. At Time 2 (10
weeks in), 8 students maximally engaged, 5 students
moderately engaged, and 0 minimally engaged.
PE Group At Time 1 (5 weeks in) 6 students were
maximally engaged, 8 were moderately engaged, and 0 were
minimally engaged. At Time 2 (10 weeks in), 8 students
maximally engaged, 5 students moderately engaged, and 0
minimally engaged. No sig differences in engagement
between groups.
No
Bent on
Learning
CC- No

LM - No
Berger et al. (2009) Students Design: QED, pre-
post

Level: After-school
program

IU: 39 students in 1
after-school program.
Total #: 12 (1 per
week)

Weeks: 12

Session Length 1 hour
Not Reported Not Reported Measures. Attendance for
each student (yoga group
only). Recorded by after-
school program teachers.
Also recorded if child
unable to participate due
to injury or if the yoga
teacher was absent.
Collected for 10 of the 12
classes.
Not Reported No Dosage. Children attended 68.5% (SD=21.6) of yoga classes.
This is an estimate as data were obtained for only 10 of 12
classes.
No
Mindful
Awareness for
Girls through
Yoga
CC- No

LM - No
White (2012) Students Design: RCT

Level School

IU: 1 School (190
students)
Total # 8 sessions
(plus 60 minutes of
homework
practice/week)

Weeks: 8

Session Length: 60
minutes weekly session
+ 10 min of HW 6
days/week
Unclear if Assessed

Measures. Study fidelity
maintained through Intervention
manual 2) journal kept by
interventionist c) an
intervention checklist monitored
by research assistants, d)
written instructions, and e)
homework with pictures and
audio instructions, and f)
feedback during sessions
Not Reported Measures. 1) Participant
attendance at sessions and
2) Self-reported home
practice of yoga.
However, measures not
described so don't know
number of items or
quant/qual.
Not Reported No Dosage. 1) Session attendance reported as a range (ranged
between 3–8 sessions) and percent of participants completing
all eight sessions (61.4%) & 2) Amount of home practice
(which is defined as part of "dosage": Average frequency
(10.8 times; SD=+/− 9.6). Ranged from 0–42 times

Amount of home practice (which is defined as part of
"dosage": Average frequency (10.8 times; SD=+/− 9.6).
Ranged from 0–42 times

Examined correlation between both participant dosage
variables and all outcome variables. 1 was significant. That is,
there was a positive correlation between home yoga practice
and perceived stress (r=.29, p< .05)
Yes

Program and study characteristics

The 48 studies included here evaluated the impact of 35 different mindfulness and yoga programs implemented in school settings. Of these 35 programs, 22 (63%) were primarily meditation-based; many of these were adapted from the standard MBSR program (Kabat-Zinn 1990). Eight programs (23%) were primarily yoga-based, focusing on physical postures (asanas), deep breathing, relaxation, and some meditation. The remaining 5 programs (14%) focused equally on meditation and yoga practices. Twenty-four programs (69%) targeted students, 8 programs (23%) targeted teachers, and 3 programs (8%) targeted both students and teachers.

The manner in which these programs were structured and delivered varied across the 48 studies. Specifically, the total number of sessions delivered ranged from 5 to 180 and the length of sessions ranged from “a few minutes” to weekend-long retreats. The most common session length was between 30 to 60 minutes (approximately one class-period). The intensity of program delivery varied from program components being delivered every school day to every couple of weeks. The shortest program duration (from start to end of program delivery) was 2 weeks while the longest duration was 12-months. Finally, programs utilized various session formats including individual sessions, group meetings and/or lessons, individual coaching calls, full-day long sessions, and weekend residential retreats.

Nineteen studies (40%) evaluated programs implemented in elementary schools, 8% in middle schools, 31% in high schools, and 10% across multiple K-12 school settings. Five studies (10%) did not report the grade levels in which programs were implemented. Thirty-five studies (73%) implemented programs during school hours, either integrated into classroom activities, during health class, physical education, a resource period, or briefly at the start or end of the school day. Eleven studies (23%), most of which targeted teachers, implemented programs outside school hours, either directly after school, in the evenings, or on weekends. Two studies (4%)--both programs targeting students and teachers--implemented the student component during school hours and the teacher component outside school hours. Four of the studies (8%) implemented programs either during summer camp within a school setting or during summer teacher professional development.

In addition to program and implementation differences, there was variation in study designs and sample sizes. Of the 48 studies included, 26 (54%) were experimental designs or randomized control trials (RCTS), 13 (27%) were quasi-experimental (QEDS) and 10 (21%) were single-group designs (the total number of study designs equals 49 because one article (Jennings et al. 2011) included a larger study comprised of two sub-studies). Sample sizes ranged from 8 to 409. Three-fourths (or 75%) of studies had a total sample size of less than 100. Most studies were implemented in 1 or 2 schools or a few classrooms, although several studies implemented a program in more than 15 classrooms (Black & Fernando 2013; Lantieri et al. 2011), suggesting variation in scope of program implementation.

Specification of program core components and their association with relevant outcomes

Most often, potential program core components were not clearly articulated in studies. Almost all of the studies provided a general description of program content by summarizing the major lesson themes or content in the order taught, the instructional or pedagogical techniques used to engage participants in learning, the key practices taught (e.g. awareness of breathing or physical postures), and/or the overall program goals. Many programs were described as being “adapted” from more established interventions such as MBSR (Kabat-Zinn 1990), Semple’s work (Semple et al. 2005), or Mind-Body Awareness which combines aspects of MBSR and Social Emotional Learning (SEL). For these adapted programs, many studies described the program in terms of how they differed or were adapted from the original program. Only a handful of studies identified program components in terms of being “key,” “core,” or “essential.” There were no studies that formally distinguished potential structural core components from process core components.

While the majority of studies outlined a general theory of anticipated programmatic impacts based on the effects of mindfulness or yoga programs more broadly, only 3 (6%) published or referenced a logic model or theory of change (Jennings et al. 2013; Mendelson et al. 2010; Roeser et al. 2013). In addition, only a handful of studies included more specified programmatic theory – that is, theory specifying the rationale for inclusion of specific program components and how those components were intended to produce specific outcomes or contribute to participant engagement. Not surprisingly, these were also the studies that distinguished program components in terms of being “key” or “core.” These programs and studies included: Learning to BREATHE ((Metz et al, 2013), Mindfulness Education (ME) Program (Schonert-Reichl & Lawlor 2010), Cultivating Awareness and Resilience in Education (CARE) (Jennings et. al. 2013), Moving into Learning (MIL) (Klatt et al. 2013), SMART-in-Education Program (Roeser et al. 2013), and a Kripalu-based Yoga Program (Noggle et al. 2012).

The Cultivating Awareness and Resilience in Education (CARE) program (Jennings et al. 2010; Jennings et al. 2013) and the SMART-in-Education Program (Roeser et al. 2013) are noteworthy with respect to articulating program components and theoretical underpinnings. Jennings and her colleagues (2013) outlined a CARE intervention logic model that specifies the main program components and the proximal and long-term outcomes hypothesized to result from program implementation. Each of the three main components - Emotion Skills Instruction, Mindfulness Practices, and Compassion Practices – was described in terms of the rationale and empirical evidence behind its inclusion, the approximate percentage of the program devoted to it, as well as the specific kinds of activities delivered as part of each. Roeser and his colleagues outline very specific programmatic theory in terms of how mindful self-regulation skills and self-compassionate mind-sets for coping are hypothesized to impact specific mechanisms underlying regulation. They also outline the main program components in terms of teaching/pedagogical techniques and specific practices to facilitate experiential learning. In addition, their programmatic logic model includes program fidelity as an important facilitator of producing hypothesized program effects.

FOI rigor and reporting

Based on our criteria and coding, the majority of studies - 30 out of 48 or 63% of the studies reviewed -assessed at least one dimension of FOI. Nine studies (just under 20%) assessed 2 or 3 dimension of FOI. No study we reviewed assessed all 4 dimensions of FOI. Eighteen studies (37%) did not assess any aspect of FOI. Table 1 provides a summary of the number and percent of studies that assessed and reported FOI data in a rigorous manner.

Table 1.

Number and Percent of Reviewed Studies Collecting and Reporting FOI Data in Rigorous Manner for FOI Dimensions

FOI Dimension
Sub-Dimension
Studies
Measuring
Studies Using
Observational
Measures
Studies
Where
>1
Source
Used
Studies
Assessing
Reliability
or
Validity
Studies
Establishing
A-Priori
Cut-offs
Studies
Monitoring
Comparison
Condition
Studies
Reporting
Adaptations
Studies
Reporting
Level of
FOI
Studies
Linking
Aspect(s)
of FOI to
Outcomes
Program
Adherence
9 (19%) 7 (15%) 3 (6%) 1 (2%) 1 (2%) 0 (0%) 3 (6%) 8 (17%) 0 (0%)
Program
Quality
5 (10%) 2 (4%) 1 (2%) 0 (0%) 0 (0%) 1 (2%) 0 (0%) 4 (8%) 0 (0%)
Participant
Dosage
23 (48%) 1 (2%) 0 (0%) 0 (0%) 3 (6%) 5 (10%) 0 (0%) 22 (46%) 6 (13%)
    Session
Attendance
16 (33%) 1 (2%) 0 (0%) 0 (0%) 3 (6%) 5 (10%) 0 (0%) 15 (31%) 3 (6%)
    Outside
Practice
16 (33%) 0 (0%) 0 (0%) 0 (0%) 0 (0%) 1 (2%) 0 (0%) 15 (31%) 3 (6%)
Participant
Responsiveness
7 (15%) 3 (6%) 1 (2%) 0 (0%) 0 (0%) 2 (4%) 0 (0%) 5 (10%) 0 (0%)

The most commonly assessed dimension of FOI was participant dosage. Dosage was evaluated in two ways: participant attendance at program sessions and participant outside practice (i.e., the frequency of mindfulness practice at home or outside of formal program time). Almost half the studies (23 of 48) assessed one or both of these aspects of participant dosage. Fewer than 20% of studies assessed program adherence, program quality, or participant responsiveness (see Table 1 for greater detail).

Rigorous FOI assessment is also characterized by at least two rating sources for a single dimension, observational measures, testing of reliability and validity, a priori cut-offs for program delivery “as intended,” monitoring of control/comparison conditions, and reporting any adaptations made during program delivery. Nine studies (19%) used some kind of observational measure to assess an aspect of FOI, although only two studies (Koenig et al. 2012 & Peck et al. 2005) reported the number of items comprising an observational measure and/or how often observations were conducted. Five studies (10%) used more than one source of data to assess a single dimension of FOI, using both a self-report checklist for the intervention facilitator and an observational assessment, generally of program adherence. None evaluated the convergent validity of these measures. One study assessed the reliability of an observational measure across two independent coders (Koenig et al. 2012).

Four studies (8%) established cut-offs for some aspect of FOI. Three of the four studies defined “program completers” based on the number of sessions attended, specifying that participants must attend at least 66%, 73%, or 100% of sessions in order to qualify as a “program completer.” The other study (Koenig et al. 2012) established an a priori cut off for what “good” implementation would entail. This study used 5 categories to construct a 16-point scale on which a score of 12–16 indicates “good” implementation. Five studies (13% of studies including a control/comparison condition) assessed an aspect of FOI in both experimental and control conditions (Barnes et al. 2001; Barnes et al. 2004; Gregogski et al. 2011; Hagins et al. 2013; & Napoli et al. 2005). All five experimental studies assessed dosage, namely attendance, in both experimental and active control conditions. Two of the studies assessed participant responsiveness and one study instructor quality in both experimental and control conditions. Three studies (6%) reported adaptations made to program delivery (Jennings et al. 2011; Khalsa et al. 2012, Lagor et al. 2013). Program adaptations included modifying curriculum delivery to fit a 50-minute format rather than the originally designed 60-minute format, cancelling a number of sessions due to school events, and cancelling a training session due to a heavy snow storm and condensing that material into one of the final sessions.

The most common way to report participant dosage data was the average percent of lessons attended by participants or the percent of participants attending a certain proportion of lessons (e.g. over 75% or all lessons offered). Across studies these average attendance rates varied and variation was typically reported as a range or standard deviation around the mean. For outside practice most studies reported the average number of days per week or average number of minutes per day participants engaged in practice outside of class or at home. Several studies reported “compliance” meaning the percent of participants reporting that they complied with suggested guidelines for outside practice.

Adherence was generally reported quantitatively as an average and/or range of lessons or percent of lesson components implemented by instructors. The vast majority of studies that assessed adherence in this manner reported “moderate” to “high” fidelity – with “moderate” the label for 70–80% of lessons/content and “high” as being over 80% adherence. Numerous studies reported instructors implemented a program with “high fidelity” without any numerical quantification or qualification, including several studies that stated a program was implemented with “high” or 100% fidelity because the program was implemented by the program developers.

For responsiveness, most studies reported the average participant engagement or percent of participants “maximally,” “moderately,” or “minimally” engaged or the percent of poses in which participants were at least moderately engaged. Several studies reported the average class enthusiasm and attentiveness. Most studies have reported fairly good participant engagement – although one study looked at engagement over the course of a program, and engagement decreased slightly over time (Hagins et al. 2013). One study used qualitative data to describe the process of engagement occurring over course of program (Linden 1973). Program Quality was typically reported as average competency ratings for all instructors or average participant-reported quality, content, and structure of lessons.

FOI associations with program outcomes

Six studies (13%) examined the association between some aspect of FOI and at least one participant outcome. All six assessed an aspect(s) of participant dosage in relation to participant outcomes. In one study, for students practicing four or more days per week outside of class compared to those who only practiced in class, overall somatic complaints were reduced and specific somatic complaints of dizziness and feeling over-tired increased (Broderick & Metz 2009). In another study, teachers’ amount of meditation practice (operationalized as the number of days teachers reported practicing meditation 20 min or more) was associated with lower blood pressure reactivity and greater reductions in physiological arousal in response to threat, lower trait anxiety, and higher mindfulness, although not with social behavior in a marital task or compassionate responding (Kemeny et al. 2012).

Khalsa and colleagues examined correlations between session attendance at a yoga program and several outcome measures and found that, as hypothesized, better attendance was associated with greater well-being and life satisfaction (Khalsa et al. 2012). They also found a significant inverse correlation of attendance with anxiety and negative attitudes toward school. In another study, the effects of Breathing Awareness Meditation on sodium handling, an indicator of behavioral stress and a precursor to high blood pressure, in African American youth at risk for high blood pressure were only evident when they excluded participants attending less than 70% of sessions (Barnes et al. 2008). White (2012) examined the association between participant dosage variables (attendance and amount of home practice) and four outcome variables (stress, coping, self-esteem, and self-regulation). Findings indicated a positive correlation between home yoga practice and perceived stress. Not all studies assessing the relationship between dosage and outcomes, however, have reported significant associations (e.g., Noggle et al. 2012).

Discussion

The aim of this review was to assess the current state of science with respect to inclusion and reporting of FOI in studies of school-based mindfulness and yoga interventions in North America. The review was framed around the CORE process model, which emphasizes the iterative nature of developing and testing program theory using increasingly rigorous FOI assessment as research moves from the formative stage to large scale effectiveness trials (Feagans Gould et al. 2014; IES 2013). Consequently, it is important to consider the rigor of study designs as a whole, when evaluating the “rigor” of FOI to this point. The 48 studies reviewed here were predominantly small-scale efficacy studies to test the preliminary feasibility and potential outcomes of school-based mindfulness and/or yoga programs with sample sizes of less than 100 participants in a few classrooms or schools. To that end, the state of the field overall can be considered relatively preliminary or “emerging” (Greenberg & Harris 2012; Meiklejohn et al. 2012; Weare 2013).

Given the preliminary stage of research in this field as a whole, we were encouraged by several pockets of “rigor” within each of the criteria outlined. These included: 1) several programs outlined potential “core” program components and articulated the theoretical rationale behind their inclusion and their relation to hypothesized outcomes, 2) over half of the studies (63%) assessed at least one dimension of FOI, which most often entailed assessing an aspect of participant dosage – either attendance or outside practice, 3) just under 20% of studies utilized some kind of observational assessment to measure an aspect of FOI, 4) 10% of studies monitored an aspect of dosage within control/comparison conditions, and 5) 13% of studies examined the relationship between an aspect of FOI – in all cases participant dosage – and relevant outcomes and reported on both significant and non-significant findings. These elements of rigor indicate that there is some important groundwork being laid for the assessment and reporting of FOI for school-based mindfulness and yoga studies in North America.

More rarely occurring aspects of rigor (appearing in 6% or less of studies unless otherwise noted) included: 1) referencing a programmatic logic model, 2) distinguishing between structural and process program elements, 3) assessing other potentially meaningful dimensions of FOI - including program adherence, quality, and participant responsiveness - which were reported in less than 20% of published studies, 4) using multiple data sources for a single FOI construct, 5) examining reliability and validity of FOI measures, 6) establishing a priori cut-offs for “adequate” or “low” FOI, and 7) reporting any significant adaptations made to program delivery. These less frequently occurring areas of rigor represent important areas for continued focus, discussion and development.

In our view, the emerging state of the field presents a critical window of opportunity to increase the intentionality, theoretical reflection, and rigor of FOI for school-based mindfulness and yoga programming. Rigorous FOI is critical because evidence suggests that program fidelity leads to better outcomes (approximately 2 to 3 times the magnitude) and program outcomes are sensitive to variation in implementation fidelity (Durlak & DuPre 2008; Kutash et al. 2012). School contexts pose multiple challenges for consistent and high quality implementation of yoga and mindfulness programs (Mendelson et al. 2013). Therefore, without rigorous assessment of FOI, the evidence base upon which our conclusions rest will fall prey to many threats to internal validity including the potential to falsely conclude a program failed to find effects because its central components are ineffective, rather than poorly implemented (Domitrovich & Greenberg 2000).

Increased attention to FOI is timely because of the growing popularity and preliminary promising effects of school-based mindfulness and yoga programs. The field is now moving toward larger scale studies in which the number of classrooms and variation in implementation contexts will increase, making issues of implementation paramount both to the conclusions we can draw and our ability to understand how and under what conditions these programs may impact outcomes. The need for more rigorous FOI work is supported by the current review in which we found a relative paucity of rigorous FOI assessment and reporting as well as substantial variation in program characteristics, foci, and levels of FOI (when reported). In service of optimizing this window of opportunity, we offer below a series of recommendations for increasing both the intentionality and rigor of FOI for mindfulness and yoga programs implemented in school settings.

Recommendation 1: Clearly define core program components

Clearly articulating potential core program components and their underlying theoretical rationale, both within and across studies, is potentially the most important next step for the field. Only a handful of studies (10% of those reviewed) articulated potential core components and even fewer (6%) referenced or included a logic model or theory of action. Articulating potential core program components can, and should, be initiated even in formative studies because it defines the essential features of the program being delivered and provides a road-map for both FOI measures as well as training of facilitators to effectively deliver program curricula (Fixsen et al. 2005). Without clearly defining essential program components, researchers cannot develop and refine appropriate FOI measures. As studies move from small-scale efficacy to effectiveness and scale-up, potential core components provide guidance for what “should” be implemented across instructor, setting, and context and how variation in FOI of those components might relate to relevant participant outcomes. Consequently, researchers and program developers should reflect on a program’s essential features, articulate the theoretical and empirical rationale for their inclusion, and formally articulate how those components individually or in combination should relate to hypothesized proximal and distal outcomes, ideally in a formal logic model. The Cultivating Awareness and Resilience in Education (CARE) (Jennings et. al. 2013) and the SMART-in-Education Program (Roeser et al. 2013) may provide helpful examples of school-based mindfulness programs for which potential core components and logic models have been well articulated.

In addition, clearly articulating the core components of mindfulness and yoga programs can help improve the replicability of programs and move us from effective programs to effective practices (Jones & Bouffard 2012). That is, clearly specifying core components helps clarify what practices and approaches various programs have in common and what distinguishes them from one another and from traditional SEL programs. This process facilitates comparisons across program, which are key for identifying and testing the relative effectiveness of common program elements. For example, leading scholars categorize mindfulness practices into various forms of meditation which include: focused attention practices (focusing on the in and out cycle of breathing or another chosen anchor), open monitoring practices (observing sights, sounds and other sensations or whatever arises in one’s present moment experience), and compassion or directive practices (cultivating a feeling of benevolence and kindness toward oneself and/or others) (Ricard et al. 2014; Roeser & Pinela 2014). Mindful movement which includes yoga and tai-chi, is another category of practice in which attention and awareness is focused on the breath, whole body, and movement (Roeser & Pinela 2014). By conceptually grouping practices into these core categories – research can test the unique contribution of each kind of practice to individual outcomes as well as how these practices might have common impact (see Ricard et al. 2014).

As part of the current review, we attempted to evaluate whether certain kinds of components were common across studies, but found that a lack of specificity and shared language made it difficult to determine the extent to which components were the same or different. Generally speaking, program components articulated across studies appeared to fall into several categories: a) themes or lesson content (e.g. slowing down, self-awareness and knowledge, integrating mindfulness into everyday life), b) actual practices taught (e.g. awareness of breath practices, asanas (or postures), compassion practices), and c) pedagogical methods used (e.g. facilitating group discussion, guided practice, experiential learning techniques). Classifying core program components into these broad categories might help facilitate a common way of conceptualizing potential core program components within the field. The program elements that fall into the first two categories (themes or lesson content and the practices taught) are consistent with potential core content components in that they address “what” should be implemented. These content components may inform and provide the backbone of a program manual (Century et al. 2010). The kinds of program elements that fall into the third category, pedagogical methods, are more consistent with potential core process components in that they address the “how” or the manner in which the content is delivered – say through skillfully guiding a group discussion and inquiry. Such core process components are more likely to inform the training of program facilitators to effectively deliver material.

Recommendation 2: Clearly articulate core process components

Within the peer-reviewed literature on school-based mindfulness and yoga, there has been a relative over-focus on defining core content components (and assessing adherence to them) and an under-focus on the importance of process components. For example, most studies clearly articulated the rationale for teaching mindfulness or awareness practices such as breathing techniques, concentration practices, physical postures, directive practices, and meditation and provided empirical support for their inclusion. Less often fully articulated and supported by rationale and empirical support are the potential core process components – like facilitating appreciative inquiry or modeling human compassion. Yet, the manner and skill with which instructors are able to deliver the material are likely just, if not more, essential program components to implement with fidelity. Thoughtfully articulating core process components, the rationale for their inclusion, and incorporating them into logic models that specify how such processes might play a role in enhancing participant responsiveness and relevant outcomes is a much-needed next step in the field.

Two studies reviewed here may serve as useful examples of outlining a core process component of a program, including Klatt and colleagues (2013) who described appreciative inquiry as part of the Moving into Learning (MIL) Program and Roeser and colleagues (2013) who describe experiential learning as a key aspect of the SMART-in-Education Program. Both of these possible core process components are not necessarily unique to mindfulness-based interventions and may reflect “active forms of learning,” which has been identified as one of the four key elements of effective SEL programs (Jones & Bouffard 2012). We encourage researchers and program developers to look to more established literatures like SEL and prevention and promotion programs to help inform potential core process components of mindfulness and yoga programs.

Process components specific to mindfulness or yoga programs will also need to be elucidated and tested. For example, ideally, instructors of mindfulness-based programs embody qualities of mindfulness as a vehicle for teaching it and engaging students in participatory learning in their own lives (Crane et al. 2012a; Kabat-Zinn 2011). It is consistent with the nature of mindfulness to be “in the moment” and to respond flexibly to students at a given point in time, rather than adhering rigidly to a manualized curriculum (Kabat-Zinn 2011). Indeed, flexibility and the ability of an instructor to respond in the present moment to what is unfolding should be an essential component of FOI of mindfulness and yoga programs. Yet defining and operationalizing such process components pose some interesting and complex challenges for assessing what instructors “should” adhere to faithfully.

Several lines of work might help inform this important inquiry. First, Rebecca Crane and her colleagues have developed assessment criteria for levels of competence in teaching mindfulness-based interventions (MBIs) (Crane et al. 2010; 2012a; 2012b). The criteria cover six domains of competence that include: coverage, pacing and organization of session curriculum, relational skills, embodiment of mindfulness, guiding mindfulness practices, conveying course themes throughout interactive inquiry and didactic teaching, and holding the group learning environment. These kinds of competencies are excellent potential core process elements to be considered and assessed for mindfulness-based programs more generally because they cover how programs should be delivered, while incorporating flexibility to adapt to the situation as essential aspects of competence itself. Although not formally articulated as essential program features, one study reviewed here adapted Crane and colleagues’ work as part of their adherence checklists completed at each session (Metz et al. 2013). Second, Patricia Dobkin and her colleagues have recently outlined a framework for balancing “fidelity” to the four chambers of MBSR, which include: form, content, instructor, and intention with the “imagination” of making appropriate adaptations to populations, occupation, and institutional regulations (Dobkin et al., 2013). This approach offers another way to conceptualize the essential features or core program components of mindfulness-based programs, allowing room for both fidelity and adaptation. We invite discussion regarding how these different teaching competencies—which balance faithfulness to a core set of standards with the ability to flexibly adapt to the current situation and participants--might serve as the backbone of potential core program process components, FOI assessment, and instructor training

Recommendation 3: Assess and report multiple dimensional of FOI

To date, there has been a heavy focus on dosage as a potentially meaningful aspect of FOI and less focus on adherence, quality, and responsiveness. Dosage was by far the most commonly assessed and reported dimension of FOI in the current review. Dosage is important to assess and report because the number of sessions attended by participants is consistently associated with stronger program impacts in other kinds of prevention programs (Berkel et al. 2011), and there is some evidence in yoga research in adults that dosage, particularly home practice, may be a strong indicator of health outcomes (Ross et al. 2012). In addition, neuro-scientific evidence provides a “scientific warrant” for educational interventions that aim, through sustained regular practice to cultivate attention, emotional regulation and empathy (Davidson et al. 2012; Diamond & Lee 2011). Consequently, dosage is and will continue to be an important aspect of FOI for school-based contemplative programs to examine. In the future, researchers may want to consider the multi-dimensional nature of dosage itself, which can include duration, number of classes per week or month, number of minutes per class, duration, and home practice (see Cook-Cottone 2013).

Dosage alone, however, is not an adequate measure of FOI. Other key aspects of FOI include program adherence, quality, and participant responsiveness. Fewer than 20% of the studies reviewed here assessed one of these potentially influential dimensions of FOI. In other areas of prevention research, these aspects of FOI have shown positive relations with program outcomes (Berkel et al. 2011). For example, program quality--which in other social programming entails the facilitators’ use of interactive teaching methods, clinical process skills, and facilitating cohesion among participants--has been an important predictor of program outcomes (Coatsworth et al. 2006; Dane & Schneider 1998; Durlak & Dupre 2008; Forgatch et al. 2005). These kind of skills are theoretically consistent with some of the potential core process components described in the studies reviewed here – which included interactive learning, establishing trust in group as a safe place for disclosure, and activating student interest as important elements of program content. Assessing multiple dimensions of FOI for mindfulness and yoga programs implemented in school settings can help identify additionally important aspects of FOI, outside of dosage. Useful examples of studies that have assessed and reported at least three dimensions of FOI include Roeser et al. 2013, Benn et al. 2012, Gregoski et al. 2011, Steiner et al. 2013, and Feagans Gould et al. 2014.

Recommendation 4: Develop observational assessment systems and common FOI measures

It is also important to increase the precision and validity with which we measure specific dimensions of FOI. Very few studies have started to address these aspects of FOI rigor which likely reflects both the preliminary nature of many of the studies and limited resources to develop such resource-intense assessment systems. Yet, the validity of the conclusions we draw depends on accurate measurement of relevant FOI constructs. The development of sound observational measures is particularly important as they are more highly correlated with program outcomes than instructor-reported measures, which are prone to bias (Dane & Schneider 1998). In addition, observational measures are particularly important for assessing core process components. Consequently, developing observational assessments systems to assess meaningful FOI dimensions is an important next undertaking to advance the field.

Given both the importance and resource-intensive nature of developing reliable and valid FOI measures, particularly observational assessments, it might be useful to develop common FOI assessments that can be shared across research teams and programs. For example, researcher teams with more resources to devote to FOI could adapt rubrics like the ones developed by Crane and her colleagues--which are available at no cost online (Crane et al. 2012)--for school-based programs. Other programs might then incorporate such observational measures of core program processes into their own FOI assessment system and continue to test and provide feedback on their reliability and validity. In this way, sharing measures helps conserve resources, encourages dialogue across teams, and increases the potential to produce reliable and valid instruments that have been evaluated across a diverse range of programs and participants.

Recommendation 5: Build common FOI language and frameworks

There was considerable variability in the studies reviewed regarding the definition of FOI and related constructs. For example, in some studies “dosage” referred to the number of sessions delivered, while in others it signified the number of sessions attended by participants. In some studies, the term “fidelity” was synonymous with “adherence,” in others it referred to a combination of FOI dimensions, and other studies did not clearly define the term. These inconsistencies make it difficult to build a coherent knowledge base on FOI. For example, we had to decide whether to categorize outside practice as an aspect of participant dosage or of participant responsiveness. Our choice, based mostly on what has been done in yoga and mindfulness research to date, has ramifications for the conclusions we draw about dosage as an important aspect of FOI for school-based mindfulness and yoga programs. Establishing commonality in FOI terms and definitions as well as generating discussion around FOI frameworks may facilitate a more coherent knowledge base to work from. Frameworks can help build a common understanding of the essential FOI elements to be considered, the interconnectedness of those elements and how they are related to each other and to participant outcomes. It would be beneficial to draw from other related literatures that have more longstanding histories of studying FOI.

Berkel and colleagues (2011) have proposed a framework that may serve as a useful starting point, in which they distinguished between FOI dimensions related to facilitator and participant behaviors. Specifically, dimensions of adherence, quality, and adaptations are all considered aspects of facilitator behaviors, which occur within the delivery of program sessions and represent potential sources of disconnect between the program as designed and as intended. These facilitator behaviors are related to participant responsiveness (including attendance, active participation, home practice, and satisfaction), which, in turn, relate to program outcomes. There are several strengths of this FOI framework. First, it highlights the interconnectedness of FOI dimensions and their joint influence on outcomes, consistent with literature in prevention science (Rohrback et al. 2010). Second, this framework delineates what is within the control of program implementers (and hence what can be improved through training, monitoring or facilitator selection) and what is related to how participants respond (which depends on both the quality of facilitator behaviors and the characteristics of the participants being targeted). Within this framework, participant behaviors (e.g. attendance, home practice, and active participation) would be considered more proximal predictors of program outcomes that are influenced by facilitator behaviors (e.g. program quality and adherence).

Limitations

Despite the systematic nature of this review, it is important to highlight potential limitations of the findings reported and conclusions drawn here. First, findings reported in the current review are limited by what is reported in a particular peer-review publication or book chapter. Evaluation teams must decide what to include within the limited journal space allotted (particularly in health and medical fields) to describe a particular intervention and its implementation. Fully describing program components, FOI measures, and reporting FOI findings must be weighed against reporting other meaningful aspects of study designs and findings. Consequently, our assessment of the rigor of FOI is based only on what is being published in peer-reviewed article, which may not reflect the full amount of FOI work being conducted. Although we did not review additional sources of information on program components, several studies referenced other publications or provided hyperlinks to online supplemental materials (Hagins et al. 2013; Sibinga et al. 2013). These are useful strategies to balance space limitations with providing more comprehensive explanations of program theory or FOI measures.

Second, the current review was limited to programs implemented in school settings in North America. We made this decision in order to limit potential heterogeneity related, for example, to diverse educational cultures and/or historical lineage of mindfulness and yoga programs. We recognize however, that there are many rigorous and informative studies being conducted in countries outside North America that deserve careful attention. For example, Kuyken and colleagues evaluating the Mindfulness in Schools Programme (MiSP), have articulated some key “principles” of effective school-based programs teaching social emotional competence – one of which is program implementation that pays close attention to fidelity (Kuyken et al. 2013). They also conducted analyses to test the relationship between outside practice and several outcomes and found that more frequent use of mindfulness practices outside of class was associated with higher well-being scores at post-intervention and follow-up, lower depression scores at post-intervention and lower stress scores at follow-up. Because we did not include such studies in the current review, we do not know the extent to which programs implemented outside North America are rigorously assessing FOI. These studies merit attention, and the core program components and methods used to assess FOI they report should undoubtedly be included in the ongoing conversation in the field.

Final reflections

Rigorously assessing FOI is an essential component of building a rigorous evidence base. As Joseph Durlak and Emily DuPre eloquently stated in their seminal 2008 paper, “science cannot study what it cannot measure accurately and cannot measure what it does not define” (p. 342). Therefore, if we fail to systematically identify and measure the FOI of core program components of school-based mindfulness and yoga programs harnessing the power of the scientific method, we will not understand the potential importance of these aspects to producing the very outcomes we aim to foster. At the same time, we, as scientists and practitioners, must remain open to the full ramifications of mindfulness as a way of being and knowing, and acknowledge the limitations of the scientific method to know everything. Indeed science offers one meaningful voice, in a larger conversation of what works, why, and under what conditions.

The current review highlights the current state of the science as well as some potential next steps in service of that larger conversation. Pressing next steps for the field include clearly articulating core program components and how they relate to hypothesized outcomes in a formal theory of change, focusing more explicitly on core process components, as well as assessing and reporting multiple dimensions of FOI (using observational measures when possible). It is critical that we work together to construct and share common FOI measures, language, and frameworks, utilizing the extensive literature in SEL as well as elaborating aspects unique to mindfulness and yoga interventions. Our intention is that this review will serve as an invitation for dialogue and a call to action, prompting more rigorous assessment of FOI for school-based mindfulness and yoga programs and encouraging researchers and program developers to engage in an iterative learning cycle together.

Acknowledgments

This project was supported by and made possible through a grant from the National Institute on Drug Abuse (R34DA029237). We would like to thank Tony Garofano for his gracious help with graphics and layout.

Footnotes

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare they have no conflict of interest.

Contributor Information

Laura Feagans Gould, Email: laura@mindsincorporated.org, Minds Incorporated, 47 Girard St., NE, Washington DC, 20002, USA.

Jacinda K. Dariotis, College of Education, Criminal Justice, and Human Services & Evaluation Services Center, University of Cincinnati, Cincinnati, OH, USA

Mark T. Greenberg, Prevention Research Center, Pennsylvania State University, State College, PA, USA

Tamar Mendelson, Department of Mental Health, Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore, MD, USA.

References

  1. Anderson VL, Levinson EM, Barker W, Kiewra KR. The effects of meditation on teacher perceived occupational stress, state and trait anxiety, and burnout. School Psychology Quarterly. 1999;14(1):3–25. [Google Scholar]
  2. Barnes BL, Nagarkar S. Yoga education and scholastic achievement. Indian Journal of Clinical Psychology. 1989;16(2):96–98. [Google Scholar]
  3. Barnes VA, Davis HC, Murzynowski JB, Treiber FA. Impact of meditation on resting and ambulatory blood pressure and heart rate in youth. Psychosomatic medicine. 2004;66(6):909–914. doi: 10.1097/01.psy.0000145902.91749.35. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Barnes VA, Pendergrast RA, Harshfield GA, Treiber FA. Impact of breathing awareness meditation on ambulatory blood pressure and sodium handling in prehypertensive African American adolescents. Ethnicity & disease. 2008;18(1):1–5. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Beauchemin J, Hutchins TL, Patterson F. Mindfulness meditation may lessen anxiety, promote social skills, and improve academic performance among adolescents with learning disabilities. Complementary Health Practice Review. 2008;13(1):34–45. [Google Scholar]
  6. Benn R, Akiva T, Arel S, Roeser RW. Mindfulness training effects for parents and educators of children with special needs. Developmental psychology. 2012;48(5):1476–1487. doi: 10.1037/a0027537. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Berger DL, Silver EJ, Stein RE. Effects of yoga on inner-city children's well-being: a pilot study. Alternative therapies in health and medicine. 2008;15(5):36–42. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Berkel C, Mauricio AM, Schoenfelder E, Sandler IN. Putting the pieces together: An integrated model of program implementation. Prevention Science. 2011;12:23–33. doi: 10.1007/s11121-010-0186-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Black DS, Fernando R. Mindfulness training and classroom behavior among lower-income and ethnic minority elementary school children. Journal of Child and Family Studies. 2013:1–5. doi: 10.1007/s10826-013-9784-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Broderick PC, Metz S. Learning to BREATHE: A pilot trial of a mindfulness curriculum for adolescents. Advances in School Mental Health Promotion. 2009;2(1):35–46. [Google Scholar]
  11. Carroll C, Patterson M, Wood S, Booth A, Rick J, Balain S. A conceptual framework for implementation fidelity. Implementation Science. 2007;2:40–48. doi: 10.1186/1748-5908-2-40. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Century J, Rudnick M, Freeman C. A framework for measuring fidelity of implementation: A foundation for shared language and accumulation of knowledge. American Journal of Evaluation. 2010;31(2):199–218. [Google Scholar]
  13. Coatsworth JD, Duncan LG, Pantin H, Szapocznik J. Retaining ethnic minority parents in a preventive intervention: The quality of group process. Journal of Primary Prevention. 2006;27:367–389. doi: 10.1007/s10935-006-0043-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Conboy LA, Noggle JJ, Frey JL, Kudesia RS, Khalsa SBS. Qualitative evaluation of a high school yoga program: Feasibility and perceived benefits. Explore: The Journal of Science and Healing. 2013;9(3):171–180. doi: 10.1016/j.explore.2013.02.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Cook-Cottone C. Dosage as a critical variable in yoga therapy research. International Journal of Yoga Therapy. 2013;23(2):11–12. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Crane RS, Kuyken W, Hastings RP, Rothwell N, Williams JMG. Training teachers to deliver mindfulness-based interventions: Learning from the UK experience. Mindfulness. 2010;1:74–86. doi: 10.1007/s12671-010-0010-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Crane RS, Kuyken W, Williams JMG, Hastings RP, Cooper L, Fennell MJV. Competence in teaching mindfulness-based courses: Concepts, development and assessment. Mindfulness. 2012a;3:76–84. doi: 10.1007/s12671-011-0073-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Crane RS, Soulsby JG, Kuyken W, Williams JMG, Eames C, Silverton S. The Bangor, Exeter & Oxford Mindfulness-based Interventions Teaching Assessment Criteria (MBI:TAC) 2012b doi: 10.1177/1073191113490790. Doi: http://www.mindfulnessteachersuk.org.uk/pdf/MBI-TACJune2012.pdf. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Dane AV, Schneider BH. Program integrity in primary and early secondary prevention: Are implementation effects out of control? Clinical Psychology Review. 1998;18(1):23–45. doi: 10.1016/s0272-7358(97)00043-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Davidson RJ Mind and Life Education Research Network. Contemplative practices and mental training: Prospects for American education. Child Development Perspectives. 2012;6(2):146–153. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-8606.2012.00240.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Deming WE. Out of Crisis. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 1986. [Google Scholar]
  22. Diamond A, Lee K. Interventions shown to aid executive function development in children 4 to 12 years old. Science. 2011;333(6045):959–964. doi: 10.1126/science.1204529. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Dobkin PL, Hickman S, Monshat K. Holding the heart of mindfulness-based stress-reduction: Balancing fidelity and imagination when adapting MBSR. Mindfulness. 2013 [Google Scholar]
  24. Domitrovich CE, Greenberg MT. The study of implementation: Current findings from effective programs that prevent mental disorders in school-aged children. Journal of Educational and Psychological Consultation. 2000;11:193–221. [Google Scholar]
  25. Durlak JA. School-based prevention programs for children and adolescents. New York: Plenum; 1985. [Google Scholar]
  26. Durlak JA. Why program implementation is important. Journal of Prevention & Intervention in the Community. 1998;17:5–18. [Google Scholar]
  27. Durlak JA, DuPre EP. Implementation matters: A review of research on the influence of implementation on program outcomes and the factors affecting implementation. American Journal of Community Psychology. 2008;41:327–350. doi: 10.1007/s10464-008-9165-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Dusenbury L, Brannigan R, Falco M, Hansen WB. A review of research on fidelity of implementation: Implications for drug abuse prevention in school settings. Health Education Research Theory & Practice. 2003;18(2):237–256. doi: 10.1093/her/18.2.237. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Elder C, Nidich S, Colbert R, Hagelin J, Grayshield L, Oviedo-Lim D, Gerace D. Reduced Psychological Distress in Racial and Ethnic Minority Students Practicing the Transcendental Meditation Program. Journal of Instructional Psychology. 2011;38(2):109–116. [Google Scholar]
  30. Feagans Gould L, Dariotis JK, Mendelson T, Greenberg M. A school-based mindfulness intervention for urban youth: exploring moderators of intervention effects. Journal of Community Psychology. 2012;40(8):968–982. [Google Scholar]
  31. Feagans Gould L, Mendelson T, Dariotis JK, Ancona M, Smith ASR, Gonzalez AA, Smith AA, Greenberg MT. Assessing Fidelity of Core Components in a Mindfulness and Yoga Intervention for Urban Youth: Applying the CORE Process. New Directions for Youth Development. 2014;142:59–81. doi: 10.1002/yd.20097. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Fixsen DL, Blasé KA, Naoom SF, Wallace F. Core implementation components. Research on Social Work Practice. 2009;19:531–540. [Google Scholar]
  33. Fixsen DL, Naoom SF, Blase KA, Friedman RM, Wallace F. Implementation Research: A Synthesis of the Literature. Tampa, FL: University of South Florida, Louis de la Parte Florida Mental Health Institute, The National Implementation Research Network (FMHI Publication #231); 2005. [Google Scholar]
  34. Flook L, Goldberg SB, Pinger L, Bonus K, Davidson RJ. Mindfulness for teachers: A pilot study to assess effects on stress, burnout, and teaching efficacy. Mind, Brain, and Education. 2013;7(3):182–195. doi: 10.1111/mbe.12026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Flook L, Smalley SL, Kitil MJ, Galla BM, Kaiser-Greenland S, Locke J, Kasari C. Effects of mindful awareness practices on executive functions in elementary school children. Journal of Applied School Psychology. 2010;26(1):70–95. [Google Scholar]
  36. Forgatch MS, Patterson GR, DeGarmo DS. Evaluating fidelity: Predictive validity for a measure of competent adherence to the Oregon model of parent management training (PMTO) Behavior Therapy. 2005;36:3–13. doi: 10.1016/s0005-7894(05)80049-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Frank JL, Bose B, Schrobenhauser-Clonan A. Effectiveness of a School-Based Yoga Program on Adolescent Mental Health, Stress Coping Strategies, and Attitudes Toward Violence: Findings From a High-Risk Sample. Journal of Applied School Psychology. 2014;30(1):29–49. [Google Scholar]
  38. Frank JL, Reibel D, Broderick P, Cantrell T, Metz S. The Effectiveness of Mindfulness-Based Stress Reduction on Educator Stress and Well-Being: Results from a Pilot Study. Mindfulness. 2013:1–9. [Google Scholar]
  39. Gelderloos P, Lockie RJ, Chuttoorgoon S. Field independence of students at Maharishi School of the Age of Enlightenment and a Montessori school. Perceptual and Motor Skills. 1987;65(2):613–614. [Google Scholar]
  40. Ghahremani DG, Oh EY, Dean AC, Mouzakis K, Wilson KD, London ED. Effects of the Youth Empowerment Seminar on impulsive behavior in adolescents. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2013;53(1):139–141. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2013.02.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Greenberg MT, Harris AR. Nurturing mindfulness in children and youth: Current state of research. Child Development Perspectives. 2012;6(2):161–166. [Google Scholar]
  42. Gregoski MJ, Barnes VA, Tingen MS, Harshfield GA, Treiber FA. Breathing awareness meditation and LifeSkills training programs influence upon ambulatory blood pressure and sodium excretion among African American adolescents. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2011;48:59–64. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2010.05.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Hagins M, Haden SC, Daly LA. A randomized controlled trial on the effects of yoga on stress reactivity in 6th grade students. Evidence-Based Complementary and Alternative Medicine. 2013;2013 doi: 10.1155/2013/607134. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Institute of Education Sciences. Common guidelines for education research and development: A report from the Institute of Education Sciences. U.S. Department of Education and the National Science Foundation; 2013. Retried from: http://ies.ed.gov/pdf/CommonGuidelines.pdf. [Google Scholar]
  45. Jennings PA, Frank JL, Snowberg KE, Coccia MA, Greenberg MT. Improving classroom learning environments by Cultivating Awareness and Resilience in Education (CARE): Results of a randomized controlled trial. School Psychology Quarterly. 2013;28(4):374–390. doi: 10.1037/spq0000035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Jennings PA, Greenberg MT. The prosocial classroom: Teacher social and emotional competence in relation to student and classroom outcomes. Review of educational research. 2009;79(1):491–525. [Google Scholar]
  47. Jennings PA, Snowberg KE, Coccia MA, Greenberg MT. Improving classroom learning environments by cultivating awareness and resilience in education (CARE): Results of two pilot studies. Journal of Classroom Interaction. 2011;46(1):37–48. doi: 10.1037/spq0000035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Jones SM, Bouffard SM. Social Policy Report. Number 4. Vol. 26. Society for Research in Child Development; 2012. Social and Emotional Learning in Schools: From Programs to Strategies. [Google Scholar]
  49. Kabat-Zinn J. Full Catastrophe Living: Using the wisdom of your body and mind to face stress, pain, and illness. New York: Bantam Dell; 1990. [Google Scholar]
  50. Kabat-Zinn J. Some reflections on the origins of MBSR, skillful means, and the trouble with maps. Contemporary Buddhism. 2011;12(1):281–306. [Google Scholar]
  51. Kemeny ME, Foltz C, Cavanagh JF, Cullen M, Giese-Davis J, Jennings P, Ekman P. Contemplative/emotion training reduces negative emotional behavior and promotes prosocial responses. Emotion. 2012;12(2):338–350. doi: 10.1037/a0026118. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Khalsa SBS, Hickey-Schultz L, Cohen D, Steiner N, Cope S. Evaluation of the mental health benefits of yoga in a secondary school: a preliminary randomized controlled trial. The journal of behavioral health services & research. 2012;39(1):80–90. doi: 10.1007/s11414-011-9249-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Klatt M, Harpster K, Browne E, White S, Case-Smith J. Feasibility and preliminary outcomes for Move-Into-Learning: An arts-based mindfulness classroom intervention. The Journal of Positive Psychology. 2013;8(3):233–241. [Google Scholar]
  54. Koenig KP, Buckley-Reen A, Garg S. Efficacy of the Get Ready to Learn yoga program among children with autism spectrum disorders: A pretest-posttest control group design. American Journal of Occupational Therapy. 2012;66(5):538–546. doi: 10.5014/ajot.2012.004390. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Kutash K, Cross B, Madias A, Duchonowski AJ, Green AL. Description of a fidelity implementation system: An example from a community-based children’s mental health program. Journal of Child and Family Studies. 2012;21:1028–1040. [Google Scholar]
  56. Kuyken W, Weare K, Ukoumunne OC, Vicary R, Motton N, Burnett R, Cullen C, Hennelly S, Huppert F. Effectiveness of the Mindfulness in Schools Programme: non-randomized controlled feasibility study. The British Journal of Psychiatry. 2013;203(2):126–131. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.113.126649. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Lagor AF, Williams DJ, Lerner JB, McClure KS. Lessons Learned From a Mindfulness-Based Intervention With Chronically Ill Youth. Clinical Practice in Pediatric Psychology. 2013;1(2):146–158. [Google Scholar]
  58. Lantieri L, Nagler Kyse E, Harnett S, Malmus C. Building inner resilience in teachers and students. Personality, stress, and coping: Implications for education. 2011:267–292. [Google Scholar]
  59. Le TN, Gobert JM. Translating and Implementing a Mindfulness-Based Youth Suicide Prevention Intervention in a Native American Community. Journal of Child and Family Studies. :1–12. [Google Scholar]
  60. Liehr P, Diaz N. A pilot study examining the effect of mindfulness on depression and anxiety for minority children. Archives of Psychiatric Nursing. 2010;24(1):69–71. doi: 10.1016/j.apnu.2009.10.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Linden W. Practicing of meditation by school children and their levels of field dependence-independence, test anxiety, and reading achievement. Journal of consulting and clinical psychology. 1973;41(1):139–143. doi: 10.1037/h0035638. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Meiklejohn J, Phillips C, Freedman ML, Griffin ML, Biegel G, Roach A, et al. Integrating mindfulness training into K-12 education: fostering the resilience of teachers and students. Mindfulness. 2012;3(4):291–307. [Google Scholar]
  63. Mendelson T, Greenberg MT, Dariotis JK, Gould LF, Rhoades BL, Leaf PJ. Feasibility and preliminary outcomes of a school-based mindfulness intervention for urban youth. Journal of abnormal child psychology. 2010;38(7):985–994. doi: 10.1007/s10802-010-9418-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Mendelson T, Dariotis JK, Feagans Gould L, Smith ASR, Smith AA, Gonzalez AA, Greenberg MT. Implementing mindfulness and yoga in urban schools: A community-academic partnership. Journal of Children’s Services. 2013;8(4):276–291. [Google Scholar]
  65. Metz SM, Frank JL, Reibel D, Cantrell T, Sanders R, Broderick PC. The effectiveness of the Learning to BREATHE program on adolescent emotion regulation. Research in Human Development. 2013;10(3):252–272. [Google Scholar]
  66. Napoli M, Krech PR, Holley LC. Mindfulness training for elementary school students: The attention academy. Journal of Applied School Psychology. 2005;21(1):99–125. [Google Scholar]
  67. Nidich SI, Nidich RJ, Rainforth M. School effectiveness: achievement gains at the Maharishi School of the Age of Enlightenment. Education. 1986;107(1):49–54. [Google Scholar]
  68. Noggle JJ, Steiner NJ, Minami T, Khalsa SBS. Benefits of yoga for psychosocial well-being in a US high school curriculum: a preliminary randomized controlled trial. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics. 2012;33(3):193–201. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0b013e31824afdc4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. O’Donnell CL. Defining, conceptualizing, and measuring fidelity of implementation and its relationship to outcomes in K-12 curriculum intervention research. Review of Educational Research. 2008;78(1):33–84. [Google Scholar]
  70. Peck HL, Kehle TJ, Bray MA, Theodore LA. Yoga as an Intervention for Children with Attention Problems. School Psychology Review. 2005;34(3):415–424. [Google Scholar]
  71. Reid E, Miller L. An exploration in mindfulness: Classroom of detectives. The Teachers College Record. 2009;111(12):2775–2785. [Google Scholar]
  72. Ricard M, Lutz A, Davidson RJ. Mind of the mediator. Scientific American. 2014;311(5):39–45. doi: 10.1038/scientificamerican1114-38. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Roeser RW, Schonert-Reichl KA, Jha A, Cullen M, Wallace L, Wilensky R, Harrison J. Mindfulness training and reductions in teacher stress and burnout: Results from two randomized, waitlist-control field trials. Journal of Educational Psychology. 2013;105(3):787–804. [Google Scholar]
  74. Roeser RW, Pinela C. Mindfulness and compassion training in adolescence: A developmental contemplative science perspective. New Directions for Youth Development. 2014;142:9–30. doi: 10.1002/yd.20094. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Rohrback LA, Gunning M, Sun P, Sussman S. The Project Towards No Drug Abuse (TND) dissemination trail: Implementation fidelity and immediate outcomes. Prevention Science. 2010;11:77–88. doi: 10.1007/s11121-009-0151-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Rosaen C, Benn R. The experience of transcendental meditation in middle school students: a qualitative report. Explore: The Journal of Science and Healing. 2006;2(5):422–425. doi: 10.1016/j.explore.2006.06.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Ross A, Friedman E, Bevans M, Thomas S. Frequency of yoga practice predicts health: Results from a national survey of yoga practice. Evidence-based Complementary and Alternative Medicine. 2012;10 doi: 10.1155/2012/983258. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Ruiz-Primo MA. A multi-method and multi-score approach for studying fidelity of implementation. 2005 Unpublished manuscript. [Google Scholar]
  79. Schonert-Reichl KA, Lawlor MS. The effects of a mindfulness-based education program on pre-and early adolescents’ well-being and social and emotional competence. Mindfulness. 2010;1(3):137–151. [Google Scholar]
  80. Semple RJ, Reid EFG, Miller LF. Treating anxiety with mindfulness: An open trial of mindfulness training for anxious children. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy: An International Quarterly. 2005;19:379–392. [Google Scholar]
  81. Serwacki ML, Cook-Cottone C. Yoga in the schools: A systematic review of the literature. International Journal of Yoga Therapy. 2012;22:101–109. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Sibinga E, Perry-Parrish C, Chung SE, Johnson SB, Smith M, Ellen JM. School-based mindfulness instruction for urban male youth: A small randomized controlled trial. Preventive medicine. 2013;57(6):799–801. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2013.08.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Steiner NJ, Sidhu TK, Pop PG, Frenette EC, Perrin EC. Yoga in an urban school for children with emotional and behavioral disorders: A feasibility study. Journal of Child and Family Studies. 2013;22(6):815–826. [Google Scholar]
  84. Wall RB. Tai chi and mindfulness-based stress reduction in a Boston public middle school. Journal of Pediatric Health Care. 2005;19(4):230–237. doi: 10.1016/j.pedhc.2005.02.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Weare K. Developing mindfulness with children and young people: A review of the evidence and policy context. Journal of Children’s Services. 2013;8(2):141–153. [Google Scholar]
  86. White LS. Reducing stress in school-age girls through mindful yoga. Journal of Pediatric Health Care. 2012;26(1):45–56. doi: 10.1016/j.pedhc.2011.01.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Winzelberg AJ, Luskin FM. The effect of a meditation training in stress levels in secondary school teachers. Stress and Health. 1999;15(2):69–77. [Google Scholar]
  88. Wisner BL. An Exploratory Study of Mindfulness Meditation for Alternative School Students: Perceived Benefits for Improving School Climate and Student Functioning. Mindfulness. 2013:1–13. [Google Scholar]
  89. Wisner BL, Norton CL. Capitalizing on Behavioral and Emotional Strengths of Alternative High School Students Through Group Counseling to Promote Mindfulness Skills. The Journal for Specialists in Group Work. 2013;38(3):207–224. [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES