Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2017 Jun 1.
Published in final edited form as: Neurobiol Aging. 2016 Mar 14;42:101–109. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2016.03.006

THE IMPACT OF AGING, HEARING LOSS AND BODY WEIGHT ON MOUSE HIPPOCAMPAL REDOX STATE, MEASURED IN BRAIN SLICES USING FLUORESCENCE IMAGING

KA Stebbings 1, HW Choi 2, A Ravindra 2, DA Llano 1,2
PMCID: PMC4857205  NIHMSID: NIHMS776456  PMID: 27143426

Abstract

The relationships between oxidative stress in the hippocampus and other aging-related changes such as hearing loss, cortical thinning or changes in body weight are not yet known. We measured the redox ratio in a number of neural structures in brain slices taken from young and aged mice. Hearing thresholds, body weight and cortical thickness were also measured. We found striking aging-related increases in the redox ratio that were isolated to the stratum pyramidale, while such changes were not observed in thalamus or cortex. These changes were driven primarily by changes in FAD, not NADH. Multiple regression analysis suggested that neither hearing threshold nor cortical thickness independently contributed to this change in hippocampal redox ratio. However, body weight did independently contribute to predicted changes in hippocampal redox ratio. These data suggest that aging-related changes in hippocampal redox ratio are not a general reflection of overall brain oxidative state, but are highly localized, while still being related to at least one marker of late aging, weight loss at the end of life.

1.0 Background

Aging is associated with both cognitive changes as well as specific sensory changes, such as peripheral hearing loss. For example, aging-related deficits in episodic and spatial memory have been well-documented in humans and animals (Albert, 1997, Buckner, 2004, Frick, et al., 1995, Gallagher and Pelleymounter, 1988, Hedden and Gabrieli, 2004, Levine, et al., 2002), as have changes in peripheral hearing function (Cruickshanks, et al., 1998, Frisina and Zhu, 2010, Moscicki, et al., 1985, Ohlemiller, et al., 2010, Simpson, et al., 1985, Syka, 2010). These changes in memory function are associated with changes in hippocampal structure and function that occur during aging (Driscoll, et al., 2006, Golomb, et al., 1994, Maguire and Frith, 2003, Persson, et al., 2006), while the changes in auditory function are associated with loss of function at multiple levels of the auditory system, from the peripheral hearing apparatus to multiple levels of the central auditory system (Del Campo, et al., 2012, Henry, et al., 1980, Ling, et al., 2005, Mendelson and Ricketts, 2001, Parthasarathy, et al., 2010, Walton, et al., 1998). In addition, hearing loss and cognitive loss during aging are correlated (Lin, et al., 2011a, Lin, et al., 2011b), suggesting a potential common underlying etiology. It is not yet known whether changes in peripheral hearing are related in a direct and causal way to changes in general cognitive function, or if both are reflections of general metabolic factors, such as oxidative stress, which occur during aging (Beal, 1995, Berlett and Stadtman, 1997, Dröge and Schipper, 2007). Therefore the goal of this study is to examine the relationships between aging, peripheral hearing loss and oxidative stress in the hippocampus in an animal model.

Multiple approaches have been used to measure oxidative stress in the brain. Previous investigators have measured redox state via direct measurements of oxygen consumption in homogenized tissues (Kudin, et al., 2002, Lores-Arnaiz, et al., 2005). Potential limitations to this direct respirometric approach are the lack of spatial resolution of the sampling of the tissue, and the subsequent inability to conduct other experiments on the same tissue. Another approach to quantify redox state in brain tissue is to measure the ratio of oxidized flavin adenine dinucleotide (FAD) to reduced nicotinamide adenine dinucleotide (NADH). FAD can be quantified by measuring the emitted fluorescence in tissue at approximately 525 nm after excitation with wavelengths of approximately 450–480 nm (Benson, et al., 1979, Huang, et al., 2002, Kunz and Gellerich, 1993). Cellular NADH can be quantified by measuring the emitted fluorescence in tissue at approximately 450 nm after excitation with wavelengths of approximately 350 nm (Duchen, et al., 2003, White and Wittenberg, 1993). Many investigators have used this ratio to measure oxidative stress across multiple tissue types, including brain (Gerich, et al., 2009, Gerich, et al., 2006, Maleki, et al., 2012, Mayevsky, 1984, Parihar, et al., 2008, Ranji, et al., 2009) and this ratio has been established to be directly associated with tissue oxidative stress (Crowley, et al., 2015, Salehpour, et al., 2015). We and others found that this ratio can be measured in acutely-prepared brain slices, and is sensitive to small perturbations of molecules known to cause oxidative stress, such as hydrogen peroxide and cyanide (Gerich, et al., 2009, Gerich, et al., 2006, Stebbings, et al., 2016). Consequently, using a standardized approach to cut brain slices that are similar animal-to-animal (validated by using electrical stimulation to establish synaptic connectivity) we can use the slice preparation to compare the redox state, measured as a ratio of FAD/NADH, in various brain subregions of the slices in a non-destructive way with high spatial resolution.

Therefore, in the current study, we measured the redox ratio of various hippocampal subfields, two auditory forebrain structures (auditory cortex [AC] and medial geniculate body [MGB]), as well as peripheral hearing thresholds in young and aged mice. This was done in brain slices taken from CBA/CAj mice, which are known to undergo aging-related hearing loss, and loss of auditory efferent function (Frisina, et al., 2011, Frisina and Zhu, 2010, Ohlemiller, et al., 2010). In addition, body weight, which has a complex and age-dependent relationship with general health and longevity (Anisimov, et al., 2004, Goodrick, et al., 1990, Samorajski, et al., 1985), was also measured. These data were obtained as part of a study of aging-related changes in inhibitory synaptic function in the AC whose results were already partially published (Stebbings, et al., 2016).

2.0 Methods

2.1 Mice

A total of 29 male CBA/CAj mice, obtained through the National Institute on Aging aged rodent colonies, were employed in these studies. The young group comprised 16 animals: five at 4 months, five at 5.5 months, five at 9 months and one at 13 months of age. The aged group comprised 13 animals: four at 20 months, five at 22 months, three at 23 months and one at 28.4 months. Body weight was measured in 22 animals. Other measures of general metabolic health were not assessed. All procedures were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee at the University of Illinois. All animals were housed in animal care facilities approved by the American Association for Assessment and Accreditation of Laboratory Animal Care.

2.2 Auditory brainstem responses (ABRs)

As previously described (Stebbings, et al., 2016), ABRs were obtained at frequencies of 4, 8, 16, 32, 45, and 64 kHz as well as broadband noise. It was not possible to obtain all frequencies for all animals. Animals were anesthetized with 100mg/kg ketamine + 3 mg/kg xylazine intraperitoneally before the insertion of two subdermal electrodes, one at the vertex and one behind the left ear. Stimuli were presented using a Tucker-Davis (TDT) system 3, ES1 free field speaker, with waveforms being generated by RPvdsEx software. The output of the TDT speaker was calibrated at all the relevant frequencies, using a Bruel and Kjaer type 4135 microphone and a Bruel and Kjaer measuring amplifier (Model 2610). Each frequency was presented for 5ms (3ms flat with 1ms for both rise and fall times), at a rate of 2–6Hz, varied within each animal, with a 100ms analysis window. Raw potentials were obtained with a Dagan 2400A amplifier and preamplifier headstage combination, and filtered between 100Hz and 3000Hz. An AD instruments power lab 4/30 system was used to average these waveforms 500 times. Significant deflections, assessed via visual inspection, within 10ms after the end of the stimulus were deemed a response. Hearing threshold in response to a noise stimulus was chosen as the main hearing metric because all animals had a measurable ABR response to noise and because a principal components analysis showed that noise thresholds were essentially interchangeable with virtually any combination of pure-tone thresholds or thresholds averaged across frequencies (data not shown). Initial attempts were made to do the ABRs blinded, as we have done previously (Llano, et al., 2012). However, we found this to be impossible given the physical differences between the young and aged mice (i.e., aged mice were larger and had substantially less whiskers on their snouts). Given the large differences seen in the ABR thresholds seen in a larger dataset using these same animals (19 dB difference, p< 0.0001, Stebbings et al. 2016), it is unlikely that functional unblinding played a role in the differences reported here.

2.3 Brain Slicing

As previously described(Stebbings, et al., 2016), we modified the thalamocortical brain slice preparation developed by Cruikshank et al. to increase the likelihood of retaining the lateral extent of thalamocortical afferents to the left AC (Cruikshank, et al., 2002). To ensure maximum slice viability, mice were initially anesthetized with ketamine (100 mg/kg) + xylazine (3mg/kg) intraperitoneally and perfused with chilled (4°C) sucrose-based slicing solution (in mM): 234 sucrose, 11 glucose, 26 NaHCO3, 2.5 KCl, 1.25 NaH2PO4, 10 MgCl2, 0.5 CaCl2. Brains were blocked by removing of the olfactory bulbs and the anterior 2 mm of frontal cortex with a razor blade. The brain was then tipped onto the coronal cut and an off-horizontal cut was made on the dorsal surface, removing a sliver of brain angled at 20° from the horizontal plane. This increase in cutting angle of the thalamocortical slice has also been employed previously in older gerbils (Takesian, et al., 2012). The brain was then glued onto the cut angled surface, and 600 μm thick sections were taken. Sections were incubated for one hour in 32°C incubation solution before being imaged (26 NaHCO3, 2.5 KCl, 10 glucose, 126 NaCl, 1.25 NaH2PO4, 3 MgCl2, and 1 CaCl2). After incubation, slices were transferred to a chamber with an elevated slice platform so that the tissue could be perfused from both above and below with artificial cerebrospinal fluid (ACSF,26 NaHCO3, 2.5 KCl, 10 glucose, 126 NaCl, 1.25 NaH2PO4, 2 MgCl2, and 2 CaCl2, 22°C and bubbled with 95% oxygen/5% carbon dioxide).

Endogenous flavoprotein (FAD+) autofluorescence was measured using a stable DC fluorescence illuminator (Prior Lumen 200 ) and a U-M49002Xl E-GFP Olympus filter cube (Excitation: 470–490 nm, Dichroic 505 nm, emission 515 nm long pass). Endogenous nicotinamide adenine dinucleotide (NADH) imaging was performed using a Semrock Sirius A 000 cube with 350–390 nm excitation and 415–465 nm emission. Redox ratios were expressed as a ratio of FAD+ to NADH (Sepehr, et al., 2012). All data were collected using an infinity-corrected Olympus MacroXL 4X objective (NA 0.28) and a Retiga EXi camera using 4 x 4 binning (with resulting image size of 348 x 260 pixels) and StreamPix software.

2.4 Analysis

Regions of interest (ROI) in the hippocampus were defined anatomically by visualization of CA1, CA2 and CA3 in the slice. Initial hippocampal ROIs were 2 x 10 or 2 x 12 pixel rectangles completely contained within the stratum pyramidale or stratum radiatum, respectively, in each hippocampal subfield (see Figure 2A). Once it was determined that the most conspicuous changes were seen in CA2 and CA3, a new ROI containing these fields was constructed (see Figure 2B). The ROI in the dentate gyrus (DG) was drawn analogously to the ROIs in CA1, CA2 and CA3. The MGB ROI was constructed using a 20 x 40 pixel rectangle totally contained within the MGB. The AC ROI corresponds to a 2 mm rostro-caudal region centered around the area of activation seen after stimulation of auditory thalamocortical afferents, done as part of a previous study with the same slices (Stebbings, et al., 2016). All pixels in each ROI were averaged in the calculation of redox ratios. Cortical thickness was evaluated by drawing a line tangent to the rostral-most extent of the hippocampus, from the white/grey matter border of the cortex to the pia.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

ROIs used in this study. A) The original ROIs used were those from the AC and MGB, as well as the stratum pyramidale and adjacent stratum radiatum in regions CA1, CA2 and CA3. B) Once it was determined that the largest aging-related signal changes were seen in CA2 and CA3, a new region of interest capturing the stratum pyramidale and stratum radiatum of both of CA2 and CA3 was used for subsequent analyses.

2.5 Statistics

Given the non-uniform distribution of ages in the study (mostly young or aged animals, with few in between), nonparametric statistics (two tailed Wilcoxon-Rank Sum test and Spearman’s correlation) were used. For multivariate comparisons, multiple linear regression (run in SPSS) was used to test relative contributions of age, hearing loss, cortical thickness and body weight on hippocampal redox ratios. An optimal multivariate model was chosen by including all combinations of age, hearing, cortical thickness and body weight as independent variables and computing the adjusted R2 values. Regression coefficients and p-values were then computed using the optimal model. Wald Chi-squared testing was run in SAS to test for interaction effects. Correction for multiple comparisons was done using the Holm-Bonferroni method (Holm, 1979).

3.0 Results

Figure 1 shows representative images from brain slices taken from animals ranging from 4 months to 28.4 months of age. Shown are FAD images (Figures 1A–D), NADH images (Figures 1E–H) and images of the redox ratio (Figures 1I–L). These exemplar data indicate that there is a progressive increase in the FAD signal and redox ratio seen with aging which is most conspicuous in the stratum pyramidale of the hippocampus, particularly in the CA2 and CA3 regions (denoted by arrows in Figure 1L).

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Representative examples of FAD images (A–D), NADH images (E–H) and redox ratio images (I–L) from mice taken from different age groups. The notable finding in this figure is the rise of the FAD signal and redox ratio observed in the stratum pyramidale of the hippocampus (see arrows in L).

We examined the relationship between age and redox ratio across multiple brain structures. Figure 2A illustrates the regions of interest (ROI) for the AC, MGB, hippocampus CA1, CA2 and CA3 regions (both for the stratum pyramidale and adjacent stratum radiatum). Figure 2B shows a modified ROI containing a large strip extending from the CA1/CA2 border, through all of visible CA3, in both the stratum pyramidale and adjacent stratum radiatum. As will be shown below, these were the areas containing the most conspicuous aging-related changes, and were therefore used for all subsequent analyses.

Figure 3A shows the redox ratios of each of these brain structures (AC, MGB, CA1, CA2 and CA3) for both young and aged animals. There are no significant differences between young and aged in the DG, MGB or AC. There is an aging-related increase in the redox ratio in CA2 (young = 0.56 +/− 0.08 vs. aged = 0.65 +/− 0.08, p=0.007) and a trend for significance in CA3 (young = 0.55 +/− 0.07 vs. aged = 0.64 +/− 0.08, p=0.021) which did not survive correction for multiple comparisons.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

A) Box-whisker plots showing median, quartiles 1 and 3, minimum and maximum values overlaid on scatterplots of the individual values of the redox ratio across five different brain regions in young (blue) and aged (red) mice. Significance for each region is as follows: AC: p=0.369, U=83, MGB: p=0.913, U=101, CA1: p=0.228, U=76, CA2: p=0.007, U = 41, CA3: p=0.021, U = 51. In all cases, DF = 27. B) Box and whisker plots of normalized redox ratios from CA1, CA2, CA3, a combined ROI containing both CA2 and CA3 and the DG Significance for each region is as follows: CA1: p=0.0001, U=17.0, CA2: p<0.0001, U=1, CA3: p<0.0001, U=13, CA2-CA3: p<0.0001, U=3, DG: p=0.101, U = 66. In all cases, DF = 27. * p<0.01, ** p<0.001, *** p<0.0001.

To achieve a more standardized measurement that would be less sensitive to differences in slice preparation, the redox ratio in the stratum pyramidale of the hippocampal subfields was normalized to the redox ratio in the stratum radiatum. These normalized redox ratios are shown in Figure 3B. As shown, there were significant increases in the normalized redox ratio seen with aging in all subfields, and pairwise comparisons (young vs. aged) showed that this increase was most highly significant (p < 0.0001) in CA2 and CA3 subfields. We therefore used this ratio from a combined CA2 + CA3 ROI for all subsequent analyses and refer to this is as the “CA2-CA3 normalized redox ratio.” Note that we have also used the normalized redox ratio of either CA2 or CA3 and a combined CA1-CA2-CA3 ROI as a dependent variable, and have found no substantial differences from analysis with the CA2-CA3 normalized redox. The trends for CA1 ratio were generally in the same direction as CA2 and CA3, but weaker and are not analyzed further (data not shown).

To determine if the increase in redox ratio was driven by a change in FAD or NADH, or both, we have separately analyzed aging-related changes in the FAD and NADH signal. Since FAD and NADH measurements were not standardized (i.e., different exposure times were used in each slice and were optimized for each slice), the ratio of each component in the stratum pyramidale was normalized to the value measured in the stratum radiatum. We observed a highly significant relationship between age and the FAD ratio obtained using the combined ROI encompassing CA2 and CA3 (Spearman’s rho = 0.823, p<0.0001, degrees of freedom (DF) = 27), but no significant relationship for NADH (Spearman’s rho = 0.098, p<0.61, DF = 27). These data suggest that the aging-dependent rise in the redox ratio is driven by the FAD component of the signal, not the NADH component.

Multiple other endpoints also show aging-dependent changes in mice, such as hearing threshold, cortical thickness and body weight (Frisina, et al., 2011, Frisina and Zhu, 2010, Goodrick, et al., 1990, Ohlemiller, et al., 2010, Samorajski, et al., 1985, Stebbings, et al., 2016). Therefore, we examined whether any of these factors were associated with hippocampal redox signal changes. Figures 4A–D show the individual correlation functions between each of these factors and the CA2-CA3 normalized redox ratio. As shown, age, cortical thickness and hearing threshold are strongly correlated with CA2-CA3 normalized redox ratio (p < 0.001 in all cases), while body weight is not (p = 0.636). Note in Figure 4C that the normalized redox ratio appears to asymptote at approximately 1.1 at hearing thresholds above 50 dB SPL, and is better fit by a logarithmic function (R = 0.695).

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Correlation functions for A) age, B) cortical thickness, C) hearing threshold and D) body weight, all vs. CA2-CA3 normalized redox ratio. Correlation coefficients were determined using Spearman’s correlation and denoted as rho. DF=27 for A–C. DF = 20 for D.

Given the multiple potential predictors of the CA2-CA3 normalized redox ratio shown in Figure 4, we attempted to separate out the effect of each one using multiple regression. Standard multiple linear regression was done to determine if cortical thickness, hearing threshold or body weight independently contributed to aging-related changes in the CA2-CA3 normalized redox ratio. All combinations of age, cortical thickness, hearing threshold and body weight were used non-sequentially to develop an optimal model based on adjusted R2 values. The largest adjusted R2 value was found when combining age and bodyweight (adjusted R2 = 0.855). For this model both age (standardized beta = 1.003, p < 0.001) and body weight (standardized beta = −0.254, p = 0.011) were significant independent predictors of CA2-CA3 normalized redox ratio (Table 1). Variance inflation factors (VIFs) were computed for each of these factors and were found to be low (VIFs=1.19), suggesting little collinearity between these variables (O’Brien, 2007).

Table 1.

Regression coefficients (standardized) and p-values from optimal multiple regression models including age and body weight as regressors.

All animals (n=22)
Factor Beta p value
Age 1.003 <0.001
Body weight −0.254 0.011
Aged animals (n=12)
Factor Beta p value
Age 0.054 0.820
Body weight −0.736 0.011

To further explore the relationships between hearing loss, body weight, aging and CA2-CA3 normalized redox ratio, we plotted body weight vs. hearing loss and body weight vs. CA2-CA3 normalized redox ratio, and analyzed these separately for young and aged animals. Figure 5A shows such a plot for hearing loss. As shown, the regression slope for body weight vs. hearing loss is essentially flat in the young animals (p=0.34), but a strong correlation between body weight and hearing loss exists in the aged animals (p = 0.0005). That is, older, lighter animals had the most significant hearing loss, while older, heavier animals had hearing thresholds similar to young animals. Similar interactions were seen for CA2-CA3 redox ratio (Figure 5B). As was the case for hearing loss, the correlation function for body weight vs. hearing loss is essentially flat in the young animals (p=0.78), but shows a strong correlation in the aged animals (p = 0.003). Although it is possible the lack of significant correlations observed with the young animals is related to type 2 error, the slopes of the regression lines are essentially flat, suggesting weak, if any, relationships between these variables. Note that none of the aging- and body weight-related findings can be explained by correlations between age and weight, since these are uncorrelated in both the whole population (Spearman’s rho = 0.353, p=0.11, DF=20), as well as the aged animals (Spearman’s rho = −0.361, p=0.25, DF=10).

Figure 5.

Figure 5

Correlation functions for A) body weight vs. hearing loss and B) body weight vs. CA2-CA3 normalized redox ratio, analyzed separately for young (blue) and aged (red) mice. Figure B is the same figure as Figure 4D, except that young and aged mice are separated by color. Correlation coefficients were determined using Spearman’s correlation and denoted as rho. DF = 10 for aged and 8 for young.

These data suggest the presence of interaction effects between age, body weight and hippocampal redox ratio. That is, older, lighter animals had the highest hippocampal redox ratios while older, heavier animals had hippocampal redox ratios approaching those of young animals. Interaction effects were examined in both cases using the Wald Chi-square test. In both cases, it was determined that body weight significantly affected aging-related changes in hearing loss and CA2-CA3 normalized redox ratio, though the interaction was stronger with hearing loss (p<0.0001, Chi-Square=23.7) than CA2-CA3 normalized redox ratio (p = 0.037, Chi-Square=4.4).

Given the interactions between body weight, age and normalized CA2-CA3 redox ratios described above, we used multiple linear regression to examine the contribution of body weight, but now only in the aged animals. As was done above, all permutations of age, cortical thickness, hearing threshold and body weight were used to develop an optimal model based on adjusted R2 values. The largest adjusted R2 value was found when combining age, hearing loss and bodyweight (adjusted R2 = 0.502). However, VIFs for this model were somewhat elevated for weight (VIF = 5.62) and noise threshold (VIF = 5.74). Therefore, we estimated the VIFs for the model with the second highest R2 value (i.e., the model containing age and body weight) and found the VIFs to be in a more acceptable range (VIFs = 1.10). For this model, adjusted R2 = 0.472, and only body weight (p=0.011) was a significant independent predictor of CA2-CA3 normalized redox ratio (Table 1).

4.0 Discussion

In the current study, we report aging-related changes in the redox ratio (FAD/NADH) of the CA subfields of the hippocampus using fluorescence imaging of intrinsic FAD and NADH signals from slices taken from the CBA/CAj mouse. These redox changes are almost totally driven by changes in the relative intensity of the FAD signal, were less prominent in CA1, and were not seen in the DG, AC or the MGB. The normalized CA2-CA3 redox ratio also correlated positively with hearing loss and inversely with cortical thickness, though neither of these contributed significantly to this ratio beyond the contribution of aging. Body weight was found to independently contribute to the normalized CA2-CA3 redox changes; particularly so in the aging animals. These data are consistent with a body of literature demonstrating selective aging-related vulnerability to oxidative stress in the hippocampus, suggest that peripheral hearing loss and hippocampal redox changes may share common mechanisms, and point to the possibility of using body weight as a window into the oxidative state of hippocampal neurons.

4.1 Methodological considerations

The process of perfusing a mouse and cutting brain slices almost certainly introduces significant metabolic stress in brain tissue, and, in fact, we have previously shown that certain portions of the slice do not survive this trauma (Llano, et al., 2014). In addition, there have been numerous previous studies demonstrating that the aging brain, particularly the hippocampus, is highly sensitive to metabolic insults (El Mohsen, et al., 2005, Huang, et al., 2015, Navarro, et al., 2008, Parihar, et al., 2008, Siqueira, et al., 2005). Therefore, it is possible that the aging-related differences in the CA2 and CA3 redox state seen in the current study are driven by the metabolic stress induced by the production of brain slices, and may not reflect differences that would be seen in situ in a living organism. The finding that the aging-related increase in redox ratio is isolated to a particular layer of the hippocampus suggests that, at the very least, our results are not related to global metabolic stress on tissue caused by the slicing process. However, it is possible that our results are the result of some combination of intrinsic differences in the redox state of CA2 and CA3 neurons, and redox changes in selectively vulnerable neurons that were induced by the slicing process. This question could be answered with the development of a method to provide high-resolution redox imaging of deep tissues in vivo, which, to our knowledge, is not yet possible.

Another methodological consideration is that the cellular source of the FAD and NADH signals observed in this study are not yet known. It has been speculated that most of the FAD signal seen in the brain is from neuronal, rather than glial, origin (Husson and Issa, 2009). This is because, at least in the primate visual cortex, it has been shown that approximately 2% of the mitochondria are found in glia, and the rest are found in dendrites (Wong-Riley, 1989). However, other work has demonstrated that at least part of the FAD signal seen in vivo in the cerebellar cortex after electrical stimulation is of glial origin (Reinert, et al., 2007). Neither of these studies examined the potential glial contribution to FAD or NADH autofluoresence signals in the hippocampus. Therefore the potential contribution of cell type to the current results remains unknown.

Finally, as a correlative study, no specific interventions were done in the current work to determine if causal relationships exist between any of the measured variables. For example, in future work, it would be interesting to specifically induce hearing loss or body weight changes in an experimental animal and ask if either of these two alter hippocampal redox ratios.

4.2 Implications for hippocampal function and aging

The current data add to a growing body of literature demonstrating aging-related increases in mitochondrial dysfunction and that this dysfunction is particularly notable in the hippocampus (El Mohsen, et al., 2005, Huang, et al., 2015, Navarro, et al., 2008, Parihar and Brewer, 2007, Parihar, et al., 2008, Siqueira, et al., 2005). One difference between our work and at least one other study is that the aging-related changes in the current study appear to be driven by a rise in the FAD signal, rather than changes in the NADH signal seen in cultured hippocampal neurons (Parihar, et al., 2008). The differences between our findings and those of previous investigators may be related to the different approaches used. As mentioned above, it is likely that each method used to measure oxidative stress in brain tissue (brain slicing, cell culture, tissue homogenization), introduces new metabolic insults which may be reflected in the redox state of the tissue.

Peripheral hearing loss is strongly epidemiologically linked to aging-related cognitive changes (Lin, et al., 2011a, Lin, et al., 2011b) and, consistent with this finding, the current data also suggest that hearing loss is correlated with hippocampal redox changes. The current data provide no additional evidence that natural hearing loss (presbycusis), which is highly correlated with age, contributes independently to redox changes. This stands in potential contrast with previous studies demonstrating that direct induction of acoustic trauma using noise stimuli also produce general cognitive changes in experimental animals (Cernak, et al., 2001, Cui, et al., 2012). It is important to note in this context that the relationship, if any, between the redox changes seen in CA2 and CA3 in the current study, and cognitive performance, is not yet known. The alternative hypothesis, that hearing loss and hippocampal changes share a common metabolic etiology, is more consistent with current data and other previous work. Recent studies have demonstrated increases in markers of metabolic stress in the peripheral hearing apparatus seen during presbycusis (Jiang, et al., 2007, Yamasoba, et al., 2007). These data suggest that aging is associated with metabolic stress, which impacts both the peripheral hearing apparatus and the hippocampus. In the current study we observed that body weight is inversely correlated with hearing loss and with CA2-CA3 redox ratio, but only in the aged animals (Figure 5). This finding was unexpected given the body of literature demonstrating cognitive, hearing and lifespan benefits of caloric restriction (Sohal and Weindruch, 1996, Someya, et al., 2007). Under normal conditions, body weight increases with age in mice, but then drifts downward at the oldest ages (Goodrick, et al., 1990, Miller, et al., 2002, Piantanelli, et al., 2001, Samorajski, et al., 1985). Although lower body weights at mid-life may predict better late-life health outcomes in rodents, this relationship weakens for body weights measured once animals are senescent (Miller, et al., 2002, Piantanelli, et al., 2001) and shows variability based on strain and specific health outcomes measured (Anisimov, et al., 2004). Therefore, one potential explanation of these data is that older, lighter animals may be generally less healthy, manifesting in lowered body weight, worsened hearing, and higher hippocampal redox ratios. Alternatively, it is possible that lowered body weight represents a compensatory mechanism on the part of the animal to counteract neural and hearing dysfunction by diminishing caloric intake. Further studies involving measurements of other determinants of metabolic health and calorie consumption or studies using induction of caloric restriction and observation of changes in redox ratio and hearing function correlations will help to answer these questions.

The mechanism(s) underlying the aging-related elevation of the FAD/NADH ratio in the stratum pyramidale of the CA portions of the hippocampus are not yet known. The hippocampus is home to multiple forms of synaptic plasticity which may involve elevations of intracellular calcium and the necessary production of reactive oxygen species (ROS), placing hippocampal neurons at general risk for oxidative stress (Hu, et al., 2006, Paula-Lima, et al., 2014). Intrinsic ROS-scavenging defenses undergo changes over the lifespan (Serrano and Klann, 2004), leaving the hippocampus more vulnerable to ROS during aging (El Mohsen, et al., 2005, Rodrigues Siqueira, et al., 2005). The hippocampus also displays aging-related changes in the production of enzymes needed for oxidative phosphorylation. The main enzymes involved in activity-dependent FAD oxidation (and therefore increase in FAD fluorescence) are the pyruvate decarboxylase complex and the enzymes of complex I and complex II of oxidative phosphorylation (Husson and Issa, 2009). The stratum pyramidale of the hippocampus, in particular, expresses high levels of complex I enzymes (Manczak, et al., 2005), however these enzymes also undergo complex aging-related changes (Navarro, et al., 2008), with one group of investigators suggesting that aging-related increases in complex I activity may be compensatory (Manczak, et al., 2005). These data suggest that the aging-related elevations seen in the FAD signal could be a consequence of these compensatory increases, though one would also expect there to be a concomitant drop in the NADH signal, given the role of complex I in oxidizing NADH. Alternatively, rather than a compensatory response, a tonic shift in the redox potential may be driven by aging-related mitochondrial dysfunction and an increased supply of ROS. Such a shift in redox potential would lead to a larger proportion of FADH2 and NADH molecules to exist in their oxidized forms, leading to an increase in the FAD/NADH ratio in tissues most sensitive to oxidative stress. Such vulnerable tissues would include hippocampal neurons, as described above, and cells in the peripheral hearing apparatus, whose dysfunction wouldlead to hearing loss (Jiang, et al., 2007, L Poirrier, et al., 2010, Staecker, 2001). This hypothesis would be consistent with our observations that hearing loss is correlated to hippocampal redox changes with aging (Figure 4C). Neither explanation adequately accounts for the absence in NADH signal change with aging, unless changes in NADH fluorescence are an insensitive marker compared to FAD. To our knowledge, this has not been studied systematically. However, we have previously observed that FAD signal changes are substantially larger than NADH signal changes when being used as a surrogate marker for neural activity (Llano, et al., 2014).

4.3 Conclusions

The current data extend previous work demonstrating aging-related changes in the redox state of the hippocampus. This increased vulnerability of the hippocampus relative to other brain structures may be responsible for the early aging-related losses in hippocampal volume, loss of episodic memory and susceptibility to neurotoxic insults, such as to glutamate or amyloid beta, the agent found to be widely deposited in the brain in Alzheimer Disease (Brewer, et al., 2010). Further, the current studies represent an additional approach to measure quantitative hippocampal redox ratio in living tissue with major portions of hippocampal circuitry retained. This approach may permit future studies that examine the relationship between aging, hippocampal redox state and the function of neural circuits within the hippocampus.

Highlights.

  • Hippocampal redox state was measured in brain slices from young and aged mice

  • Hearing thresholds, body weight and cortical thickness were also measured

  • Redox state markedly increased with aging and was isolated to stratum pyramidale

  • Hearing and cortical thickness did not independently contribute to redox changes

  • Body weight was an independent predictor of hippocampal changes and hearing loss

Acknowledgments

The authors wish to acknowledge Dr. Masha Ranji for her advice about redox imaging and comments on an earlier version of this manuscript, Dr. Gregory Brewer for his comments on an earlier version of this manuscript, Xuan Bi from the Illinois Statistics Consulting Office for his statistical advice and assistance and Jiarong “Jerry” Fu for his assistance with Matlab coding. The authors thank Drs. Don Caspary and Jeremy Turner for useful discussions regarding this work. This work was supported by DC012125 as well as the American Federation of Aging Research and the Alzheimer’s Association.

Footnotes

Commercial Interest: None.

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

Literature cited

  1. Albert MS. The ageing brain: normal and abnormal memory. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences. 1997;352(1362):1703–9. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1997.0152. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Anisimov VN, Arbeev KG, Popovich IG, Zabezhinksi MA, Rosenfeld SV, Piskunova TS, Arbeeva LS, Semenchenko AV, Yashin AI. Body weight is not always a good predictor of longevity in mice. Experimental gerontology. 2004;39(3):305–19. doi: 10.1016/j.exger.2003.12.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Beal MF. Aging, energy, and oxidative stress in neurodegenerative diseases. Annals of neurology. 1995;38(3):357–66. doi: 10.1002/ana.410380304. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Benson RC, Meyer RA, Zaruba ME, McKhann GM. Cellular autofluorescence–is it due to flavins? Journal of Histochemistry & Cytochemistry. 1979;27(1):44–8. doi: 10.1177/27.1.438504. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Berlett BS, Stadtman ER. Protein oxidation in aging, disease, and oxidative stress. Journal of Biological Chemistry. 1997;272(33):20313–6. doi: 10.1074/jbc.272.33.20313. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Brewer GJ, Torricelli JR, Lindsey AL, Kunz EZ, Neuman A, Fisher DR, Joseph JA. Age-related toxicity of amyloid-beta associated with increased pERK and pCREB in primary hippocampal neurons: reversal by blueberry extract. The Journal of nutritional biochemistry. 2010;21(10):991–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jnutbio.2009.08.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Buckner RL. Memory and executive function in aging and AD: multiple factors that cause decline and reserve factors that compensate. Neuron. 2004;44(1):195–208. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.09.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Cernak I, Wang Z, Jiang J, Bian X, Savic J. Cognitive deficits following blast injury-induced neurotrauma: possible involvement of nitric oxide. Brain Injury. 2001;15(7):593–612. doi: 10.1080/02699050010009559. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Crowley A, Yang C, Zheleznova NN, Staruschenko A, Kurth T, Rein L, Sadovnikov K, Dayton A, Hoffman M, Ryan RP, Skelton MM, Salehpour F, Ranji M, Geurts A. Evidence of the importance of Nox4 in production of hypertension in Dahl salt-sensitive rats. Hypertension. 2015 doi: 10.1161/HYPERTENSIONAHA.115.06280. Accepted, in press. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Cruickshanks KJ, Wiley TL, Tweed TS, Klein BE, Klein R, Mares-Perlman JA, Nondahl DM. Prevalence of hearing loss in older adults in Beaver Dam, Wisconsin the epidemiology of hearing loss study. American Journal of Epidemiology. 1998;148(9):879–86. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a009713. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Cruikshank SJ, Rose HJ, Metherate R. Auditory thalamocortical synaptic transmission in vitro. Journal of neurophysiology. 2002;87(1):361–84. doi: 10.1152/jn.00549.2001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Cui B, Wu M, She X, Liu H. Impulse noise exposure in rats causes cognitive deficits and changes in hippocampal neurotransmitter signaling and tau phosphorylation. Brain research. 2012;1427:35–43. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2011.08.035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Del Campo HM, Measor K, Razak K. Parvalbumin immunoreactivity in the auditory cortex of a mouse model of presbycusis. Hearing research. 2012;294(1):31–9. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2012.08.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Driscoll I, Howard S, Stone J, Monfils M, Tomanek B, Brooks W, Sutherland R. The aging hippocampus: a multi-level analysis in the rat. Neuroscience. 2006;139(4):1173–85. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2006.01.040. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Dröge W, Schipper HM. Oxidative stress and aberrant signaling in aging and cognitive decline. Aging cell. 2007;6(3):361–70. doi: 10.1111/j.1474-9726.2007.00294.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Duchen MR, Surin A, Jacobson J. Methods in Enzymology. Academic Press; 2003. [17] Imaging mitochondrial function in intact cells; pp. 353–89. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. El Mohsen MMA, Iravani MM, Spencer JP, Rose S, Fahim AT, Motawi TM, Ismail NA, Jenner P. Age-associated changes in protein oxidation and proteasome activities in rat brain: modulation by antioxidants. Biochemical and biophysical research communications. 2005;336(2):386–91. doi: 10.1016/j.bbrc.2005.07.201. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Frick KM, Baxter MG, Markowska AL, Olton DS, Price DL. Age-related spatial reference and working memory deficits assessed in the water maze. Neurobiology of aging. 1995;16(2):149–60. doi: 10.1016/0197-4580(94)00155-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Frisina RD, Singh A, Bak M, Bozorg S, Seth R, Zhu X. F1 (CBA× C57) mice show superior hearing in old age relative to their parental strains: Hybrid vigor or a new animal model for “Golden Ears”? Neurobiology of aging. 2011;32(9):1716–24. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2009.09.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Frisina RD, Zhu X. Auditory sensitivity and the outer hair cell system in the CBA mouse model of age-related hearing loss. Open access animal physiology. 2010;2:9. doi: 10.2147/OAAP.S7202. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Gallagher M, Pelleymounter MA. Spatial learning deficits in old rats: a model for memory decline in the aged. Neurobiology of aging. 1988;9:549–56. doi: 10.1016/s0197-4580(88)80112-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Gerich FJ, Funke F, Hildebrandt B, Faßhauer M, Müller M. H2O2-mediated modulation of cytosolic signaling and organelle function in rat hippocampus. Pflügers Archiv-European Journal of Physiology. 2009;458(5):937–52. doi: 10.1007/s00424-009-0672-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Gerich FJ, Hepp S, Probst I, Müller M. Mitochondrial inhibition prior to oxygen-withdrawal facilitates the occurrence of hypoxia-induced spreading depression in rat hippocampal slices. Journal of neurophysiology. 2006;96(1):492–504. doi: 10.1152/jn.01015.2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Golomb J, Kluger A, de Leon MJ, Ferris SH, Convit A, Mittelman MS, Cohen J, Rusinek H, De Santi S, George AE. Hippocampal formation size in normal human aging: a correlate of delayed secondary memory performance. Learning & Memory. 1994;1(1):45–54. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Goodrick C, Ingram D, Reynolds M, Freeman J, Cider N. Effects of intermittent feeding upon body weight and lifespan in inbred mice: interaction of genotype and age. Mechanisms of ageing and development. 1990;55(1):69–87. doi: 10.1016/0047-6374(90)90107-q. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Hedden T, Gabrieli JDE. Insights into the ageing mind: a view from cognitive neuroscience. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2004;5(2):87–96. doi: 10.1038/nrn1323. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Henry KR, McGinn MD, Chole RA. Age-related auditory loss in the Mongolian gerbil. Archives of oto-rhino-laryngology. 1980;228(4):233–8. doi: 10.1007/BF00660735. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Holm S. A simple sequentially rejective multiple test procedure. Scandinavian journal of statistics. 1979:65–70. [Google Scholar]
  29. Hu D, Serrano F, Oury TD, Klann E. Aging-dependent alterations in synaptic plasticity and memory in mice that overexpress extracellular superoxide dismutase. The Journal of neuroscience. 2006;26(15):3933–41. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5566-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Huang S, Heikal AA, Webb WW. Two-Photon Fluorescence Spectroscopy and Microscopy of NAD(P)H and Flavoprotein. Biophysical Journal. 2002;82(5):2811–25. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3495(02)75621-X. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0006-3495(02)75621-X. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Huang T-T, Leu D, Zou Y. Oxidative stress and redox regulation on hippocampal-dependent cognitive functions. Archives of biochemistry and biophysics. 2015 doi: 10.1016/j.abb.2015.03.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Husson TR, Issa NP. Functional Imaging with Mitochondrial Flavoprotein Autofluorescence: Theory, Practice, and Applications. In: Frostig RD, editor. In Vivo Optical Imaging of Brain Function. CRC Press; New York: 2009. pp. 221–53. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Jiang H, Talaska AE, Schacht J, Sha SH. Oxidative imbalance in the aging inner ear. Neurobiology of aging. 2007;28(10):1605–12. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2006.06.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Kudin AP, Kudina TA, Seyfried J, Vielhaber S, Beck H, Elger CE, Kunz WS. Seizure-dependent modulation of mitochondrial oxidative phosphorylation in rat hippocampus. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2002;15(7):1105–14. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2002.01947.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Kunz WS, Gellerich FN. Quantification of the content of fluorescent flavoproteins in mitochondria from liver, kidney cortex, skeletal muscle, and brain. Biochemical medicine and metabolic biology. 1993;50(1):103–10. doi: 10.1006/bmmb.1993.1051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Poirrier AL, Pincemail J, Van Den Ackerveken P, Lefebvre PP, Malgrange B. Oxidative stress in the cochlea: an update. Current medicinal chemistry. 2010;17(30):3591–604. doi: 10.2174/092986710792927895. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Levine B, Svoboda E, Hay JF, Winocur G, Moscovitch M. Aging and autobiographical memory: dissociating episodic from semantic retrieval. Psychology and aging. 2002;17(4):677. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Lin FR, Ferrucci L, Metter EJ, An Y, Zonderman AB, Resnick SM. Hearing loss and cognition in the Baltimore Longitudinal Study of Aging. Neuropsychology. 2011a;25(6):763. doi: 10.1037/a0024238. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Lin FR, Metter EJ, O’Brien RJ, Resnick SM, Zonderman AB, Ferrucci L. Hearing loss and incident dementia. Archives of neurology. 2011b;68(2):214–20. doi: 10.1001/archneurol.2010.362. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Ling L, Hughes L, Caspary D. Age-related loss of the GABA synthetic enzyme glutamic acid decarboxylase in rat primary auditory cortex. Neuroscience. 2005;132:1103–13. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2004.12.043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Llano DA, Slater BJ, Lesicko AM, Stebbings KA. An auditory colliculothalamocortical brain slice preparation in mouse. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2014;111(1):197–207. doi: 10.1152/jn.00605.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Llano DA, Turner J, Caspary DM. Diminished Cortical Inhibition in an Aging Mouse Model of Chronic Tinnitus. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2012;32(46):16141–8. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.2499-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Lores-Arnaiz S, Perazzo JC, Prestifilippo JP, Lago N, D’Amico G, Czerniczyniec A, Bustamante J, Boveris A, Lemberg A. Hippocampal mitochondrial dysfunction with decreased mtNOS activity in prehepatic portal hypertensive rats. Neurochemistry international. 2005;47(5):362–8. doi: 10.1016/j.neuint.2005.05.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Maguire EA, Frith CD. Aging affects the engagement of the hippocampus during autobiographical memory retrieval. Brain. 2003;126(7):1511–23. doi: 10.1093/brain/awg157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Maleki S, Sepehr R, Staniszewski K, Sheibani N, Sorenson CM, Ranji M. Mitochondrial redox studies of oxidative stress in kidneys from diabetic mice. Biomedical optics express. 2012;3(2):273–81. doi: 10.1364/BOE.3.000273. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Manczak M, Jung Y, Park BS, Partovi D, Reddy PH. Time-course of mitochondrial gene expressions in mice brains: implications for mitochondrial dysfunction, oxidative damage, and cytochrome c in aging. Journal of neurochemistry. 2005;92(3):494–504. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.2004.02884.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Mayevsky A. Brain NADH redox state monitored in vivo by fiber optic surface fluorometry. Brain Research Reviews. 1984;7(1):49–68. doi: 10.1016/0165-0173(84)90029-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Mendelson J, Ricketts C. Age-related temporal processing speed deterioration in auditory cortex. Hearing research. 2001;158(1):84–94. doi: 10.1016/s0378-5955(01)00294-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Miller RA, Harper JM, Galecki A, Burke DT. Big mice die young: early life body weight predicts longevity in genetically heterogeneous mice. Aging cell. 2002;1(1):22–9. doi: 10.1046/j.1474-9728.2002.00006.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Moscicki EK, Elkins EF, Baurn HM, McNarnara PM. Hearing loss in the elderly: an epidemiologic study of the Framingham Heart Study Cohort. Ear and Hearing. 1985;6(4):184–90. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Navarro A, López-Cepero JM, Bández MJ, Sanchez-Pino MJ, Gómez C, Cadenas E, Boveris A. Hippocampal mitochondrial dysfunction in rat aging. American Journal of Physiology-Regulatory, Integrative and Comparative Physiology. 2008;294(2):R501–R9. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.00492.2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. O’Brien RM. A caution regarding rules of thumb for variance inflation factors. Quality & Quantity. 2007;41(5):673–90. [Google Scholar]
  53. Ohlemiller KK, Dahl AR, Gagnon PM. Divergent aging characteristics in CBA/J and CBA/CaJ mouse cochleae. Journal of the Association for Research in Otolaryngology. 2010;11(4):605–23. doi: 10.1007/s10162-010-0228-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Parihar MS, Brewer GJ. Simultaneous age-related depolarization of mitochondrial membrane potential and increased mitochondrial reactive oxygen species production correlate with age-related glutamate excitotoxicity in rat hippocampal neurons. Journal of neuroscience research. 2007;85(5):1018–32. doi: 10.1002/jnr.21218. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Parihar MS, Kunz EA, Brewer GJ. Age-related decreases in NAD (P) H and glutathione cause redox declines before ATP loss during glutamate treatment of hippocampal neurons. Journal of neuroscience research. 2008;86(10):2339–52. doi: 10.1002/jnr.21679. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Parthasarathy A, Cunningham PA, Bartlett EL. Age-related differences in auditory processing as assessed by amplitude-modulation following responses in quiet and in noise. Frontiers in aging neuroscience. 2010:2. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2010.00152. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Paula-Lima AC, Adasme T, Hidalgo C. Contribution of Ca2+ release channels to hippocampal synaptic plasticity and spatial memory: potential redox modulation. Antioxidants & redox signaling. 2014;21(6):892–914. doi: 10.1089/ars.2013.5796. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Persson J, Nyberg L, Lind J, Larsson A, Nilsson LG, Ingvar M, Buckner RL. Structure–function correlates of cognitive decline in aging. Cerebral Cortex. 2006;16(7):907–15. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhj036. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Piantanelli L, Zaia A, Rossolini G, Piantanelli A, Basso A, Anisimov VN. Long-live euthymic BALB/c-nu mice. I. Survival study suggests body weight as a life span predictor. Mechanisms of ageing and development. 2001;122(5):463–75. doi: 10.1016/s0047-6374(00)00256-6. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0047-6374(00)00256-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Ranji M, Matsubara M, Leshnower BG, Hinmon RH, Jaggard DL, Chance B, Gorman RC, Gorman JH. Quantifying Acute Myocardial Injury Using Ratiometric Fluorometry. Biomedical Engineering, IEEE Transactions on. 2009;56(5):1556–63. doi: 10.1109/TBME.2008.2006029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Reinert KC, Gao W, Chen G, Ebner TJ. Flavoprotein autofluorescence imaging in the cerebellar cortex in vivo. Journal of Neuroscience Research. 2007;85(15):3221–32. doi: 10.1002/jnr.21348. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Rodrigues Siqueira I, Fochesatto C, da Silva Torres IL, Dalmaz C, Alexandre Netto C. Aging affects oxidative state in hippocampus, hypothalamus and adrenal glands of Wistar rats. Life sciences. 2005;78(3):271–8. doi: 10.1016/j.lfs.2005.04.044. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.lfs.2005.04.044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Salehpour F, Ghanian Z, Yang C, Zheleznova NN, Kurth T, Dash RK, Cowley AW, Ranji M. Effects of p67phox on the mitochondrial oxidative state in the kidney of Dahl salt-sensitive rats: optical fluorescence 3-D cryoimaging. American Journal of Physiology-Renal Physiology. 2015;309(4):F377–F82. doi: 10.1152/ajprenal.00098.2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Samorajski T, Delaney C, Durham L, Ordy J, Johnson J, Dunlap W. Effect of exercise on longevity, body weight, locomotor performance, and passive-avoidance memory of C57BL/6J mice. Neurobiology of aging. 1985;6(1):17–24. doi: 10.1016/0197-4580(85)90066-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Sepehr R, Staniszewski K, Maleki S, Jacobs ER, Audi S, Ranji M. Optical imaging of tissue mitochondrial redox state in intact rat lungs in two models of pulmonary oxidative stress. Journal of biomedical optics. 2012;17(4):046010. doi: 10.1117/1.JBO.17.4.046010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Serrano F, Klann E. Reactive oxygen species and synaptic plasticity in the aging hippocampus. Ageing Research Reviews. 2004;3(4):431–43. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2004.05.002. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.arr.2004.05.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Simpson GV, Knight RT, Brailowsky S, Prospero-Garcia O, Scabini D. Altered peripheral and brainstem auditory function in aged rats. Brain research. 1985;348(1):28–35. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(85)90355-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Siqueira IR, Fochesatto C, da Silva Torres IL, Dalmaz C, Netto CA. Aging affects oxidative state in hippocampus, hypothalamus and adrenal glands of Wistar rats. Life sciences. 2005;78(3):271–8. doi: 10.1016/j.lfs.2005.04.044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Sohal RS, Weindruch R. Oxidative stress, caloric restriction, and aging. Science. 1996;273(5271):59–63. doi: 10.1126/science.273.5271.59. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Someya S, Yamasoba T, Weindruch R, Prolla TA, Tanokura M. Caloric restriction suppresses apoptotic cell death in the mammalian cochlea and leads to prevention of presbycusis. Neurobiology of aging. 2007;28(10):1613–22. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2006.06.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Staecker QYZ, Thomas R, Van De Water Hinrich. Oxidative stress in aging in the C57B16/J mouse cochlea. Acta oto-laryngologica. 2001;121(6):666–72. doi: 10.1080/00016480152583593. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Stebbings K, Choi H, Ravindra A, Caspary D, Turner J, Llano D. Ageing-related changes in GABAergic inhibition in mouse auditory cortex, measured using in vitro flavoprotein autofluorescence imaging. The Journal of physiology. 2016;594(1):207–21. doi: 10.1113/JP271221. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Syka J. The Fischer 344 rat as a model of presbycusis. Hearing research. 2010;264(1):70–8. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2009.11.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Takesian AE, Kotak VC, Sanes DH. Age-dependent effect of hearing loss on cortical inhibitory synapse function. Journal of neurophysiology. 2012;107(3):937–47. doi: 10.1152/jn.00515.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Walton JP, Frisina RD, O’Neill WE. Age-Related Alteration in Processing of Temporal Sound Features in the Auditory Midbrain of the CBA Mouse. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1998;18(7):2764–76. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-07-02764.1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. White R, Wittenberg B. NADH fluorescence of isolated ventricular myocytes: effects of pacing, myoglobin, and oxygen supply. Biophysical Journal. 1993;65(1):196. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3495(93)81058-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Wong-Riley MT. Cytochrome oxidase: an endogenous metabolic marker for neuronal activity. Trends in neurosciences. 1989;12(3):94–101. doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(89)90165-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Yamasoba T, Someya S, Yamada C, Weindruch R, Prolla TA, Tanokura M. Role of mitochondrial dysfunction and mitochondrial DNA mutations in age-related hearing loss. Hearing research. 2007;226(1):185–93. doi: 10.1016/j.heares.2006.06.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES