Abstract
Somatoform disorders (SD) are common medical disorders with prevalence rates between 3.5% and 18.4%, depending on country and medical setting. SD as outlined in the ICD-10 exhibits various biological, social, and psychological pathogenic factors. Little is known about the neural correlates of SD. The aims of this meta-analysis are to identify neuronal areas that are involved in SD and consistently differ between patients and healthy controls. We conducted a systematic literature research on neuroimaging studies of SD. Ten out of 686 studies fulfilled the inclusion criteria and were analyzed using activation likelihood estimation. Five neuronal areas differ between patients with SD and healthy controls namely the premotor and supplementary motor cortexes, the middle frontal gyrus, the anterior cingulate cortex, the insula, and the posterior cingulate cortex. These areas seem to have a particular importance for the occurrence of SD. Out of the ten studies two did not contribute to any of the clusters. Our results seem to largely overlap with the circuit network model of somatosensory amplification for SD. It is conceivable that functional disorders, independent of the clinical impression, show similar neurobiological processes. While overlaps do occur it is necessary to understand single functional somatic syndromes and their aetiology for future research, terminology, and treatment guidelines.
Keywords: Somatoform disorders, Somatoform pain disorders, Neuroimaging, MRI, ALE, Activation likelihood estimation, Meta-analysis, Premotor area, Supplementary motor cortex, Middle frontal gyrus, Anterior cingulate cortex, Insula, Posterior cingulate cortex
Graphical abstract
Highlights
-
•
Five neurobiological areas correlate with the occurrence of somatoform disorders (SD).
-
•
Areas are crucial for emotional, evaluative and cognitive aspects of pain processing.
-
•
Prefrontal areas indicate involvement of pain memory in SD.
-
•
Functional disorders might have central sensitization as an underlying factor.
-
•
Results do not support the classification of SD as medically unexplained symptoms.
1. Introduction
Somatoform disorders (SD) are highly prevalent in many medical settings. In German psychosomatic hospitals, 18.4% of the inpatients fulfilled the criteria of the ICD-10 (WHO, 1992) for SD (Pieh et al., 2010). It is estimated that SD have a prevalence of 16.1% in primary care settings in the Netherlands (de Waal et al., 2004), a life-time-prevalence of 12.9% in Germany (Meyer et al., 2000) and a 12-month prevalence of 6.3% (Wittchen et al., 2011) in Germany. General practitioners overestimate the prevalence of patients with SD in their medical practice considerably with 27.7% (Boeckle et al., 2014). Because of high medical costs and indirect costs, such as those caused by times of un-employability and early retirement, patients with somatoform disorder generate high economic costs for the health system (Rief and Henningsen, 2011). Patients with somatoform disorders (F45.-) have physical symptoms that suggest a medical condition but are not or not fully explained by any other medical condition and are related to psychological factors (WHO, 1992). The symptoms must cause impairment in occupational, social or other areas, or clinically relevant stress (WHO, 1992).
While many potential pathogenic factors have been discussed for somatoform disorders (SD) and related diagnoses, the pathogenesis of the symptoms is still unclear. There is some evidence that physically and sexually abused people have a higher risk of developing a somatoform disorder (Paras et al., 2009), as are persons with insecure or disorganized/disoriented attachment style (Waller et al., 2004). Barsky (1992) describes a cognitive style called “somatosensory amplification”, which can be applied to many patients with somatoform disorder. Patients with somatoform disorder often exhibit a heightened focus on their own bodies, perceiving their bodily complaints quicker as illness than healthy people do. The term “central sensitization” has recently been used to describe a neurobiological process, which assumes that symptom onset is associated with a hyper-responsive neural network in high-risk individuals (Bourke et al., 2015, Nijs et al., 2012, Phillips and Clauw, 2011). Patients with SD rate normally innocuous stimuli as painful stimulation due to an alteration of the network (Bourke et al., 2015). Additionally, patients perceive their complaints as illness and thus display augmented bodily attention (Barsky, 1992). Risk factors for SD are, amongst others, personality traits like neuroticism and alexithymia, childhood adversity, physical trauma, infections as triggers, and changes in the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis or in the immune system (American-Psychiatric-Association, 2013, Bourke et al., 2015, Burba et al., 2006, Rief and Henningsen, 2011).
Neuroimaging methods, such as functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), positron emission tomography (PET) and voxel-based morphometry (VBM) enable us to investigate neuronal activities and structural differences between different subpopulations. For instance, Yildirim et al. (2012) found smaller pituitary volumes in somatization patients than in a healthy control group, while Atmaca et al. (2011) examined differences in hippocampal and amygdalar volumes. Bourke et al. (2015) report that the most commonly found components of the neural network in central sensitization are, on the one hand the insula, which is involved in multimodal pain input (Peyron et al., 2002), threat detection (Critchley et al., 2002), interception (Craig, 2003a) emotional regulation (Gray et al., 2007), and motivation (Balleine and Dickinson, 2000), and on the other hand, the striatum (Zink et al., 2003), functioning in salience detection (Itti and Koch, 2001) in relation to onset speed (Iannetti et al., 2006), novelty (Iannetti et al., 2008), and context (Näätänen et al., 2007). Although neuroimaging findings have led to a better understanding of the pathogenesis of SD and other functional somatic syndromes (FSS), the evidence of neurobiological changes of FSS is still contradictory, as is true for SD. Additionally, there is evidence to support that FSS might be based on changes of the central nervous system, resulting in a common central augmentation of innocuous stimuli to pain (Bourke et al., 2015). The aims of this meta-analysis are to identify consistently differing neuronal areas that are involved in SD. Additionally, we hypothesize that identified areas coincide with theoretical models, e.g. central sensitization (Bourke et al., 2015, Perez et al., 2015).
2. Material and methods
2.1. Literature search
We searched the scientific databases Pubmed, ISI web of knowledge, Scopus, Cochrane database, Psycinfo, and Psyndex for relevant publications with the following terms: (“somatic symptom disorder” OR “somatoform disorder” OR “somatization” OR “functional somatic symptoms” OR “functional somatic syndrome” OR “somatization disorder”) AND (“neuro imaging” OR (“magnetic resonance imaging” OR (“magnetic” AND “resonance” AND “imaging”) OR “magnetic resonance imaging” OR “fmri”) OR (“magnetic resonance imaging” OR (“magnetic” AND “resonance” AND “imaging”) OR “magnetic resonance imaging” OR “mri”) OR PET OR VBM). Abstracts and titles were scanned by MB and CP, according to the a priori defined criteria (English publication; human adult subjects; no reviews, case reports, letters to editors or editorials: only original research; imaging methods PET, MRI, SPECT; including coordinates in Talairach space or MNI; differences between healthy subjects and patients; SD). Publications were included in the full text review if one of the raters found no exclusion criterion, resulting in a conservative approach and preventing the exclusion of possible full texts. We adopted the guidelines of the “PRISMA Statement” to provide transparent data selection (Moher et al., 2009). Only studies on somatoform disorder according to ICD or DSM matching ICD criteria were included. Although other pain related functional disorders, such as irritable bowel syndrome (IBS) or fibromyalgia syndrome (FMS) have partially similar diagnostic criteria (Eich et al., 2008). We specifically excluded IBS and FMS even though they are often described as syndromes sharing specific aspects (Häuser and Henningsen, 2014, Henningsen et al., 2007) while it is repeatedly emphasized that these syndromes are not the same (Bourke et al., 2015, Häuser and Henningsen, 2014, Lacourt et al., 2013, Rief and Isaac, 2014, White, 2010, White, 2013). Even though there are arguments for lumping FSS together as well as arguments for separate syndromes, we decided to base our selection criteria for the meta-analysis to a clear diagnosis in order to enhance interpretation of our results.
2.2. Meta-analysis
All coordinates from eligible publications were analyzed with an Activation Likelihood Estimation (ALE) using a Java-Version of GingerALE 2.3.5 (Eickhoff et al., 2009, Eickhoff et al., 2012, Turkeltaub et al., 2012). ALE is a quantitative and meta-analytical approach to calculate consistent differences in gray matter volume and/or activation, based on reported statistical differences in the literature. ALE is possible to conduct on structural as well as functional neuroimaging studies solely as well as in combination of structural and functional neuroimaging studies. We calculated two analysis, one including functional and structural studies, another focusing on functional studies. A subcategory analysis for structural imaging modality or certain paradigm was not feasible due to sample size. We included all coordinates that presented relevant differences between healthy controls and patients into our analysis.
All coordinates reported in MNI were recalculated into Talairach space via the icbm2tal tool (Laird et al., 2010, Lancaster et al., 2007) provided by brainmap.org (Eickhoff, 2014). We calculated a cluster-level of 0.05 with 1000 threshold permutations, a minimum cluster size of 240 mm3 and Cluster forming value of 0.001. ALE maps were visualized with Mango version 3.7 (1415) (Mango, 2014) to investigate threshold maps that were superimposed on a standard anatomical image (Colin.1.1.1.nii).
We summed healthy controls and patients to quantify our number of subjects. To calculate the most conservative ALE analysis, for each dataset we entered the number of individuals participating in the experiment with the lowest number of participants. Publications analyzing the same individuals were subsumed under one publication, in order to prevent overestimating the influence of these individuals on the whole sample. All reported foci of these publications entered ALE analysis, including the foci lying outside of the mask, as small proportions of outside foci do not influence results. In the results, the nearest gray matters within ± 5 mm are reported.
3. Results
3.1. Study selection
The systematic literature search yielded 686 research articles on SD (Fig. 1). Search results included 143 duplicates, resulting in 543 studies eligible for abstract and title review. The title and abstract review resulted in 507 excluded studies, leaving 36 studies for full text analysis. After the exclusion of non-eligible publications (n = 27), ten studies (Table 1) were included in the meta-analysis. Several studies were based on the same population; namely two by de Greck and colleagues (de Greck et al., 2012, de Greck et al., 2011) as well as three publications by a working group from China (Song et al., 2015, Su et al., 2014, Zhang et al., 2015). We subsumed all of the results based on the sample within one dataset to prevent pseudo-replication and overestimating the influence of these individuals on the results. We analyzed seven datasets when using all studies, six when excluding structural studies. Thus, 243 individuals entered the full analysis including structural and functional studies, while 204 subjects the analysis focusing on functional studies (Table 1). Five foci were outside the mask in both analyses, whereby functional and structural analysis has a total of 107 foci and functional analysis 90 foci.
Table 1.
Citation | Type of disorder | Number of participants | Imaging method | Task |
---|---|---|---|---|
de Greck et al. (2011) | Somatoform disorder | P:20 (12f); HC: 20 (12f) | MRI (1.5T) | Monetary reward task |
de Greck et al. (2012) | Somatoform disorder | P:20 (12f); HC: 20 (12f) | MRI (1.5T) | Emotional empathy |
Gündel et al. (2008) | Somatoform pain disorder | P:12 (12f); HC: 20 (13f) | MRI (1.5T) | Noxious heat stimuli |
Egloff et al. (2009) | Chronic pain, not explained by peripheral tissue damage | P: 11 (6f); HC: 12 (6f) | PET | Glucose metabolism resting state |
Song et al. (2015) | Somatization disorder | P: 25 (21f); HC: 28 (22) | MRI (3.0T) | Resting state (ReHo) |
Stoeter et al. (2007) | Somatoform pain disorder | P:17 (11f); HC: 17 (11f) | MRI (1.5T) | Pin-prick stimulation, emotional and cognitive stress |
Su et al. (2014) | Somatization disorder | P: 25 (21f); HC: 28 (22) | MRI (3.0T) | Resting state (fALFF) |
Valet et al. (2009) | Pain disorder, without any somatic cause | P:14 (14f); HC: 14 (14f) | MRI (1.5T) | VBM structural scan |
Yoshino et al. (2014) | Somatoform pain disorder | P: 9 (4); HC: 20 (13) | MRI (1.5T) | Resting state (ReHo) |
Zhang et al. (2015) | Somatization disorder | P: 25 (21f); HC: 28 (22) | MRI (3.0T) | DTI structural scan |
P: patients; HC: healthy controls; T: Tesla; fMRI: magnetic resonance tomography; PET: positron emission tomography; f: number of females; ReHo: regional homogeneity; fALFF: fractional amplitude of low-frequency fluctuations.
3.1.1. ALE clusters of somatoform disorders
Five brain areas have been identified to repeatedly differ between patients with SD and healthy controls (Table 2) according to the calculated clusters. Out of the ten publications contributing to the calculations two did not contribute to any of the ALE clusters (Gündel et al., 2008, Yoshino et al., 2014).
Table 2.
Cluster # | # of datasets | Gray matter at center | Additional gray matter within 5 mm | Center | Vol. mm3 | Studies included | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Functional studies | Cluster 1 | 2 | BA 31: dorsal posterior CC | BA 30: part of posterior CC | x = − 13.2 y = − 62.6 z = 15.1 |
384 | Egloff et al. (2009), Song et al. (2015), Su et al. (2014), Zhang et al. (2015) |
Cluster 2 | 2 | BA 10: anterior prefrontal cortex | x = 23.4 y = 50.3 z = 7.3 |
304 | de Greck et al., 2011, de Greck et al., 2012, Stoeter et al. (2007) | ||
Cluster 3 | 2 | BA 13: Insula | x = − 37.3 y = − 21.1 z = 19.8 |
256 | Egloff et al. (2009), Stoeter et al. (2007) | ||
Structural & functional studies | Cluster 1 | 3 | BA 10: anterior prefrontal cortex | x = 21.8 y = 50.9 z = 7.5 |
808 | de Greck et al., 2011, de Greck et al., 2012, Song et al. (2015), Stoeter et al. (2007), Su et al. (2014),Zhang et al. (2015) | |
Cluster 2 | 2 | BA 6: premotor and supplementary motor cortex | BA 8: lateral and medial supplementary motor area | x = − 43.2 y = 14.6 z = 48.7 |
376 | de Greck et al., 2011, de Greck et al., 2012, Egloff et al. (2009) | |
Cluster 3 | 2 | BA 31: dorsal posterior CC | BA 30: part of posterior cingulate cortex | x = 13.3 y = − 62.5 z = 15.1 |
352 | Egloff et al. (2009), Song et al. (2015), Su et al. (2014), Zhang et al. (2015) | |
Cluster 4 | 2 | BA 32: anterior CC | BA 24: part of anterior CC | x = − 6.2 y = 33.7 z = − 2.2 |
272 | Egloff et al. (2009), Valet et al. (2009) |
Brodmann Area (BA), Cingutale cortex (CC), number (#), x-, y-, z-coordinates in Talairach space.
3.1.2. Functional areas of ALE clusters of SD in functional studies
In cluster 1 the dorsal posterior cingulate cortex (dPCC, Brodmann area (BA) 30 & 31) was identified to be significantly different across studies comparing patients and HC (Egloff et al., 2009, Song et al., 2015, Su et al., 2014, Zhang et al., 2015), while in Cluster 2 (de Greck et al., 2011, de Greck et al., 2012, Stoeter et al., 2007) the anterior prefrontal cortex (aPFC, BA 10) and in cluster 3 (Egloff et al., 2009, Stoeter et al., 2007) the insula (BA 13).
3.1.3. Functional areas of ALE clusters of SD in structural and functional studies
In cluster 1 the aPFC (BA 10) was identified to be significantly different between patients and HC across several studies (de Greck et al., 2011, de Greck et al., 2012, Song et al., 2015, Stoeter et al., 2007, Su et al., 2014, Zhang et al., 2015). The premotor and supplementary motor cortex (PSMC, BA 6) including the lateral and medial supplementary motor area (BA 8), which is significant in cluster 2 (de Greck et al., 2011, de Greck et al., 2012, Egloff et al., 2009), is also within the 5 mm boundary in SD. While in cluster 3 (Egloff et al., 2009, Song et al., 2015, Su et al., 2014, Zhang et al., 2015) the dPCC (BA 30 & 31) is differentially activated. In cluster 4 the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC, BA 24 & 32) (Egloff et al., 2009, Valet et al., 2009) varies significantly between patients with SD and healthy controls (Fig. 2).
4. Discussion
In this meta-analysis, we present the results of an ALE-analysis on SD using the results of published neuro-imaging (MRI, SPECT, PET) studies. We herewith provide a first attempt to combine various studies to find consistent significant neurobiological differences between healthy controls and patients with SD. In both analyses combined we identified five areas showing functional and/or structural differences between the two groups of interest, namely the dorsal posterior cingulate cortex, anterior cingulate cortex, the anterior prefrontal cortex, the insula and the premotor and supplementary motor cortex.
We found significant differences between patients and controls in the ACC (Egloff et al., 2009, Valet et al., 2009). Functional studies showed that the ACC processes amongst others emotion (Kawamoto et al., 2012, Killgore and Yurgelun-Todd, 2007), attention (Nebel et al., 2005), and pain (Büchel et al., 2002, Lloyd et al., 2004). Changes in the ACC have been reported in cases of pain-induced depression (Barthas et al., 2014) and anxiety disorder (Shinoura et al., 2013). Patients with FSS experience alterations of attention, anticipation, and pain memories that correlate with increased activity of the ACC. These also correlate with the prefrontal areas and the ACC when stimulated below pain thresholds in both patients with FMS (Cook et al., 2004, Peyron et al., 2000) and IBS (Andresen et al., 2005, Naliboff et al., 2001). Additionally, studies on FMS repeatedly report reduced activation of the ACC during noxious stimulation (e.g.: Gracely et al., 2002, Jensen et al., 2009, Lee et al., 2013). The interaction of the ACC with prefrontal areas is in correlation with the negative forecasting while catastrophizing (Seminowicz and Davis, 2006).
The changed interaction between ACC and prefrontal areas might be represented in our data, as we also found changes in the aPFC, a part of the middle frontal gyrus (de Greck et al., 2011, de Greck et al., 2012, Stoeter et al., 2007). Chronic back pain disrupts normal functioning in the default mode network (DMN) also represented in the middle frontal gyrus of our results (Laird et al., 2009, Tagliazucchi et al., 2010). Changes in cortical thickness in the middle frontal gyrus have been shown to be reversible with back pain surgery (Seminowicz et al., 2011), whereby the resulting increases in gray matter in the MFG resulted in a reduction of pain and physical disability (Seminowicz et al., 2011). Patients with IBS show reductions of gray matter (Seminowicz et al., 2010) similar to the reductions of gray matter in the MFG in patients with SD, as well as reductions of activation in the middle frontal gyrus (de Greck et al., 2011, de Greck et al., 2012, Song et al., 2006, Stoeter et al., 2007).
Another important area for the occurrence of SD and that shares important interactions with the ACC is the insula. The insula is known to be important for pain processing (Fitzek et al., 2004, Kong et al., 2006, Schoedel et al., 2008, Strigo et al., 2003) and in paradigms when painful stimulation is expected (Sawamoto et al., 2000). Activation of the insula has been reported primarily for cutaneous pain rather than visceral pain (Strigo et al., 2003). In patients with FMS, the activation of the insula was connected to experiments on pain expectation (Gracely et al., 2002, Kim et al., 2013). Egloff et al. (2009) show that in cases of non-dermatomal somatosensory deficits in patients with chronic pain, the posterior part of the insula shows hypo-metabolic activation. This is especially significant as the area is essential for vibro-tactile and temperature discrimination tasks (Brooks et al., 2005). Contrary to the findings of Egloff et al. (2009); Stoeter et al. (2007) describe an increased activation of the insula as a result of the first application of pain in patients in comparison to healthy controls. The lateral nociceptive system, of which the insula is a part, indicated consistent hyper-perfusion, whereas the ACC and the more affective-motivational system did not (Stoeter et al., 2007). The increased activation in the insula may be correlated with the exaggerated expectancy of pain and attention in SD patients (Stoeter et al., 2007). This might be correlated with the anterior part of the insula, which is associated with cognitive-affective aspects of pain (Craig, 2003b). In FSS patients, the increased insular activation is correlated with heightened sensory coding of stimuli, which are coded as innocuous in healthy patients (Cook et al., 2004, Peyron et al., 2000). This effect has also been shown in patients with IBS (Berman et al., 2008). Medial and posterior parts of the insula are hypothesized to be involved in these somatosensory discriminative abilities (Craig, 2003b). It is hypothesized that an increased interoception is at work and is mediated via missing inhibitory descending prefrontal input and/or continued ascending arousal (Berman et al., 2008). The insula specifically the posterior part does receive somatosensory information of the thalamus and was brought into connection for a neural circuit model of somatosensory amplification (Perez et al., 2015). This circuit model proposes the thalamus to play an important part in SD (Perez et al., 2015). While the thalamus was not significantly different in our meta-analysis it shares many connections as important relaying area with areas that we found to be significantly changed in SD patients like the insula, cingulate cortex, prefrontal areas, and motor areas. Of the motor network we found the PSMC, which shares afferent and efferent connections with the thalamus, to be significantly changed across studies about SD.
Changes in the PSMC have previously been described in patients with chronic somatoform pain (Noll-Hussong et al., 2010). These changes often correlate with chronic pain because patients reduce movement in order to avoid painful stimulation (Rodriguez-Raecke et al., 2013). This explanation is supported by the increase of gray matter in correlation with time and improvement of motor function found in patients following a successful treatment for chronic pain, (Rodriguez-Raecke et al., 2013, Seminowicz et al., 2011). Outside changes in the motor cortex due to general chronic pain, alterations in the motor cortex are documented for patients with pain syndromes (Flor et al., 2001, Maihöfner et al., 2007, Swart et al., 2009). FSSs are related to modifications in the PSMC. IBS patients show urge-related differences in the PSMC (Kwan et al., 2005). Additionally, patients with fibromyalgia-syndrome show a reduction in the gray matter in the motor cortex (Puri et al., 2010).
Another significantly different area that is connected with the thalamus but also frontal brain areas like the BA 10 is the PCC (Maddock, 1999, Vann et al., 2009). The PCC is found to correlate with functional or structural changes in patients with SD. Increased activation in BA 23 correlates negatively with BA 30 activation, and BA 30 has a positive correlation with BA 31 (Cauda et al., 2010). In IBS and FMS, the PCC (BA 23, BA 31) and agranular retrolimbic area (ARA) differ between HC and patients (Boeckle et al., 2016). As PCC and ARA seem to be important parts of the DMN (Vann et al., 2009), a function already consistently found in 2-week-old infants (Gao et al., 2009), changes in DMN might underlie SD in general. Hyper-activation and hyper-connectivity in the DMN in schizophrenia are related to overtly increased self-reference, attention deficits, and impairments of working memory, while in depressive patients hyper-activation of the DMN might correlate with negative rumination (Whitfield-Gabrieli and Ford, 2012). Out of the four core regions (Whitfield-Gabrieli and Ford, 2012) of the DMN (medial prefrontal cortex (BA 10, 24, 23); PCC and retrosplenial cortex (BA 29, 30, 23, 13); left and right inferior parietal lobules (BA 39, 40)) all except the inferior parietal lobules seem to be dysfunctional in patients with SD. The DMN is also related to personal introspection, autobiographical memories, and thoughts about the future. Thus, disruptions of the DMN can lead to alexithymia as well as increased introspection and reflections of others (Buckner et al., 2008, Saxe et al., 2004, Whitfield-Gabrieli and Ford, 2012), aspects often observed in patients with SD.
In our analysis we found that evaluative regional areas like PCC, ACC, and insula are likely to have a high influence on the occurrence of SD. Thus it might be similar to sleep deprived individuals, who show an increased pain sensitivity (Schrimpf et al., 2015) that is based on changes of the evaluative brain network, which influences clinical pain complaints but not pain thresholds (Busch et al., 2012). Therefore, it seems that central mechanisms like central sensitization might have a higher impact than peripheral mechanisms like spinal sensitization. This unspecific network of higher brain functions was also called ‘neuromatrix’ (Melzack, 2001). This network that previously was also called ‘painmatrix’ (Iannetti and Mouraux, 2010) is not specific for pain as it is active in various conscious processes (Melzack, 2001). The ‘neuromatrix’ has been divided in three subsystems, namely the lateral (sensory and discriminative), medial (affective and motivational) and frontal (cognitive and evaluative) system (Apkarian et al., 2005, Melzack, 2001). SD patients appear to have mainly changes in medial and frontal parts of the neuromatrix, responsible for affective and evaluative responses. This has specifically shown for pain where affective components were processed in dACC, while sensory components are processed by primary and secondary somatosensory cortex (S1, S2) and the posterior part of the insula (PI) (Treede et al., 1999). According to our meta-analysis the somatosensory cortex is not found to be different between SD patients and HC in our study. Though, differences in the posterior part of the insula, responsible for somatosensory processes of pain, do show differences.
4.1. Limitations
By undertaking an ALE analysis of SD, we were able to include a sample of 243 (204 functional) individuals from ten respectively 8 functional studies, and nine (7 functional) datasets. In our analysis two studies did not contribute to any of the resulting clusters (Gündel et al., 2008, Yoshino et al., 2014). This resulted in a sample, with which we were able to identify neurobiological similarities within this disorder. We have to report, that the foci included in our study are uncorrected for intervening factors, such as varying experimental design, imaging parameters, analysis software, etc. Still, we think that due to the meta-analytic approach, it was possible to identify some commonalities amongst patients via this ALE analysis. Especially when comparing our results with previous theoretical models based on various imaging studies across paradigms and neuroimaging methods, our meta-analytical perspective bases its conclusion on statistical testing and can thus contrast to merely theoretically driven functional models. Still, as some of the studies that were included for the proposal of the circuit model did not report any coordinates and were thus not included in our analysis, some differences between the model of Perez et al. (2015) and ours might be based on the difference between inclusion criteria. Additionally, most studies used mainly female participants. Thus, all results may be applicable to female patients with SD only. In future studies, it would be highly advantageous to look for studies including male patients with SD, as it has been shown that differences between male and female patients do exist in somatoform disorders (Labus et al., 2008, Labus et al., 2013). Finally, specificity of our results for SD might be limited, as Hsu et al. (2009) has shown that gray matter changes in FMS patients are dependent on affective disorder rather than on FMS. A similar effect might underlie the selected studies. This might be especially critical, as it has been found that SD and depression are highly comorbid (Busch et al., 2012). Thus, future functional and structural studies should control for affective disorders. Additionally, a recent meta-analysis revealed that changes in the dorsal ACC, as well as the left and right insula are common to all psychiatric diagnoses (Goodkind et al., 2015) and might not be specific to SD.
5. Conclusions
This meta-analysis underlines neurobiological aspects of SD. In SD, the premotor and supplementary motor cortex, the middle frontal gyrus, the anterior cingulate cortex, the insula and the posterior cingulate cortex seem to be of particular importance. Furthermore, these results seem in line with the hypothesis of “central sensitization” (CS) (Bourke et al., 2015) especially when looking at the neurobiological model (Perez et al., 2015). Still discrepancies do exist and future research is needed in order to better understand the underlying brain network and mechanisms. However, it is important to also consider the influence of a co-morbid depression. Hsu et al. (2009), for example, found no consistent difference in gray matter volume between FMS patients when controlling for affective disorder (Hsu et al., 2009). Thus, future neuroimaging studies should consider the role of affective disorders when investigating somatoform disorder. Still, it is conceivable that many FSS, independent of the clinical impression, show similar neurobiological processes. However, we expect specificities to be at work in each single FSS and thus presented results for SD only. Investigating neurobiological mechanisms of various FSS like SD will help to understand the aetiology of the disorders, lead away from medically unexplained syndromes, and might in the long run help to develop better treatment programs for patients suffering from SD.
Conflict of interests
The authors have no conflicts of interest to report.
Acknowledgements
We would like to thank our interns for helping in numerous ways during the research process. For supporting us with an initial abstract analysis form, we thank Prof. Gerald Gartlehner. We would also like to thank Stephanie Larumbe for her comments on the manuscript.
References
- American-Psychiatric-Association . Amer Psychiatric Pub Incorporated; 2013. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: Dsm-5. [Google Scholar]
- Andresen V., Bach D.R., Poellinger A., Tsrouya C., Stroh A., Foerschler A., Georgiewa P., Zimmer C., Monnikes H. Brain activation responses to subliminal or supraliminal rectal stimuli and to auditory stimuli in irritable bowel syndrome. Neurogastroenterol. Motil. 2005;17:827–837. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2982.2005.00720.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Apkarian A.V., Bushnell M.C., Treede R.D., Zubieta J.K. Human brain mechanisms of pain perception and regulation in health and disease. Eur. J. Pain. 2005;9:463–484. doi: 10.1016/j.ejpain.2004.11.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Atmaca M., Sirlier B., Yildirim H., Kayali A. Hippocampus and amygdalar volumes in patients with somatization disorder. Prog. Neuro-Psychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry. 2011;35:1699–1703. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2011.05.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Balleine B.W., Dickinson A. The effect of lesions of the insular cortex on instrumental conditioning: evidence for a role in incentive memory. J. Neurosci. 2000;20:8954–8964. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-23-08954.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barsky A.J. Amplification, somatization, and the somatoform disorders. Psychosomatics. 1992;33:28–34. doi: 10.1016/S0033-3182(92)72018-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barthas F., Sellmeijer J., Hugel S., Waltisperger E., Barrot M., Yalcin I. The anterior cingulate cortex is a critical hub for pain-induced depression. Biol. Psychiatry. 2014 doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.08.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berman S.M., Naliboff B.D., Suyenobu B., Labus J.S., Stains J., Ohning G., Kilpatrick L., Bueller J.A., Ruby K., Jarcho J., Mayer E.A. Reduced brainstem inhibition during anticipated pelvic visceral pain correlates with enhanced brain response to the visceral stimulus in women with irritable bowel syndrome. J. Neurosci. 2008;28:349–359. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2500-07.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boeckle M., Liegl G., Leitner A., Pieh C. How burdensome is the treatment of patients with somatoform disorders? Z. Psychosom. Med. Psychother. 2014;60:383–391. doi: 10.13109/zptm.2014.60.4.383. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Boeckle M., Schrimpf M., Liegl G., Lahmann C., Pieh C. 2016. Do Fibromyalgia Syndrome and Irritable Bowel Syndrome Share Neural Correlates? A Meta-Analysis of Neuroimaging Studies. (in preparation) [Google Scholar]
- Bourke J.H., Langford R.M., White P.D. The common link between functional somatic syndromes may be central sensitisation. J. Psychosom. Res. 2015;78:228–236. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2015.01.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brooks J., Zambreanu L., Godinez A., Tracey I. Somatotopic organisation of the human insula to painful heat studied with high resolution functional imaging. NeuroImage. 2005;27:201–209. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.03.041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Büchel C., Bornhövd K., Quante M., Glauche V., Bromm B., Weiller C. Dissociable neural responses related to pain intensity, stimulus intensity, and stimulus awareness within the anterior cingulate cortex: a parametric single-trial laser functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J. Neurosci. 2002;22:970–976. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-03-00970.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buckner R.L., Andrews-Hanna J.R., Schacter D.L. The brain's default network. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2008;1124:1–38. doi: 10.1196/annals.1440.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burba B., Oswald R., Grigaliunien V., Neverauskiene S., Jankuviene O., Chue P. A controlled study of alexithymia in adolescent patients with persistent somatoform pain disorder. Can. J. Psychiatr. 2006;51:468–471. doi: 10.1177/070674370605100709. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Busch V., Haas J., Crönlein T., Pieh C., Geisler P., Hajak G., Eichhammer P. Sleep deprivation in chronic somatoform pain-effects on mood and pain regulation. Psychiatry Res. 2012;195:134–143. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2011.07.021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cauda F., Geminiani G., D'Agata F., Sacco K., Duca S., Bagshaw A.P., Cavanna A.E. Functional connectivity of the posteromedial cortex. PLoS One. 2010;5 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0013107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cook D.B., Lange G., Ciccone D.S., Liu W.C., Steffener J., Natelson B.H. Functional imaging of pain in patients with primary fibromyalgia. J. Rheumatol. 2004;31:364–378. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Craig A. Interoception: the sense of the physiological condition of the body. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2003;13:500–505. doi: 10.1016/s0959-4388(03)00090-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Craig A. A new view of pain as a homeostatic emotion. Trends Neurosci. 2003;26:303–307. doi: 10.1016/s0166-2236(03)00123-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Critchley H.D., Mathias C.J., Dolan R.J. Fear conditioning in humans: the influence of awareness and autonomic arousal on functional neuroanatomy. Neuron. 2002;33:653–663. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(02)00588-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Greck M., Scheidt L., Boelter A.F., Frommer J., Ulrich C., Stockum E., Enzi B., Tempelmann C., Hoffmann T., Northoff G. Multimodal psychodynamic psychotherapy induces normalization of reward related activity in somatoform disorder. World J. Biol. Psychiatry. 2011;12:296–308. doi: 10.3109/15622975.2010.539269. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Greck M., Scheidt L., Boelter A.F., Frommer J., Ulrich C., Stockum E., Enzi B., Tempelmann C., Hoffmann T., Han S., Northoff G. Altered brain activity during emotional empathy in somatoform disorder. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2012;33:2666–2685. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21392. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Waal M.W., Arnold I.A., Eekhof J.A., van Hemert A.M. Somatoform disorders in general practice: prevalence, functional impairment and comorbidity with anxiety and depressive disorders. Br. J. Psychiatry. 2004;184:470–476. doi: 10.1192/bjp.184.6.470. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Egloff N., Sabbioni M.E., Salathé C., Wiest R., Juengling D.D. Nondermatomal somatosensory deficits in patients with chronic pain disorder: clinical findings and hypometabolic pattern in FDG-PET. Pain. 2009;145:252–258. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2009.04.016. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eich W., Häuser W., Friedel E., Klement A., Herrmann M., Petzke F., Offenbächer M., Schiltenwolf M., Sommer C., Tölle T. Definition, Klassifikation und Diagnose des Fibromyalgiesyndroms. Schmerz. 2008;22:255–266. doi: 10.1007/s00482-008-0671-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eickhoff S.B. 2014. brainmap.org
- Eickhoff S.B., Laird A.R., Grefkes C., Wang L.E., Zilles K., Fox P.T. Coordinate-based activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis of neuroimaging data: a random-effects approach based on empirical estimates of spatial uncertainty. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2009;30:2907–2926. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20718. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eickhoff S.B., Bzdok D., Laird A.R., Kurth F., Fox P.T. Activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis revisited. NeuroImage. 2012;59:2349–2361. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fitzek S., Fitzek C., Huonker R., Reichenbach J.R., Mentzel H.J., Witte O.W., Kaiser W.A. Event-related fMRI with painful electrical stimulation of the trigeminal nerve. Magn. Reson. Imaging. 2004;22:205–209. doi: 10.1016/j.mri.2003.08.031. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Flor H., Denke C., Schaefer M., Grüsser S. Effect of sensory discrimination training on cortical reorganisation and phantom limb pain. Lancet. 2001;357:1763–1764. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(00)04890-X. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gao W., Zhu H., Giovanello K.S., Smith J.K., Shen D., Gilmore J.H., Lin W. Evidence on the emergence of the brain's default network from 2-week-old to 2-year-old healthy pediatric subjects. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2009;106:6790–6795. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0811221106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goodkind M., Eickhoff S.B., Oathes D.J., Jiang Y., Chang A., Jones-Hagata L.B., Ortega B.N., Zaiko Y.V., Roach E.L., Korgaonkar M.S. Identification of a common neurobiological substrate for mental illness. JAMA Psychiatry. 2015;72:305–315. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2014.2206. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gracely R.H., Petzke F., Wolf J.M., Clauw D.J. Functional magnetic resonance imaging evidence of augmented pain processing in fibromyalgia. Arthritis Rheum. 2002;46:1333–1343. doi: 10.1002/art.10225. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gray M.A., Harrison N.A., Wiens S., Critchley H.D. Modulation of emotional appraisal by false physiological feedback during fMRI. PLoS One. 2007;2 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0000546. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gündel H., Valet M., Sorg C., Huber D., Zimmer C., Sprenger T., Tölle T.R. Altered cerebral response to noxious heat stimulation in patients with somatoform pain disorder. Pain. 2008;137:413–421. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2007.10.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Häuser W., Henningsen P. Fibromyalgia syndrome: a somatoform disorder? Eur. J. Pain. 2014;18:1052–1059. doi: 10.1002/j.1532-2149.2014.00453.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Henningsen P., Zipfel S., Herzog W. Management of functional somatic syndromes. Lancet. 2007;369:946–955. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(07)60159-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hsu M.C., Harris R.E., Sundgren P.C., Welsh R.C., Fernandes C.R., Clauw D.J., Williams D.A. No consistent difference in gray matter volume between individuals with fibromyalgia and age-matched healthy subjects when controlling for affective disorder. Pain. 2009;143:262–267. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2009.03.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Iannetti G., Mouraux A. From the neuromatrix to the pain matrix (and back) Exp. Brain Res. 2010;205:1–12. doi: 10.1007/s00221-010-2340-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Iannetti G., Zambreanu L., Tracey I. Similar nociceptive afferents mediate psychophysical and electrophysiological responses to heat stimulation of glabrous and hairy skin in humans. J. Physiol. 2006;577:235–248. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2006.115675. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Iannetti G.D., Hughes N.P., Lee M.C., Mouraux A. Determinants of laser-evoked EEG responses: pain perception or stimulus saliency? J. Neurophysiol. 2008;100:815–828. doi: 10.1152/jn.00097.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Itti L., Koch C. Computational modelling of visual attention. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2001;2:194–203. doi: 10.1038/35058500. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jensen K.B., Kosek E., Petzke F., Carville S., Fransson P., Marcus H., Williams S.C., Choy E., Giesecke T., Mainguy Y., Gracely R., Ingvar M. Evidence of dysfunctional pain inhibition in fibromyalgia reflected in rACC during provoked pain. Pain. 2009;144:95–100. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2009.03.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kawamoto T., Onoda K., Nakashima K., Nittono H., Yamaguchi S., Ura M. Is dorsal anterior cingulate cortex activation in response to social exclusion due to expectancy violation? An fMRI study. Front. Evol. Neurosci. 2012;4:11. doi: 10.3389/fnevo.2012.00011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Killgore W.D., Yurgelun-Todd D.A. Unconscious processing of facial affect in children and adolescents. Soc. Neurosci. 2007;2:28–47. doi: 10.1080/17470910701214186. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim S.H., Lee Y., Lee S., Mun C.W. Evaluation of the effectiveness of pregabalin in alleviating pain associated with fibromyalgia: using functional magnetic resonance imaging study. PLoS One. 2013;8 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0074099. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kong J., White N.S., Kwong K.K., Vangel M.G., Rosman I.S., Gracely R.H., Gollub R.L. Using fMRI to dissociate sensory encoding from cognitive evaluation of heat pain intensity. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2006;27:715–721. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20213. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kwan C., Diamant N., Pope G., Mikula K., Mikulis D., Davis K. Abnormal forebrain activity in functional bowel disorder patients with chronic pain. Neurology. 2005;65:1268–1277. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000180971.95473.cc. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Labus J.S., Naliboff B.N., Fallon J., Berman S.M., Suyenobu B., Bueller J.A., Mandelkern M., Mayer E.A. Sex differences in brain activity during aversive visceral stimulation and its expectation in patients with chronic abdominal pain: a network analysis. NeuroImage. 2008;41:1032–1043. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.03.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Labus J.S., Gupta A., Coveleskie K., Tillisch K., Kilpatrick L., Jarcho J., Feier N., Bueller J., Stains J., Smith S., Suyenobu B., Naliboff B., Mayer E.A. Sex differences in emotion-related cognitive processes in irritable bowel syndrome and healthy control subjects. Pain. 2013;154:2088–2099. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2013.06.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lacourt T., Houtveen J., van Doornen L. “Functional somatic syndromes, one or many?”: an answer by cluster analysis. J. Psychosom. Res. 2013;74:6–11. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2012.09.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Laird A.R., Eickhoff S.B., Li K., Robin D.A., Glahn D.C., Fox P.T. Investigating the functional heterogeneity of the default mode network using coordinate-based meta-analytic modeling. J. Neurosci. 2009;29:14496. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4004-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Laird A.R., Robinson J.L., McMillan K.M., Tordesillas-Gutiérrez D., Moran S.T., Gonzales S.M., Ray K.L., Franklin C., Glahn D.C., Fox P.T., Lancaster J.L. Comparison of the disparity between Talairach and MNI coordinates in functional neuroimaging data: validation of the Lancaster transform. NeuroImage. 2010;51:677–683. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.02.048. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lancaster J.L., Tordesillas-Gutiérrez D., Martinez M., Salinas F., Evans A., Zilles K., Mazziotta J.C., Fox P.T. Bias between MNI and Talairach coordinates analyzed using the ICBM-152 brain template. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2007;28:1194–1205. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20345. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee S.J., Song H.J., Decety J., Seo J., Kim S.H., Kim S.H., Nam E.J., Kim S.K., Han S.W., Lee H.J., Do Y., Chang Y. Do patients with fibromyalgia show abnormal neural responses to the observation of pain in others? Neurosci. Res. 2013;75:305–315. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2013.01.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lloyd D., Di Pellegrino G., Roberts N. Vicarious responses to pain in anterior cingulate cortex: is empathy a multisensory issue? Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 2004;4:270–278. doi: 10.3758/cabn.4.2.270. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maddock R.J. The retrosplenial cortex and emotion: new insights from functional neuroimaging of the human brain. Trends Neurosci. 1999;22:310–316. doi: 10.1016/s0166-2236(98)01374-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Maihöfner C., Baron R., DeCol R., Binder A., Birklein F., Deuschl G., Handwerker H.O., Schattschneider J. The motor system shows adaptive changes in complex regional pain syndrome. Brain. 2007;130:2671–2687. doi: 10.1093/brain/awm131. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mango . 2014. Res Imaging Inst website. [Google Scholar]
- Melzack R. Pain and the neuromatrix in the brain. J. Dent. Educ. 2001;65:1378–1382. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meyer C., Rumpf H.J., Hapke U., Dilling H., John U. Lebenszeitprävalenz psychischer Störungen in der erwachsenen Allgemeinbevölkerung Ergebnisse der TACOS-Studie. Nervenarzt. 2000;71:535–542. doi: 10.1007/s001150050623. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moher D., Liberati A., Tetzlaff J., Altman D.G. Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. Ann. Intern. Med. 2009;151:264–269. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-151-4-200908180-00135. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Näätänen R., Paavilainen P., Rinne T., Alho K. The mismatch negativity (MMN) in basic research of central auditory processing: a review. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2007;118:2544–2590. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2007.04.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Naliboff B.D., Derbyshire S.W., Munakata J., Berman S., Mandelkern M., Chang L., Mayer E.A. Cerebral activation in patients with irritable bowel syndrome and control subjects during rectosigmoid stimulation. Psychosom. Med. 2001;63:365–375. doi: 10.1097/00006842-200105000-00006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nebel K., Wiese H., Stude P., de Greiff A., Diener H.C., Keidel M. On the neural basis of focused and divided attention. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 2005;25:760–776. doi: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.09.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nijs J., Meeus M., Van Oosterwijck J., Ickmans K., Moorkens G., Hans G., De Clerck L.S. In the mind or in the brain? Scientific evidence for central sensitisation in chronic fatigue syndrome. Eur. J. Clin. Investig. 2012;42:203–212. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2362.2011.02575.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Noll-Hussong M., Otti A., Laeer L., Wohlschlaeger A., Zimmer C., Lahmann C., Henningsen P., Toelle T., Guendel H. Aftermath of sexual abuse history on adult patients suffering from chronic functional pain syndromes: an fMRI pilot study. J. Psychosom. Res. 2010;68:483–487. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2010.01.020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paras M.L., Murad M.H., Chen L.P., Goranson E.N., Sattler A.L., Colbenson K.M., Elamin M.B., Seime R.J., Prokop L.J., Zirakzadeh A. Sexual abuse and lifetime diagnosis of somatic disorders: a systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA. 2009;302:550–561. doi: 10.1001/jama.2009.1091. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Perez D.L., Barsky A.J., Vago D.R., Baslet G., Silbersweig D.A. A neural circuit framework for somatosensory amplification in somatoform disorders. J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 2015;27:e40–e50. doi: 10.1176/appi.neuropsych.13070170. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peyron R., Laurent B., Garcia-Larrea L. Functional imaging of brain responses to pain. A review and meta-analysis (2000) Neurophysiol. Clin. 2000;30:263–288. doi: 10.1016/s0987-7053(00)00227-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Peyron R., Frot M., Schneider F., Garcia-Larrea L., Mertens P., Barral F., Sindou M., Laurent B., Mauguiere F. Role of operculoinsular cortices in human pain processing: converging evidence from PET, fMRI, dipole modeling, and intracerebral recordings of evoked potentials. NeuroImage. 2002;17:1336–1346. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1315. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Phillips K., Clauw D.J. Central pain mechanisms in chronic pain states–maybe it is all in their head. Best Pract. Res. Clin. Rheumatol. 2011;25:141–154. doi: 10.1016/j.berh.2011.02.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pieh C., Lahmann C., Heymann F., Tritt K., Loew T., Busch V., Probst T. Prevalence and comorbidity of somatoform disorder in psychosomatic inpatients: a multicentre study. Z. Psychosom. Med. Psychother. 2010;57:244–250. doi: 10.13109/zptm.2011.57.3.244. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Puri B., Agour M., Gunatilake K., Fernando K., Gurusinghe A., Treasaden I. Reduction in left supplementary motor area grey matter in adult female fibromyalgia sufferers with marked fatigue and without affective disorder: a pilot controlled 3-T magnetic resonance imaging voxel-based morphometry study. J. Int. Med. Res. 2010;38:1468–1472. doi: 10.1177/147323001003800429. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rief W., Henningsen P. Somatoforme Störung (ICD-10 F45) In: Voderholzer U., Hohagen F., editors. Therapie psychischer Erkrankungen: State of the Art Urban & Fischer. 2011. pp. 233–241. [Google Scholar]
- Rief W., Isaac M. The future of somatoform disorders: somatic symptom disorder, bodily distress disorder or functional syndromes? Curr. Opin. Psychiatry. 2014;27:315–319. doi: 10.1097/YCO.0000000000000089. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rodriguez-Raecke R., Niemeier A., Ihle K., Ruether W., May A. Structural brain changes in chronic pain reflect probably neither damage nor atrophy. PLoS One. 2013;8 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0054475. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sawamoto N., Honda M., Okada T., Hanakawa T., Kanda M., Fukuyama H., Konishi J., Shibasaki H. Expectation of pain enhances responses to nonpainful somatosensory stimulation in the anterior cingulate cortex and parietal operculum/posterior insula: an event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J. Neurosci. 2000;20:7438–7445. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-19-07438.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saxe R., Carey S., Kanwisher N. Understanding other minds: linking developmental psychology and functional neuroimaging. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2004;55:87–124. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.142044. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schoedel A.L., Zimmermann K., Handwerker H.O., Forster C. The influence of simultaneous ratings on cortical BOLD effects during painful and non-painful stimulation. Pain. 2008;135:131–141. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2007.05.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schrimpf M., Liegl G., Boeckle M., Leitner A., Geisler P., Pieh C. The effect of sleep deprivation on pain perception in healthy subjects: a meta-analysis. Sleep Med. 2015 doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2015.07.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seminowicz D.A., Davis K.D. Cortical responses to pain in healthy individuals depends on pain catastrophizing. Pain. 2006;120:297–306. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2005.11.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seminowicz D.A., Labus J.S., Bueller J.A., Tillisch K., Naliboff B.D., Bushnell M.C., Mayer E.A. Regional gray matter density changes in brains of patients with irritable bowel syndrome. Gastroenterology. 2010;139:48–57. doi: 10.1053/j.gastro.2010.03.049. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Seminowicz D.A., Wideman T.H., Naso L., Hatami-Khoroushahi Z., Fallatah S., Ware M.A., Jarzem P., Bushnell M.C., Shir Y., Ouellet J.A. Effective treatment of chronic low back pain in humans reverses abnormal brain anatomy and function. J. Neurosci. 2011;31:7540–7550. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5280-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shinoura N., Yamada R., Tabei Y., Shiode T., Itoi C., Saito S., Midorikawa A. The right dorsal anterior cingulate cortex may play a role in anxiety disorder and visual function. Neurol. Res. 2013;35:65–70. doi: 10.1179/1743132812Y.0000000101. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Song G.H., Venkatraman V., Ho K.Y., Chee M.W., Yeoh K.G., Wilder-Smith C.H. Cortical effects of anticipation and endogenous modulation of visceral pain assessed by functional brain MRI in irritable bowel syndrome patients and healthy controls. Pain. 2006;126:79–90. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2006.06.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Song Y., Su Q., Jiang M., Liu F., Yao D., Dai Y., Long L., Yu M., Liu J., Zhang Z., Zhang J., Xiao C., Guo W. Abnormal regional homogeneity and its correlations with personality in first-episode, treatment-naive somatization disorder. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 2015;97:108–112. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2015.05.012. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stoeter P., Bauermann T., Nickel R., Corluka L., Gawehn J., Vucurevic G., Vossel G., Egle U.T. Cerebral activation in patients with somatoform pain disorder exposed to pain and stress: an fMRI study. NeuroImage. 2007;36:418–430. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.01.052. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Strigo I.A., Duncan G.H., Boivin M., Bushnell M.C. Differentiation of visceral and cutaneous pain in the human brain. J. Neurophysiol. 2003;89:3294–3303. doi: 10.1152/jn.01048.2002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Su Q., Yao D., Jiang M., Liu F., Jiang J., Xu C., Dai Y., Yu M., Long L., Li H., Liu J., Zhang Z., Zhang J., Xiao C., Guo W. Dissociation of regional activity in default mode network in medication-naive, first-episode somatization disorder. PLoS One. 2014;9 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0099273. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Swart C., Stins J.F., Beek P.J. Cortical changes in complex regional pain syndrome (CRPS) Eur. J. Pain. 2009;13:902–907. doi: 10.1016/j.ejpain.2008.11.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tagliazucchi E., Balenzuela P., Fraiman D., Chialvo D.R. Brain resting state is disrupted in chronic back pain patients. Neurosci. Lett. 2010;485:26–31. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2010.08.053. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Treede R.-D., Kenshalo D.R., Gracely R.H., Jones A.K. The cortical representation of pain. Pain. 1999;79:105–111. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3959(98)00184-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Turkeltaub P.E., Eickhoff S.B., Laird A.R., Fox M., Wiener M., Fox P. Minimizing within-experiment and within-group effects in activation likelihood estimation meta-analyses. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2012;33:1–13. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21186. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Valet M., Guendel H., Sprenger T., Sorg C., Muehlau M., Zimmer C., Henningsen P., Toelle T.R. Patients with pain disorder show gray-matter loss in pain-processing structures: a voxel-based morphometric study. Psychosom. Med. 2009;71:49–56. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e31818d1e02. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vann S.D., Aggleton J.P., Maguire E.A. What does the retrosplenial cortex do? Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2009;10:792–802. doi: 10.1038/nrn2733. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Waller E., Scheidt C.E., Hartmann A. Attachment representation and illness behavior in somatoform disorders. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis. 2004;192:200–209. doi: 10.1097/01.nmd.0000116463.17588.07. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- White P.D. Chronic fatigue syndrome: is it one discrete syndrome or many? Implications for the “one vs. many” functional somatic syndromes debate. J. Psychosom. Res. 2010;68:455–459. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2010.01.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- White P.D. Functional somatic syndromes may be either “polysyndromic” or “monosyndromic”. J. Psychosom. Res. 2013;74:2–3. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2012.10.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whitfield-Gabrieli S., Ford J.M. Default mode network activity and connectivity in psychopathology. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 2012;8(8) doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032511-143049. (49-+) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- WHO . World Health Organization; Geneva: 1992. The ICD-10 Classification of Mental and Behavioural Disorders: Clinical Descriptions and Diagnostic Guidelines. [Google Scholar]
- Wittchen H.-U., Jacobi F., Rehm J., Gustavsson A., Svensson M., Jönsson B., Olesen J., Allgulander C., Alonso J., Faravelli C. The size and burden of mental disorders and other disorders of the brain in Europe 2010. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2011;21:655–679. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2011.07.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yildirim H., Atmaca M., Sirlier B., Kayali A. Pituitary volumes are reduced in patients with somatization disorder. Psychiatry Investig. 2012;9:278–282. doi: 10.4306/pi.2012.9.3.278. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Yoshino A., Okamoto Y., Kunisato Y., Yoshimura S., Jinnin R., Hayashi Y., Kobayakawa M., Doi M., Oshita K., Nakamura R. Distinctive spontaneous regional neural activity in patients with somatoform pain disorder: a preliminary resting-state fMRI study. Psychiatry Res. Neuroimaging. 2014;221:246–248. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2013.12.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhang J., Jiang M., Yao D., Dai Y., Long L., Yu M., Liu J., Zhang Z., Xiao C., Guo W. Alterations in white matter integrity in first-episode, treatment-naive patients with somatization disorder. Neurosci. Lett. 2015 doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2015.05.037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zink C.F., Pagnoni G., Martin M.E., Dhamala M., Berns G.S. Human striatal response to salient nonrewarding stimuli. J. Neurosci. 2003;23:8092–8097. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-22-08092.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]