Abstract
Despite emerging interest in gene–environment interaction (GxE) effects, there is a dearth of studies evaluating its potential relevance apart from specific hypothesized environments and biometrical variance trends. Using a monozygotic within-pair approach, we evaluated evidence of G×E for body mass index (BMI), depressive symptoms, and cognition (verbal, spatial, attention, working memory, perceptual speed) in twin studies from four countries. We also evaluated whether APOE is a ‘variability gene’ across these measures and whether it partly represents the ‘G’ in G×E effects. In all three domains, G×E effects were pervasive across country and gender, with small-to-moderate effects. Age-cohort trends were generally stable for BMI and depressive symptoms; however, they were variable—with both increasing and decreasing age-cohort trends—for different cognitive measures. Results also suggested that APOE may represent a ‘variability gene’ for depressive symptoms and spatial reasoning, but not for BMI or other cognitive measures. Hence, additional genes are salient beyond APOE.
Keywords: Gene–environment interaction, Twins, BMI, Depression, Cognitive performance, APOE, Variability gene
Introduction
Emerging evidence suggests that gene–environment interplay, including gene–environment interactions (G×E), may contribute to multiple life domains. Here we focus on measures sampled from three domains: physical [body mass index (BMI)]; psychological (depressive symptoms); and cognitive (verbal, spatial, attention/working memory, perceptual speed). The role of G×E among these domains has been variously studied by examining the interaction of a specific environmental exposure with a specific gene variant, how genetic variance may differ due to a specific exposure, or how environmental variance may differ as a function of a specific gene variant. For each of these domains, the current paper concerns establishing evidence of G×E, evaluating age-cohort differences in G×E effects, and testing whether APOE is a variability gene, i.e., in phenotypes where there is evidence of G×E, whether sensitivity to environmental influences varies with APOE gene variants.
As concerns obesity, a variety of twin studies have shown how genetic risk for obesity-related traits may be mitigated (or facilitated) by specific environmental factors. For example, in a Danish Twin Registry study, higher education levels corresponded with substantially reduced genetic variance, as well as shared and nonshared environmental variance, for BMI in women, with similar reductions in shared and nonshared environmental variance for BMI in men (Johnson et al. 2011). Vigorous exercise has also been associated with reduced genetic variance in BMI (McCaffery et al. 2009) in middle-aged men, and higher levels of physical activity have been associated with reduced genetic variance for BMI, waist-hip ratio, and percent body fat (Mustelin et al. 2009; Silventoinen et al. 2009) in young adult twins from Finland and adult twins from Denmark.
In the psychological domain, lower SES indexed by income level has been associated with magnified total variance for internalizing psychopathology in middle adulthood, indexed by major depression, generalized anxiety disorder, panic attacks, and neuroticism (South and Krueger 2011). However, the moderation of total variance for internalizing psychopathology was mainly due to magnification of unique environmental variance at the lowest SES levels. The finding of moderation of internalizing psychopathology via SES in middle adulthood builds on earlier work evaluating G×E for depression and indices of adversity (see Rutter 2012; Rutter and Silberg 2002). In particular, a greater risk of depression has been observed in the presence of a combination of prior stress, particularly childhood maltreatment, and a variant in the serotonin transporter gene promoter region (5-HTTLPR) (Caspi et al. 2003; Karg et al. 2011), although not all studies replicate this finding (see Duncan and Keller 2011).
A potential signal of the presence of G×E for adult cognitive performance has come from observations that unique environmental influences may accelerate in importance with age across multiple cognitive tests (Pahlen et al. under review; Reynolds et al. 2005, 2007), although others have reported stability of twin similarity on a cognitive composite score (McGue and Christensen 2013). Moreover, twin studies examining G×E for mid to late adult cognition are limited compared to childhood and early adulthood. There is some evidence that higher levels of childhood SES are associated with greater genetic influences on general cognitive ability (Turkheimer and Horn 2014), although this effect has not been observed when assessed in adulthood (Grant et al. 2010). In adult male twins, across greater years of parental education, total variance and particularly common environmental variance for word recognition was reduced; whereas genetic variance was relatively stable (Kremen et al. 2005). A personality trait, Experience Seeking (ES), a subscale of the Sensation Seeking Scale (Dutch translation) has been evaluated as a moderator of genetic and environmental variance in cognitive ability in an adult twin sample with results suggesting reduced genetic variance but increased nonshared environmental variance at the highest levels of ES (Vinkhuyzen et al. 2012).
‘Agnostic’ tests of G×E
Typically G×E is tested with a selected environmental feature or exposure, or a specific gene target in mind, or both. However, an agnostic test has been available, without identified genes or environments, as first proposed by Fisher (1925; see Martin et al. 1983 for correction). Specifically, Fisher delineated a test of heterogeneity that relies on evaluating monozygotic (MZ) within-pair differences (Fisher 1925; Martin et al. 1983), i.e., the test compares mean squared pair differences for a trait with the mean absolute pair differences squared. The extent to which these values differ supports a mixture of distributions of the within-pair differences rather than one distribution of differences and suggests there is possible G×E interaction. This indicates a differential sensitivity of genotypes to environments such that the MZ pair differences, which reflect nonshared environment, vary according to particular genotypes. MZ within-pair approaches are rarely used (Cornes et al. 2008; Martin 2000; Martin et al. 1983; Reynolds et al. 2007; Surakka et al. 2012), particularly since the advent of genome-wide genotyping, but such an approach may usefully quantify the extent of heterogeneity and identify the likely presence of G×E. Coupling an agnostic general test with potential genetic markers using MZ pairs can be more powerful than evaluating GxE in population-based samples of unrelated individuals (Visscher and Posthuma 2010).
Variability genes, i.e., the ‘G’ in G×E
A significant Fisher test of heterogeneity could indicate the presence of G×E interaction, i.e., differential sensitivity of particular genotypes to particular environments, or could reflect a shared environment by nonshared environment interaction, C×E. To support that an observed significant heterogeneity test is due to G×E, it is useful to consider measured genes that may explain such heterogeneity (Berg et al. 1989; Martin 2000; Martin et al. 1983). The genes of interest may be regarded as ‘variability genes’ (Berg et al. 1989), i.e., genes that are associated with trait variation and not simply associated with trait mean (Martin 2000). APOE may be of particular interest in this regard. The APOE gene, coding for the major cholesterol transporter in the brain, and its ε4 haplotype in particular, has demonstrated associations with cognitive decline, Alzheimer’s disease (AD) and dementia (e.g., Bennet et al. 2010; Davies et al. 2014; Reynolds et al. 2006; Schellenberg and Montine 2012). In addition, APOE has also shown some evidence of associations with, or moderation of, risk factors that are predictive of cognitive decline and dementia, including BMI (e.g., Besser et al. 2014; Keller et al. 2011) and depression (e.g., Karlsson et al. 2015; Skoog et al. 2015).
APOE has shown evidence that it may act as a variability gene; that is, the effects of environmental risk and protective factors have been shown to differ according to APOE genotype. For example, MZ twin pairs who were APOE ε4—were more variable in their semantic memory trajectories, whereas those who were ε4+ were less variable (Reynolds et al. 2007). Additionally, individuals with particular APOE haplotypes may be differentially sensitive to dietary and exercise interventions, albeit not consistently (Brown et al. 2013a Carvalho-Wells et al. 2012; Gomez-Pinilla and Hillman 2013; Hotting and Roder 2013). For example, in those who lead sedentary lives, amyloid burden is greater for those with ε4+ compared to other APOE haplotypes, whereas for those who engage in physical activity, amyloid burden does not vary across APOE haplotypes (Brown et al. 2013b; Head et al. 2012). Moreover, a recent experimental study in sedentary women suggested a particular benefit of acute exercise to ε4+ carriers on a cognitive inhibition task (Stroop) in comparison to a spatial attention task (Posner) that engages the prefrontal region to a lesser extent, but no benefit accrued for non-ε4 individuals across tasks (De Marco et al. 2015). MZ twin pair differences in semantic memory change have also been associated with twin-pair differences in depressive symptoms but in this case only among non-ε4 individuals (Reynolds et al. 2007). Thus, taken together, emerging evidence across multiple traits and domains supports the role of APOE as a variability gene and suggest that the associations of APOE may be complex and depend in part on environmental factors. Indeed, for BMI APOE may show differing patterns of evidence for sensitivity, as compared to cognition or depression traits.
The aims of the current study were to evaluate general evidence of G×E for BMI, depressive symptoms, and cognitive performance in twin studies participating in the Interplay of Genes and Environment across Multiple Studies (IGEMS) consortium (Pedersen et al. 2013). We further considered whether there were age-cohort trends in G×E. Once general evidence for G×E was evaluated, we considered specific genetic aspects further, by testing the extent to which APOE was a variability gene across these traits. That is, we evaluated whether different APOE haplotypes were more or less sensitive to environmental factors and thereby showed differences in the variance of pair differences in depressive symptoms, BMI and cognitive performance.
Methods
Samples
The current analysis sample includes individuals from up to nine twin studies representing four countries: the United States, Sweden, Denmark and Finland, from the IGEMS consortium (Pedersen et al. 2013). The primary analyses considered complete MZ twin pairs to evaluate heterogeneity of within-pair differences and homogeneity of within-pair variance by APOE haplotypes (see Table 1). Each of the respective studies described below obtained approvals by their Institutional Review Boards, or equivalent, to carry out the original data collection, obtaining informed consent from participants as required.
Table 1.
MZ pairs contributing to G×E Analyses
Study | Country | Sex | BMI |
Depressive Sx |
Cognitive (1+ test) |
|||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
N | NAPOE | Mean age | SD | N | NAPOE | Mean age | SD | N | NAPOE | Mean age | SD | |||
VETSA | USA | M | 349 | 340 | 55.35 | 2.53 | 346 | 337 | 55.36 | 2.52 | 347 | 339 | 55.33 | 2.49 |
MTSADA | USA | M | 69 | – | 56.70 | 12.26 | 117 | – | 58.98 | 9.93 | 66 | – | 56.19 | 12.12 |
F | 150 | – | 54.33 | 13.35 | 210 | – | 56.65 | 12.33 | 149 | – | 54.41 | 13.08 | ||
MIDUS | USA | M | 132 | – | 45.56 | 11.46 | 33 | – | 54.70 | 11.70 | 83 | – | 55.19 | 11.12 |
F | 155 | – | 44.35 | 12.39 | 48 | – | 52.63 | 10.98 | 96 | – | 52.87 | 11.56 | ||
SATSA | SWE | M | 112 | 52 | 55.69 | 13.87 | 102 | 53 | 57.00 | 13.40 | 59 | 52 | 62.69 | 7.45 |
F | 138 | 70 | 59.80 | 13.59 | 116 | 72 | 59.97 | 13.28 | 83 | 73 | 64.15 | 9.03 | ||
Octo-Twin | SWE | M | 41 | 37 | 82.81 | 2.47 | 47 | 43 | 82.59 | 2.90 | 42 | 38 | 82.88 | 2.63 |
F | 66 | 62 | 83.48 | 2.95 | 67 | 63 | 83.19 | 3.27 | 55 | 50 | 82.83 | 2.31 | ||
TOSS | SWE | M | 120 | – | 46.48 | 4.50 | 101 | – | 46.57 | 4.53 | 121 | – | 46.50 | 4.49 |
F | 104 | – | 43.40 | 5.27 | 213 | – | 43.69 | 4.64 | 259 | – | 43.68 | 4.66 | ||
MADT | DEN | M | 335 | 191 | 56.57 | 6.41 | 333 | 191 | 56.54 | 6.40 | 330 | 189 | 56.45 | 6.40 |
F | 327 | 198 | 56.36 | 6.41 | 328 | 197 | 56.36 | 6.41 | 326 | 195 | 56.33 | 6.41 | ||
LSADT | DEN | M | 171 | 52 | 75.05 | 4.57 | 172 | 51 | 74.92 | 4.54 | 132 | 37 | 74.31 | 3.73 |
F | 274 | 98 | 76.05 | 4.79 | 266 | 97 | 75.87 | 4.73 | 190 | 77 | 74.95 | 4.03 | ||
FTC | FIN | M | 405 | – | 60.14 | 3.69 | 407 | – | 59.71 | 3.70 | – | – | – | – |
F | 602 | – | 59.85 | 3.69 | 602 | – | 59.38 | 3.69 | – | – | – | – | ||
Total pairs | – | – | 3550 | 1100 | – | – | 3508 | 1104 | – | – | 2338 | 1050 | – | – |
Sx Symptoms, USA United States of America, SWE Sweden, DEN Denmark, FIN Finland
USA
Data were available from the Vietnam Era Twin Study of Aging (VETSA) (Kremen et al. 2013), Minnesota Twin Study of Adult Development and Aging (MTSADA) (Finkel et al. 1995), and the Midlife Development in the United States (MIDUS) twin study (Kendler et al. 2000; Radler 2014). The VETSA study included only male twin pairs (51–60 years), while from MTSADA (25–92 years) and MIDUS (34–82 years) we included same-sex male and female pairs.
Sweden
Data were available from three population-based samples of same-sex male and female twins that originated from the Swedish Twin Registry (Lichtenstein et al. 2006; Magnusson et al. 2013): the Swedish Adoption/Twin Study of Aging (SATSA) (Pedersen et al. 1991), the Origins of Variance in the Oldest-Old (OCTO-twin) (McClearn et al. 1997), and the Twin-Offspring Study in Sweden (TOSS) (Neiderhiser et al. 2007). Data for SATSA twins (39–88 years) came from the first available questionnaire or in-person testing wave, available during one of 6 respective assessment waves. Data on OCTO-twin participants (79–99 years) came from the first assessment. Twin data from the parent generation (32–60 years) of the TOSS study were used in the current study.
Denmark
The Longitudinal Study of Aging Danish Twins (LSADT) (70–100 years) and the Middle Aged Danish Twins (MADT) (45–68 years) included pairs drawn from the Danish Twin Register (McGue and Christensen 2013; Skytthe et al. 2013). Data from the first assessment wave were used in the present study.
Finland
The Finnish Adult Twin Cohort (FTC; Kaprio and Koskenvuo 2002) sample included data from the fourth assessment wave of twins born 1945–1957, done as a postal questionnaire survey in 2011 to 2012 (Kaprio 2013).
Measures
All studies had data from at least one of the following three domains.
BMI
BMI was computed in standard fashion as weight, measured in kilograms, divided by height squared, measured in meters (kg/m2). BMI scores were adjusted for self-report versus measured assessments (Johnson et al. 2012) given that self-reports are biased towards over-reporting of height yet under-reporting of weight (Dahl et al. 2010), i.e., Adjusted BMI = 0.35 + 1.038*(BMIself-rept). Studies in the current analysis with measured height and weight assessments included OCTO-Twin and VETSA, the remainder of the studies provided self-reported data. Prior to analysis, BMI scores were rank-normalized to reduce non-normality (c.f., Reynolds et al. 2007; Surakka et al. 2012).
Depression
Depressive symptoms were measured with either the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression (CESD) scale (Radloff 1977) or the Cambridge Mental Disorders of the Elderly Examination (CAMDEX) as modified by McGue and Christensen (McGue and Christensen 1997). To create a common metric, both scales were collected from a separate crosswalk sample, and item response theory methods were applied in order to compare items from the two measures and create a conversion table between the scales (Gatz et al. in press). We retained those items from both CESD and CAMDEX that loaded on the respective affect and somatic subscales. The co-calibrated score is expressed in CAMDEX units, such that the total score can range from 16 for someone who endorses no symptoms of depression to 46. After harmonization, scores were rank-normalized to reduce non-normality.
Cognitive performance
Five measures of cognitive ability spanning four cognitive domains were considered in the current study: verbal (Synonyms), spatial (Block Design), attention and working memory (Digit Span Forward and Backward), and perceptual speed (Symbol Digit). Each measure was available in at least two studies. Number of individuals available for each test was therefore variable, reflecting the differential availability of the tests across studies. Cognitive tests and harmonization procedures have been described previously (Pahlen et al. under review). In short, those in the analysis sample completed at least one of the cognitive tests and scored 24 or above on the Mini-Mental State Exam (MMSE; Folstein et al. 1975); a total of 7.3 % of the total sample were excluded based on the MMSE criteria. Scores were residualized for sex and transformed to T-score scaling (M = 50.0 and SD = 10.0) against the reference age group 50 to 59.99 years (Pahlen et al. under review) and subjected to winsorizing within age group for values falling outside of ±3 SDs. Prior to within-pair analyses, scores were rank-normalized to reduce non-normality.
Genotyping
APOE haplotypes were available for a subset of studies and were categorized as ε2+ (ε22, ε23, ε24), ε33, and ε4+ (ε34, ε44). Samples with MZ pairs and genotyping included: VETSA (US) SATSA and OCTO-Twin (Swedish), MADT and LSADT (Danish). Genotyping procedures for VETSA, SATSA and OCTO-Twin have been described elsewhere (Reynolds et al. 2013; Schultz et al. 2008). For the Danish samples, APOE haplotypes were formed from two genotyped SNPs, rs429358 and rs7412, that for MADT were based on TaqMan® SNP Genotyping Assays (Applied Biosystems, Foster City, CA, USA) and for LSADT were based on custom-designed assays.
APOE haplotype frequencies are reported in supplementary Table 1 (Table S1). Hardy–Weinberg Equilibrium based on computations for a three allele system were calculated for each study and met (p ≥ 0.121). MZ twins who were not directly genotyped were assigned their cotwin’s value.
Statistical analysis
We evaluated the presence of G×E by applying a test of mixture distributions of MZ within-pair differences overall, and separately by country, sex, and age group. Given that the data are cross-sectional such that age group and birth cohort are unable to be dissociated, we refer to age group as age-cohort. Specifically, we applied a test first proposed by Fisher (Fisher 1925; Martin et al. 1983). The test evaluates the difference between mean squared pair differences for a trait and the mean absolute pair differences squared as follows (Fisher 1925; Martin et al. 1983):
(1.0) |
and corresponding standard error as (Fisher 1925; Martin et al. 1983):
(1.1) |
A one-tailed t test was used to evaluate significance (Δ/se), given that the expected values were assumed to be positive (Martin et al. 1983), with df equal to the number of pairs minus 1.0. To address multiple testing, we conducted false-discovery rate (FDR) tests (Benjamini and Hochberg 1995; Weinkauf 2012) and provided Holm-Bonferroni adjusted p values as well for each set of tests by trait (Gaetano 2013; Holm 1979).
In addition, effect size rs were calculated from the t statistics (Rosenthal 1991) to consider the potential impact of G×E across country, sex, and age-cohort, apart from power considerations:
(2) |
A measure of the heterogeneity of effect size rs (ESrs) were calculated according to the Chi square test outlined by Snedecor & Cochran (1989; as cited in Rosenthal 1991).
In a subset of available samples, we considered measured genes to substantiate G×E and not C×E. Specifically, heterogeneity of variance by APOE haplotype was evaluated using SAS Proc Mixed (SAS Inc, Cary, NC) specifying between and within pair random effects. Analyses of within-pair variation were adjusted for average effects of APOE haplotype, country, sex and age. A series of model constraints were tested on within pair variances, considering APOE haplotype differences within and across country or sex. Given the potential differential regional and within-country impact of e4 on health outcomes, such as mortality (Ewbank 2004) and Alzheimer’s disease (Ward et al. 2012), as well as differential impact of APOE on cognitive outcomes for women versus men (Altmann et al. 2014; Damoiseaux et al. 2012; Farrer et al. 1997), we evaluated whether APOE effects could be generalized. Hence, we tested whether within-pair variances for each APOE haplotype could be constrained: (1) across men and women within country (i.e., ε2+m = ε2+f, ε33m=ε33 f, ε4+m=ε4+f), and (2) across country (i.e., ε2+US = ε2+SWE = ε2+DEN, etc.). Last, we tested whether within-pair variances could be constrained equal within country across the three APOE haplotype groups (i.e., ε2+=ε33 = ε4+) to evaluate the significance of an APOE effect on variability. Sensitivity analyses considered adjustments when dropping individuals with the APOE ε24 haplotype. We did not evaluate age trends in within pair variances by APOE haplotype, primarily due to the reductions in sample sizes of those with both phenotypic data and genotyping and to resultant confounding of age-cohort and country.
Follow-up tests of association at the mean level based on APOE haplotype were undertaken in SAS Proc Mixed (SAS Inc, Cary, NC) allowing for within and between pair variances to differ by country; analyses adjusted for average effects of age, sex and country. Specifically, we tested whether entering the APOE haplotype (ε2+, ε33, ε4+) led to a significant improvement in fit based on a two-degree of freedom test.
Results
Fisher heterogeneity test
The full sample heterogeneity tests for BMI included 3550 complete MZ pairs and for depressive symptoms 3508 MZ pairs. For the cognitive measures, 2338 MZ pairs had at least one cognitive test where both members participated and met MMSE criterion; test availability across studies the analysis samples ranged from 390 to 1727 MZ pairs. The Fisher (1925) test suggested significant within-pair heterogeneity in the full sample for BMI, p = 3.54E-34, and depressive symptoms, p = 1.99E-41 (see Table 2), with significant within-pair heterogeneity for each age-cohort (p ≤ 6.87E–03; see Table 2), as well as both sexes and all four countries (p ≤ 3.90E–04; see supplement Table S2). Overall effect size rs (ESrs) were small for both BMI and depressive symptoms (median = .19, .21, respectively). Effect sizes were consistent across age-cohort groups for both BMI and depressive symptoms [χ2 (4) ≤ 2.55, p ≥ 6.36E–01] (see Table 2). BMI showed consistent small ESrs across country [χ2 (3) = 3.68, p = 3.68E–01]. Although depressive symptoms showed small and signficant evidence for G×E for each country, the ESrs were significantly variable with lower effect sizes for Sweden and Finland and higher effects for US and Demark [χ2 (3) = 18.77, p = 3.06E–04] (see supplement Table S2).
Table 2.
Test of mixture distributions of MZ within pair differences in full sample and by age-cohort
d̅ | delta | se | t | p | p′ | ESr | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
BMI (Npair) | ||||||||
Full sample (3550) | 0.61 | 0.65 | 0.07 | 0.01 | 12.26 | 3.54E–34 | 4.25E–33 | 0.20 |
<50 years (643) | 0.51 | 0.45 | 0.04 | 0.01 | 4.65 | 2.05E–06 | 8.21E–06 | 0.18 |
50–59 (1351) | 0.60 | 0.64 | 0.07 | 0.01 | 7.93 | 2.36E–15 | 2.12E–14 | 0.21 |
60–69 (929) | 0.61 | 0.63 | 0.05 | 0.01 | 4.49 | 3.97E–06 | 1.19E–05 | 0.15 |
70–79 (429) | 0.69 | 0.84 | 0.09 | 0.02 | 4.09 | 2.60E–05 | 5.21E–05 | 0.19 |
80+ (198) | 0.76 | 1.01 | 0.10 | 0.04 | 2.49 | 6.87E–03 | 6.87E–03 | 0.17 |
Depressive Sx (Npair) | ||||||||
Full sample (3508) | 0.81 | 1.19 | 0.14 | 0.01 | 13.61 | 1.99E–41 | 2.39E–40 | 0.22 |
<50 years (550) | 0.80 | 1.12 | 0.13 | 0.03 | 4.99 | 4.06E–07 | 2.03E–06 | 0.21 |
50–59 (1326) | 0.81 | 1.16 | 0.14 | 0.02 | 7.98 | 1.57E–15 | 1.25E–14 | 0.21 |
60–69 (995) | 0.77 | 1.08 | 0.14 | 0.02 | 7.88 | 4.37E–15 | 3.06E–14 | 0.24 |
70–79 (436) | 0.92 | 1.51 | 0.17 | 0.04 | 4.52 | 3.91E–06 | 1.57E–05 | 0.21 |
80+ (201) | 0.86 | 1.30 | 0.14 | 0.05 | 2.80 | 2.81E–03 | 2.81E–03 | 0.19 |
Synonyms (Npair) | ||||||||
Full sample (912) | 0.64 | 0.68 | 0.04 | 0.01 | 3.53 | 2.16E–04 | 2.16E–03 | 0.12 |
<50 years (318) | 0.56 | 0.50 | 0.01 | 0.01 | 0.92 | 1.79E–01 | 3.59E–01 | 0.05 |
50–59 years (463) | 0.69 | 0.79 | 0.03 | 0.02 | 1.74 | 4.13E–02 | 2.39E–01 | 0.08 |
60–69 years (48) | 0.57 | 0.64 | 0.13 | 0.05 | 2.71 | 4.64E–03 | 3.25E–02 | 0.37 |
70–79 years (32) | 0.64 | 0.62 | −0.01 | 0.06 | −0.19 | 5.75E–01 | 5.75E–01 | 0.03 |
80+ years (51) | 0.72 | 0.94 | 0.13 | 0.07 | 1.79 | 3.98E–02 | 2.39E–01 | 0.25 |
Block design (Npair) | ||||||||
Full sample (393) | 0.56 | 0.57 | 0.07 | 0.02 | 4.61 | 2.75E–06 | 2.75E–05 | 0.23 |
<50 years (60) | 0.46 | 0.37 | 0.04 | 0.03 | 1.43 | 7.85E–02 | 1.57E–01 | 0.18 |
50–59 years (85) | 0.47 | 0.37 | 0.03 | 0.02 | 1.36 | 8.94E–02 | 1.57E–01 | 0.15 |
60–69 years (124) | 0.59 | 0.60 | 0.06 | 0.03 | 1.98 | 2.47E–02 | 1.23E–01 | 0.18 |
70–79 years (37) | 0.59 | 0.66 | 0.11 | 0.06 | 1.84 | 3.69E–02 | 1.23E–01 | 0.29 |
80+ years (87) | 0.68 | 0.82 | 0.09 | 0.05 | 1.89 | 3.08E–02 | 1.23E–01 | 0.20 |
Digits forward (Npair) | ||||||||
Full Sample (1551) | 0.83 | 1.21 | 0.12 | 0.02 | 7.28 | 2.60E–13 | 2.86E–12 | 0.18 |
<50 years (124) | 0.91 | 1.39 | 0.07 | 0.07 | 1.13 | 1.31E–01 | 1.94E–01 | 0.10 |
50–59 years (695) | 0.80 | 1.13 | 0.12 | 0.02 | 5.31 | 7.54E–08 | 6.78E–07 | 0.20 |
60–69 years (285) | 0.79 | 1.01 | 0.04 | 0.03 | 1.30 | 9.69E–02 | 1.94E–01 | 0.08 |
70–79 years (313) | 0.94 | 1.51 | 0.12 | 0.05 | 2.56 | 5.52E–03 | 1.66E–02 | 0.14 |
80+ years (134) | 0.80 | 1.26 | 0.26 | 0.06 | 4.46 | 8.67E–06 | 5.20E–05 | 0.36 |
Digits backward (Npair) | ||||||||
Full sample (1722) | 0.83 | 1.21 | 0.14 | 0.02 | 8.81 | 1.51E–18 | 1.66E–17 | 0.21 |
<50 years (194) | 0.82 | 1.25 | 0.18 | 0.05 | 3.84 | 8.23E–05 | 3.20E–04 | 0.27 |
50–59 years (745) | 0.82 | 1.18 | 0.12 | 0.02 | 5.38 | 4.87E–08 | 3.41E–07 | 0.19 |
60–69 years (323) | 0.82 | 1.11 | 0.06 | 0.03 | 1.83 | 3.42E–02 | 3.42E–02 | 0.10 |
70–79 years (327) | 0.88 | 1.39 | 0.16 | 0.04 | 3.82 | 8.01E–05 | 3.20E–04 | 0.21 |
80+ years (133) | 0.74 | 1.10 | 0.25 | 0.05 | 4.92 | 1.28E–06 | 7.70E–06 | 0.39 |
Symbol digit (Npair) | ||||||||
Full sample (1256) | 0.58 | 0.57 | 0.04 | 0.01 | 5.17 | 1.39E–07 | 1.39E–06 | 0.14 |
<50 years (190) | 0.54 | 0.51 | 0.04 | 0.02 | 2.26 | 1.26E–02 | 7.56E–02 | 0.16 |
50–59 years (360) | 0.59 | 0.61 | 0.07 | 0.02 | 4.20 | 1.66E–05 | 1.49E–04 | 0.22 |
60–69 years (371) | 0.59 | 0.58 | 0.03 | 0.02 | 1.86 | 3.18E–02 | 1.59E–01 | 0.10 |
70–79 years (256) | 0.57 | 0.54 | 0.03 | 0.02 | 1.85 | 3.25E–02 | 1.59E–01 | 0.12 |
80+ years (79) | 0.63 | 0.63 | 0.01 | 0.04 | 0.33 | 3.70E–01 | 8.92E–01 | 0.04 |
D absolute pair difference, delta ; , ESr effect size where df= Npair-1. p-values are based on one-tailed t-tests; bolded p values are signficant according to FDR tests. p ′ = Holm-Bonferroni sequentially adjusted p values, where bolded are significant
For cognitive performance, G×E was suggested in the full sample (p ≤ 2.16E–04) (see Table 2). The ESrs were small, ranging from .12 to .23, and were not significantly heterogeneous from one another [χ2 (4) = 7.71, p = 1.03E–01] (see Table 2, supplement Table S2). As depicted in Fig. 1, three prototypical age-cohort trends in ESRs were noticeable: (a) Block Design represented a linear pattern of increasingly stronger effect sizes across age groups: (b) Digits Backward represented a nonlinear u-shaped pattern with peaks before age of 50 (ESr = .27) and after age of 80 (ESr = .39) with a similar trend for Digits Forward (not shown), and (c) Symbol Digit displayed a pattern of decreasing effect sizes with age-cohort, with the peak at ages 50–59 (ESr = .22). The pattern for Synonyms was less consistent and is not shown in Fig. 1 (but see Table 2). The FDR tests and Holm-Bonferroni adjusted p-values generally supported the age-based patterns described in terms of significance (see Table 2); however, heterogeneity tests of ESrs among age-cohorts suggested that only Digits Backward reached significance [χ2 (4) = 10.14, p ≤ 3.81E–02], with a trend effect in Digits Forward (p = 5.07E–02).
Fig. 1.
Effect size r (ESr) for evidence for mixture distribution suggesting possible G×E: representative cognitive tests
G×E was indicated on all cognitive measures both for women (p ≤ 5.48E–03) and for men (p ≤ 4.56E–05), apart from Synonyms (p = 6.26E–02). For all five measures, there was evidence of significant heterogeneity of within pair differences across all countries, although for Symbol Digit, only Denmark showed a significant effect (p = 8.62E–12; ESr = .23), and for Synonyms, only Sweden (p = 1.11E–03; ESr = .13) (see supplemental Table S2).
Measured G×E: APOE
In the primary analyses of APOE as a variability gene, we focused on testing for heterogeneity in the variance of pair differences among APOE haplotypes evaluating whether variances could be constrained by country and sex, adjusting for average effects of age, sex and APOE haplotypes on the trait scores (see Table 3). We did not evaluate age trends in within pair variances by APOE haplotype for BMI, depression or for cognition, primarily due to the reductions in sample sizes of those with phenotypic data and genotyping and consequent confounding of age-cohort and country. Moreover, we note that the general age-cohort consistency of the evidence for G×E observed for BMI and depressive symptoms. Significant findings are described further below. Analyses of mean level associations (i.e., whether individuals score higher or lower on the trait on average) are reported (see Table 4), with no significant associations observed; description of mean trends is provided below for traits showing significant evidence for APOE x variance effects. Dropping APOE ε24 individuals from the analysis did not alter any of the conclusions.
Table 3.
Homogeneity of within pair variance by APOE
Measure | Country | Within pair σ2 | Likelihood ratio tests | Total Npair |
||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
APOE |
Equate males &
females |
Equate countries |
Equate ε2+, ε33,
ε4+ |
|||||||||||
ε2+ | ε33 | ε4+ | Δχ2 | df | p | Δχ2 | df | p | Δχ2 | df | p | |||
BMI | USA (m) | 0.39 | 0.30 | 0.31 | 19.64 | 6 | 3.21E–03 | 10.85 | 6 | 9.31E–02 | 3.67 | 6 | 7.22E–01 | 672 |
SWE (m) | 0.32 | 0.26 | 0.32 | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | |
DEN (m) | 0.24 | 0.19 | 0.24 | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | |
SWE (f) | 0.26 | 0.45 | 0.52 | – | – | – | 4.50 | 3 | 2.12E–01 | 6.53 | 4 | 1.63E–01 | 428 | |
DEN (f) | 0.30 | 0.31 | 0.40 | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | |
Depressive Sx | USA | 0.91 | 0.44 | 0.39 | 2.05 | 6 | 9.15E–01 | 44.99 | 6 | 4.70E–08 | 19.78 | 6 | 3.04E–03 | 1104 |
SWE | 0.55 | 0.47 | 0.41 | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | |
DEN | 0.76 | 0.71 | 0.91 | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | |
Synonyms | USA | 0.45 | 0.43 | 0.38 | 1.91 | 3 | 5.92E–01 | 1.62 | 3 | 6.54E–01 | 1.77 | 4 | 7.79E–01 | 506 |
SWE | 0.32 | 0.40 | 0.32 | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | |
Block design | SWE | 0.40 | 0.33 | 0.15 | 5.76 | 3 | 1.24E–01 | – | – | – | 11.91 | 2 | 2.60E–03 | 203 |
Digits forward | USA | 0.51 | 0.46 | 0.50 | 3.32 | 6 | 7.68E–01 | 9.24 | 6 | 1.60E–01 | 4.58 | 6 | 5.98E–01 | 1038 |
SWE | 0.53 | 0.66 | 0.63 | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | |
DEN | 0.66 | 0.64 | 0.49 | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | |
Digits back | USA | 0.43 | 0.63 | 0.52 | 6.73 | 6 | 3.46E–01 | 10.04 | 6 | 1.23E–01 | 6.71 | 6 | 3.48E–01 | 1031 |
SWE | 0.32 | 0.47 | 0.44 | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | |
DEN | 0.57 | 0.51 | 0.58 | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | |
Symbol digit | SWE | 0.24 | 0.30 | 0.31 | 6.03 | 6 | 4.20E–01 | 0.08 | 3 | 9.94E–01 | 1.71 | 4 | 7.88E–01 | 618 |
DEN | 0.26 | 0.31 | 0.31 | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – | – |
USA United States of America, SWE Sweden, DEN Denmark, m male, f female, Sx symptoms. Random effects model adjusted for average effect of APOE haplotype, country, sex and age
Table 4.
Average effects of APOE: adjusted for age, sex and country
Measure |
APOE
haplotype |
Test of APOE
effect |
Npair | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
ε2+ | ε33 | ε4+ | Δχ2 | df | p | ||
BMI | −0.19 | −0.12 | −0.13 | 1.04 | 2 | 5.94E–01 | 1100 |
Depressive Sx | −0.62 | −0.53 | −0.50 | 2.53 | 2 | 2.83E–01 | 1104 |
Synonyms | −0.06 | 0.09 | 0.03 | 2.33 | 2 | 3.12E–01 | 506 |
Block design | 0.04 | 0.29 | 0.26 | 2.78 | 2 | 2.49E–01 | 364 |
Digits forward | −0.04 | −0.02 | 0.09 | 3.98 | 2 | 1.37E–01 | 1038 |
Digits backward | 0.12 | 0.08 | 0.18 | 2.81 | 2 | 2.45E–01 | 1031 |
Symbol digit | 0.17 | 0.06 | 0.18 | 3.78 | 2 | 1.51E–01 | 618 |
Sx symptoms. Random effects models adjusted for average effects of age (centered at 65 years), sex (males = −0.5, females = +0.5), and country (reference = Denmark)
BMI
Variances of absolute pair differences by APOE haplotype could not be constrained across sex within country [χ2 (6) = 19.64, p = 3.21E–03] (see Table 3 for within pair variance estimates and test statistics); hence, further analyses were conducted separately for men and women. Nonsignificant country differences in within pair variances within APOE haplotype were observed for men and women (p = 9.31E–02). In addition, within pair variances could be constrained across APOE haplotype within country (p = 7.22E–01). In sum, within pair variances for the APOE haplotypes differed between men and women, but across country the APOE haplotype effects were not statistically different from each other. Hence, there was no support for an APOE effect on within pair variability, but there was heterogeneity of within pair variances across men and women suggesting that female pairs are more variable than male pairs in terms of the degree to which twins differ from their cotwin in BMI.
Depressive symptoms
Variances of absolute pair differences by APOE haplotype could be constrained across sex within country [χ2(6) = 2.05, p = 9.15E–01]; hence analyses were conducted collapsing men and women together (see Table 3). Haplotype-based within pair variances could not be constrained across country [χ2(6) = 44.99, p = 4.70E–08]. Thus, haplotype-based within pair variances were allowed to vary within country and significant differences by APOE haplotype were observed [χ2(6) = 19.78, p = 3.04E–03]. Figure 2a indicates that APOE effects could be observed in the US and in the Swedish samples, with smaller variances of pair differences for APOE ε4+ compared to larger variances for APOE ε33 and ε2+ . This pattern suggests that those with APOE ε4 + may be less affected by environmental factors compared to the other haplotypes. Last, we followed up these variance tests of within-pair differences to consider whether APOE effects were evident for average depressive symptom scores, with no significant differences observed (p = 2.83E–01).
Fig. 2.
Variance of absolute MZ within pair differences adjusted for age by APOE: a depressive symptoms, b block design
Cognitive performance
Among the five cognitive measures considered, only Block Design showed evidence of significant haplotype differences in within pair variances (see Table 3). Variances by APOE haplotype could be constrained across sex [χ2(3) = 5.76, p = 1.24E–01]; hence, analyses were conducted collapsing men and women together. As Block Design and APOE genotyping were only available in two Swedish samples, no country comparisons could be conducted. Significant differences in within pair variances by APOE haplotype were observed [χ2(2) = 11.91, p = 2.60E–03]. Smaller within pair variances of pair differences for APOE ε4 + versus larger variances for APOE ε2 + were observed (see Fig. 2b). This pattern indicates that those with APOE ε4 + may be less affected by environmental factors compared to those with APOE ε33 and APOE ε2 + , and is consistent with the overall pattern observed for depressive symptoms above. Last, we followed up these within-pair variance tests to consider whether APOE effects were evident for average Block Design performance scores, and no significant differences were observed (p = 2.49E–01).
Discussion
We evaluated general evidence of G×E for BMI, depressive symptoms, and cognitive performance in twin studies from four countries, i.e., US, Sweden, Denmark, and Finland. We further evaluated whether APOE is a variability gene across these traits and represents, in part, the G in the G×E effects. We observed that across physical, psychological, and cognitive domains, G×E was pervasive across country and sex showing small to moderate effect sizes. While modest, the presence of these effects across domains argues for the importance of more routinely considering gene–environment interaction in biometric models. Generally stable age-cohort trends were observed for BMI and depressive symptoms. However, age-cohort trends varied by cognitive trait domains with some showing decreasing G×E effects and some showing increasing G×E effects. Last, APOE may represent one variability gene for depressive symptoms and spatial reasoning, but not for BMI or other cognitive tests. Hence additional variability genes are salient beyond APOE.
BMI
BMI evidenced small G×E effects, and these effects were consistent across country, sex, and age-cohort. This is perhaps not surprising in that the candidate G×E studies evaluating education or exercise on genetic variations in BMI have reported G×E in samples from various countries represented in our study (US, Denmark, Finland; Johnson et al. 2011; Lajunen et al. 2012; McCaffery et al. 2009; Mustelin et al. 2009; Silventoinen et al. 2009; Silventoinen et al. 2004). Others have suggested that the genetic variance for BMI may be increasing in later born Swedish cohorts (Rokholm et al. 2011), perhaps suggesting a complex cohort/generational G×E given changing dietary and activity patterns amongst others. Further examinations of longitudinal data across multiple cohorts would be informative as to the extent to which G×E for BMI is dynamic across age versus birth cohort.
Despite agnostic evidence of G×E, no APOE associations were observed with within-pair variability for BMI. Prior studies have noted interactions of APOE with BMI, obesity, or of BMI variants (e.g., FTO) with outcomes such as metabolic traits (Elosua et al. 2003), dementia risk (Keller et al. 2011) or dementia progression (Besser et al. 2014). However, GWAS have not observed direct genetic association of APOE with mean BMI (Locke et al. 2015). Nonetheless, our lack of findings of APOE in the current analysis suggests that other variability genes, e.g., perhaps based on a polygenic risk score of 97 BMI loci (Locke et al. 2015), are relevant to pursue given evidence of G×E we observed in the agnostic Fisher analysis.
Depressive symptoms
Depressive symptoms showed consistently small but significant G×E effect sizes for sex and age-cohort, with lower effect sizes for Sweden and Finland and higher for US and Demark. Our findings of ubiquitous small G×E effects furthers earlier evidence there is not simply an effect of the environment (E) on depressive symptom levels but that there is genetically influenced sensitivity to environmental factors that may foster (or mitigate) depression (c.f., Kendler et al. 1995).
We observed associations of APOE with within-pair variability in depression symptoms but no effect on mean depression scores. Results varied across country; evidence for APOE as the ‘G’ in G×E was found for the U.S. and Sweden, but not the Danish sample. Indeed, APOE associations with average depression symptoms and risk for a diagnosis of depression have been mixed across studies, perhaps due to differential population effects or study designs (Skoog et al. 2015). APOE has been associated with depressive symptomatology and depression diagnosis in late adulthood in a prospective study of Swedish individuals even when excluding prevalent or incident dementia cases (Skoog et al.). Other comparably sized (or larger) cross-sectional and longitudinal studies have not found such effects (e.g., Locke et al. 2013; Schultz et al. 2008; Surtees et al. 2009); however, the average sample age tended to be between ages 55 and 61, suggesting that the association of APOE and depressive symptoms may tend towards older adults.
Our results suggest that the effect of APOE on depression would appear to lie, not in main effects, but in the role of APOE in magnifying or reducing the effects of environmental risk factors for depressive symptoms. Specifically, MZ pairs carrying the ε4 haplotypes showed the smallest within-pair differences while those carrying the ε2 haplotypes the largest within-pair differences in depression scores. Hence, the depressive symptoms experienced by those with APOE ε4 + may be less driven by environmental factors, and more by familial or endogenous factors, compared to depressive symptoms experienced by those with other APOE haplotypes. Together with the observed age-cohort trends, such an interpretation would be consistent with the role of vascular factors and white matter changes in late onset depression (Nebes et al. 2001; Taylor et al. 2013).
Cognition
Different cognitive performance domains showed different patterns of results with respect to the agnostic Fisher G×E tests, with the pattern possibly reflecting the difference between age-sensitive cognitive tests versus more age-robust tests. The most age-sensitive test, perceptual speed indexed by Symbol Digit task performance, showed peak G×E effects in the younger age-cohorts compared to later age cohorts; whereas tests of attention, working memory, and spatial performance showed higher G×E in later age-cohorts. These latter tests tend to show later declines, accelerating across the adult lifespan (Salthouse 2009; Schaie 1994). We note that the complexity of findings underscores the need to consider specific cognitive abilities beyond general measures of ability.
In the APOE analyses, where we adjusted for age given the restricted sample size, we observed an effect for the spatial task, Block Design, but no other tasks. For Block Design, as for depressive symptoms, those with APOE ε4 + may be less affected by environmental factors compared to the other APOE haplotypes. It is worth noting that Block Design performance may be a salient predictor of subsequent cognitive dysfunction (e.g., Andel et al. 2001; Bozoki et al. 2001; Hamilton et al. 2008; Tabert et al. 2006). Hence those at risk for dysfunction or decline may show relatively less sensitivity to environmental factors compared to those without this risk allele, whose performance does reflect environmental influences.
The lack of association of APOE with variability for other cognitive measures could be viewed as puzzling. APOE associations with cognitive performance levels in non-demented adults have been mixed overall. However, we note that age-related change may be more salient than cross-sectional differences in performance level in terms of gene associations (e.g., Davies et al. 2014; Finkel et al. 2011; Salmon et al. 2013) as well as observing G×E effects (Reynolds et al. 2007). For example, in longitudinal work in SATSA using the within MZ pair methods, we observed significant G×E effects on semantic, episodic, and working memory trajectory features (e.g., linear and nonlinear change) but negligible effects on overall performance level (Reynolds et al. 2007). Hence, longitudinal examinations may reveal unique effects not apparent in baseline performance data. Another interpretation, given the longitudinal findings, might suggest that effects may not show up strongly until later ages. If age is adjusted for, then age periods where APOE or another gene or genes have a particular effect may be missed.
The smaller within-pair differences for those with APOE ε4 may seem to be counter-intuitive given that in some instances ε4 individuals may show greater rather than lesser sensitivity to particular environments that are relevant to brain reserve, not only dietary and exercise factors as mentioned above (Brown et al. 2013a, b; Carvalho-Wells et al. 2012; De Marco et al. 2015; Head et al. 2012), but also head injury and neuropsychological functioning and dementia (e.g., Sundstrom et al. 2004; Sundstrom et al. 2007; Tang et al. 1996) and combat exposure and PTSD (Kimbrel et al. 2015; Lyons et al. 2013). While a diathesis-stress model would expect ε4 always to act in the same direction, others have proposed the concept of a plasticity gene (Belsky et al. 2009; Belsky and Pluess 2009). Such an interpretation would be consistent with smaller within-pair differences for ε4 and greater sensitivity to some exposures or contexts but lessened sensitivity to other exposures or contexts.
Strengths, limitations, and future directions
The strengths of the current study include the relatively large samples of MZ pairs and the ability to evaluate (and replicate) G×E trends in physical, psychological, and cognitive domains across up to four countries, by sex, and age cohorts. Moreover, in a subset of studies we were able to evaluate a well-characterized gene, APOE, as a potential variability gene. The primary limitation was that a single-occasion was available for evaluation of G×E for BMI, depressive symptoms and cognition. Moreover, not all studies had available APOE genotyping, hampering age-cohort investigations. Moreover, we had a limited set of cognitive measures and, hence, future studies would benefit from inclusion of measures of executive function and episodic memory.
Overall, future research directions should consider the possible measured environmental factors, i.e., the ‘E’ in G×E, given that G×E was ubiquitously observed albeit with generally small impact. Indeed, particularly for depression and spatial reasoning, the impact of any measured environmental factors may be modified by the APOE gene.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
IGEMS is supported by the National Institutes of Health Grant no. R01 AG037985. SATSA was supported by Grants R01 AG04563, R01 AG10175, the MacArthur Foundation Research Network on Successful Aging, the Swedish Council For Working Life and Social Research (FAS) (97:0147:1B, 2009-0795) and Swedish Research Council (825-2007-7460, 825-2009-6141). OCTO-Twin was supported by Grant R01 AG08861. Gender was supported by the MacArthur Foundation Research Network on Successful Aging, The Axel and Margaret Ax:son Johnson’s Foundation, The Swedish Council for Social Research, and the Swedish Foundation for Health Care Sciences and Allergy Research. TOSS was supported by Grant R01 MH54610 from the National Institute of Health. The Danish Twin Registry is supported by Grants from The National Program for Research Infrastructure 2007 from the Danish Agency for Science and Innovation, the Velux Foundation and the US National Institute of Health (P01 AG08761). The Minnesota Twin Study of Adult Development and Aging was supported by NIA Grant R01 AG 06886. VETSA was supported by National Institute of Health Grants NIA R01 AG018384, R01 AG018386, R01 AG022381, and R01 AG022982, and, in part, with resources of the VA San Diego Center of Excellence for Stress and Mental Health. The Cooperative Studies Program of the Office of Research & Development of the United States Department of Veterans Affairs has provided financial support for the development and maintenance of the Vietnam Era Twin (VET) Registry. This MIDUS study was supported by the John D. and Catherine T. MacArthur Foundation Research Network on Successful Midlife Development and by National Institute on Aging Grant AG20166. The Finnish Twin Cohort study has been supported by Academy of Finland Center of Excellence in Complex Disease Genetics (Grant numbers: 213506, 129680), the Academy of Finland (Grants 265240, 263278 & 264146 to JK) and ENGAGE – European Network for Genetic and Genomic Epidemiology, FP7-HEALTH-F4-2007, Grant agreement number 201413. The content of this manuscript is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the NIA/NIH, or the VA.
Appendix
Members of the consortium on Interplay of Genes and Environment across Multiple Studies (IGEMS) include: Nancy L. Pedersen (Department of Medical Epidemiology and Biostatistics, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden, and Department of Psychology, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA), Kaare Christensen (Department of Epidemiology, University of Southern Denmark, Odense, Denmark), Anna K. Dahl Aslan (Institute of Gerontology, School of Health Sciences, Jönköping University, Jönköping, Sweden), Deborah Finkel (Department of Psychology, Indiana University Southeast, New Albany, IN), Carol E. Franz (Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, CA), Margaret Gatz (Department of Medical Epidemiology and Biostatistics, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden, and Department of Psychology, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA), Briana N. Horwitz (Department of Psychology, California State University, Fullerton, CA), Boo Johansson (Department of Psychology, University of Gothenburg, Gothenburg, Sweden), Wendy Johnson (Department of Psychology and Centre for Cognitive Ageing and Cognitive Epidemiology, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, UK), Jaakko Kaprio, Department of Public Health, University of Helsinki, Helsinki, Finland), William S. Kremen (Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, and VA Center of Excellence for Stress and Mental Health, La Jolla, CA), Robert Krueger (Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN), Michael J. Lyons (Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Boston University, Boston, MA), Matt McGue (Department of Psychology, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN), Jenae M. Neiderhiser (Department of Psychology, ThePennsylvania State University, University Park, PA), Matthew S. Panizzon (Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, CA), Inge Petersen (Department of Epidemiology, University of Southern Denmark, Odense, Denmark), and Chandra A. Reynolds (Department of Psychology, University of California-Riverside, Riverside, CA).
Footnotes
Electronic supplementary material The online version of this article (doi:10.1007/s10519-015-9761-3) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.
Compliance with Ethical Standards
Conflict of interest Dr. Korhonen has served as a consultant on nicotine dependence for Pfizer (Finland) in 2011–2015.
Research involving Human Participants and/or Animals All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the respective institutional and/or national research committees for each participating study providing archival data, and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.
Informed consent Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants from their respective parent study.
References
- Altmann A, Tian L, Henderson VW, Greicius MD Alzheimer’s Disease Neuroimaging Initiative Investigators. Sex modifies the APOE-related risk of developing Alzheimer disease. Ann Neurol. 2014;75(4):563–573. doi: 10.1002/ana.24135. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andel R, Gatz M, Pedersen NL, Reynolds CA, Johansson B, Berg S. Deficits in controlled processing may predict dementia: a twin study. J Gerontol B. 2001;56B(6):P347. doi: 10.1093/geronb/56.6.p347. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Belsky J, Pluess M. Beyond diathesis stress: differential susceptibility to environmental influences. Psychol Bull. 2009;135(6):885–908. doi: 10.1037/a0017376. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Belsky J, Jonassaint C, Pluess M, Stanton M, Brummett B, Williams R. Vulnerability genes or plasticity genes? Mol Psychiatry. 2009;14(8):746–754. doi: 10.1038/mp.2009.44. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J Royal Stat Soc B. 1995;57(1):289–300. [Google Scholar]
- Bennet AM, Reynolds CA, Gatz M, Blennow K, Pedersen NL, Prince JA. Pleiotropy in the presence of allelic heterogeneity: alternative genetic models for the influence of APOE on serum LDL, CSF amyloid-beta42, and dementia. J Alzheimer’s Dis. 2010;22(1):129–134. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2010-100864. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berg K, Kondo I, Drayna D, Lawn R. “Variability gene” effect of cholesteryl ester transfer protein (CETP) genes. Clin Genet. 1989;35(6):437–445. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-0004.1989.tb02969.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Besser LM, Gill DP, Monsell SE, Brenowitz W, Meranus DH, Kukull W, Gustafson DR. Body mass index, weight change, and clinical progression in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer disease. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2014;28(1):36–43. doi: 10.1097/WAD.0000000000000005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bozoki A, Giordani B, Heidebrink JL, Berent S, Foster NL. Mild cognitive impairments predict dementia in nondemented elderly patients with memory loss. Arch Neurol. 2001;58(3):411–416. doi: 10.1001/archneur.58.3.411. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown BM, Peiffer JJ, Martins RN. Multiple effects of physical activity on molecular and cognitive signs of brain aging: can exercise slow neurodegeneration and delay Alzheimer’s disease? Mol Psychiatry. 2013a;18(8):864–874. doi: 10.1038/mp.2012.162. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brown BM, Peiffer JJ, Taddei K, Lui JK, Laws SM, Gupta VB, Taddei T, Ward VK, Rodrigues MA, Burnham S, Rainey-Smith SR, Villemagne VL, Bush A, Ellis KA, Masters CL, Ames D, Macaulay SL, Szoeke C, Rowe CC, Martins RN. Physical activity and amyloid-beta plasma and brain levels: results from the Australian imaging, biomarkers and lifestyle study of ageing. Mol Psychiatry. 2013b;18(8):875–881. doi: 10.1038/mp.2012.107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carvalho-Wells AL, Jackson KG, Lockyer S, Lovegrove JA, Minihane AM. APOE genotype influences triglyceride and C-reactive protein responses to altered dietary fat intake in UK adults. Am J Clin Nutr. 2012;96(6):1447–1453. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.112.043240. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Caspi A, Sugden K, Moffitt TE, Taylor A, Craig IW, Harrington H, McClay J, Mill J, Martin J, Braithwaite A, Poulton R. Influence of life stress on depression: moderation by a polymorphism in the 5-HTT gene. Science. 2003;301(5631):386–389. doi: 10.1126/science.1083968. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cornes BK, Lind PA, Medland SE, Montgomery GW, Nyholt DR, Martin NG. Replication of the association of common rs9939609 variant of FTO with increased BMI in an Australian adult twin population but no evidence for gene by environment (G [times] E) interaction. Int J Obes. 2008;33(1):75–79. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2008.223. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dahl AK, Hassing LB, Fransson EI, Pedersen NL. Agreement between self-reported and measured height, weight and body mass index in old age—a longitudinal study with 20 years of follow-up. Age Ageing. 2010;39(4):445–451. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afq038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Damoiseaux JS, Seeley WW, Zhou J, Shirer WR, Coppola G, Karydas A, Rosen HJ, Miller BL, Kramer JH, Greicius MD. gender modulates the APOE ε4 effect in healthy older adults: convergent evidence from functional brain connectivity and spinal fluid tau levels. J Neurosci. 2012;32(24):8254–8262. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0305-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Davies G, Harris SE, Reynolds CA, Payton A, Knight HM, Liewald DC, Lopez LM, Luciano M, Gow AJ, Corley J, Henderson R, Murray C, Pattie A, Fox HC, Redmond P, Lutz MW, Chiba-Falek O, Linnertz C, Saith S, Haggarty P, McNeill G, Ke X, Ollier W, Horan M, Roses AD, Ponting CP, Porteous DJ, Tenesa A, Pickles A, Starr JM, Whalley LJ, Pedersen NL, Pendleton N, Visscher PM, Deary IJ. A genome-wide association study implicates the APOE locus in nonpathological cognitive ageing. Mol Psychiatry. 2014;19(1):76–87. doi: 10.1038/mp.2012.159. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- De Marco M, Clough PJ, Dyer CE, Vince RV, Waby JS, Midgley AW, Venneri A. Apolipoprotein E epsilon4 allele modulates the immediate impact of acute exercise on prefrontal function. Behav Genet. 2015;45(1):106–116. doi: 10.1007/s10519-014-9675-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Duncan LE, Keller MC. A critical review of the first 10 years of candidate gene-by-environment interaction research in psychiatry. Am J Psychiatry. 2011;168(10):1041–1049. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.11020191. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Elosua R, Demissie S, Cupples LA, Meigs JB, Wilson PW, Schaefer EJ, Corella D, Ordovas JM. Obesity modulates the association among APOE genotype, insulin, and glucose in men. Obes Res. 2003;11(12):1502–1508. doi: 10.1038/oby.2003.201. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ewbank DC. The APOE gene and differences in life expectancy in Europe. J Gerontol A. 2004;59(1):B16–B20. doi: 10.1093/gerona/59.1.b16. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Farrer LA, Cupples LA, Haines JL, Hyman B, Kukull WA, Mayeux R, Myers RH, Pericak-Vance MA, Risch N, van Duijn CM. Effects of age, sex, and ethnicity on the association between apolipoprotein E genotype and Alzheimer disease. A meta-analysis. APOE and Alzheimer disease meta analysis consortium. JAMA. 1997;278(16):1349–1356. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Finkel D, Whitfield K, McGue M. Genetic and environmental influences on functional age: a twin study. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 1995;50(2):P104–P113. doi: 10.1093/geronb/50b.2.p104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Finkel D, Reynolds CA, Larsson M, Gatz M, Pedersen NL. Both odor identification and ApoE-epsilon4 contribute to normative cognitive aging. Psychol Aging. 2011;26(4):872–883. doi: 10.1037/a0023371. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fisher RA. The resemblance between twins, a statistical examination of Lauterbach’s measurements. Genetics. 1925;10:569–579. doi: 10.1093/genetics/10.6.569. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. “Mini-mental state”: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 1975;12(3):189–198. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gaetano J. Holm-Bonferroni Sequential Correction: An EXCEL Calculator. 1.1.xlsx HsccB (ed) 2013 https://www.researchgate.net/publication/236969037_Holm-Bonferroni_Sequential_Correction_An_EXCEL_Calculator.
- Gatz M, Reynolds CA, Finkel D, Hahn CJ, Zhou Y, Zavala C for the IGEMS Consortium. Data harmonization in aging research: Not so fast. Experimental aging research. in press doi: 10.1080/0361073X.2015.1085748. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gomez-Pinilla F, Hillman C. The influence of exercise on cognitive abilities. Compr Physiol. 2013;3(1):403–428. doi: 10.1002/cphy.c110063. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grant MD, Kremen WS, Jacobson KC, Franz C, Xian H, Eisen SA, Toomey R, Murray RE, Lyons MJ. Does parental education have a moderating effect on the genetic and environmental influences of general cognitive ability in early adulthood? Behav Genet. 2010;40(4):438–446. doi: 10.1007/s10519-010-9351-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hamilton JM, Salmon DP, Galasko D, Raman R, Emond J, Hansen LA, Masliah E, Thal LJ. Visuospatial deficits predict rate of cognitive decline in autopsy-verified dementia with Lewy bodies. Neuropsychology. 2008;22(6):729–737. doi: 10.1037/a0012949. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Head D, Bugg JM, Goate AM, Fagan AM, Mintun MA, Benzinger T, Holtzman DM, Morris JC. Exercise engagement as a moderator of the Effects of APOE genotype on amyloid deposition. Arch Neurol. 2012;69(5):636–643. doi: 10.1001/archneurol.2011.845. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holm S. A simple sequentially rejective multiple test procedure. Scand J Stat. 1979;6(2):65–70. [Google Scholar]
- Hotting K, Roder B. Beneficial effects of physical exercise on neuroplasticity and cognition. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2013;37(9 Pt B):2243–2257. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.04.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Johnson W, Dahl A for the IGEMS consortium. Education moderates genetic and environmental influences on body mass index: Findings from the consortium on Interplay of Genes and Environment across Multiple Studies (IGEMS) Gerontological Society of America, San Diego. 2012 [Google Scholar]
- Johnson W, Kyvik KO, Skytthe A, Deary IJ, Sorensen TI. Education modifies genetic and environmental influences on BMI. PLoS One. 2011;6(1):e16290. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0016290. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaprio J. The finnish twin cohort study: an update. Twin Res Hum Genet. 2013;16(1):157–162. doi: 10.1017/thg.2012.142. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaprio J, Koskenvuo M. Genetic and environmental factors in complex diseases: the older Finnish Twin Cohort. Twin Res. 2002;5(5):358–365. doi: 10.1375/136905202320906093. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Karg K, Burmeister M, Shedden K, Sen S. The serotonin transporter promoter variant (5-httlpr), stress, and depression meta-analysis revisited: evidence of genetic moderation. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2011;68(5):444–454. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.189. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Karlsson IK, Bennet AM, Ploner A, Andersson TML, Reynolds CA, Gatz M, Pedersen NL. Apolipoprotein E ε4 genotype and the temporal relationship between depression and dementia. Neurobiol Aging. 2015;36:1751–1756. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2015.01.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Keller L, Xu W, Wang HX, Winblad B, Fratiglioni L, Graff C. The obesity related gene, FTO, interacts with APOE, and is associated with Alzheimer’s disease risk: a prospective cohort study. J Alzheimer’s Dis. 2011;23(3):461–469. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2010-101068. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kendler KS, Kessler RC, Walters EE, MacLean C, Neale MC, Heath AC, Eaves LJ. Stressful life events, genetic liability, and onset of an episode of major depression in women. Am J Psychiatry. 1995;152(6):833–842. doi: 10.1176/ajp.152.6.833. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kendler KS, Thornton LM, Gilman SE, Kessler RC. Sexual orientation in a U.S. national sample of twin and nontwin sibling pairs. Am J Psychiatry. 2000;157(11):1843–1846. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.157.11.1843. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kimbrel NA, Hauser MA, Garrett M, Ashley-Koch A, Liu Y, Dennis MF, Klein RC, Veterans Affairs Mid-Atlantic Mental Illness Research Clinical E, Center W, Beckham JC. Effect of the APOE epsilon4 allele and combat exposure on PTSD among Iraq/Afghanistan-Era Veterans. Depression and anxiety. 2015 [Google Scholar]
- Kremen WS, Jacobson KC, Xian H, Eisen SA, Waterman B, Toomey R, Neale MC, Tsuang MT, Lyons MJ. Heritability of word recognition in middle-aged men varies as a function of parental education. Behav Genet. 2005;35(4):417–433. doi: 10.1007/s10519-004-3876-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kremen WS, Franz CE, Lyons MJ. VETSA: the Vietnam era twin study of aging. Twin Res Hum Genet. 2013;16(1):399–402. doi: 10.1017/thg.2012.86. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lajunen HR, Kaprio J, Rose RJ, Pulkkinen L, Silventoinen K. Genetic and environmental influences on BMI from late childhood to adolescence are modified by parental education. Obesity (Silver Spring) 2012;20(3):583–589. doi: 10.1038/oby.2011.304. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lichtenstein P, Sullivan PF, Cnattingius S, Gatz M, Johansson S, Carlstrom E, Bjork C, Svartengren M, Wolk A, Klareskog L, de Faire U, Schalling M, Palmgren J, Pedersen NL. The Swedish twin registry in the third millennium: an update. Twin Res Hum Genet. 2006;9(6):875–882. doi: 10.1375/183242706779462444. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Locke DE, Dueck AC, Stonnington CM, Knopman DS, Geda YE, Caselli RJ. Depressive symptoms in healthy apolipoprotein E epsilon4 carriers and noncarriers: a longitudinal study. J Clin Psychiatry. 2013;74(12):1256–1261. doi: 10.4088/JCP.13m08564. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Locke AE, Kahali B, Berndt SI, Justice AE, Pers TH, Day FR, Powell C, Vedantam S, Buchkovich ML, Yang J, Croteau-Chonka DC, Esko T, Fall T, Ferreira T, Gustafsson S, Kutalik Z, Luan J, Magi R, Randall JC, Winkler TW, Wood AR, Workalemahu T, Faul JD, Smith JA, Hua Zhao J, Zhao W, Chen J, Fehrmann R, Hedman AK, Karjalainen J, Schmidt EM, Absher D, Amin N, Anderson D, Beekman M, Bolton JL, Bragg-Gresham JL, Buyske S, Demirkan A, Deng G, Ehret GB, Feenstra B, Feitosa MF, Fischer K, Goel A, Gong J, Jackson AU, Kanoni S, Kleber ME, Kristiansson K, Lim U, Lotay V, Mangino M, Mateo Leach I, Medina-Gomez C, Medland SE, Nalls MA, Palmer CD, Pasko D, Pechlivanis S, Peters MJ, Prokopenko I, Shungin D, Stancakova A, Strawbridge RJ, Ju Sung Y, Tanaka T, Teumer A, Trompet S, van der Laan SW, van Setten J, Van Vliet-Ostaptchouk JV, Wang Z, Yengo L, Zhang W, Isaacs A, Albrecht E, Arnlov J, Arscott GM, Attwood AP, Bandinelli S, Barrett A, Bas IN, Bellis C, Bennett AJ, Berne C, Blagieva R, Bluher M, Bohringer S, Bonnycastle LL, Bottcher Y, Boyd HA, Bruinenberg M, Caspersen IH, Ida Chen YD, Clarke R, Daw EW, de Craen AJ, Delgado G, Dimitriou M, Doney AS, Eklund N, Estrada K, Eury E, Folkersen L, Fraser RM, Garcia ME, Geller F, Giedraitis V, Gigante B, Go AS, Golay A, Goodall AH, Gordon SD, Gorski M, Grabe HJ, Grallert H, Grammer TB, Grassler J, Gronberg H, Groves CJ, Gusto G, Haessler J, Hall P, Haller T, Hallmans G, Hartman CA, Hassinen M, Hayward C, Heard-Costa NL, Helmer Q, Hengstenberg C, Holmen O, Hottenga JJ, James AL, Jeff JM, Johansson A, Jolley J, Juliusdottir T, Kinnunen L, Koenig W, Koskenvuo M, Kratzer W, Laitinen J, Lamina C, Leander K, Lee NR, Lichtner P, Lind L, Lindstrom J, Sin Lo K, Lobbens S, Lorbeer R, Lu Y, Mach F, Magnusson PK, Mahajan A, McArdle WL, McLachlan S, Menni C, Merger S, Mihailov E, Milani L, Moayyeri A, Monda KL, Morken MA, Mulas A, Muller G, Muller-Nurasyid M, Musk AW, Nagaraja R, Nothen MM, Nolte IM, Pilz S, Rayner NW, Renstrom F, Rettig R, Ried JS, Ripke S, Robertson NR, Rose LM, Sanna S, Scharnagl H, Scholtens S, Schumacher FR, Scott WR, Seufferlein T, Shi J, Vernon Smith A, Smolonska J, Stanton AV, Steinthorsdottir V, Stirrups K, Stringham HM, Sundstrom J, Swertz MA, Swift AJ, Syvanen AC, Tan ST, Tayo BO, Thorand B, Thorleifsson G, Tyrer JP, Uh HW, Vandenput L, Verhulst FC, Vermeulen SH, Verweij N, Vonk JM, Waite LL, Warren HR, Waterworth D, Weedon MN, Wilkens LR, Willenborg C, Wilsgaard T, Wojczynski MK, Wong A, Wright AF, Zhang Q, LifeLines Cohort S, Brennan EP, Choi M, Dastani Z, Drong AW, Eriksson P, Franco-Cereceda A, Gadin JR, Gharavi AG, Goddard ME, Handsaker RE, Huang J, Karpe F, Kathiresan S, Keildson S, Kiryluk K, Kubo M, Lee JY, Liang L, Lifton RP, Ma B, McCarroll SA, McKnight AJ, Min JL, Moffatt MF, Montgomery GW, Murabito JM, Nicholson G, Nyholt DR, Okada Y, Perry JR, Dorajoo R, Reinmaa E, Salem RM, Sandholm N, Scott RA, Stolk L, Takahashi A, Tanaka T, Van’t Hooft FM, Vinkhuyzen AA, Westra HJ, Zheng W, Zondervan KT, Consortium AD, Group A-BW, Consortium CAD, Consortium CK, Glgc Icbp, Investigators M, Mu TC, Consortium MI, Consortium P, ReproGen C, Consortium G, International Endogene C, Heath AC, Arveiler D, Bakker SJ, Beilby J, Bergman RN, Blangero J, Bovet P, Campbell H, Caulfield MJ, Cesana G, Chakravarti A, Chasman DI, Chines PS, Collins FS, Crawford DC, Cupples LA, Cusi D, Danesh J, de Faire U, den Ruijter HM, Dominiczak AF, Erbel R, Erdmann J, Eriksson JG, Farrall M, Felix SB, Ferrannini E, Ferrieres J, Ford I, Forouhi NG, Forrester T, Franco OH, Gansevoort RT, Gejman PV, Gieger C, Gottesman O, Gudnason V, Gyllensten U, Hall AS, Harris TB, Hattersley AT, Hicks AA, Hindorff LA, Hingorani AD, Hofman A, Homuth G, Hovingh GK, Humphries SE, Hunt SC, Hypponen E, Illig T, Jacobs KB, Jarvelin MR, Jockel KH, Johansen B, Jousilahti P, Jukema JW, Jula AM, Kaprio J, Kastelein JJ, Keinanen-Kiukaanniemi SM, Kiemeney LA, Knekt P, Kooner JS, Kooperberg C, Kovacs P, Kraja AT, Kumari M, Kuusisto J, Lakka TA, Langenberg C, Le Marchand L, Lehtimaki T, Lyssenko V, Mannisto S, Marette A, Matise TC, McKenzie CA, McKnight B, Moll FL, Morris AD, Morris AP, Murray JC, Nelis M, Ohlsson C, Oldehinkel AJ, Ong KK, Madden PA, Pasterkamp G, Peden JF, Peters A, Postma DS, Pramstaller PP, Price JF, Qi L, Raitakari OT, Rankinen T, Rao DC, Rice TK, Ridker PM, Rioux JD, Ritchie MD, Rudan I, Salomaa V, Samani NJ, Saramies J, Sarzynski MA, Schunkert H, Schwarz PE, Sever P, Shuldiner AR, Sinisalo J, Stolk RP, Strauch K, Tonjes A, Tregouet DA, Tremblay A, Tremoli E, Virtamo J, Vohl MC, Volker U, Waeber G, Willemsen G, Witteman JC, Zillikens MC, Adair LS, Amouyel P, Asselbergs FW, Assimes TL, Bochud M, Boehm BO, Boerwinkle E, Bornstein SR, Bottinger EP, Bouchard C, Cauchi S, Chambers JC, Chanock SJ, Cooper RS, de Bakker PI, Dedoussis G, Ferrucci L, Franks PW, Froguel P, Groop LC, Haiman CA, Hamsten A, Hui J, Hunter DJ, Hveem K, Kaplan RC, Kivimaki M, Kuh D, Laakso M, Liu Y, Martin NG, Marz W, Melbye M, Metspalu A, Moebus S, Munroe PB, Njolstad I, Oostra BA, Palmer CN, Pedersen NL, Perola M, Perusse L, Peters U, Power C, Quertermous T, Rauramaa R, Rivadeneira F, Saaristo TE, Saleheen D, Sattar N, Schadt EE, Schlessinger D, Slagboom PE, Snieder H, Spector TD, Thorsteinsdottir U, Stumvoll M, Tuomilehto J, Uitterlinden AG, Uusitupa M, van der Harst P, Walker M, Wallaschofski H, Wareham NJ, Watkins H, Weir DR, Wichmann HE, Wilson JF, Zanen P, Borecki IB, Deloukas P, Fox CS, Heid IM, O’Connell JR, Strachan DP, Stefansson K, van Duijn CM, Abecasis GR, Franke L, Frayling TM, McCarthy MI, Visscher PM, Scherag A, Willer CJ, Boehnke M, Mohlke KL, Lindgren CM, Beckmann JS, Barroso I, North KE, Ingelsson E, Hirschhorn JN, Loos RJ, Speliotes EK. Genetic studies of body mass index yield new insights for obesity biology. Nature. 2015;518(7538):197–206. doi: 10.1038/nature14177. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lyons MJ, Genderson M, Grant MD, Logue M, Zink T, McKenzie R, Franz CE, Panizzon M, Lohr JB, Jerskey B, Kremen WS. Gene–environment interaction of ApoE genotype and combat exposure on PTSD. Am J Med Gen B Neuropsych Gen. 2013;162B(7):762–769. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.32154. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Magnusson PK, Almqvist C, Rahman I, Ganna A, Viktorin A, Walum H, Halldner L, Lundstrom S, Ullen F, Langstrom N, Larsson H, Nyman A, Gumpert CH, Rastam M, Anckarsater H, Cnattingius S, Johannesson M, Ingelsson E, Klareskog L, de Faire U, Pedersen NL, Lichtenstein P. The Swedish twin registry: establishment of a biobank and other recent developments. Twin Res Hum Genet. 2013;16(1):317–329. doi: 10.1017/thg.2012.104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Martin NG. Gene–environment interaction and twin studies. In: Spector T, Snieder H, Gregor Mac-A, editors. Advances in twin and sib-pair analysis. London: Greenwich Medical Media Ltd; 2000. pp. 143–150. [Google Scholar]
- Martin NG, Rowell DM, Whitfield JB. Do the MN and Jk systems influence environmental variability in serum lipid levels? Clin Genet. 1983;24(1):1–14. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-0004.1983.tb00061.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McCaffery JM, Papandonatos GD, Bond DS, Lyons MJ, Wing RR. Gene X environment interaction of vigorous exercise and body mass index among male Vietnam-era twins. Am J Clin Nutr. 2009;89(4):1011–1018. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.2008.27170. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McClearn GE, Johansson B, Berg S, Pedersen NL, Ahern F, Petrill SA, Plomin R. Substantial genetic influence on cognitive abilities in twins 80 or more years old. Science. 1997;276(5318):1560–1563. doi: 10.1126/science.276.5318.1560. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGue M, Christensen K. Genetic and environmental contributions to depression symptomatology: evidence from Danish twins 75 years of age and older. J Abnorm Psychol. 1997;106(3):439–448. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.106.3.439. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McGue M, Christensen K. Growing old but not growing apart: Twin similarity in the latter half of the lifespan. Behav Gen. 2013;43:1–12. doi: 10.1007/s10519-012-9559-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mustelin L, Silventoinen K, Pietilainen K, Rissanen A, Kaprio J. Physical activity reduces the influence of genetic effects on BMI and waist circumference: a study in young adult twins. Int J Obes. 2009;33(1):29–36. doi: 10.1038/ijo.2008.258. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nebes RD, Vora IJ, Meltzer CC, Fukui MB, Williams RL, Kamboh MI, Saxton J, Houck PR, DeKosky ST, Reynolds CF. Relationship of deep white matter hyperintensities and apolipoprotein E genotype to depressive symptoms in older adults without clinical depression. Am J Psychiatry. 2001;158(6):878–884. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.158.6.878. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Neiderhiser JA, Reiss D, Lichtenstein P, Spotts EL, Ganiban J. Father-adolescent relationships and the role of genotype-environment correlation. J Fam Psychol. 2007;21(4):560–571. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.21.4.560. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pahlen S, Hamdi NR, Dahl Aslan AK, Horwitz BN, Panizzon MS, Petersen I, Zavala C, Christensen K, Finkel D, Franz CE, Gatz M, Johnson W, Kremen WS, Krueger RF, Neiderhiser JM, Reynolds CA, Pedersen NL, McGue M. Widespread evidence of age-moderation of genetic and environmental contributions to cognitive functioning in mid- and late-life. Psychol Sci. under review doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2017.12.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pedersen NL, McClearn GE, Plomin R, Nesselroade JR, Berg S, DeFaire U. The Swedish Adoption twin study of aging: an update. Acta Genet Med Gemellol. 1991;40(1):7–20. doi: 10.1017/s0001566000006681. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pedersen NL, Christensen K, Dahl AK, Finkel D, Franz CE, Gatz M, Horwitz BN, Johansson B, Johnson W, Kremen WS, Lyons MJ, Malmberg B, McGue M, Neiderhiser JM, Petersen I, Reynolds CA. IGEMS: the consortium on interplay of genes and environment across multiple studies. Twin Res Hum Genet. 2013;16(1):481–489. doi: 10.1017/thg.2012.110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Radler BT. The midlife in the United States (MIDUS) series: A national longitudinal study of health and well-being. Open health data. 2014;2(1):e3. doi: 10.5334/ohd.ai. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Radloff LS. The CES-D scale a self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Appl Psychol Meas. 1977;1(3):385–401. [Google Scholar]
- Reynolds CA, Finkel D, McArdle JJ, Gatz M, Berg S, Pedersen NL. Quantitative genetic analysis of latent growth curve models of cognitive abilities in adulthood. Dev Psychol. 2005;41(1):3–16. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.41.1.3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reynolds CA, Prince JA, Feuk L, Brookes AJ, Gatz M, Pedersen NL. Longitudinal memory performance during normal aging: twin association models of APOE and other Alzheimer candidate genes. Behav Genet. 2006;36(2):185–194. doi: 10.1007/s10519-005-9027-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reynolds CA, Gatz M, Berg S, Pedersen NL. Genotype-environment interactions: cognitive aging and social factors. Twin Res Hum Genet. 2007;10(2):241–254. doi: 10.1375/twin.10.2.241. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reynolds CA, Zavala C, Gatz M, Vie L, Johansson B, Malmberg B, Ingelsson E, Prince JA, Pedersen NL. Sortilin receptor 1 predicts longitudinal cognitive change. Neurobiol Aging. 2013;34(6):1710.e1711–1710.e1718. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2012.12.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rokholm B, Silventoinen K, Tynelius P, Gamborg M, Sørensen TIA, Rasmussen F. Increasing genetic variance of body mass index during the swedish obesity epidemic. PLoS One. 2011;6(11):e27135. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0027135. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rosenthal R. Meta-analytic procedures for social research. Newbury Park: Sage; 1991. [Google Scholar]
- Rutter M. Achievements and challenges in the biology of environmental effects. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2012;109(2):17149–17153. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1121258109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rutter M, Silberg J. Gene-environment interplay in relation to emotional and behavioral disturbance. Annu Rev Psychol. 2002;53:463–490. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135223. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Salmon DP, Ferris SH, Thomas RG, Sano M, Cummings JL, Sperling RA, Petersen RC, Aisen PS. Age and APOE genotype influence rate of cognitive decline in non-demented elderly. Neuropsychology. 2013 doi: 10.1037/a0032707. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Salthouse TA. When does age-related cognitive decline begin? Neurobiol Aging. 2009;30(4):507–514. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2008.09.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schaie KW. The course of adult intellectual development. Am Psychol. 1994;49(4):304–313. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.49.4.304. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schellenberg GD, Montine TJ. The genetics and neuropathology of Alzheimer’s disease. Acta Neuropathol. 2012;124:305–323. doi: 10.1007/s00401-012-0996-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Schultz MR, Lyons MJ, Franz CE, Grant MD, Boake C, Jacobson KC, Xian H, Schellenberg GD, Eisen SA, Kremen WS. Apolipoprotein E genotype and memory in the sixth decade of life. Neurology. 2008;70(19 Pt 2):1771–1777. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000286941.74372.cc. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Silventoinen K, Sarlio-Lahteenkorva S, Koskenvuo M, Lahelma E, Kaprio J. Effect of environmental and genetic factors on education-associated disparities in weight and weight gain: a study of Finnish adult twins. Am J Clin Nutr. 2004;80(4):815–822. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/80.4.815. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Silventoinen K, Hasselbalch AL, Lallukka T, Bogl L, Pietilainen KH, Heitmann BL, Schousboe K, Rissanen A, Kyvik KO, Sorensen TI, Kaprio J. Modification effects of physical activity and protein intake on heritability of body size and composition. Am J Clin Nutr. 2009;90(4):1096–1103. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.2009.27689. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Skoog I, Waern M, Duberstein P, Blennow K, Zetterberg H, Börjesson-Hanson A, Östling S, Guo X, Kern J, Gustafson D, Gudmundsson P, Marlow T, Kern S. A 9-year prospective population-based study on the association between the APOE*E4 allele and late-life depression in Sweden. Biol Psychiatry. 2015 doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.01.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Skytthe A, Christiansen L, Kyvik KO, Bodker FL, Hvidberg L, Petersen I, Nielsen MM, Bingley P, Hjelmborg J, Tan Q, Holm NV, Vaupel JW, McGue M, Christensen K. The Danish Twin Registry: linking surveys, national registers, and biological information. Twin Res Hum Genet. 2013;16(1):104–111. doi: 10.1017/thg.2012.77. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- South SC, Krueger RF. Genetic and environmental influences on internalizing psychopathology vary as a function of economic status. Psychol Med. 2011;41(1):107–117. doi: 10.1017/S0033291710000279. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sundstrom A, Marklund P, Nilsson LG, Cruts M, Adolfsson R, Van Broeckhoven C, Nyberg L. APOE influences on neu-ropsychological function after mild head injury: within-person comparisons. Neurology. 2004;62(11):1963–1966. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000129268.83927.a8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sundstrom A, Nilsson LG, Cruts M, Adolfsson R, Van Broeckhoven C, Nyberg L. Increased risk of dementia following mild head injury for carriers but not for non-carriers of the APOE epsilon4 allele. Int Psychoger. 2007;19(1):159–165. doi: 10.1017/S1041610206003498. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Surakka I, Whitfield JB, Perola M, Visscher PM, Montgomery GW, Falchi M, Willemsen G, de Geus EJ, Magnusson PK, Christensen K, Sorensen TI, Pietilainen KH, Rantanen T, Silander K, Widen E, Muilu J, Rahman I, Liljedahl U, Syvanen AC, Palotie A, Kaprio J, Kyvik KO, Pedersen NL, Boomsma DI, Spector T, Martin NG, Ripatti S, Peltonen L, Genom EP. A genome-wide association study of monozygotic twin-pairs suggests a locus related to variability of serum high-density lipoprotein cholesterol. Twin Res Hum Genet. 2012;15(6):691–699. doi: 10.1017/thg.2012.63. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Surtees PG, Wainwright NWJ, Bowman R, Luben RN, Wareham NJ, Khaw K-T, Bingham SA. No association between APOE and major depressive disorder in a community sample of 17,507 adults. J Psychiatr Res. 2009;43(9):843–847. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2008.12.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tabert MH, Manly JJ, Liu X, Pelton GH, Rosenblum S, Jacobs M, Zamora D, Goodkind M, Bell K, Stern Y, Devanand DP. Neuropsychological prediction of conversion to Alzheimer disease in patients with mild cognitive impairment. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2006;63(8):916–924. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.63.8.916. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tang MX, Maestre G, Tsai WY, Liu XH, Feng L, Chung WY, Chun M, Schofield P, Stern Y, Tycko B, Mayeux R. Effect of age, ethnicity, and head injury on the association between APOE genotypes and Alzheimer’s disease. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1996;802:6–15. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1996.tb32593.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Taylor WD, Aizenstein HJ, Alexopoulos GS. The vascular depression hypothesis: mechanisms linking vascular disease with depression. Mol Psychiatry. 2013;18(9):963–974. doi: 10.1038/mp.2013.20. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Turkheimer E, Horn EE. Interactions between socioeconomic status and components of variation in cognitive ability. In: Finkel D, Reynolds CA, editors. Behavior genetics of cognition across the lifespan. Riverside: Springer; 2014. pp. 41–68. [Google Scholar]
- Vinkhuyzen AA, van der Sluis S, Posthuma D. Interaction between experience seeking and genetic and environmental influences on general cognitive ability. Twin Res Hum Genet. 2012;15(1):87–96. doi: 10.1375/twin.15.1.87. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Visscher PM, Posthuma D. Statistical power to detect genetic Loci affecting environmental sensitivity. Behav Genet. 2010;40(5):728–733. doi: 10.1007/s10519-010-9362-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ward A, Crean S, Mercaldi CJ, Collins JM, Boyd D, Cook MN, Arrighi HM. Prevalence of apolipoprotein E4 genotype and homozygotes (APOE e4/4) among patients diagnosed with Alzheimer’s disease: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neuroepidemiology. 2012;38(1):1–17. doi: 10.1159/000334607. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weinkauf M. BenjaminiHochberg.xlsx (ed) 2012. False discovery rate Excel spreadsheet (41,86 KB)—Marum. https://www.marum.de/Binaries/Binary745/BenjaminiHochberg.xlsx. [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.