Skip to main content
Frontiers in Psychology logoLink to Frontiers in Psychology
. 2016 May 6;7:631. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00631

Animal Assisted Therapy (AAT) Program As a Useful Adjunct to Conventional Psychosocial Rehabilitation for Patients with Schizophrenia: Results of a Small-scale Randomized Controlled Trial

Paula Calvo 1,2,*, Joan R Fortuny 3, Sergio Guzmán 3, Cristina Macías 3, Jonathan Bowen 1,4, María L García 3, Olivia Orejas 3, Ferran Molins 3, Asta Tvarijonaviciute 5,6, José J Cerón 5, Antoni Bulbena 1,2,3, Jaume Fatjó 1,2
PMCID: PMC4858645  PMID: 27199859

Abstract

Currently, one of the main objectives of human–animal interaction research is to demonstrate the benefits of animal assisted therapy (AAT) for specific profiles of patients or participants. The aim of this study is to assess the effect of an AAT program as an adjunct to a conventional 6–month psychosocial rehabilitation program for people with schizophrenia. Our hypothesis is that the inclusion of AAT into psychosocial rehabilitation would contribute positively to the impact of the overall program on symptomology and quality of life, and that AAT would be a positive experience for patients. To test these hypotheses, we compared pre–program with post–program scores for the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) and the EuroQoL-5 dimensions questionnaire (EuroQol-5D), pre–session with post–session salivary cortisol and alpha–amylase for the last four AAT sessions, and adherence rates between different elements of the program. We conducted a randomized, controlled study in a psychiatric care center in Spain. Twenty–two institutionalized patients with chronic schizophrenia completed the 6–month rehabilitation program, which included individual psychotherapy, group therapy, a functional program (intended to improve daily functioning), a community program (intended to facilitate community reintegration) and a family program. Each member of the control group (n = 8) participated in one activity from a range of therapeutic activities that were part of the functional program. In place of this functional program activity, the AAT–treatment group (n = 14) participated in twice–weekly 1–h sessions of AAT. All participants received the same weekly total number of hours of rehabilitation. At the end of the program, both groups (control and AAT–treatment) showed significant improvements in positive and overall symptomatology, as measured with PANSS, but only the AAT–treatment group showed a significant improvement in negative symptomatology. Adherence to the AAT-treatment was significantly higher than overall adherence to the control group’s functional rehabilitation activities. Cortisol level was significantly reduced after participating in an AAT session, which could indicate that interaction with the therapy dogs reduced stress. In conclusion, the results of this small-scale RCT suggest that AAT could be considered a useful adjunct to conventional psychosocial rehabilitation for people with schizophrenia.

Keywords: animal-assisted therapy, psychosocial rehabilitation, adherence to treatment, schizophrenia, PANSS, EuroQol-5 dimensions, salivary cortisol, salivary alpha-amylase

Introduction

Interactions with companion animals appear to have positive effects on physiological, psychological, and social aspects of human wellbeing (Fine, 2010). Animal assisted therapies (AAT) seem to produce therapeutic benefits in different kinds of patients, from those with physical ailments, such as cardiovascular disease, to those with mental disorders ranging from dementia to depression (Pedersen et al., 2011) and schizophrenia (Barak et al., 2001). It has been suggested that AAT might help to develop the therapeutic relationship between patients and healthcare professionals, and could improve the therapeutic atmosphere (Fine, 2010; Julius et al., 2013); animals in AAT can act as social facilitators, social modulators, and amplifiers of emotional reactivity (Fine, 2010).

However, scientific evidence for the benefits of AAT is still very limited (Nimer and Lundahl, 2007; Kamioka et al., 2014), partially due to intrinsic difficulties of performing research with AAT (Nimer and Lundahl, 2007; Kamioka et al., 2014). Typical methodological limitations of AAT include: small sample size, difficulties of blinding, lack of an adequate control group, selection bias due to including only participants who like animals, lack of physiological evaluation, short program duration and the limited number of professionals and animals that currently participate in AAT. Some of these limitations are very difficult to overcome, because of the nature of AAT interventions. For example, in AAT, it is very difficult to find a comparable therapeutic activity for the control group, and it is impossible to blind for the presence of the animal. Since AAT is still considered an alternative therapeutic approach, very few resources are dedicated to it within the health system (Kaplan and Sadock, 1989). As a consequence of these limitations it is important to compile studies with partial evidence for AAT efficacy and applicability (Fine, 2010) and to improve and standardize research methodologies (Kamioka et al., 2014).

Recent reviews of AAT research indicate that mental health disorders are a good target for AAT interventions (Nimer and Lundahl, 2007; Villalta-Gil and Ochoa, 2007; Rossetti and King, 2010; Kamioka et al., 2014). Some studies have shown that AAT programs could benefit patients being treated for schizophrenia (Kovács et al., 2004, 2006; Nathans-Barel et al., 2005; Chu et al., 2009). Suggested benefits include effects on self-esteem, self-determination, positive symptomatology, emotional symptomatology, anhedonia, and daily functioning (Nathans-Barel et al., 2005; Villalta-Gil and Ochoa, 2007; Villalta-Gil et al., 2009; Kamioka et al., 2014).

The aim of this study was to assess the effect of an AAT program as an adjunct to conventional psychosocial rehabilitation for people with schizophrenia.

Based on the hypothesis that inclusion of AAT in a rehabilitation program would have a beneficial effect, our study had three objectives; to analyze the impact on symptomatology and quality of life, to evaluate the patient’s experience of the AAT sessions, and to assess stress relief during the AAT sessions. For the first objective, the measures used were the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS; Kay et al., 1989; Peralta and Cuesta, 1994), and EuroQoL-5 Dimensions questionnaire (EQ-5D; Bobes et al., 2005). For the second objective, we used adherence (proportion of programmed sessions that a patient attended). Adherence was used as an indicator of the relative appeal of the AAT sessions, by comparing adherence for the AAT sessions with combined adherence for the functional program attended by the control group. For the last objective, since stress management is one of the main objectives for the treatment of inpatients with mental disorders (Klainin-Yobas et al., 2015), we evaluated the stress-relieving aspect of the sessions by making a pre- versus post-session comparison of values for salivary cortisol and alpha-amylase for the last four AAT sessions. To our knowledge, previous research on the effects of AAT for patients with schizophrenia has not included the combination of these three different types of objectives (and the associated measures).

Our general objective was to present evidence that was different and complementary to existing research and to identify interesting target measures, such as adherence to treatment and physiological measures, that could be used for future research.

Materials and Methods

Study Design

The study was a randomized, controlled trial (RCT).

In this study, primary outcomes for all participants were changes in symptomatology (measured with PANSS) and changes in quality of life (measured with EQ-5D). Secondary outcomes of this study consisted of adherence to AAT sessions (AAT-treatment group) versus adherence to other activities of functional rehabilitation (control group), and changes in salivary cortisol and alpha-amylase during AAT sessions, as a measure of stress relief (AAT-treatment group only).

Patients were randomly assigned to the control or AAT-treatment group.

The laboratory technicians who analyzed the saliva samples were only given the patients’ ID numbers, and were blinded to whether patients were in the control or AAT-treatment group. For practical reasons and for issues relating to the availability of resources and personnel, the rest of the process of the study could not be blinded. It was not possible for patients to be blinded to the presence of dogs, and only one hospital neuropsychologist was able to participate in the study (in charge of all of the pre-treatment and post-treatment evaluations of the study, and follow-up of all of the patients). A single researcher not only carried out the collection of the data and saliva samples, but also acted as a guide for the therapy dogs during the AAT sessions.

Sample

The study was conducted in a public psychiatric hospital within an urban area of Spain. In order to avoid the confounding effects of environmental variation, only patients from the same unit were included (MILLE: Long and medium-stay unit). All eligible patients from the MILLE unit who fulfilled the following criteria were included:

  • Diagnosis of schizophrenia, according to the Revised forth edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV-TR; American Psychiatric Association, 2000).

  • Enrolled in a psychosocial rehabilitation process.

  • With a projected minimum hospitalization term of 6 months.

A set of exclusion criteria was also applied, which included:

  • Compromised mobility.

  • Presence of allergies to animals.

  • Rejection of contact with companion animals.

  • Confirmed diagnosis of a coagulopathy.

These inclusion and exclusion criteria were adapted from previous AAT protocols (Barak et al., 2001; Kovács et al., 2004; Nathans-Barel et al., 2005; Villalta-Gil et al., 2009; Fine, 2010; Lang et al., 2010). All patients in the unit who met the criteria were included in the study.

Twenty-four adult patients (Mean age = 47.8 years of age; SD = 6.7) fulfilled the requirements and were included in the study. The patients’ mean age at diagnosis of schizophrenia was 20.5 years of age (SD = 5.0). The patients’ mean scores for PANSS were: 43.8 (SD = 12.3) for General PANSS, 24 (SD = 6.6) for Negative PANSS and 20.6 (SD = 6.6) for Positive PANSS. The EQ-5D total score mean was 1.8 (SD = 1.5). See Table 1 for an overview of all of the characteristics of the sample population.

Table 1.

Characteristics of the sample.

Patient Group Age Age of onset Gender PANSS General PANSS Negative PANSS Positive EQ-5D total Psychotropic medication Other medication
1 T 54 17 Man 31 25 11 0 Aripripazol, levomepromazine Pravastatin, repaglinide, pantoprazole
2 T 66 20 Woman 38 18 22 3 Clozapine, venlafaxine Pantoprazole, lactulose
3 T 37 21 Man 32 17 9 1 Clozapine, amisulpride, lorazepam, pregabalin
4 T 58 22 Man 46 36 24 2 Fluphenazine, levomepromazine, biperiden
5 T 52 26 Man 50 32 23 1 Clozapine, sodium valproate Metformin
6 T 47 20 Man 38 19 13 1 Zuclopenthixol, biperiden Atorvastatin
7 T 41 15 Man 77 39 28 3 Levomepromazine, periciazine, paroxetine, biperiden
8 T 33 23 Woman 31 12 15 0 Zuclopenthixol, clonazepam, sodium valproate, biperiden Gemfibrozil
9 T 35 24 Man 46 25 18 2 Clozapine, fluphenazine, pimozide, clotiapine
10 T 47 21 Man 53 29 25 1 Fluphenazine, risperidone, biperiden Lactulose
11 T 54 15 Woman 37 29 17 1 Risperidone, zuclopenthixol, sodium valproate
12 T 44 22 Woman 37 22 27 3 Clozapine, risperidone Atorvastatin, salmeterol
13 T 50 17 Man 43 24 17 7 Lithium carbonate, clozapine, methylphenidate, sertraline, topiramate, biperiden Monohydrate lactitol
14 T 55 39 Man 61 27 15 2 Olanzapine, paroxetine, dipotassium cloracepate, biperiden Hydroxyzine, loratadine
15 T 43 21 Man 47 30 30 2 Fluphenazine, risperidone, sodium valproate
16 T 45 19 Man 42 18 34 2 Clozapine, diazepam, sodium valproate Pantoprazole, lactulose, gemfibrozil
17 C 48 18 Woman 45 26 18 4 Clozapine
18 C 47 16 Man 50 20 14 1 Haloperidol, dipotassium clorazepate, biperiden Lactulose
19 C 53 21 Man 46 16 25 2 Risperidone, quetiapine, sodium valproate, clonazepam Atenolol, pravastatin, pantoprazole, metformin
20 C 59 25 Man 44 22 21 2 Clozapine, gabapentin, biperiden
21 C 51 16 Man 61 26 28 0 Zuclopenthixol, ziprasidone, sodium valproate, biperiden
22 C 50 20 Man 15 22 12 0 clozapine Enalapril
23 C 44 18 Woman 48 15 22 2 Zuclopenthixol, levomepromazine, clotiapine
24 C 50 16 Woman 32 27 26 1 Olanzapine, levomepromazine, sodium valproate, phenytoin
MEAN (SD) 47.8 (6.7) 20.5 (5.0) 43.7 (12.3) 20.6 (6.6) 24 (6.6) 1.8 (1.5)

The 24 patients who met the inclusion criteria were randomly assigned to three groups, with eight patients in each group (AAT-treatment groups A and B, and a control group C) (See Figure 1). Given the length of the study (6 months), a high drop out rate was expected. Other authors recommend that group size is kept small for AAT sessions (Kovács et al., 2004; Nathans-Barel et al., 2005; Chu et al., 2009; Fine, 2010). To comply with this recommendation, the 16 patients who were to be given AAT were randomly allocated to one of two small therapy groups (eight people in each). There were no differences in the characteristics of these groups, or in the AAT-therapy they received. In the analysis, data from patients in both therapy groups (A and B) was therefore combined into a single group.

FIGURE 1.

FIGURE 1

Shows the modified CONSORT flow diagram for individual RCTs of non-pharmacologic treatment (Boutron et al., 2008; Schulz et al., 2010) applied to this study.

Five therapy dogs that had previously been assessed and trained, and had experience of participation in AAT work were used for the study. There is no official dog therapy certification in Spain. A thorough physical and behavioral examination of each dog was performed by a panel of three board-certified specialists in veterinary behavioral medicine. This examination included the Ethotest (Lucidi et al., 2005), a test designed to identify suitable therapy dogs, and the C-BARQ (Hsu and Serpell, 2003), a questionnaire for measuring behavior and temperament traits in dogs.

Interventions

The study took place between October 2012 and May 2013. At the psychiatric hospital where the study was conducted, the global psychosocial rehabilitation process consisted of five types of programs: individual psychotherapy, group therapy program, functional program (to improve daily functioning), community program (with social reintegration objectives), and family program. From Monday to Friday every week, all patients treated in this global psychosocial rehabilitation process had to participate in all five types of program.

Patients in all groups participated in the same total weekly number of hours of activity within the psychosocial rehabilitation process. For the AAT-therapy groups (A and B) the AAT program was one of these activities. The AAT program consisted of 6-months of twice-weekly 1-h sessions (Tuesday and Friday), so that each patient attended a total of 40 AAT sessions (taking into account public holidays). Control group patients attended the same number of sessions in the functional program.

The AAT-treatment involved three types of sessions:

  • (a)

    Sessions to develop the emotional bond between participants and dogs: The participants were taught to handle and take care of the dogs correctly. In this type of session, concepts of animal welfare and responsible ownership were explained and practiced.

  • (b)

    Sessions involving walking the dogs: During the first half of the program, the dogs were walked in a large natural park, so that the patients could learn to walk the dogs in a calm and controlled manner. For the rest of the program, the participants walked the dogs in the city, where they could experience dog-walking in a social context that is typical of that which is experienced by dog owners.

  • (c)

    Sessions to train and play with dogs: Patients learned to give instructions to the dogs and train them using positive reinforcement training techniques.

During an AAT session 4 of the 5 therapy dogs were always present to interact with the patients. At the beginning of each session, participants were asked to work in pairs. Each working pair was assigned a dog, which they worked with for the remaining hour of the session. During the program there was a rotation between the three types of sessions (emotional bonding, dog walking, and dog training with play).

Each patient in the control group was assigned to a single activity from the functional program on the basis of their therapist’s criteria, but taking into account the individual’s preferences. The choice was between art therapy, group sports (football or basketball), dynamic psycho-stimulation, and gymnastics. These activities were organized so that they closely matched certain important characteristics of the AAT program:

  • They were conducted outside the hospital unit where the patients were resident.

  • They all involved a similar element of group work.

  • Group sizes were small (similar to the AAT sessions).

  • Patients were accompanied and supervised off-site by a mental health professional (nurse or similar).

  • The activities continued throughout the period of trial (they were unaffected by season).

  • The sessions were twice-weekly and of 1-h duration.

The difference between functional program activities and the AAT sessions was, as far as was possible, restricted to content.

Instruments

To compare evolution in psychiatric symptoms between AAT-treatment and control patients during the 6-month duration of the program, we used the previously validated Spanish version of the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS; Kay et al., 1989). PANSS has been found to be a reasonably valid psychometric tool for people with schizophrenia (Kay et al., 1989; Peralta and Cuesta, 1994), and is one of the most widely used tools for the assessment of therapeutic results in schizophrenia treatment. PANSS was administered to all patients during individualized interviews with the hospital neuropsychologist. It was completed for each patient several times in the month before the study started, during the program and in the month after the end of the program.

The same interview approach was used to assess quality of life, using the EQ-5D (Bobes et al., 2005). The EQ-5D has been found to be reasonably valid for use in people with schizophrenia (König et al., 2007) and is a standard assessment instrument used in this hospital. The neuropsychologist completed the EQ-5D twice with each patient, in the month before the study started and in the month after the end of the program.

Individual attendance at sessions of AAT and the functional program was recorded. Adherence was calculated as the proportion of programmed sessions that a patient attended during the 6-month program period, expressed as a percentage.

In order to study the physiological effects of contact with the dogs during an AAT session, pre- and post-session saliva samples were collected for the last four AAT sessions of the program. Salivary alpha-amylase (sAA) and cortisol were measured. As a biomarker of psychosocial stress, salivary alpha-amylase can be considered to be a measure of the level of activation of the sympathetic nervous system (SNS; Rohleder et al., 2006; Holt-Lunstad et al., 2008). Salivary cortisol is an indicator of the state of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis and is a general physiological biomarker of stress (Fortunato et al., 2008; Holt-Lunstad et al., 2008). Saliva samples were collected using a commercial saliva collection kit (Salivettes®, Sarstedt), with the Salivette remaining in the patient’s mouth for 1 min per sample. Two samples were collected from each patient at each of the four sessions; one was collected 30 min before the AAT session and the other 10 min after the AAT session had finished. Saliva samples were stored in a dry-ice cooled mobile fridge, in which they were delivered to the laboratory to be processed and frozen to −80°C for later testing. The maximum pre-freezing storage time was 4 h. After the study was completed, all saliva samples were thawed and analyzed. Cortisol was extracted and analyzed using a commercial immunoassay (Siemens IMMULITE 2000, Siemens Healthcare Diagnostics. Deerfield, IL, USA; Owen and Roberts, 2011; Tecles et al., 2014), and alpha-amylase was analyzed using a commercial spectrophotometric assay (Olympus AU2700. Olympus America Inc. Center Valley, PA, USA; Tecles et al., 2014).

Statistical Analysis

We analyzed data from all the participants who completed the 6-month period of the study (N = 22). In the present study, patients were included in the analysis regardless of their level of adherence to their medication regime or any of the five elements of the psychosocial rehabilitation process, and adherence to the AAT program was a main outcome measure. As a result, the present study does not comply with the requirements for a ‘per protocol’ analysis, in which patients would be excluded for any deviation from treatment. However, because we excluded two patients who did not complete the study we also did not carry out an ‘intention to treat’ analysis, and so our protocol could be described as a ‘modified intention to treat.’

Between-group (control and AAT-treatment) contrasts of PANSS and EQ-5D scores were analyzed using Statistica 10 and GraphPad Prism 6. Data was tested for normality using the Shapiro–Wilk test; parametric data was tested using a t-test, and non-parametric data was tested using the Mann–Whitney U (for unpaired data) or Wilcoxon test (for paired data). For dichotomous variables (patient sex), a chi-square test was used to compare proportions between groups. Multiple comparisons were made in the EQ-5D analysis, so the Bonferroni correction was used to adjust the value of p that was accepted for significance (for example, for 20 comparisons, p = 0.05/20 = 0.0025).

Pre-program PANSS and EQ-5D scores were compared with post-program scores, for the AAT-treatment and control groups separately. After checking normality of data (with the Shapiro–Wilk test), a paired-samples t-test was used with parametric data and the Wilcoxon test was used with non-parametric data.

Adherence to treatment data was checked for normality using the Shapiro–Wilk test. An unpaired t-test (for parametric data) or Mann–Whitney U (for non-parametric data) was used to compare adherence levels between the AAT-treatment group and either overall compliance or compliance for individual activities within the functional programs (control group).

A paired t-test was used to compare pre- with post-session levels of cortisol and alpha-amylase in the AAT-treatment group (data had been found to be normally distributed using the Shapiro–Wilk test).

Ethics

The Clinical Research Ethics Committee of the Hospital del Mar Medical Research Institute (IMIM) approved the clinical-protocol, patient management, and participation of the patients.

The Department of Agriculture and Natural Environment of the Catalonia Government approved the animal management protocol for this study. All dogs that participated in the project were given a thorough medical, behavioral, and welfare assessment before, during, and after the AAT program.

All patients who were eligible for the study received documentation that outlined the study, and they signed an informed consent form. They were able to withdraw from the study at any time.

Animal assisted therapy technicians signed an informed consent form that detailed their responsibilities (confidentiality and conformity) within the project.

Spanish law 15/99 (regarding personal data protection) was applied to all data collection.

Results

Sample Characteristics

There were no differences between control and AAT-treatment groups with respect to sex [Chi-square test; χ2(1) = 0.40], age or initial scores of PANSS and EQ-5D (Mann–Whitney U; p < 0.05; See Table 2 for full details).

Table 2.

Initial scores of PANSS and EQ-5D of the analyzed patients of this study.

Mean (SD)
INITIAL SCORES Treatment (N = 14) Control (N = 8) U Z adjusted 2 sided exact p
AGE 48.9 (6.7) 46.7 (7.3) 40.5 1.02 0.29
PANSS positive 18.9 (6.0) 20.7 (5.7) 46 −0.64 0.52
PANSS negative 25.3 (7.5) 21.70 (4.5) 39.5 1.09 0.26
PANSS general 44.3 (12.3) 42.6 (13.7) 51.5 −0.27 0.76
EQ-5D Total score 1.9 (1.8) 1.5 (1.3) 49.5 0.42 0.66
EQ-5D- Health Today (0–100) 80.7 (24.9) 78.7 (18.3) 42 0.94 0.36
EQ-5D- F1 Mobility 0.1 (0.3) 0.1 (0.3) 53 −0.34 0.86
EQ-5D- F2 Personal Care 0.1 (0.4) ∗∗ 48 1.02 0.61
EQ-5D- F3 Daily Activities 0.1 (0.3) 0.2 (0.7) 52.5 −0.41 0.81
EQ-5D- F4 Pain/Discomfort 0.4 (0.6) 0.5 (0.5) 50 -0.43 0.71
EQ-5D- F6 Health State 12 m 0.4 (0.6) 0.4 (0.7) 51.5 0.33 0.76

Comparison between control and treatment group. ∗Mann–Whitney U Test. Tests are significant at p < 0.05. No significant results were found. ∗∗Data for the initial item EQ-5D F5 Anxiety was not included because some patients were not able to fully understand the meaning of this item.

During the program, two patients within the AAT-treatment group withdrew from the study. One patient was discharged from the hospital before the end of the AAT program. The other patient exhibited behaviors that threatened to compromise the welfare of the therapy dogs, and therefore stopped participating in the AAT activity (See Figure 1).

Schizophrenic Symptomatology (PANSS)

At the end of the program, no significant differences were found between control and AAT-treatment groups (Mann–Whitney U test, p < 0.05) with respect to final PANSS or change in PANSS (see Table 3 for full details). However, there were significant differences in PANSS pre-treatment and post-treatment scores in both control and AAT-treatment groups (t-test; p < 0.05). In the AAT-treatment group, scores for all PANSS subscales (positive, negative, and general) were significantly lower after the AAT program (t-test; p < 0.05). In the control group, only positive and general PANSS scores showed a significant decrease after treatment (t-test; p < 0.05). For full details, see Table 4.

Table 3.

Differences between control and treatment groups with respect to final PANSS (after 6 months of treatment) or change in PANSS.

Mean (SD)
Treatment (N = 14) Control (N = 8) U Z adjusted 2 sided exact p
PANSS positive score FINAL 13.6 (3.8) 12.9 (5.2) 52 0.24 0.81
PANSS positive change 5.3 (4.8) 7.9 (4.3) 38.5 1.16 0.23
PANSS negative score FINAL 19.6 (7.0) 19.9 (5.4) 55 −0.03 0.97
PANSS negative change −11.7 (7.4) −8.9 (4.8) 41 −0.99 0.33
PANSS general score FINAL 34.3 (8.6) 30.0 (6.0) 37 1.26 0.21
PANSS general score change 5.6 (8.9) 1.9 (3.4) 45 0.71 0.48

∗Mann–Whitney U Test. Tests are significant at p < 0.05. No significant results were found.

Table 4.

Differences in PANSS pre-treatment and post-treatment scores in both control and treatment (AAT) groups.

Group Variable Number of pairs (pre vs. post) Mean (SD) t (df) p-value
Control PANSS positive 8 7.87 (4.29) t(7) = 5.19 0.001
Control PANSS negative 8 1.87 (3.44) t(7) = 1.54 0.167
Control PANSS general 8 12.63 (13.57) t(7) = 2.63 0.033
AAT PANSS positive 14 5.28 (4.78) t(13) = 4.13 0.001
AAT PANSS negative 14 5.64 (8.19) t(13) = 2.57 0.022
AAT PANSS general 14 10.00 (8.70) t(13) = 4.30 0.001

∗t-test (considered significant at p < 0.05).

Quality of Life (EQ-5D)

No significant difference was found between AAT-treatment and Control groups (Mann–Whitney U test; p < 0.0025 after Bonferroni correction). In addition, almost none of the EQ-5D items were significantly different after treatment (Wilcoxon test; p < 0.05; Table 5). Only the score for the general health item (compared with 12 months before) of the EQ-5D was significantly lower after the program in the AAT-treatment group (Wilcoxon test; p < 0.05). For this item, low scores indicate higher health status, meaning that AAT-treatment group patients perceived themselves to be in a better state of health after the program. However, after applying a Bonferroni correction none of the results of EQ-5D was significant different after treatment (for eight comparisons, p = 0.05/8 = 0.0625).

Table 5.

Differences in EQ-5D pre-treatment and post-treatment scores in both control and treatment groups.

Group Variable Number of pairs Type of test t (df) or W p-value
Control EQ-5D Total score 8 T t(7) = 1.8 0.11
Control EQ-5D Health today 12 m 8 W W = 9 0.53
Control EQ-5D Mobility 8 W W = 0 >0.99
Control EQ-5D Pain/discomfort 8 W W = 3 0.50
Control EQ-5D Health State today 8 W W = 0 >0.99
Control EQ-5D Anxiety/Depression 8 W W = −3 0.50
Control EQ-5D Daily Activities 8 W W = −1 >0.99
Control EQ-5D Personal Care 8 ∗∗ ∗∗ ∗∗
Treatment EQ-5D Total score 14 W W = −3 0.91
Treatment EQ-5D Health today 12 m 14 W W = 37 0.03
Treatment EQ-5D Mobility 14 ∗∗ ∗∗ ∗∗
Treatment EQ-5D Pain/discomfort 14 W W = −3 0.76
Treatment EQ-5D Health State today 14 W W = 0 >0.99
Treatment EQ-5D Anxiety/Depression 14 W W = 0 0.34
Treatment EQ-5D Daily Activities 14 W W = −10 0.07
Treatment EQ-5D Personal Care 14 W W = 3 0.34

(T) t-test (data passed normality test). (W) Wilcoxon test (data did not pass normality test). ∗Tests are significant at p < 0.05 (If apply Bonferroni correction: p < 0.05/8 < 0.00625). ∗∗Could not calculate because the values to compare were the same.

Adherence to Treatment

Although patients were encouraged, and expected, to attend all scheduled activities, attendance was entirely voluntary. In the AAT-treatment group, there was an overall 92.9% (SD = 4.7) adherence to treatment for the AAT sessions. The majority of absences from the AAT sessions were due to family or health issues. Only once did a patient not want to attend an AAT session. In the control group, there was an overall 61.2% (SD = 24.8) adherence to treatment for the assigned activity from the functional program. This higher level of adherence to the AAT sessions, compared with overall adherence to the functional activities, was significant [t-test: t(20) = 4.7; p = 0.0001]. We could only compare adherence to AAT-treatment with specific functional program activities for which the number of attending patients was large enough to justify a statistical test (art therapy and gymnastics). AAT showed significantly better adherence than art therapy (Mann–Whitney U test; U = 2; p = 0.01) and gymnastics therapy (Mann–Whitney U test; U = 2; p = 0.01). All detailed data on adherence to treatment are presented in Tables 6 and 7 and see Figure 2.

Table 6.

Patients’ adherence to treatment.

Patient ID Group Number of programmed sessions Number of attended sessions Percentage of adherence
1 Treatment 40 38 95
2 Treatment 40 37 92.5
3 Treatment 40 39 97.5
4 Treatment 40 40 100
5 Treatment 40 37 92.5
6 Treatment 40 34 85
7 Treatment 40 39 97.5
8 Treatment 40 36 90
9 Treatment 40 37 92.5
10 Treatment 40 38 95
11 Treatment 40 37 92.5
12 Treatment 40 34 85
13 Treatment 40 39 97.5
14 Treatment 40 35 87.5
15 Control 28 16 57.1
16 Control 28 6 21.4
17 Control 28 13 46.4
18 Control 21 18 85.7
19 Control 28 25 89.3
20 Control 28 18 64.3
21 Control 56 22 39.3
22 Control 56 48 85.7
MEAN(SD) All patients 37.9 (8.4) 31.1 (10.1) 81.3 (21.5)
Treatment group 40.0 (0.0) 37.1 (1.9) 92.9 (4.7)
Control group 34.1 (13.7) 20.8 (12.4) 61.2 (24.8)

Table 7.

Differences in adherence to treatment between AAT and other types of functional rehabilitation interventions.

Type of compared functional intervention Number of participants in the control group Type of test t (df) or U p
AAT vs. Art therapy 3 U U = 2 0.010
AAT vs. Gymnastics 3 U U = 2 0.010
AAT vs. Psychodynamic therapy 1 U ∗∗ ∗∗
AAT vs. Group sport 1 U ∗∗ ∗∗
AAT vs. all other 8 T t(20) = 4.7 0.001

(T) t-test (data passed normality test). (U) Mann–Whitney U Test (data did not pass normality test). ∗Tests are considered significant at p < 0.05. ∗∗Not possible to calculate because not enough values in the control type of intervention.

FIGURE 2.

FIGURE 2

Differences between AAT and other functional rehabilitation interventions.

Salivary Cortisol and Alpha-Amylase

We collected 61 pre-session and 60 post-session saliva samples from the AAT-treatment group. However, some of the saliva samples were too small for analysis and were discarded. Cortisol analysis was performed with 48 matched pairs of samples (matching every corresponding pre-session and post-session sample for each session for which sufficient sample was available). There was a significant decrease in cortisol after participation in an AAT session (Wilcoxon Test; p < 0.05. Pair-matching was confirmed using the Spearman test; p < 0.05). Fifty pairs of matching samples were used to measure the effect of the intervention on salivary alpha-amylase. sAA was increased after the AAT sessions, but the difference was not quite significant (Wilcoxon Test; p = 0.059. Pair-matching was confirmed using the Spearman test; p < 0.0001).

Discussion

In terms of age and gender, our sample of patients was consistent with the general population of people with schizophrenia, as well as the population of institutionalized people with schizophrenia (Jablensky, 2000; Uggerby et al., 2011). All participants were receiving at least one psychotropic drug, as is common in people treated for this condition (Jablensky, 2000; Uggerby et al., 2011). Our results could therefore be relevant to other similar institutions that are considering the implementation of an AAT program.

With regard to population size, our study was comparable with similar studies that have investigated the effect of AAT in the treatment of schizophrenia, suggesting some common methodological limitations (Barak et al., 2001; Nathans-Barel et al., 2005; Kovács et al., 2006; Berget, 2008; Chu et al., 2009; Villalta-Gil et al., 2009). Apart from the constraint of working with a limited total population of patients within a single hospital unit, and the application of exclusion/inclusion criteria, it should be remembered that AAT has to be conducted in small groups for practical reasons such as the need for proper supervision and a high animal-to-patient ratio (Fine, 2010).

One patient withdrew from the study due to the risk of harm to the therapy dogs. This kind of problem should have been anticipated and taken into account within the exclusion criteria. This should be considered in future studies. Another patient withdrew very early in the study (week 3), and prior to the collection of any outcome data. The recommended approach for superiority studies is an intention to treat analysis, whereby all patients included in the randomization are included in the analysis, and by deviating from this approach in our study we risk an overestimation of the treatment effect (Armijo-Olivo and Magee, 2009). So, whilst the results are interesting and point to a potential effect of treatment, they cannot be relied upon as general evidence of efficacy in a clinical population.

People with a diagnosis of chronic schizophrenia who live in institutionalized settings have very low levels of social functioning and social activity (Kovács et al., 2004). Individual or combined measures of symptomatology, quality of life and adherence to treatment are commonly used to assess the efficacy of a psychosocial rehabilitation process for patients with schizophrenia (Wilson-d’Almeida et al., 2013), but not together in the same study. By including these measures and adding an assessment of salivary cortisol and alpha-amylase, our study provides an interesting insight into the use of combined measures.

In terms of symptomatology, in the AAT-treatment group we observed an improvement in negative symptoms of schizophrenia like apathy, asociality, anhedonia and alogia, that could be partially explained by the regular interaction between patients and animals. Previous work suggests that AAT programs may be effective in the control of negative symptoms of schizophrenia (Barker and Dawson, 1998; Barak et al., 2001; Kovács et al., 2004; Nathans-Barel et al., 2005). Therapy dogs have been described as social catalysts or mediators of interactions between patients and between patients and their therapists, and these benefits could be extended outside the AAT sessions (Fine, 2010). Since negative symptoms of schizophrenia are relatively insensitive to pharmacological therapies and are associated with a chronic course and high levels of social disability, it is very important to find effective alternative interventions that can be added to standard treatment protocols (Hammer et al., 1995; Liddle, 2000; Gråwe and Levander, 2001). The beneficial effects of AAT on negative symptoms of schizophrenia is therefore worthy of further investigation.

The trend toward an increase in alpha-amylase combined with the significant decrease in cortisol after the AAT sessions suggests that the interaction patients had with the dogs was perceived to be not only engaging, but also relaxing. Increases in alpha-amylase and the activation of the SNS can occur in positive emotional states (Fortunato et al., 2008; Payne et al., 2014), and recent research indicates that people with schizophrenia may experience a dysregulation of SNS tone (Monteleone et al., 2015).

The lack of significance for the change in salivary alpha-amylase could be due to the absence of an effect, but also due to the small population size and the small number of collected saliva samples (saliva was only collected for the last four AAT sessions, sample collection was not always successful, and approximately 17% of collected samples had to be rejected due to inadequate sample volume for analysis).

Regarding stress and cortisol levels, previous research has found decreases in salivary cortisol during AAT sessions in other types of patients, such as autistic children (Viau et al., 2010) and insecure attached males (Beetz et al., 2012a). In a previous study with people being treated for schizophrenia, cortisol levels were not been found to change after interaction with animals (Nepps et al., 2014). However, in comparison to our study, the AAT protocol for that study did not include repeated sessions for each patient and the ratio of dogs per patient was lower. Long-term and dose effects of AAT on stress levels of patients with schizophrenia still need to be studied. Future studies could take advantage of our experience by extending the measurement of salivary cortisol to all AAT sessions within a program, and a control group, while also looking for long-term and dose effects.

There were some difficulties in collecting saliva samples in this study, both in terms of quantity and quality of saliva. The pharmaceutical treatment of schizophrenia involves drugs that suppress salivation, and as a consequence of their symptomatology, many people with schizophrenia are smokers (Rae et al., 2014). Smoking increases cortisol and decreases alpha-amylase (Granger et al., 2007a), so this could be a confounding factor. Future studies should include data on patients’ smoking level, particularly when comparing saliva measures between groups, as between group matching could be important. In addition, personal hygiene and dental care seems to be poor in many people with schizophrenia (Velligan et al., 1997), and the presence of impurities in saliva samples could interfere with the reliability of the measurements (Granger et al., 2007b). Ideally, a patient should have rinsed his or her mouth with water some minutes before saliva collection, but due to a lack of patient cooperation this was rarely possible. Future research should try to extend and optimize saliva sample extraction and analysis, as it seems cortisol and alpha-amylase could be good markers of AAT effects in people being treated for schizophrenia.

Quality of life measurements did not differ between pre-treatment and post-treatment conditions in either of the two groups. Improvement in symptomatology is not always related to improvement in quality of life in people with schizophrenia as the latter can be affected by other factors such us the level of insight (Wilson-d’Almeida et al., 2013; Hayhurst et al., 2014; Margariti et al., 2015). Previous research has shown that even patients with schizophrenia who are undergoing treatment can experience a progressive decline in their quality of life (Medici et al., 2015). Therefore, a lack of decline in overall quality of life measurements could be interpreted to be a benefit of psychosocial rehabilitation, particularly in chronic patients. Future research could focus on specific domains of quality of life where AAT seems to have a direct effect, such as anxiety and depression (Barker and Dawson, 1998) and social relationships (Villalta-Gil et al., 2009).

In the present study, mean adherence to the alternative functional rehabilitation interventions (art therapy, group sports, dynamic psycho-stimulation, or gymnastics) was lower in the control group than the AAT-treatment. Previously reported adherence rates to therapeutic sport programs for people being treated for schizophrenia range from 50 to 82% (Beebe et al., 2005; Warren et al., 2011). In the present study, there were intrinsic differences between the activities included in the functional program, but they all shared certain features, such as frequency, duration, and being conducted outside the hospital. Although the added value of AAT sessions in terms of adherence could be due to a novelty effect, attendance to sessions did not decline during the program. Information about adherence is rarely reported in AAT research, but it could be a very useful indicator in the context of psychosocial rehabilitation, and deserves further research (Kamioka et al., 2014).

Another factor that could be of importance in adherence to ATT is the human–dog relationship (Nagasawa et al., 2015). An initial bond may be quickly established between a person and a dog, and this bond has a strongly emotional element (Dwyer et al., 2006; Fine, 2010; Beetz et al., 2012b), that leads to the development of attachment to the dog (Zasloff, 1996). This attachment could contribute to a person’s sustained interest in attending AAT sessions, but could potentially lead to problems when the human–animal bond is disrupted at the end of the program. Further research could monitor the development of the patient–dog bond during an AAT program, and the effects of ending such programs.

Taken together, the various significant results reported in this study (reduction of negative symptomatology, high adherence to the AAT program, and cortisol reduction after AAT sessions) could be explained by the biology of human–animal interactions (Beetz et al., 2012b; Nagasawa et al., 2015). When a person has a enjoyable contact with a dog there is a release of oxytocin, dopamine, and endorphins, as well as a decrease in cortisol (Beetz et al., 2012a,b; Julius et al., 2013). This overall reaction seems to enhance pro-social behavior and decrease anxiety and stress, mainly via the hypothalamic-pituitary axis (HPA; Neumann et al., 2000). Oxytocin administration has previously been proposed as a treatment for psychiatric patients because of its broad pro-social effects on behavior and cognition (Zik and Roberts, 2014). Through the release of oxytocin, positive contact with dogs could produce such psychosocial and psychophysiological benefits. Future research in AAT might also try to study changes in oxytocin levels of people being treated for schizophrenia during contact with animals.

The results of our study raise some questions that could be addressed in future work. Adherence to treatment is a significant problem, especially in lengthy rehabilitation programs with challenging patients. It would be interesting to investigate whether the high level of adherence to AAT that we observed is replicated in other therapeutic situations, and whether adherence really is different from other closely matched activities. It is possible that the mere presence of a dog in any type of therapy session could improve adherence, especially if the patient has developed a relationship with the dog during AAT, and this effect should be investigated. In all rehabilitation programs resources are limited and the inclusion of AAT could represent an opportunity cost by displacing other activities. It is therefore important to find out whether patients who have participated in AAT go on to experience significant long-term benefits after the rehabilitation program has concluded, compared with patients who have been involved in other activities.

Conclusion

Animal assisted therapy seems to be a worthwhile adjunct therapeutic approach for people being treated for schizophrenia in a conventional psychosocial rehabilitation process, with potential positive outcomes in symptomatology, adherence to AAT program, and stress reduction during AAT sessions.

Author Contributions

The paper itself was written by PC, JF, and JB. The paper was reviewed before submission by AB, JRF, SG, CM, OO, FM, AT, JC, and MG. All authors contributed to the initiation and design of the study. PC, JF, JRF, SG, CM, MG, and AB monitored the progress of the study. PC, JF, and JB decided on the analytic strategy. JB, JRF, SG and CM equally contributed to the total production of the study. PC is the guarantor of the study.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The Chair Affinity Foundation Animals and Health is sponsored by a non-profit Foundation (Affinity Foundation). Any research The Chair Affinity Foundation Animals and Health develops is not related to any commercial product.

Acknowledgments

The authors are grateful to all of the patients, nurses, psychologists, psychiatrists and animal assisted therapy technicians. We would particularly like to thank the following professionals and technicians who collaborated in this project: Rosa Cirac, Elena García, Ana Güimil, Natalia Iorlano, Miriam Pérez, Elia Sierra.

Footnotes

Funding. This study was supported by the Affinity Foundation.

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association (2000). DSM-IV-TR. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th Edn. Arlington, TX: American Psychiatric Association. [Google Scholar]
  2. Armijo-Olivo S., Magee D. J. (2009). Intention to treat analysis, compliance, drop-outs and how to deal with missing data in clinical research: a review. Phys. Ther. Rev. 14 36–49. 10.1179/174328809X405928 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  3. Barak Y., Savorai O., Mavashev S., Ben A. (2001). Animal-assisted therapy for elderly schizophrenic patients: a one-year controlled trial. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 9 439–442. 10.1097/00019442-200111000-00013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Barker S. B., Dawson K. S. (1998). The effects of animal-assisted therapy on anxiety ratings of hospitalized psychiatric patients. Psychiatr. Serv. 49 797–801. 10.1176/ps.49.6.797 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Beebe L. H., Tian L., Morris N., Goodwin A., Allen S. S., Kuldau J. (2005). Effects of exercise on mental and physical health parameters of persons with schizophrenia. Issues Ment. Health Nurs. 26 661–676. 10.1080/01612840590959551 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Beetz A., Julius H., Turner D., Kotrschal K. (2012a). Effects of social support by a dog on stress modulation in male children with insecure attachment. Front. Psychol. 3:352 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00352 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Beetz A., Uvnäs-Moberg K., Julius H., Kotrschal K. (2012b). Psychosocial and psychophysiological effects of human-animal interactions: the possible role of oxytocin. Front. Psychol. 3:234 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00234 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Berget B. (2008). Attitudes to animal-assisted therapy with farm animals. J. Psychiatr. Ment. Health Nurs. 15 576–581. 10.1111/j.1365-2850.2008.01268.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bobes J., García-Portilla P., Sáiz P. A., Bascarán T., Bousoño M. (2005). Quality of life measures in schizophrenia. Eur. Psychiatry 20 313–317. 10.1016/S0924-9338(05)80182-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Boutron I., Moher D., Altman D. G., Schulz K. F., Ravaud P. (2008). Extending the CONSORT statement to randomized trials of nonpharmacologic treatment: explanation and elaboration. Ann. Intern. Med. 148 295–309. 10.1002/bjs.4954 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Chu C. I., Liu C. Y., Sun C. T., Lin J. (2009). The effect of animal-assisted activity on inpatients with schizophrenia. J. Psychosoc. Nurs. Ment. Health Serv. 47 42–48. 10.3928/02793695-20091103-96 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Dwyer F., Bennett P. C., Coleman G. J. (2006). Development of the monash dog owner relationship scale (MDORS). Anthrozoös 19 243–256. 10.2752/089279306785415592 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  13. Fine A. (ed.) (2010). Handbook on Animal-Assisted Therapy, 3rd Edn. London: Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
  14. Fortunato C. K., Dribin A. M., Douglas A. G., Kristin A. B. (2008). Salivary alpha-amylase and cortisol in toddlers: differential relations to affective behavior. Dev. Psychobiol. 50 807–818. 10.1002/dev.20326 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Granger D. A., Blair C., Willoughby M., Kivlighan K. T., Hibel L. C., Fortunato C. K., et al. (2007a). Individual differences in salivary cortisol and alpha-amylase in mothers and their infants: relation to tobacco smoke exposure. Dev. Psychobiol. 49 692–701. 10.1002/dev.20247 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Granger D. A., Kivlighan K. T., Fortunato C. T., Harmon A. G., Hibel L. C., Schwartz E. B., et al. (2007b). Integration of salivary biomarkers into developmental and behaviorally-oriented research: problems and solutions for collecting specimens. Physiol. Behav. 92 583–590. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.05.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Gråwe R. W., Levander S. (2001). Neuropsychological impairments in patients with schizophrenia: stability and prediction of outcome. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 104 60–64. 10.1034/j.1600-0447.2001.104s408060.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Hammer M. A., Katsanis J., Iacono W. G. (1995). The relationship between negative symptoms and neuropsychological performance. Biol. Psychiatry 37 828–830. 10.1016/0006-3223(95)00040-N [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Hayhurst K. P., Massie J. A., Dunn G., Lewis S. W., Drake R. J. (2014). Validity of subjective versus objective quality of life assessment in people with schizophrenia. BMC Psychiatry 14:365 10.1186/s12888-014-0365-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Holt-Lunstad J., Birmingham W. A., Light K. C. (2008). Influence of a ‘Warm Touch’ support enhancement intervention among married couples on ambulatory blood pressure, oxytocin, alpha amylase, and cortisol. Psychosom. Med. 70 976–985. 10.1097/PSY.0b013e318187aef7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Hsu Y., Serpell J. A. (2003). Development and validation of a questionnaire for measuring behavior and temperament traits in pet dogs. J. Am. Vet. Med. Assoc. 223 1293–1300. 10.2460/javma.2003.223.1293 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Jablensky A. (2000). Epidemiology of schizophrenia: the global burden of disease and disability. Eur. Arch. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. 250 274–285. 10.1007/s004060070002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Julius H., Beetz A., Kotrschal K., Turner D., Uvnäs-Moberg K. (eds) (2013). Attachment to Pets: An Integrative View of Human-Animal Relationships with Implications for Therapeutic Practice. Göttingen: Hogrefe Publishing. [Google Scholar]
  24. Kamioka H., Okada S., Tsutani K., Park H., Okuizumi H., Handa S., et al. (2014). Effectiveness of animal-assisted therapy: a systematic review of randomized controlled trials. Complement. Ther. Med. 22 371–390. 10.1016/j.ctim.2013.12.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Kaplan H. I., Sadock B. J. (1989). Comprehensive Textbook of Psychiatry, Vols. 1 and 2 5th Edn. Baltimore, MA: Williams and Wilkins Co. [Google Scholar]
  26. Kay S. R., Opler L. A., Lindenmayer J. P. (1989). The positive and negative syndrome scale (PANSS): rationale and standardisation. Br. J. Psychiatry Suppl. 7 59–67. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Klainin-Yobas P., Ignacio J., He H. G., Lau Y., Ngooi B. X., Koh S. Q. (2015). Effects of a stress-management program for inpatients with mental disorders: a feasibility study. Biol. Res. Nurs. 18 213–220. 10.1177/1099800415595877 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. König H. H., Roick C., Angermeyer M. C. (2007). Validity of the EQ-5D in assessing and valuing health status in patients with schizophrenic, schizotypal or delusional disorders. Eur. Psychiatry 22 177–187. 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2006.08.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Kovács Z., Bulucz J., Kis R., Simon L. (2006). An exploratory study of the effect of animal-assisted therapy on nonverbal communication in three schizophrenic patients. Anthrozoos 19 353–364. 10.2752/089279306785415475 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  30. Kovács Z., Kis R., Rózsa S., Rózsa L. (2004). Animal-assisted therapy for middle-aged schizophrenic patients living in a social institution. A Pilot Study. Clin. Rehabil. 18 483–486. 10.1191/0269215504cr765oa [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Lang U. E., Jansen J. B., Wertenauer F., Gallinat J., Rapp M. A. (2010). Reduced anxiety during dog assisted interviews in acute schizophrenic patients. Eur. J. Integr. Med. 2 123–127. 10.1016/j.eujim.2010.07.002 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  32. Liddle P. F. (2000). Cognitive impairment in schizophrenia: its impact on social functioning. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 101 11–16. 10.1111/j.0065-1591.2000.007s021[dash]3.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Lucidi P., Bernabò N., Panunzi M., Dalla Villa P., Mattioli M. (2005). Ethotest: a new model to identify (shelter) dogs’ skills as service animals or adoptable pets. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 95 103–122. 10.1016/j.applanim.2005.04.006 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  34. Margariti M., Ploumpidis D., Economou M., Christodoulou G. N., Papadimitriou G. N. (2015). Quality of life in schizophrenia spectrum disorders: associations with insight and psychopathology. Psychiatry Res. 225 695–701. 10.1016/j.psychres.2014.11.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Medici C. R., Vestergaard C. H., Hjorth P., Hansen M. V., Shanmuganathan J. W., Viuff A. G., et al. (2015). Quality of life and clinical characteristics in a nonselected sample of patients with schizophrenia. Int. J. Soc. Psychiatry 62 12–20. 10.1177/0020764015585330 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Monteleone A. M., Monteleone P., Di Filippo C., Pellegrino F., Grillo F., Maj M. (2015). Investigation of the endogenous stress response system in patients with chronic schizophrenia. Neuropsychobiology 72 1–7. 10.1159/000437437 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Nagasawa M., Mitsui S., En S., Ohtani N., Ohta M., Sakuma Y., et al. (2015). Oxytocin-gaze positive loop and the coevolution of human-dog bonds. Science 348 333–336. 10.1126/science.1261022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Nathans-Barel I., Feldman P., Berger B., Modai I., Silver H. (2005). Animal-assisted therapy ameliorates anhedonia in schizophrenia patients: a controlled pilot study. Psychother. Psychosom. 74 31–35. 10.1159/000082024 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Nepps P., Stewart C. N., Bruckno S. R. (2014). Animal-assisted activity: effects of a complementary intervention program on psychological and physiological variables. J. Evid. Based Complementary Altern. Med. 19 211–215. 10.1177/2156587214533570 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Neumann I. D., Wigger A., Torner L., Holsboer F., Landgraf R. (2000). Brain oxytocin inhibits basal and stress-induced activity of the hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal axis in male and female rats: partial action within the paraventricular nucleus. J. Neuroendocrinol. 12 235–243. 10.1046/j.1365-2826.2000.00442.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Nimer J., Lundahl B. (2007). Animal-assisted therapy: a meta-analysis. Anthrozoos 20 225–238. 10.2752/089279307X224773 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  42. Owen W. E., Roberts W. L. (2011). Performance characteristics of a new immulite 2000 system erythropoietin assay. Clin. Chim. Acta 412 480–482. 10.1016/j.cca.2010.11.023 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Payne L. A., Hibel L., Granger D., Tsao J., Zeltzer L. (2014). Relationship of salivary alpha amylase and cortisol to social anxiety in healthy children undergoing laboratory pain tasks. J. Child Adolesc. Behav. 2 1000129 10.4172/jcalb.1000129 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Pedersen I., Nordaunet T., Martinsen E. W., Berget B., Braastad B. O. (2011). Farm animal-assisted intervention: relationship between work and contact with farm animals and change in depression, anxiety, and self-efficacy among persons with clinical depression. issues ment. Health Nurs. 32 493–500. 10.3109/01612840.2011.566982 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Peralta V., Cuesta M. J. (1994). Psychometric properties of the PANSS in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res. 53 31–40. 10.1016/0165-1781(94)90093-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Rae J., Pettey D., Aubry T., Stol J. (2014). Factors affecting smoking cessation efforts of people with severe mental illness: a qualitative study. J. Dual Diagn. 11 42–49. 10.1080/15504263.2014.992096 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Rohleder N., JWolf J. M., Maldonado E. F., Kirschbaum C. (2006). The psychosocial stress-induced increase in salivary alpha-amylase is independent of saliva flow rate. Psychophysiology 43 645–652. 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2006.00457.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  48. Rossetti J., King C. (2010). Use of animal-assisted therapy with psychiatric patients. J. Psychosoc. Nurs. Ment. Health Serv. 48 44–48. 10.3928/02793695-20100831-05 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Schulz K. F., Altman D. G., Moher D. (2010). CONSORT 2010 statement: updated guidelines for reporting parallel group randomised trials. BMC Med. 8:1 10.1186/1741-7015-8-18 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Tecles F., Fuentes-Rubio M., Tvarijonaviciute A., Martínez-Subiela S., Fatjó J., Cerón J. J. (2014). Assessment of stress associated with an oral public speech in veterinary students by salivary biomarkers. J. Vet. Med. Educ. 41 37–43. 10.3138/jvme.0513-073R1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Uggerby P., Nielsen R. E., Correll C. U., Nielsen J. (2011). Characteristics and predictors of long-term institutionalization in patients with schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 131 120–126. 10.1016/j.schres.2011.03.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Velligan D. I., Mahurin R. K., Diamond P. L., Hazleton B. C., Eckert S. L., Miller A. L. (1997). The functional significance of symptomatology and cognitive function in schizophrenia. Schizophr. Res. 25 21–31. 10.1016/S0920-9964(97)00010-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Viau R., Arsenault-Lapierre G., Fecteau S., Champagne N., Walker S. C. D., Lupien S. (2010). Effect of service dogs on salivary cortisol secretion in autistic children. Psychoneuroendocrinology 35 1187–1193. 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2010.02.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Villalta-Gil V., Ochoa S. (2007). La terapia facilitada por animales de compañía como programa de rehabilitación adjunto para personas con diagnóstico de esquizofrenia crónica. Papeles Del Psicólogo 28 49–56. [Google Scholar]
  55. Villalta-Gil V., Roca M., Domènec E., Escanilla A., Asensio M. R., Esteban M. E., et al. (2009). Dog-assisted therapy in the treatment of chronic schizophrenia inpatients. Anthrozoos 22 149–159. 10.2752/175303709X434176 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  56. Warren K. R., Ball M. P., Feldman S., Liu F., McMahon R. P., Kelly D. L. (2011). Exercise program adherence using a 5-kilometer (5k) event as an achievable goal in people with schizophrenia. Biol. Res. Nurs. 13 383–390. 10.1177/1099800410393272 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Wilson-d’Almeida K., Karrow A., Bralet M. C., Bazin N., Hardy-Baylé M. C., Falissard B. (2013). In patients with schizophrenia, symptoms improvement can be uncorrelated with quality of life improvement. Eur. Psychiatry 28 185–189. 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2011.10.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Zasloff R. L. (1996). Measuring attachment to companion animals: a dog is not a cat is not a bird. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 47 43–48. 10.1037/a0015213 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  59. Zik J. B., Roberts D. L. (2014). The many faces of oxytocin: implications for psychiatry. Psychiatry Res. 226 31–37. 10.1016/j.psychres.2014.11.048 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Frontiers in Psychology are provided here courtesy of Frontiers Media SA

RESOURCES