Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2016 May 11.
Published in final edited form as: Curr Biol. 2015 May 4;25(9):R358–R359. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.03.005

Immediate susceptibility to visual illusions after sight onset

Tapan Gandhi 1,2,*, Amy Kali 2, Suma Ganesh 3, Pawan Sinha 2
PMCID: PMC4863640  NIHMSID: NIHMS686006  PMID: 25942545

The dominant account for many visual illusions is based on experience-driven development of sensitivity to certain visual cues. According to this account, learned associations between observed 2D cues (say, converging lines) and the real 3D structures they represent (a surface receding in depth) render us susceptible to misperceiving some images that are cleverly contrived to contain those 2D cues. While this explanation appears reasonable, it lacks direct experimental validation. To contrast it against an account that dispenses with the need for visual experience, we have to determine whether susceptibility to the illusion is present immediately after birth. However, eliciting reliable responses from newborns is fraught with operational difficulties and studies with older infants are incapable of resolving this issue. Our work with children who gain sight after extended early-onset blindness, as part of Project Prakash, provides a potential way forward. We find that the newly sighted children, ranging in age from 8 through 16 years, exhibit susceptibility to two well-known geometrical visual illusions, Ponzo [1] and Müller-Lyer [2], immediately after the onset of sight. This finding has implications not only for the likely explanations of these illusions, but more generally, for the nature-nurture argument as it relates to some key aspects of visual processing.

In the Ponzo illusion (figure 1A, left), first demonstrated over a century ago, two identical stripes, placed on a background of converging lines, appear to be of different lengths. According to an influential account [3, 4], this anomalous percept arises from our learned association of 2D perspective cues with the distances they represent in the 3D world. Based on our past visual experience, we come to interpret the Ponzo display as depicting two objects at different depths in the 3D scene, with the stripe closer to the point of convergence seen as being further away. To reconcile this 3D interpretation with the 2D display in which both stripes subtend the same visual angle, the visual system is led to infer that the distant stripe must be physically longer. This inference is believed to influence perception, making the `distant' stripe appear longer in the display. A similar account has been offered for the even older Müller-Lyer illusion [2] (figure 1A, middle and right). The perceived disparity in line lengths is thought to be an outcome of our experience with the three-dimensional world [46], with the fins conveying a sense of lines advancing or receding in depth. Results from cross-cultural studies have provided support to experience-based explanations [7]. Although alternative accounts have been suggested [8], there has thus far been no direct test of the necessity of visual experience for engendering susceptibility to these illusions.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

The Susceptibility of Newly-Sighted Individuals to Visual Illusions. (A) The Ponzo and Muller-Lyer illusions superimposed on real images to indicate how learned perspective cues, as proxies for distance, may be the source of the effects. (Images after [5]; the railroad tracks image is by Darren Lewis and is in the public domain) (B) Results from normally sighted and newly sighted subjects on multiple displays. In each of these displays, the two lines being compared (denoted `A' and `B') are actually of identical length. Data are represented as the proportion of subjects (%) reporting each type of response.

Experience-based explanations predict that susceptibility to the Ponzo and Müller-Lyer illusions will not be evident in observers who are visually naïve, such as newborn infants. However, eliciting reliable responses from neonates is fraught with operational difficulties, and studies with older visually-experienced infants are incapable of resolving the issue. Our work in India with children who gain sight after extended congenital blindness [9] provides a potential way forward.

We tested nine children, ranging in age from 8 to 16 years (mean: 12.2 years), who were treated for blindness due to dense bilateral congenital cataracts that limited their pre-operative vision to the perception of hand movements close to their face. Given the remote rural domiciles of the patients, formal medical reports of their ophthalmic status at birth were not available. Assessments of cataract congenitality were based on multiple factors including parental reports, presence of nystagmus and nature of cataracts (please see supplementary information). The children underwent cataract removal surgery and an intraocular lens (IOL) implant. All children were tested within 48 hours after first eye surgery. Since only one eye had been treated at the time of the experiment, the patients had had no exposure to binocular depth cues. 9 normally-sighted children (age range: 6–18 years; mean: 11.9 years), with similar socio-economic status as the patients and drawn from a local municipal school, participated as controls. The stimuli comprised variations on the basic Ponzo and Müller-Lyer displays (as shown in figure 1B), subtending 50 degrees of visual angle at a viewing distance of 30 cm. In each display, the subjects' task was to point to the line that appeared longer or say that the lines were of equal length. No feedback was provided to the subjects.

As shown in figure 1b, control subjects showed a reliable susceptibility to the illusions. If the illusion is driven by a learned appreciation of perspective cues, we would expect the newly-sighted children's responses to be physically veridical and hence, inconsistent with the control subjects' choices. However, the data reveal that the newly-sighted behave akin to the control group in their choices. The pattern of responses exhibited by the Prakash children is unlikely to arise by random chance (Binomial test, Ponzo illusion: p<0.05 for six of the nine children individually; Müller-Lyer illusion: p<0.01 for all nine children individually. p ≈ 0.0 for pooled data across all children for each of these illusions). Thus, even at the very outset of their visual experience, the Prakash children already exhibit susceptibility to the Ponzo and Müller-Lyer illusions. These results are especially interesting in the context of past studies of late sight onset which have shown that the newly sighted have difficulties with spatial perception of scenes [9]. This suggests that susceptibility to the Ponzo and Muller-Lyer illusions likely does not depend upon a sophisticated spatial analysis of the scene. It is also worth considering the possibility that susceptibility to these visual illusions may be engendered by prior haptic experience. Although we cannot definitively rule out this explanation, the lack of transfer from touch to vision that we have previously observed in newly sighted patients [10], argues against this possibility.

These results argue that the susceptibility to these two classic illusions is based not on an individual's learned contingencies about the visual world, but rather on processing mechanisms that do not depend on visual experience.

Supplementary Material

01

Acknowledgments

The authors wish to thank all children who participated in these studies, members of the Project Prakash team who were instrumental in identifying treatably blind children. The research reported here was supported by the James McDonnell Foundation and the National Eye Institute of NIH.

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

Supplemental Information Supplemental Information includes experimental procedures, and one table and can be found with this article online at *bxs.

References

  • 1.Ponzo M. Archives Italiennes de Biologie. 1911. Intorno ad alcune illusioni nel campo delle sensazioni tattili sull'illusione di Aristotele e fenomeni analoghi. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Müller-Lyer FC. Optische Urteilstäuschungen. Archiv für Physiologie. 1889;(Suppl):263–270. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Gregory RL. Distortion of visual space as inappropriate constancy scaling. Nature. 1963;199:678–680. doi: 10.1038/199678a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Gillam B. Geometrical illusions. Scientific American. 1980;242:102–111. [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Gregory RL. Eye and brain. McGraw-Hill; New York: 1966. [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Howe CQ, Dale Purves D. The Müller-Lyer illusion explained by the statistics of image–source relationships. Procs. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2005;102:1234–1239. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0409314102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Segall M, Campbell D, Herskovits MJ. The Influence of Culture on Visual Perception. The Bobbs-Merrill Company; New York: 1966. [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Prinzmetal W, Shimamura AP, Mikolinski M. The Ponzo illusion and the perception of orientation. Percept. Psychophys. 2001;63:99–114. doi: 10.3758/bf03200506. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Sinha P, Held R. Sight-restoration. F1000 Medicine Reports. 2012;4:17. doi: 10.3410/M4-17. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Held R, Ostrovsky Y, deGelder B, Gandhi T, Ganesh S, Mathur U, Sinha P. Newly sighted cannot match seen with felt. Nat. Neurosci. 2011;14:551–553. doi: 10.1038/nn.2795. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

01

RESOURCES