Abstract
Introduction
Clinical pain models can be applied when investigating basic physiologic pain responses in healthy volunteers. Several pain models exist; however, only few have been adequately validated. Our primary aim with this prospective study was to investigate the intra- and inter-individual variation in secondary hyperalgesia elicited by brief thermal sensitization (45°C for 3 min) in healthy volunteers.
Material and Methods
Fifty healthy volunteers were included. Areas of secondary hyperalgesia following brief thermal sensitization were investigated by 2 observers on 4 experimental days, with a minimum interval of 7 days. Additionally, heat pain detection threshold and pain during thermal stimulation (45°C for 1 min.), and the psychological tests Pain Catastrophizing Scale and Hospital Anxiety and Depression Score were applied.
Results
For areas of secondary hyperalgesia, an intra-observer intra-person correlation of 0.85, 95% CI [0.78, 0.90], an intra-observer inter-person correlation of 0.03, 95% CI [0.00, 0.16], and a coefficient of variation of 0.17, 95% CI [0.14, 0.21] was demonstrated. Four percent of the study population had areas of secondary hyperalgesia both below the 1st and above the 3rd quartile considering all included participants. Heat pain detection threshold predicted area of secondary hyperalgesia with an adjusted R2 of 0.20 (P = 0.0006).
Conclusions
We have demonstrated a low intra-individual, and a high inter-individual variation in thermally induced secondary hyperalgesia. We conclude that brief thermal sensitization produce secondary hyperalgesia with a high level of reproducibility, which can be applied to investigate different phenotypes related to secondary hyperalgesia in healthy volunteers.
Trial Registration
clinicaltrials.gov NCT02166164
Introduction
Clinical pain models are important in order to investigate basic physiologic pain responses in both healthy volunteers and patients. Such models play an important role in translational studies and are necessary to bridge the gap between animal and human pain research.
There are numerous pain models, employed to investigate different aspects of the human physiologic pain response [1, 2]. Models applying nociceptive stimulation to the skin, by heat [3–8], cold [9] or electrical stimuli [10] can be used in investigation of injury-induced sensitization of the central nervous system. Central sensitization is believed to be an important factor in the development and maintenance of pain, and represents an uncoupling of the nociceptive stimulus and the nociceptive response [11]. Likewise, central sensitization may have a prominent role in the inter-individual differences in pain sensitivity; the concept that different individuals experience different levels of pain when exposed to identical noxious or nociceptive stimuli.
A standardized heat injury of the skin results in primary hyperalgesia at the site of injury, and secondary hyperalgesia surrounding the traumatized area [1, 3–8]. Injury-induced secondary hyperalgesia is characterized by reduced thresholds for mechanical stimulation, and is supposed to result from an altered central processing of mechano- and nociceptive input in A-fibers from the periphery, so that activation of these fibers produce painful sensations [11–14].
Moreover, a significant inter-individual difference in the size of the area of secondary hyperalgesia may persist, implying that the development of secondary hyperalgesia may be a phenotypic expression [15]. The inter-individual differences in areas of secondary hyperalgesia may be due to genetic [16], physiologic, and psychological differences [17], as well as differences in brain activation during pain stimulation [18]; however, further studies are needed to confirm this hypothesis.
Several pain models investigating cutaneous sensitization exist—each investigating different aspects of cutaneous sensitization. Brief thermal sensitization (BTS) [4–8] induces short lasting cutaneous sensitization, ideal for multiple inductions throughout a study day. With the BTS-model the skin is heated to 45°C for 3 min., resulting in mild pain perception, and short lasting secondary hyperalgesia [4–8]. Thus, the BTS-model can be applied in investigation of central sensitization. To our knowledge there have been no prospective trials investigating intra-individual, inter-individual, and inter-investigator variances of areas of secondary hyperalgesia following BTS. Validation of the models is paramount, and methodological sound studies investigating the inter-and intra-individual reproducibility, as well as the inter- and intra-investigator reproducibility are needed in order to validate the use of the models in future scientific research [19, 20].
The primary aim of this prospective cohort study was to investigate the intra-individual and inter-individual variance in secondary hyperalgesia elicited by brief thermal sensitization in healthy male volunteers.
Material and Methods
The study was registered at clinicaltrials.gov (NCT02166164), and approved by the Danish Regional Committee on Health Research Ethics (Identifier: H-4-2014-027), and the Danish Data Protection Agency (Identifier: 30–1217). Informed written consent was obtained from all participants before inclusion in the study. The study was conducted at the Department of Anaesthesiology, 4231, Rigshospitalet, Copenhagen, Denmark, in the period from June 10, 2014 to September 17, 2014.
Study design
This prospective cohort study was designed to evaluate the method of BTS, and consisted of four identical experimental days and one information/inclusion day. To prevent carry-over effects, the information day and each of the four experimental days were separated by a minimum of seven days.
The participants were tested with three procedures, brief thermal sensitization (BTS), heat pain detection threshold (HPDT), and pain during 1 min. thermal stimulation (p-TS) (for definitions, see below) on the four separate experimental days in a predefined sequence (see Randomization and allocation concealment).
All the pain models were performed with the computer-controlled Somedic Senselab MSA Thermotester™; size 2.5x5 cm.
On the information day the participants were given the psychological tests Pain Catastrophizing Scale (PCS) and Hospital Anxiety and Depression Score (HADS). The participants completed the PCS and HADS questionnaires and handed them back on the first experimental day in a concealed opaque envelope to ensure blinding. Opening of the envelopes were postponed until all participants had completed all four experimental days.
In order to investigate the inter-investigator variance, two different investigators were responsible for the testing on the different experimental days. Each participant was tested on four different study days. Two different investigators performed the testing. Every participant was thus tested by each investigator independently on two separate days—the order of the days being randomized.
The investigators were trained in performing the assessments similarly, but conducted the tests independently of each other. Test results were placed in an opaque sealed envelope to ensure blinding between the two investigators.
Study participants
50 healthy male volunteers were included (Fig 1). Informed consent was obtained from all included participants. Participants were recruited by advertisement in the medical student magazine and online at www.forsøgspersoner.dk. Inclusion criteria were: Male sex, age ≥18 years and ≤35 years, speak and understand the Danish language, and signed informed consent. Exclusion criteria were: Failure to cooperate to the tests, alcohol and/or substance abuse, consummation of analgesics within 48 hours before experimental day, consummation of prescription medicine within the last 30 days before experimental day, history of chronic pain, psychiatric diagnoses, tattoos on the extremities, and a Body Mass Index (BMI) of >30 kg/m2 and < 18 kg/m2.
Setting
The study was conducted in a quiet secluded room (temperature 22–25 degrees Celsius), where only the study participant and the responsible investigator were present. The participant was placed in a supine position during the assessment. The study was conducted during the time from 8.00 AM to 5.00 PM.
Pain models
Brief thermal sensitization (BTS)
Induction of BTS was performed anterior on the right thigh, in the midline between the anterior superior iliac spine and the base of patella [4–8]. The skin was heated to 45°C for 3 min. After 3 min., while the 45°C thermode was still placed on the skin, the assessment of secondary hyperalgesia was performed (see section “assessment of secondary hyperalgesia”). The assessment of secondary hyperalgesia took approximately 1–2 minutes, with a maximum duration of heat stimulation of 5 min.
Pain during 1 min. thermal stimulation (p-TS)
Evaluation of p-TS was performed on the anterior aspect of the non-dominant volar side of the forearm [5, 21–24]. The participant’s skin was heated to 45°C for 1 min., while the participant performed continuous evaluation of pain on an electronic visual analogue scale (VAS, 0 mm = no pain; 100 mm = worst pain imaginable). A maximum (Max.)-VAS and a VAS-Area under the curve (AUC) were registered. The participant was not able to see the computer-screen during the assessment.
Heat pain detection threshold (HPDT)
The skin on the dominant anterior volar side of the forearm was heated with an increase in temperature of 1°C/sec (initial temperature 32°C) [5, 8, 21–24]. The study participant stated when the heat was perceived as painful by pressing a button, and the temperature was registered. The HPDT was calculated as an average of 4 stimulations. Each stimulation was performed with an interval of 6–10 seconds. The participant was not able to see the computer-screen during the assessment.
Assessment of secondary hyperalgesia
The area of secondary hyperalgesia was evaluated following BTS. The area was quantified by stimulation with a 19G monofilament (Von Frey hair) in 4 linear paths arranged 90° around the center of the heat-stimulation. The monofilament stimulation was initiated in normal skin, and advanced in steps of 5 mm with 1-second intervals towards the center of the heat-stimulation until the participant stated a clear change in the sensation (burning, intense pricking, tenderness). The borders were marked with a felt pen, and the transverse and longitudinal axes were measured with a pliable measuring tape for rectangular area calculation [4–8, 15, 21–27].
The area of primary hyperalgesia was defined as the area directly heated by the thermode (2.5x5 cm). The surrounding area with decreased mechanical thresholds was defined as the area of secondary hyperalgesia. For calculations, the area of the thermode (2.5x5 cm) was not subtracted from the total area of secondary hyperalgesia.
Psychological testing
Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS)
HADS is a questionnaire consisting of 14 questions [28]. HADS evaluates depression and anxiety, and can be subdivided in HADS-Anxiety and HADS-Depression. The highest achievable score is 42.
Pain Catastrophizing Score (PCS)
PCS is a questionnaire consisting of 13 questions [29] and can be subdivided into 3 subtests that each evaluates the central elements in catastrophizing: Rumination, magnification, and helplessness. The highest achievable score is 52.
Outcomes
Primary outcome
To determine the intra- and inter-participant variance, and the intra- and inter-investigator variance of the secondary hyperalgesia areas following BTS on 4 separate experimental days with two different observers.
Secondary outcomes
To investigate:
How precise the scores of PCS and HADS predict the size of the area of secondary hyperalgesia.
How precise the subscales in PCS and HADS (PCS-rumination, PCS-magnification, PCS-helplessness, and HADS-Anxiety, HADS-Depression) predict the size of the area of secondary hyperalgesia.
How precise the HPDT evaluated on the 4 experimental days predicts the area of secondary hyperalgesia on the respective 4 experimental days.
How precise the Maximum VAS-score following p-TS evaluated on the 4 experimental days predicts the area of secondary hyperalgesia on the respective 4 experimental days.
How precise the VAS-AUC following p-TS evaluated on the 4 experimental days predicts the area of secondary hyperalgesia on the respective 4 experimental days
Sample size estimation
Estimation of the number of participants, investigators and experimental days were based on statistical simulations based on data from a previous study [4]. The simulations demonstrated that a scenario with 2 investigators, 4 experimental days and 50 study participants would enable us to discern the relevant variance components with acceptable precision.
For full documentation of statistical simulations, please see supporting information available online (S1 Appendix).
Randomization and allocation concealment
The reproducibility of the area of secondary hyperalgesia following BTS was the primary outcome. Thus, in order to avoid possible carry over effects of the HPDT and p-TS, all study days began with BTS. Testing with HPDT and p-TS were therefore subsequent to BTS on all study days; However, the sequence of testing (HPDT and p-TS) was randomized for each patient and each experimental day, so that on two of the four experimental days the sequence was: 1) BTS, 2) HPDT, 3) p-TS, and on the remaining two study days the sequence was: 1) BTS, 2) p-TS, 3) HPDT (Fig 2).
The investigator responsible for testing and registration of data on the respective experimental day was randomized, so the same investigator was not responsible for testing the same participant on two consecutive experimental days. The allocation sequence of participants to investigator, and the test allocation sequence (HPDT and p-TS) were randomly generated via a computer by the data manager at Copenhagen Trial Unit.
The randomization of the test allocation sequence was kept in opaque sealed envelopes prepared by Copenhagen Trial Unit to ensure allocation concealment. The envelopes remained sealed until immediately before the testing.
Statistical analysis
The variation in the areas of secondary hyperalgesia derived from the study participant, the experimental day, and the investigator was determined using a variance component model.
For each of the 5 secondary outcomes, the ability of PCS, HADS, HPDT, Max-VAS and VAS-AUC (following p-TS), to predict individual variations in areas of secondary hyperalgesia was investigated by linear regression on the estimated best linear unbiased predictors (EBLUPS) of individual secondary hyperalgesia extracted from the primary analysis. HPDT, Max-VAS, and VAS-AUC profiles were also summarized in terms of EBLUPS.
Significance of the predictors was assessed by Analysis of variance (ANOVA) methods and their predictive abilities were quantified with various summaries of prediction errors including 95% prediction intervals for the predictions. Model reduction was done by means of backwards elimination with a cut-off value of 5%.
The variation in the areas of secondary hyperalgesia described in the primary outcomes will be reported as Intraclass Correlations (ICC) and Coefficient of Variations (CV).
The predictive abilities of the variables described in the secondary outcomes are summarized by adjusted R2 and illustrated by 95% predictive intervals for selected values of the remaining predictor.
Reproducibility of area of secondary hyperalgesia
We planned to categorize the participants in three groups according to the mean size of the area of secondary hyperalgesia: “Small-area” (1st quartile), “medium-area” (2nd and 3rd quartile), and “large-area” (4th quartile).
Based on the study performed by Werner et al. [15] we expected measures of reproducibility as detailed below:
Intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC) around 0.74, corresponding to an intra-participant variance of approximately 25% of the inter-individual variance.
A pooled mean intra-participant CV around 0.25
No more than 10% of the study participants change group from “small-area” to “large-area” or vice versa
Results
All study participants completed the study, and all study participants were analyzed for primary and secondary outcomes (Fig 1).
Data on participants’ characteristics are presented in Table 1. The median interval between the information day, experimental day 1, 2, 3 and 4 was 12 (Range; 7–33), 9 (7–35), 15 (7–61) and 8.5 (7–37) days respectively.
Table 1. Characteristics of included participants.
Variable | Mean (SD) | Range (min.-max.) |
---|---|---|
Age (years) | 24 (3) | 18–32 |
Height (m) | 1,85 (0,1) | 1.69–1.98 |
Weight (kg) | 78 (9) | 65–105 |
BMI (m2/kg) | 22,8 (2,1) | 18.4–27.3 |
Abbreviations: BMI, Body Mass Index
No adverse or serious adverse events were reported.
The results from this study has previously been presented in abstract form [30].
Secondary hyperalgesia following BTS
The secondary hyperalgesia following BTS was evaluated on the 4 separate experimental days by 2 different investigators (1 investigator per experimental day). We found (i) an intra-investigator intra-participant correlation of 0.85, 95% CI (0.78, 0.90), (ii) an inter-investigator intra-participant correlation of 0.82 (0.69, 0.89), (iii) an intra-investigator inter-participant correlation of 0.03 (0.00, 0.16), and (iv) a coefficient of variation of 0.17 (0.14, 0.21).
Only 2 participants, 4% (1%, 13%) of the total population, had areas of secondary hyperalgesia in both below the 1st quartile and above the 3rd quartile, considering the total population. The sizes of the areas of secondary hyperalgesia as well as the results of the ICC are presented in Tables 2 and 3 and Fig 3, respectively.
Table 2. Median size of the area of secondary hyperalgesia.
QST | Experimental day 1 | Experimental day 2 | Experimental day 3 | Experimental day 4 | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Median (IQR) | Range | Median (IQR) | Range | Median (IQR) | Range | Median (IQR) | Range | |
BTS* | 311.9 (256.6–457.0) | 12.5–742.5 | 294.0 (250.5–417.3) | 12.5–748.0 | 339.3 (231.7–389.9) | 12.5–641.3 | 310.1 (244.1–413.3) | 12.5–681.5 |
Median size and range of areas of secondary hyperalgesia following BTS on the four experimental days
* Medians, IQRs and ranges are given in Cm2
Abbreviations: QST, Quantitative Sensory Testing; BTS, Brief thermal sensitization, IQR, Interquartile range
Table 3. Main results.
Parameter | Result (95% CI) |
---|---|
ICCIntra-investigator intra-participant | 0.85 (0.78–0.90) |
ICCInter-investigator intra-participant | 0.82 (0.69–0.89) |
ICCIntra-investigator inter-participant | 0.03 (0.0–0.16) |
CV | 0.17 (0.14–0.21) |
Intraclass Correlations and Coefficient of Variation. Abbreviations: ICC, Intra Class Correlation; CV, Coefficient of Variation
Predictive factors of the area of secondary hyperalgesia
The ability of HPDT, Max-VAS (following p-TS), VAS-AUC (following p-TS), PCS and HADS to predict inter-individual variations in area of secondary hyperalgesia was investigated. After backwards elimination with a cut-off value of 5% only HPDT offered a statistically significant prediction of the area of secondary hyperalgesia with an adjusted R2 of 0.20 (P = 0.0006). No other evaluated factors significantly predicted the area of secondary hyperalgesia. Results of HPDT, Max-VAS (following p-TS), VAS-AUC (following p-TS), PCS and HADS are presented in Tables 4 and 5 and Figs 4, 5 and 6.
Table 4. Heat pain detection threshold, and pain during 1 min. thermal stimulation.
QST | Experimental day 1 | Experimental day 2 | Experimental day 3 | Experimental day 4 | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Median (IQR) | Range | Median (IQR) | Range | Median (IQR) | Range | Median (IQR) | Range | |
HPDT* | 44.8 (42.4–46) | 37.6–48.4 | 44.9 (43.1–46.3) | 36.7–48.5 | 44.7 (42–46.4) | 37.5–48.9 | 45.1 (43.6–46) | 36.7–48.7 |
p-TS-max. VAS | 23.0 (16–36) | 0.0–72.0 | 27.0 (16–34) | 0.0–71 | 25.0 (16.8–37.3) | 3.0–64.0 | 27.0 (17.5–40) | 2.0–71.0 |
p-TS-AUC VAS | 853.8 (325.2–1198) | 0.0–3663.0 | 851.8 (448.3–1275.3) | 0.0–3620.50 | 936.2 (523.4–1216.2) | 8.8–3400.7 | 901.7 (421.1–1416.6) | 6.9–3294.5 |
Median, IQR and range of HPDT, p-TS-max VAS and p-TS-AUC VAS on the 4 experimental days.
* Medians, IQRs and ranges are given in °C
Abbreviations: QST, Quantitative Sensory Test; HPDT, Heat pain detection threshold; p-TS, pain during 1 min. thermal stimulation, Max., maximum; AUC, Area Under the Curve; VAS, Visual Analog Scale, IQR, Interquartile range
Table 5. Scores of Pain Catastrophizing Scale and Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale.
Psychological test | Median (IQR) | Range |
---|---|---|
PCSRumination | 6 (3.8–8) | 0–15 |
PCSMagnification | 2 (1–4) | 0–7 |
PCSHelplessness | 3 (2–7) | 0–19 |
PCSTotal | 12 (7.8–18) | 0–38 |
HADSAnxiety | 4 (1–6) | 0–12 |
HADSDepression | 1 (1–3) | 0–9 |
HADSTotal | 5 (3–10) | 0–18 |
Median, IQRs and range of the PCS and HADS. Total scores and scores of individual subtests are displayed. Abbreviations: PCS, Pain Catastrophizing Score; HADS, Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale, IQR, Interquartile range
Discussion
The aim of this study was to investigate the intra-individual, inter-individual, and inter-investigator variances of BTS-elicited areas of secondary hyperalgesia, in order to examine the reproducibility of the pain model. There are no gold standards for reproducibility; however, prior to our study we defined three criteria, hypothesized using data from previous studies. In order to confirm the reproducibility of the model, all three criteria had to be fulfilled (see methods). Firstly, we demonstrated an inter-investigator intra-participant correlation of 0.82 (0.69, 0.89), secondly we found a coefficient of variation of 0.17 (0.14, 0.21), and lastly we demonstrated that only two participants, 4% (1%, 13%) of the total population, had areas of secondary hyperalgesia in both below the 1st quartile and above the 3rd quartile, considering the total population. By evaluating the point estimates, all three criteria are fulfilled, and according to our pre-defined criteria, we have demonstrated that BTS is a reproducible model in regards to eliciting secondary hyperalgesia in healthy volunteers. The rather high ICCs and low CV demonstrate a high reliability and reproducibility respectively.
We also demonstrated that HPDT significantly predicted the area of secondary hyperalgesia. Our study was not designed to detect the correlation between HPDT and areas of secondary hyperalgesia; however, we find a highly significant result (P = 0.0006). An adjusted R2 of 0.20 is nonetheless an indication that HPDT only offers a very modest explanation of the variation in BTS, which poses the question: How precise does HPDT predict areas of secondary hyperalgesia? In the current study, we estimated a prediction interval for BTS to (53.14–515.43) given a HPDT of 46°C, indicating wide prediction intervals. The possible variation of BTS for a given HPDT value is huge and HPDT and secondary hyperalgesia following BTS may thus represent two different pain entities. HPDT has been demonstrated to be highly reproducible [31], and both HPDT and brief thermal sensitization activates peripheral A-delta and C-fibers [32]. However, the secondary hyperalgesia to punctate mechanical stimuli, that occurs as a result of central neuronal plasticity of the nociceptive system is mediated by A-fiber nociceptors, not C-fibers [11–14]. Thus, secondary hyperalgesia as a result of central sensitization elicited by BTS may be significantly distinct from HPDT. To our knowledge, no studies have investigated this issue, and further studies are needed to confirm this hypothesis.
Individual characteristics, such as sex, and obesity, may influence pain thresholds and tolerance [24, 33–37]. Likewise it remains unclear whether the menstrual cycle influences the pain sensitivity in healthy women [38]. In addition we have no knowledge of what effect tattoos have on peripheral cutaneous sensitivity. To account for these variables, we applied strict inclusion and exclusion criteria, in order to minimize the unknown factors of variation. This enabled us to focus on the ability of BTS to produce an area of secondary hyperalgesia, rather than the influence of the individual characteristics of the participant.
Several clinical studies have demonstrated an association between psychological factors and pain [39, 40]. In the present study the two psychological tests, HADS and PCS, did not significantly predict the area of secondary hyperalgesia. Our study was not designed to detect the correlation between psychological test scores and area of secondary hyperalgesia; however, post hoc analyses demonstrated that in order to investigate such a correlation with sufficiently high power, 300 healthy participants should have been included in the study. A plausible reason for the weak correlation between psychological test scores and secondary hyperalgesia may be that we included a very homogenous population of healthy volunteers, that all had relatively low scores on HADS and PCS. By including healthy volunteers in an experimental pain study, it can be speculated that only few persons with a high anxiety-index or high-catastrophizing scores are enrolled, because they rarely consider volunteering in experimental pain studies. Thus, avoidance of sampling bias may be difficult. Therefore, in order to minimize sampling bias when investigating a possible association of high psychological vulnerability and experimental pain entities, consecutive inclusion of patients awaiting surgery, or specific inclusion of healthy volunteers with high psychological vulnerability seems necessary.
When evaluating the area of secondary hyperalgesia there exist different methods [4–8, 15, 18, 21–27, 41]. We chose a pragmatic approach that can be easily applied in a clinical setting by non-specialists, and evaluated the area of secondary hyperalgesia with the same polyamide filament (19g), in all the participants. Likewise, we chose a simple approach in calculating the area of secondary hyperalgesia, by using a 4 vector rectangular area calculation that has been applied in several previous studies [4–8, 15, 21–27]. More favorable results may have been achieved by using the up-down method when applying punctate mechanical stimuli with the polyamide monofilaments [42]; however, our results demonstrate that the methods we applied were sufficient to demonstrate that BTS produce a reproducible area of secondary hyperalgesia.
The area of secondary hyperalgesia elicited by BTS may represent the level of central sensitization in the individual participant. Woolf describes that different pain hypersensitivity syndromes may share a common contribution of central sensitization, and hypothesizes that the comorbidity of different clinical pain syndromes may be explained by a “central sensitization syndrome” [11]. Thus, individuals with high pain sensitivity may share common factors that may be identified prior to the development of chronic pain. Moreover, it unlocks the possibility that individuals can be “phenotyped” in regards to their pain hypersensitivity. The assessment of secondary hyperalgesia may be a tool for investigation of central sensitization, and thus, be applied as a predictive factor of e.g. postoperative pain. To our knowledge, only few studies have investigated the assessment of secondary hyperalgesia as a possible predictive factor of postoperative pain, with one study demonstrating no correlations between area of secondary hyperalgesia following burn injury [43], and another demonstrating that postoperative secondary hyperalgesia around the surgical incision following an iliac crest bone harvest was predictive for the development of chronic postsurgical neuropathic pain [44]. Our results demonstrate a high inter-participant variance in the area of secondary hyperalgesia. The primary aim with this study was not to identify possible causal factors that could explain the high inter-participant variance; however our results are interesting and several factors could provide explanation for the remarkable inter-participant variance in an otherwise homogenous population. Factors such as stress [45], diet, including tryptophan intake [46, 47], hormone levels [45], skin receptor density, anatomical and functional brain differences [18], as well as genetics [16] could have influenced our results. The area of secondary hyperalgesia as a phenotypic indicator of pain hypersensitivity, and as a predictor for the development of acute and chronic pain is yet unexplored and further research is needed in order to clarify this.
The applicability of quantitative sensory testing (QST) and experimental pain models in translational and clinical studies has been widely debated. Should QST be implemented in the daily clinical practice, and should trials investigating analgesics implement the use of experimental pain models? When evaluating the increasingly body of research performed on QST and pain models, the main problem appears to be the heterogeneity in study methodology and statistical approaches [1, 48]. When using QST it is recommend that intra-participant reliability is determined [19]. Thus, before QST and experimental pain models can be fully implemented in clinical studies, reliability of the individual models is necessary. This means that several prospective methodological studies of the individual models should be performed [49]; evaluating intra- and inter-participant variance, as well as intra- and inter-investigator variance. Moreover, general accepted measures of reproducibility are needed. So far, no general recommendations have been proposed on how reproducible or reliable the various pain models or QSTs should be before they are implemented in translational or clinical research [50]. In the present study, we attempted to pre-define measures of reproducibility based on an earlier retrospective study [15]. This is in our opinion an important strength, and hypotheses regarding reproducibility/reliability should be implemented in study design as well as sample size analysis in future studies. If a test is not reliable and/or reproducible, then it cannot be used as tool in a diverse scientific community.
Our study has some limitations. Firstly, the two investigators were trained to perform BTS in precisely the same manner. That includes the assessment of secondary hyperalgesia, as well as the information given to the participants. This means, that if two entirely independent investigators were to perform the assessment without rigorous simultaneous training, the inter-investigator variance might increase. Moreover, even though our simulation study (see supporting information: S1 Appendix, simulation study 2, Figs 4 and 5) demonstrated that a scenario with 2 investigators and 50 study participants would enable us to discern the relevant variance components with acceptable precision, a higher number of independent observers is required to obtain final conclusions on the inter-observer variance and inter-study comparisons.
Secondly, we applied BTS on a highly homogenous population of healthy, male volunteers. Inclusion of women, elderly patients and chronic pain patients could potentially have increased the inter-participant variance with a clustering of effects in chronic pain patients. Thus, studies investigating a heterogeneous clinical population are needed in order to clarify the potential of BTS as a tool for evaluating both male and female patients, as well as the young and elderly population. Thirdly, we did not evaluate the participants’ dietary intake or hormone levels. Studies have demonstrated that tryptophan may increase the pain sensitivity [46, 47], and cortisol and testosterone levels may influence the pain sensitivity [45]. Consequently, control of the dietary intake may have decreased intra-participant variance, and evaluation of hormone levels may have been an explanatory factor in the high inter-participant variance in the area of secondary hyperalgesia.
Lastly, even though our HPDT was within the range reported in previous studies [51], it may be that the three participants with a mean HPDT below 40°C possibly misunderstood the procedure.
In conclusion, our rigorous prospective study confirms earlier retrospective indications, that BTS produce a reproducible area of secondary hyperalgesia [15]. Furthermore, we have demonstrated a low intra-participant variance and a high inter-participant variance compared to inter-observer variance. BTS can therefore be applied in investigations of secondary hyperalgesia in healthy volunteers. However, to thoroughly determine that BTS is a reliable tool for pain research, other independent research groups should continue investigation of BTS in healthy volunteers and in more heterogeneous clinical populations.
Supporting Information
Acknowledgments
We would like to thank research nurse Karen Lisa Hilsted for valuable comments to the conception and design of the study.
Data Availability
All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files.
Funding Statement
This work was supported by Doctor Fritz Karner and Wife’s Foundation (in Danish: Læge Fritz Karner og Hustrus Fond), no grant number available, no URL available: MSH and JBD received the funding. The Foundation for Medical Science’s Advancement (in Danish: Fonden for Lægevidenskabens Fremme), grant number: 14-84, URL: http://www.apmollerfonde.dk/ansoegning/laegefonden.aspx: JBD recieved the funding. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
References
- 1.Reddy KS, Naidu MU, Rani PU, Rao TR. Human experimental pain models: A review of standardized methods in drug development. J Res Med Sci. 2012;17(6):587–95. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Handwerker HO, Kobal G. Psychophysiology of experimentally induced pain. Physiol Rev. 1993;73(3):639–71. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Moiniche S, Dahl JB, Kehlet H. Time course of primary and secondary hyperalgesia after heat injury to the skin. Br J Anaesth. 1993;71(2):201–5. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Dirks J, Petersen KL, Dahl JB. The heat/capsaicin sensitization model: a methodologic study. J Pain. 2003;4(3):122–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Dirks J, Petersen KL, Rowbotham MC, Dahl JB. Gabapentin suppresses cutaneous hyperalgesia following heat-capsaicin sensitization. Anesthesiology. 2002;97(1):102–7. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Frymoyer AR, Rowbotham MC, Petersen KL. Placebo-controlled comparison of a morphine/dextromethorphan combination with morphine on experimental pain and hyperalgesia in healthy volunteers. J Pain. 2007;8(1):19–25. 10.1016/j.jpain.2006.05.010 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Petersen KL, Iyengar S, Chappell AS, Lobo ED, Reda H, Prucka WR, et al. Safety, tolerability, pharmacokinetics, and effects on human experimental pain of the selective ionotropic glutamate receptor 5 (iGluR5) antagonist LY545694 in healthy volunteers. Pain. 2014;155(5):929–36. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Petersen KL, Meadoff T, Press S, Peters MM, LeComte MD, Rowbotham MC. Changes in morphine analgesia and side effects during daily subcutaneous administration in healthy volunteers. Pain. 2008;137(2):395–404. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Ruscheweyh R, Stumpenhorst F, Knecht S, Marziniak M. Comparison of the cold pressor test and contact thermode-delivered cold stimuli for the assessment of cold pain sensitivity. J Pain. 2010;11(8):728–36. 10.1016/j.jpain.2009.10.016 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Koppert W, Dern SK, Sittl R, Albrecht S, Schuttler J, Schmelz M. A new model of electrically evoked pain and hyperalgesia in human skin: the effects of intravenous alfentanil, S(+)-ketamine, and lidocaine. Anesthesiology. 2001;95(2):395–402. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Woolf CJ. Central sensitization: implications for the diagnosis and treatment of pain. Pain. 2011;152(3 Suppl):S2–15. Epub Oct 18. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Latremoliere A, Woolf CJ. Central sensitization: a generator of pain hypersensitivity by central neural plasticity. J Pain. 2009;10(9):895–926. 10.1016/j.jpain.2009.06.012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Ziegler EA, Magerl W, Meyer RA, Treede RD. Secondary hyperalgesia to punctate mechanical stimuli. Central sensitization to A-fibre nociceptor input. Brain. 1999;122 (Pt 12):2245–57. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Treede RD. Chapter 1 Pain and hyperalgesia: definitions and theories. Handb Clin Neurol. 2006;81:3–10. 10.1016/S0072-9752(06)80005-9 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Werner MU, Petersen KL, Rowbotham MC, Dahl JB. Healthy volunteers can be phenotyped using cutaneous sensitization pain models. PLoS One. 2013;8(5):e62733 10.1371/journal.pone.0062733. Print 2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Diatchenko L, Slade GD, Nackley AG, Bhalang K, Sigurdsson A, Belfer I, et al. Genetic basis for individual variations in pain perception and the development of a chronic pain condition. Hum Mol Genet. 2005;14(1):135–43. 10.1093/hmg/ddi013 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Hansen MS, Horjales-Araujo E, Dahl JB. Associations between psychological variables and pain in experimental pain models. A systematic review. Acta Anaesthesiol Scand. 2015;59(9):1094–102. 10.1111/aas.12555 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Asghar MS, Pereira MP, Werner MU, Martensson J, Larsson HB, Dahl JB. Secondary Hyperalgesia Phenotypes Exhibit Differences in Brain Activation during Noxious Stimulation. PLoS One. 2015;10(1):e0114840 10.1371/journal.pone.0114840 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Dyck PJ. Quantitative sensory testing: a consensus report from the Peripheral Neuropathy Association. Neurology. 1993;43(5):1050–2. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Siao P, Cros DP. Quantitative sensory testing. Phys Med Rehabil Clin N Am. 2003;14(2):261–86. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Petersen KL, Jones B, Segredo V, Dahl JB, Rowbotham MC. Effect of remifentanil on pain and secondary hyperalgesia associated with the heat—capsaicin sensitization model in healthy volunteers. Anesthesiology. 2001;94(1):15–20. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Dirks J, Petersen KL, Rowbotham MC, Dahl JB. Effect of systemic adenosine on pain and secondary hyperalgesia associated with the heat/capsaicin sensitization model in healthy volunteers. Reg Anesth Pain Med. 2001;26(5):414–9. 10.1053/rapm.2001.22256a . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Cavallone LF, Frey K, Montana MC, Joyal J, Regina KJ, Petersen KL, et al. Reproducibility of the heat/capsaicin skin sensitization model in healthy volunteers. J Pain Res. 2013;6:771–84. 10.2147/JPR.S53437 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Jensen MT, Petersen KL. Gender differences in pain and secondary hyperalgesia after heat/capsaicin sensitization in healthy volunteers. J Pain. 2006;7(3):211–7. 10.1016/j.jpain.2005.10.013 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Mikkelsen S, Dirks J, Fabricius P, Petersen KL, Rowbotham MC, Dahl JB. Effect of intravenous magnesium on pain and secondary hyperalgesia associated with the heat/capsaicin sensitization model in healthy volunteers. Br J Anaesth. 2001;86(6):871–3. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Petersen KL, Brennum J, Dahl JB. Experimental evaluation of the analgesic effect of ibuprofen on primary and secondary hyperalgesia. Pain. 1997;70(2–3):167–74. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Petersen KL, Rowbotham MC. A new human experimental pain model: the heat/capsaicin sensitization model. Neuroreport. 1999;10(7):1511–6. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Zigmond AS, Snaith RP. The hospital anxiety and depression scale. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1983;67(6):361–70. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Sullivan MJL, Bishop SR, Pivik J. The Pain Catastrophizing Scale: Development and validation. Psychol Assess. 1995;7(4):524–32. [Google Scholar]
- 30.Hansen MS, Wetterslev J, Pipper CB, Asghar MS, Dahl JB, editors. The area of secondary hyperalgesia to heat stimulation characterizes healthy volunteers Conference Abstract Scandinavian Society of Anaesthesiology and Itensive care; 2015; Reykjavik, Iceland.
- 31.Khambam SK, Naidu MU, Rani PU, Rao TR. A simple contact heat experimental pain model for evaluation of analgesic agents in healthy volunteers. Curr Ther Res Clin Exp. 2011;72(6):233–42. 10.1016/j.curtheres.2011.11.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.McCormack K, Prather P, Chapleo C. Some new insights into the effects of opioids in phasic and tonic nociceptive tests. Pain. 1998;78(2):79–98. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Alabas OA, Tashani OA, Tabasam G, Johnson MI. Gender role affects experimental pain responses: a systematic review with meta-analysis. Eur J Pain. 2012;16(9):1211–23. 10.1002/j.1532-2149.2012.00121.x . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Bartley EJ, Fillingim RB. Sex differences in pain: a brief review of clinical and experimental findings. Br J Anaesth. 2013;111(1):52–8. 10.1093/bja/aet127 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Campesi I, Fois M, Franconi F. Sex and gender aspects in anesthetics and pain medication. Handb Exp Pharmacol. 2012;(214):265–78. 10.1007/978-3-642-30726-3_13 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Mogil JS. Sex differences in pain and pain inhibition: multiple explanations of a controversial phenomenon. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2012;13(12):859–66. 10.1038/nrn3360 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Okifuji A, Hare BD. The association between chronic pain and obesity. J Pain Res. 2015;8:399–408. 10.2147/JPR.S55598 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Iacovides S, Avidon I, Baker FC. Does pain vary across the menstrual cycle? A review. Eur J Pain. 2015;19(10):1389–405. Epub 2015 Apr 21. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Granot M, Ferber SG. The roles of pain catastrophizing and anxiety in the prediction of postoperative pain intensity: a prospective study. Clin J Pain. 2005;21(5):439–45. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Papaioannou M, Skapinakis P, Damigos D, Mavreas V, Broumas G, Palgimesi A. The role of catastrophizing in the prediction of postoperative pain. Pain Med. 2009;10(8):1452–9. Epub 2009 Oct 26. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Pedersen JL, Kehlet H. Secondary hyperalgesia to heat stimuli after burn injury in man. Pain. 1998;76(3):377–84. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Chaplan SR, Bach FW, Pogrel JW, Chung JM, Yaksh TL. Quantitative assessment of tactile allodynia in the rat paw. J Neurosci Methods. 1994;53(1):55–63. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Werner MU, Duun P, Kehlet H. Prediction of postoperative pain by preoperative nociceptive responses to heat stimulation. Anesthesiology. 2004;100(1):115–9; discussion 5A. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Martinez V, Ben Ammar S, Judet T, Bouhassira D, Chauvin M, Fletcher D. Risk factors predictive of chronic postsurgical neuropathic pain: the value of the iliac crest bone harvest model. Pain. 2012;153(7):1478–83. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Choi JC, Chung MI, Lee YD. Modulation of pain sensation by stress-related testosterone and cortisol. Anaesthesia. 2012;67(10):1146–51. 10.1111/j.1365-2044.2012.07267.x . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Lieberman HR, Corkin S, Spring BJ, Growdon JH, Wurtman RJ. Mood, performance, and pain sensitivity: changes induced by food constituents. J Psychiatr Res. 1982;17(2):135–45. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Abbott FV, Etienne P, Franklin KB, Morgan MJ, Sewitch MJ, Young SN. Acute tryptophan depletion blocks morphine analgesia in the cold-pressor test in humans. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 1992;108(1–2):60–6. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Werner MU, Petersen MA, Bischoff JM. Test-retest studies in quantitative sensory testing: a critical review. Acta Anaesthesiol Scand. 2013;57(8):957–63. 10.1111/aas.12150 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Geber C, Klein T, Azad S, Birklein F, Gierthmuhlen J, Huge V, et al. Test-retest and interobserver reliability of quantitative sensory testing according to the protocol of the German Research Network on Neuropathic Pain (DFNS): a multi-centre study. Pain. 2011;152(3):548–56. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Shy ME, Frohman EM, So YT, Arezzo JC, Cornblath DR, Giuliani MJ, et al. Quantitative sensory testing: report of the Therapeutics and Technology Assessment Subcommittee of the American Academy of Neurology. Neurology. 2003;60(6):898–904. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Erpelding N, Moayedi M, Davis KD. Cortical thickness correlates of pain and temperature sensitivity. Pain. 2012;153(8):1602–9. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files.