Abstract
Some past work indicates that sexual minorities may experience impairments in social health, or the perceived and actual availability and quality of one’s social relationships, relative to heterosexuals; however, research has been limited in many ways. Furthermore, it is important to investigate etiological factors that may be associated with these disparities, such as self-reported discrimination. The current work tested whether sexual minority adults in the United States reported less positive social health (i.e., loneliness, friendship strain, familial strain and social capital) relative to heterosexuals and whether self-reported discrimination accounted for these disparities. Participants for the current study (N = 579) were recruited via Amazon’s Mechanical Turk, including 365 self-identified heterosexuals (105 women) and 214 sexual minorities (103 women). Consistent with hypotheses, sexual minorities reported impaired social health relative to heterosexuals, with divergent patterns emerging by sexual orientation subgroup (which were generally consistent across sexes). Additionally, self-reported discrimination accounted for disparities across three of four indicators of social health. These findings suggest that sexual minorities may face obstacles related to prejudice and discrimination that impair the functioning of their relationships and overall social health. Moreover, because social health is closely related to psychological and physical health, remediating disparities in social relationships may be necessary to address other health disparities based upon sexual orientation. Expanding upon these results, implications for efforts to build resilience among sexual minorities are discussed.
Keywords: social disparities, social health, sexual minorities, discrimination, relationships
INTRODUCTION
Many theorists have proposed that well-functioning social relationships are a vital component of health and well-being (e.g., Baumeister & Leary, 1995; Bowlby, 1969; Durkheim, 1951) and much empirical research has confirmed this proposition (Berkman, Glass, Brissette, & Seeman, 2000; Hawkley & Cacioppo, 2010; Robles, Slatcher, Trombello, & McGinn, 2014; Ryff & Singer, 2000). Accumulating data indicate disparities in social health, or the perceived and actual availability and quality of one’s social relationships (Donald, Ware, Brook, & Davies-Avery, 1978), for members of devalued relative to dominant groups in the United States (Hatzenbuehler, Phelan, & Link, 2013; Umberson & Montez, 2010). Of note, these gaps in social health could contribute to other well-documented health disparities, including greater mental (McGuire & Miranda, 2008; United States Department of Health and Human Services [HHS], 2014) and physical health burdens (Adler & Rehkopf, 2008; HHS, 2014; Williams & Mohammed, 2009) among members of devalued groups. A group for whom social health disparities may be especially stark is sexual minorities (e.g., Andersson, Noack, Seierstad, & Weedon-Fekjær, 2006; Bos, Sandfort, de Bruyn, & Hakvoort, 2008; Fokkema & Kuyper, 2009; Valanis et al., 2000). Focusing on this population, the current work had two primary aims. First, we examined disparities in social health based upon sexual orientation. Second, we tested whether self-reported discrimination, which is predictive of impairments in psychological and physical health (Pascoe & Richman, 2009), might explain disparities in social health between sexual minorities and heterosexuals.
Social Relationships and Health
Because social relationships at multiple levels (e.g., with siblings, friends, neighbors, and even communities) are related to health outcomes (Kawachi & Berkman, 2001; Lin, Ye, & Ensel, 1999), the current research focused on social health, broadly conceptualized (Donald et al., 1978). Inherently a social species, humans rely on one another to both survive and thrive (Brewer & Caporeal, 1990; Buss & Kenrick, 1998). Perhaps unsurprisingly then, in order to understand the human condition it is necessary to understand human social relationships (Baumeister & Leary, 1995; Berscheid, 1999; Reis, Collins, & Berscheid, 2000).
Consistently, studies have shown that social isolation and loneliness are predictive of increased rates of psychological and physical illnesses (Berkman, 1995; Hawkley & Cacioppo, 2010). For example, a five-year longitudinal population-based investigation (Cacioppo, Hawkley, & Thisted, 2010) revealed that loneliness, or the subjective lack of social relationships, prospectively predicted increased depressive symptomatology. These findings held above and beyond other factors, including demographic characteristics, personality factors, and perceived stress. In an extreme example of the importance of social health, a lack of social relationships has been shown to predict increased mortality risk (Berkman & Syme, 1979; Holt-Lunstad, Smith, & Layton, 2010; House, Landis, & Umberson, 1988) to an extent that is on par with or even exceeds other powerful health risks, such as smoking (Holt-Lunstad et al., 2010; House, Landis, & Umberson, 1988).
In addition to the subjective experience of loneliness, the quality of one’s relationships is also important for social health (Brooks & Dunkel Schetter, 2011; Kiecolt-Glaser & Newton, 2001). While well functioning social relationships are protective for health outcomes, poorly functioning social relationships may damage health and well-being (Brooks & Dunkel Schetter, 2011). For example, in one study (Holt-Lunstad, Birmingham, & Jones, 2008), married individuals evidenced lower (i.e., healthier) ambulatory blood pressure compared to singles. However, this effect was moderated by marital quality such that those in higher quality marriages showed significantly lower blood pressure and those in lower quality marriages showed significantly higher blood pressure relative to singles. Similar moderating effects of relationship functioning have been reported for psychological health and well-being (e.g., Dush & Amato, 2005; Kiecolt-Glaser et al., 1987).
Over the past two decades, public health researchers have increasingly focused on another form of social health—social capital. Social capital refers to the resources to which one has access through social networks (Bourdieu, 1986). Although social capital is often posited as a higher-level social variable (e.g., neighborhood- or community-level), many researchers have investigated social capital as a resource available to the individual. According to one model (Grootaert & van Bastelaer, 2002), social capital may be divided along two axes, one ranging from structural to cognitive and the other ranging from macro to micro. Situated at the micro cognitive level (of primary interest in the current research) are constructs such as trust, local norms, and values (Grootaert & van Bastelaer, 2002). Social trust and perceptions of mutual aid are two operationalizations that have been frequently investigated in past work on micro cognitive social capital (e.g., Fujiwara et al., 2012). Importantly, the extent to which individuals perceive greater social capital has been shown to be protective for various health outcomes, including depression (Fujiwara & Kawachi, 2008), self-rated physical health (Hurtado, Kawachi, & Sudarsky, 2011), and cardiovascular and all-cause mortality (Hyyppä, Mäki, Impivaara, & Aromaa, 2007).
Evidence for Social Health Disparities by Sexual Orientation
Patterns of social health may differ somewhat consistently between members of dominant and devalued groups (Hatzenbuehler et al., 2013; Umberson & Montez, 2010). In general, past research has found that sexual minorities evidence poorer social health relative to heterosexuals (e.g., Andersson et al., 2006; Bos et al., 2008; Diamond & Lucas, 2004; Fokkema & Kuyper, 2009; Valanis et al., 2000). However, there are a number of limitations to what little research has been conducted on this topic. First, much of the work has been conducted in a few European countries (e.g., the Netherlands, Norway, Sweden) that have historically been relatively accepting of sexual minority individuals and their social relationships (Merin, 2010). Second, while some studies have been conducted with sexual minority youths and older adults, little research has been conducted with sexual minority adults (i.e., aged 18–55 years old). Finally, insufficient attention has been paid to differences in social health by sex and sexual orientation subgroup (e.g., gay/lesbian, bisexual).
Population-level data on divorce among sexual minorities are not currently available in the United States, but research in Norway and Sweden, where registered partnerships have been legal since the mid-1990s, shows that sexual minorities are at greater risk for divorce compared to heterosexuals (Andersson et al., 2006). Another study (Fokkema & Kuyper, 2009), conducted in the Netherlands, used self-report data from two large surveys of institutionalized and independently living older adults to explore disparities in social relationship outcomes by sexual orientation. Results showed that sexual minority older adults had fewer social relationships, including with romantic partners, children, and other family members, and participated less frequently in church activities compared to heterosexual older adults. Sexual minority older adults also reported significantly greater levels of loneliness. Among older adults in the United States, poorer social health has also been found among sexual minority compared to heterosexual women participating in the Women’s Health Initiative Study (Valanis et al., 2000).
A number of studies have also linked same-sex attraction in youths to impaired social health (Radkowsky & Siegel, 1997). A study in the Netherlands found that sexual minority youth reported poorer quality relationships with peers and their fathers relative to heterosexual youth (Bos et al., 2008). Sexual minority youth also report fewer peer relationships and greater fear for the loss of current friendships compared to heterosexuals (Diamond & Lucas, 2004). Other research shows that sexual minority youth spend less time with their best friends and report less closeness with their mothers (Williams, Connolly, Pepler, & Craig, 2005). In fact, population-based data from the United States suggest that sexual minority youth are more likely to experience strained relationships with their families of origin (e.g., Pearson & Wilkinson, 2013).
Self-Reported Discrimination as an Etiologic Variable
In addition to examining disparities in social health among adults in the United States based upon sexual orientation, it is important to investigate etiological factors that may produce these disparities. Laypersons and researchers alike have sometimes assumed that disparities in health outcomes between members of dominant and devalued groups are caused by innate or essential group characteristics (e.g., genetic predispositions) (Dar-Nimrod & Heine, 2011; Krieger, 2005). In the past few decades, however, theorists have challenged essentialist explanations, arguing that the stress of living in a society that devalues one’s social identity can lead to impaired health outcomes (Clark, Anderson, Clark, & Williams, 1999; Krieger, 1999; Meyer, 2003). Past work has confirmed that the stress of prejudice and discrimination is associated with impaired psychological and physical health (Paradies, 2006; Pascoe & Richman, 2009; Williams & Mohammed, 2009). Because stressors have also been shown to negatively affect social relationships (McCubbin & Patterson, 1983; Randall & Bodenmann, 2009), some scholars have proposed that prejudice and discrimination may lead to impaired social health (e.g., Doyle & Molix, 2014b; Trail, Goff, Bradbury, & Karney, 2012). However, no studies of which we are aware have tested whether self-reported discrimination explains disparities in social health between sexual minorities and heterosexuals.
Past work has linked self-reported discrimination to impaired romantic relationship functioning among sexual minorities (e.g., Doyle & Molix, 2014a; Kamen, Burns, & Beach, 2011; Otis, Rostosky, Riggle, & Hamrin, 2006). In a quantitative review of this literature (Doyle & Molix, 2015), it was revealed that across 35 studies there was evidence of a statistically significant inverse association between self-reported discrimination and romantic relationship functioning (r = −.12, 95% CI [−.16, −.08]). A few other studies have focused on associations between self-reported discrimination and other types of social relationships among sexual minorities. For sexual minority older adults in the Netherlands, it was found that self-reported discrimination was associated with greater levels of loneliness (Kuyper & Fokkema, 2010). Among sexual minority youth in the United States, self-reported discrimination was also shown to be predictive of impaired social support from family and friends (Mustanski, Newcomb, & Garofalo, 2011).
The Current Study
The first aim of the current study was to explore disparities in social health between sexual minority and heterosexual adults in the United States. Based upon limited previous research (e.g., Andersson et al., 2006; Bos et al., 2008; Fokkema & Kuyper, 2009; Valanis et al., 2000), it was hypothesized that sexual minority adults would evidence greater levels of loneliness, greater social relationship strain (with both friends and family), and lesser social capital relative to heterosexual adults. In the current research, differences in social health by sex and sexual orientation subgroup were also explored. A second aim of the current study was to examine whether disparities in social health between sexual minorities and heterosexuals would be attenuated when accounting for the effects of self-reported discrimination among sexual minorities.
METHOD
Participants
A total of 579 participants (365 heterosexual and 214 sexual minority) were recruited for the current study. Approximately two thirds of the sample identified as male (63.6%) and about one third identified as female (35.9%). The mean age of the sample was 29.92 years (SD = 9.17). The majority of participants identified as Caucasian/White (80.1%), but the sample also included individuals identified as Asian/Asian Indian (7.1%), African American/Black (6.0%), Hispanic/Latino (4.0%), multiracial (1.9%), Native American (.5%), and Middle Eastern/North African (.2%). The mean household income of the sample was $47,097 per year (SD = 47,531) and, on average, participants had completed some years of college education.
Procedure
Participants for the current study were all recruited via Amazon’s Mechanical Turk (MTurk), a popular crowdsourcing platform. Much past work in the social and behavioral sciences has successfully utilized MTurk to recruit participants for a variety of research studies (Buhrmester, Kwang, & Gosling, 2011; Goodman, Cryder, & Cheema, 2013; Shapiro, Chandler, & Mueller, 2013), including those focused on sexual minority populations (e.g., Zou, Andersen, & Blosnich, 2013). In order to conduct studies utilizing MTurk, researchers post tasks, referred to as human intelligence tasks (HITs) on the website. Anyone who is over 18 years of age and possesses a valid social security or individual tax identification number is then able to complete these tasks. However, for the current study, a restriction was also placed limiting participation to those residing within the United States.
The survey itself was hosted on another website, Qualtrics, which was linked to the MTurk HIT. All participants were provided $.50 as compensation for their time and effort. This amount was determined after evaluating a review of response rates and completion times for surveys of various lengths (Buhrmester et al., 2011). All workers residing in the United States were eligible to participate in the current study regardless of other demographic characteristics, such as age, gender or race. However, prior to participating in the study, everyone was required to respond to a sexual orientation item (described in the following section). Based upon these responses, participants were either classified as heterosexual or sexual minority. The survey instrument was programmed such that each separate group had a fixed quota set at 365 participants. Once this quota was reached for either group, further individuals selecting that group membership were told that the study was full and thanked for their interest. Settings were also programmed to avoid “ballot-stuffing” (as recommended by Mason & Suri, 2012), meaning that Qualtrics tracked IP addresses and disallowed returning individuals who had previously responded to the first item from starting the survey over. This prevented potential participants from changing their sexual orientation identification in order to get around the fixed quotas. All other components of the survey instrument were identical between groups.
Measures
All measures for the current study were chosen with consideration of limitations due to the survey platform. Specifically, MTurk is oriented toward brief tasks that can be completed easily and consecutively in a short period of time in order to accrue multiple small payments. Therefore, it is optimal to select brief and relatively straightforward measures (Goodman et al., 2013). Where possible, previously utilized and validated abbreviated scales were selected for the current study.
Sexual orientation
Based upon previous research recommending a five-category self-identification measure of sexual orientation (Vrangalova & Savin-Williams, 2012), participants were asked to select from the following points on a 5-point Kinsey-type scale: heterosexual, mostly heterosexual, bisexual, mostly gay/lesbian, gay/lesbian. For the purposes of the survey quotas, as described previously, participants selecting heterosexual were grouped as heterosexual while participants selecting all other labels were grouped as sexual minority. Sexual orientation was also assessed near the end of the survey via a free-response item asking participants, “What term best describes your sexual orientation?”
Self-reported discrimination
Items from the Everyday Discrimination Scale (Williams, Yu, Jackson, & Anderson, 1997) were chosen and adapted to address discrimination based upon sexual identity. A few of the nine items on this scale, initially designed for use with racial minorities, do not apply particularly well to sexual minorities (e.g., “People act as if they are afraid of you”). Therefore, two items with the best face validity among this population were chosen for the current study. These items were, “You are called names or insulted because of your sexual orientation” and “You are threatened or harassed because of your sexual orientation.” Participants indicated how often they experienced both of these events on a scale ranging from 1 (never) to 6 (almost everyday). These two items were highly correlated among sexual minorities, r(212) = .83, p < .001, and thus average scores were utilized to assess self-reported discrimination.
Loneliness
The three-item loneliness scale is an abbreviated version of the Revised UCLA Loneliness Scale (Russell, Peplau, & Cutrona, 1980) that has been validated in past research (Hughes, Waite, Hawkley, & Cacioppo, 2004). Participants responded to items such as, “How often do you feel isolated from others?” on a scale from 1 (hardly ever) to 3 (often). Mean scores were then calculated for the overall scale. In past work, the three-item version has been shown to have adequate internal consistency, α = .72, and correlates highly with the longer Revised UCLA Loneliness Scale, r = .82 (Hughes et al., 2004). This scale also evidenced good internal consistency in the current study, α = .86.
Friendship and familial strain
Parallel measures of strain with friends and family (Walen & Lachman, 2000) were included to assess social relationship functioning. These scales are composed of four statements, such as “How often do they get on your nerves?” with participants rating their level of agreement to each statement on a scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 4 (a lot). Mean scores across all four items were taken to assess relationship strain with friends and family, separately. Internal consistency has been shown to be adequate when considering friends, α = .79, and family, α = .80, in past research (Walen & Lachman 2000), and similarly in the current study, α = .79 and α = .81, respectively.
Social capital
In line with past research (Fujiwara et al., 2012), the cognitive domain of social capital was assessed via two items addressing social trust, “In general, would you say that your neighbors can be trusted?” and mutual aid, “Do you think that people in your neighborhood aid each other?” Responses were on a scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 4 (a lot). These two items were highly correlated in the current study, r(571) = .66, p < .001, and were therefore combined via mean score on both items to indicate social capital. Social capital is thus operationally defined here as social trust and mutual aid in one’s neighborhood.
Analyses
Missing data were first considered in the current study. Overall, very few participants had missing data on any of the measures, with the most on the measure of self-reported discrimination (n = 10). In order to assess whether missing data affected the pattern of results, missing values were estimated via multiple imputations in SPSS software (Version 21). Analyses including imputed values did not differ meaningfully from those including only the original data; therefore, we present results from the original analyses in the following sections. In order to explore the primary hypothesis of the current study, that sexual minorities would experience impaired social health relative to heterosexuals, multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) was conducted. In this analysis, sexual orientation was entered as the independent variable and loneliness, friendship strain, familial strain, and social capital were entered as the dependent variables with follow-up ANOVAs conducted on each indicator separately. Complementary analyses of differences in social health by sexual orientation subgroup and sex were conducted via ANOVAs with post-hocs employing Fisher’s least significant difference (LSD) test to control for family-wise error rates. To address the second hypothesis regarding the etiologic role of self-reported discrimination, sexual minorities were divided into two groups—those relatively higher in self-reported discrimination and those relatively lower in self-reported discrimination (via mean split on self-reported discrimination). ANOVAs with post-hocs employing Fisher’s LSD test were then conducted to compare differences in social health between heterosexuals and sexual minorities relatively higher and lower in self-reported discrimination.
RESULTS
Sexual Orientation and Participant Grouping
Table 1 shows the frequencies of each self-identification label, coded into six separate categories, by selections on the initial sexual orientation item. Participants who selected heterosexual on the initial item were overwhelmingly likely to self-identify as heterosexual. The majority of those selecting mostly heterosexual on the initial item also identified as heterosexual, but this group included a significant number of participants self-identifying with a variety of other sexual minority labels, including unsure, questioning, and bi-curious. This finding was consistent with past work demonstrating that individuals who select mostly heterosexual represent a unique sexual minority group (Vrangalova & Savin-Williams, 2012). Participants who selected bisexual on the initial item predominantly self-identified as bisexual, but a fair number also used labels such as queer, fluid or pansexual. Mirroring the mostly heterosexual group, the majority of those who selected mostly gay/lesbian on the initial item also self-identified as gay or lesbian, but included a significant number who chose other sexual minority labels. All participants who selected gay/lesbian on the initial item also self-identified as gay or lesbian. Chi-square analyses confirmed non-independence, χ2(20) = 1020.27, p < .001, that is, self-identification labels were highly dependent upon responses to the initial sexual orientation item.
Table 1.
Completely straight/heterosexual | Unsure, questioning | Bisexual, bi-curious | Queer, fluid, pansexual | Completely homosexual/gay/lesbian | Asexual | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Heterosexual (n = 365, 63%) | 345 (94.5%) | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 3 (.8%) |
Mostly heterosexual (n = 107, 18.5%) | 65 (60.7%) | 20 (18.7%) | 12 (11.2%) | 2 (1.9%) | 0 | 3 (2.8%) |
Bisexual (n = 51, 8.8%) | 1 (2.0%) | 0 | 39 (76.5%) | 10 (19.6%) | 0 | 1 (2.0%) |
Mostly gay/lesbian (n = 16, 2.8%) | 0 | 0 | 2 (12.5%) | 3 (18.8%) | 10 (62.5%) | 0 |
Gay/lesbian (n = 40, 6.9%) | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 37 (92.5%) | 0 |
Note. N = 579. This table displays cross-tabulated values for participants responses to the initial sexual orientation item and the sexual orientation self-identification item calculated by row. Counts are displayed for each cell outside of parentheses and percentages are displayed inside of parentheses. Frequencies do not necessarily total to 100% because some participants did not provide a response to the self-identification item (n = 26, 4.5%).
Participants were next divided into two groups based upon sexual orientation: heterosexual (n = 365, 63%) and sexual minority (n = 214, 37%). As described in the measures section, participants who selected heterosexual on the initial sexual orientation item were categorized as heterosexual while participants who selected any other response were categorized as sexual minority. These two categories were utilized in regression analyses related to the primary hypotheses of the study (similar results were observed when substituting self-identification as the criterion for determining heterosexual and sexual minority group categorization, with a high correlation between sexual orientation coded from the initial item and coded from self-identification responses, r(553) = .74, p < .001). Demographic data presented for each group separately is shown in Table 2. Sexual orientation was significantly associated with both sex and age, such that sexual minorities tended to be older and were more likely to be female. All results presented were robust to adjustment for these demographic covariates.
Table 2.
Heterosexual (n = 365) | Sexual minority (n = 214) | t/χ2 | df | p | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Age | 31.13 | 27.86 | 4.20 | 577 | < .001 |
Income | $47,867 | $45,778 | .51 | 576 | .61 |
Sex | 22.63 | 1 | < .001 | ||
Male | 259 | 109 | |||
Female | 105 | 103 | |||
Race | .37 | 1 | .59 | ||
White | 295 | 169 | |||
Racial Minority | 69 | 45 |
Disparities in Social Health by Sexual Orientation
Results from MANOVA revealed that, overall, sexual orientation had a significant multivariate association with social health, Wilk’s λ = .94, F(4, 566) = 8.45, p < .001, η2 = .06. Follow-up ANOVAs revealed significant associations between sexual orientation and each of the indicators, with the same general pattern of poorer social health among sexual minorities relative to heterosexuals across variables. Specifically, compared to heterosexuals, sexual minorities reported greater loneliness (M = 1.82, SD = .04 vs. M = 1.60, SD = .03), F(1, 569) = 17.54, p < .001, η2 = .03), greater friendship strain (M = 1.85, SD = .04 vs. M = 1.67, SD = .03), F(1, 569) = 13.16, p = .001, η2 = .02), greater familial strain (M = 2.20, SD = .05 vs. M = 1.97, SD = .04), F(1, 569) = 13.64, p = .001, η2 = .02), and lesser social capital (M = 2.21, SD = .05 vs. M = 2.41, SD = .04), F(1, 569) = 8.46, p = .004, η2 = .02).
Analyses of Sexual Orientation Subgroups by Sex
Results from analyses of sexual orientation subgroup by sex are shown in Table 3. Overall, these analyses replicated the main findings, with heterosexuals evidencing the greatest social health across each of the four measures. However, there were also some consistent patterns of differences between subgroups. Generally, those falling in the middle of the sexual orientation spectrum (i.e., mostly heterosexual, bisexual, mostly gay/lesbian) evidenced the poorest social health. Gays and lesbians, although scoring somewhat lower across indicators of social health relative to heterosexuals, did not differ from this group with statistical significance. The pattern of social health between sexual orientation subgroups was generally consistent across sexes with a few exceptions (e.g., mostly lesbian women reported the greatest loneliness while mostly gay men reported the least social capital).
Table 3.
Loneliness | Friendship strain | Familial strain | Social capital | |||||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
| ||||||||||||||||
Male | Female | Male | Female | Male | Female | Male | Female | |||||||||
| ||||||||||||||||
M | SD | M | SD | M | SD | M | SD | M | SD | M | SD | M | SD | M | SD | |
Heterosexual (n = 365) | 1.63a | .04 | 1.51a | .06 | 1.73a,b | .04 | 1.54a | .05 | 1.95a | .04 | 2.01a | .07 | 2.37a,b | .05 | 2.50a | .08 |
Mostly heterosexual (n = 107) | 2.05d | .09 | 1.76b | .08 | 1.90a,b,c | .08 | 1.87b | .07 | 2.10a | .10 | 2.26b | .10 | 2.16a,b | .11 | 2.25a | .11 |
Bisexual (n = 51) | 1.70a,b,c | .13 | 1.76b | .11 | 1.63a | .12 | 1.81b | .10 | 2.22a,b | .15 | 2.13a,b | .14 | 2.11a,b | .16 | 2.23a | .15 |
Mostly gay/lesbian (n = 16) | 2.03b,c,d | .20 | 2.33c | .24 | 2.23c | .18 | 1.92a,b | .22 | 2.63b | .22 | 2.58a,b | .29 | 1.90b | .24 | 2.50a | .33 |
Gay/lesbian (n = 40) | 1.61a,b | .13 | 1.56a,b | .14 | 1.94a,b,c | .12 | 1.63a,b | .12 | 2.05a | .15 | 2.17a,b | .17 | 2.50a | .16 | 2.14a | .19 |
Note. Post-hocs with Fisher’s least significant difference (LSD) test were conducted to compare group means. Differing subscripts within columns represent significant differences between sexual orientation subgroups by sex (p < .05). Sample sizes by sexual orientation subgroup for men are heterosexual (n = 257), mostly heterosexual (n = 53), bisexual (n = 22), mostly gay (n = 10), gay (n = 22). Sample sizes by sexual orientation subgroup for women are heterosexual (n = 101), mostly heterosexual (n = 51), bisexual (n = 28), mostly lesbian (n = 6), lesbian (n = 18).
Self-Reported Discrimination as an Etiologic Factor
In line with past work examining sexism as an etiological factor in mental health disparities between men and women (Klonoff, Landrine, & Campbell, 2000), we stratified on self-reported discrimination among sexual minorities to explore its role in producing observed disparities. Sexual minorities reporting higher levels compared to the mean (M = 1.60, SD = .05) were categorized as relatively higher in self-reported discrimination (n = 70) while those reporting lower levels compared to the mean were categorized as relatively lower in self-reported discrimination (n = 141). Results from analyses comparing these three groups (heterosexuals, sexual minorities lower in discrimination, sexual minorities higher in discrimination) are shown in Table 4. Consistent with hypotheses of discrimination as an etiologic factor, sexual minorities lower in discrimination did not significantly differ from heterosexuals in friendship strain, familial strain, or social capital, while sexual minorities higher in discrimination differed from heterosexuals on all three of these indicators. Conversely, there was not evidence of an etiologic role for discrimination in loneliness—both groups of sexual minorities evidenced elevated rates relative to heterosexuals that were also not significantly different from one another.
Table 4.
Heterosexual (n = 359) | Sexual minority: lower discrimination (n = 141) | Sexual minority: higher discrimination (n = 70) | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
| ||||||
M | SD | M | SD | M | SD | |
Loneliness | 1.60a | .03 | 1.82b | .05 | 1.82b | .07 |
Friendship strain | 1.67a | .03 | 1.80a,b | .05 | 1.96b | .07 |
Familial strain | 1.97a | .04 | 2.10a | .06 | 2.39b | .08 |
Social capital | 2.41a | .04 | 2.27a,b | .07 | 2.10b | .09 |
Note. Sexual minority participants were categorized as relatively lower or higher in discrimination based upon a mean split on self-reported discrimination (M = 1.60, SD = .05). Post-hocs with Fisher’s least significant difference (LSD) test were conducted to compare group means. Differing subscripts within rows represent significant differences between sexual orientations (p < .05).
DISCUSSION
Confirming and extending past work (e.g., Andersson et al., 2006; Bos et al., 2008; Fokkema & Kuyper, 2009; Valanis et al., 2000), evidence was found for disparities in social health between sexual minority and heterosexual adults in the United States, such that sexual minorities reported significantly more loneliness, friendship strain and familial strain, and less social capital on average. The consistency of this pattern across indicators of social health suggests that sexual minorities do, in fact, face unique obstacles to forming and maintaining strong, healthy social relationships. Interestingly, while analyses of sexual orientation subgroups revealed the same overall pattern between heterosexuals and sexual minorities, there were some notable differences between sexual minority subgroups. In general, those falling in the middle of the sexual orientation spectrum (i.e., mostly heterosexual, bisexual, mostly gay/lesbian) tended to report the poorest social health (with few appreciable differences by sex). This is important because these groups are sometimes overlooked in research on sexual minority health, but they may evidence the largest disparities (Dodge & Sandfort, 2007). One possible reason is that members of these groups may experience some degree of marginalization from gays and lesbians as well as heterosexuals (Weiss, 2004), making it more difficult to find supportive communities and social identification, which can be protective in the face of prejudice and discrimination (Doyle & Molix, 2014c).
Another finding to emerge from the current research provides insight into an important etiologic factor linking sexual orientation to impaired social health—self-reported discrimination. Indeed, as hypothesized, self-reported discrimination accounted for disparities in friendship strain, familial strain, and social capital. Sexual minorities reporting relatively lower levels of discrimination did not significantly differ from heterosexuals on these three indicators of social health while sexual minorities reporting relatively greater levels of discrimination did. Only loneliness continued to be elevated among all sexual minorities, irrespective of self-reported discrimination. To our knowledge, this is the first work to examine whether disparities in social health between sexual minorities and heterosexuals may be accounted for by prejudice and discrimination.
Limitations and Future Directions
It is important to consider the composition of the sample in this study, as it is both a limitation and strength of the current work. Internet-based sampling is a valid and practical epidemiologic method that has been recommended for use (with appropriate considerations), especially with commonly overlooked research populations, such as sexual minorities (Meyer & Wilson, 2009). While MTurk workers are certainly not fully representative of the population of the United States as a whole (see Paolacci & Chandler, 2014, for a description of average MTurk worker demographics), it is an informative starting point for tackling these research questions. Furthermore, it has been argued that not only are fully representative samples often unnecessary in epidemiologic research, but they may even be undesirable in some instances, such as when examining etiologic factors (cf. Rothman, Gallacher, & Hatch, 2013). More important to the current research than representativeness is comparability between groups.
It can be difficult to locate legitimate control groups of heterosexuals when studying sexual minority populations, and internet-based sampling provides a useful tool for solving this problem (Rothblum, 2007). Although in the current study heterosexuals and sexual minorities were found to differ across certain demographic characteristics, these differences may be more attributable to true sexual orientation differences than sampling biases (i.e., younger people and women are relatively more likely to identify as sexual minorities compared to older people and men). The heterosexual sample in the current study represents a valid comparison group for the sexual minority sample given there is no reason to suspect relevant systematic differences in the propensity to participate in MTurk based upon sexual orientation. However, it is worth noting that the MTurk workforce overall tends to skew younger and more educated than the general population (Paolacci & Chandler, 2014). This may be one reason for the relatively low rates of self-reported discrimination in the current sample, as attitudes toward sexual minorities tend to be improving at a steady rate in the United States. If this were the case, one would expect to see even more exaggerated social health disparities in the general population, as we showed evidence for discrimination as a critical etiologic factor.
Another strength of the current study is that the recruitment materials made no mention of the fact that the study concerned sexual orientation; therefore, selection issues related to group identification (i.e., recruiting only those who are highly identified with their sexual minority identity) were greatly attenuated. For example, in response to the self-identification item, one participant responded that he identified as “closeted gay.” Individuals who are not out regarding their sexual orientation are often excluded from research on sexual minority issues (whether intentionally or unintentionally), but the current design may have enabled such individuals to participate.
While the current study recruited a relatively large sample of heterosexual and sexual minority participants, it should be noted that some cell sizes were limited when stratifying by sex and sexual orientation subgroup (samples sizes for these cells are available in Table 3). Because the focus of this research was primarily on social health disparities between heterosexuals and sexual minorities, statistical power was not compromised for the majority of the analyses presented. However, sexual orientation subgroup analyses should be interpreted with caution as some of these point estimates included wide confidence intervals (e.g., for those identifying as mostly gay and mostly lesbian). Future research aimed at uncovering potential disparities by sex and sexual orientation subgroup should endeavor to recruit even larger samples with adequate representation across subgroups. Relatedly, in order to examine even more fine-grained distinctions (including intersections of sex, sexual orientation, race and other relevant social identities), population-based data including variables related to social health will be required, and population-based surveys should include nuanced assessments of sexual orientation and gender identity (for recommendations, see Institute of Medicine, 2011).
Conclusion
Past research has shown that sexual minorities are burdened with higher rates of psychological and physical health problems, including but not limited to depression and anxiety (Cochran & Mays, 2009; King et al., 2008; Meyer, 2003) as well as cardiovascular disease and cancer (Dibble, Roberts, & Nussey, 2004; Lick, Durso, & Johnson, 2013; Wang, Häusermann, Vounatsou, Aggleton, & Weiss, 2007). The remediation of these burdens is slowly becoming a priority among some researchers and practitioners within the United States (HHS, 2014; IOM, 2011). However, as of yet, these health disparities have proven to be relatively intractable; it may be that current policies and interventions are too narrow in scope (Hong, Espelage, & Kral, 2011). The current work should be of particular interest to those concerned with the remediation of health disparities as impaired social relationships are a known risk factor for many of the most serious diseases inequitably burdening sexual minority men and women today (Berkman, 1995; Hawkley & Cacioppo, 2010). In neglecting the importance of social relationships for sexual minority health, researchers and policy makers may be overlooking one of the most critical avenues for intervention.
Although public policy and attitude change are the ultimate goals in eradicating prejudice and discrimination, something must be done in the short-term to help empower and build resilience among sexual minorities who are struggling with prejudice and discrimination on a daily basis. According to Kwon’s (2013) review of resilience factors, three important predictors of resilience among sexual minorities are social support, emotional openness, and optimism. Kwon speculated that these resilience factors reduce reactivity to prejudice and discrimination, thus protecting sexual minorities from negative health outcomes. Clinicians and practitioners working with sexual minority individuals and couples should focus on building these resilience factors with their clients. Clinicians and practitioners can also help sexual minority individuals and couples recognize the influence that minority stressors have on their social relationships, with awareness potentially disrupting negative effects. Interventions targeted toward reducing loneliness may also be effective among sexual minorities, as they have been shown to be among heterosexuals (Masi, Chen, Hawkley, & Cacioppo, 2011). However, primary prevention is also critical (Matthews & Adams, 2009); friends and allies of sexual minority individuals can play their part in supporting positive outcomes before the negative effects of minority stress lead sexual minorities to seek professional help.
Researchers have been remiss in neglecting to recognize the influence of social health disparities on other intractable health disparities inequitably affecting sexual minorities. The present work reveals consistent evidence of a divide in social health between sexual minority and heterosexual adults in the United States, as well as the etiologic role of self-reported discrimination. By its very nature, the term “social stigma” is interpersonal, but the majority of past research on social stigma has focused on intrapersonal outcomes (Major & Sawyer, 2009). A paradigmatic shift in the way researchers think about and conceptualize stigma will be necessary to advance study on the intersection of stigma and social relationships.
Acknowledgments
David Matthew Doyle is now a post-doctoral fellow in the Department of Epidemiology, Columbia University, supported by training grant T32MH13043 from the National Institute of Mental Health. This research constituted a portion of David Matthew Doyle’s dissertation under the direction of Lisa Molix.
References
- Adler NE, Rehkopf DH. US disparities in health: Descriptions, causes, and mechanisms. Annual Review of Public Health. 2008;29:235–252. doi: 10.1146/annurev.publhealth.29.020907.090852. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andersson G, Noack T, Seierstad A, Weedon-Fekjær H. The demographics of same-sex marriages in Norway and Sweden. Demography. 2006;43:79–98. doi: 10.1353/dem.2006.0001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baumeister RF, Leary MR. The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin. 1995;117:497–529. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berkman LF. The role of social relations in health promotion. Psychosomatic Medicine. 1995;57:245–254. doi: 10.1097/00006842-199505000-00006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berkman LF, Glass T, Brissette I, Seeman TE. From social integration to health: Durkheim in the new millennium. Social Science & Medicine. 2000;51:843–857. doi: 10.1016/s0277-9536(00)00065-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berkman LF, Syme SL. Social networks, host resistance, and mortality: A nine-year follow-up study of Alameda County residents. American Journal of Epidemiology. 1979;109:186–204. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a112674. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Berscheid E. The greening of relationship science. American Psychologist. 1999;54:260–266. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.54.4.260. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bos HM, Sandfort TG, de Bruyn EH, Hakvoort EM. Same-sex attraction, social relationships, psychosocial functioning, and school performance in early adolescence. Developmental Psychology. 2008;44:59–68. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.44.1.59. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bourdieu P. The forms of capital. In: Richardson J, editor. Handbook of theory and research for the sociology of education. New York: Greenwood Press; 1986. pp. 241–258. [Google Scholar]
- Bowlby J. Attachment and loss. New York: Basic Books; 1969. [Google Scholar]
- Brewer MB, Caporael LR. Selfish genes versus selfish people: Sociobiology as origin myth. Motivation and Emotion. 1990;14:237–243. [Google Scholar]
- Brooks KP, Dunkel Schetter C. Social negativity and health: Conceptual and measurement issues. Social and Personality Psychology Compass. 2011;5:904–918. [Google Scholar]
- Buhrmester M, Kwang T, Gosling SD. Amazon’s Mechanical Turk: A new source of inexpensive, yet high-quality, data? Perspectives on Psychological Science. 2011;6:3–5. doi: 10.1177/1745691610393980. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Buss DM, Kenrick DT. Evolutionary social psychology. In: Gilbert DT, Fiske ST, Lindzey G, editors. The handbook of social psychology. New York: McGraw-Hill; 1998. pp. 982–1026. [Google Scholar]
- Cacioppo JT, Hawkley LC, Thisted RA. Perceived social isolation makes me sad: 5-year cross-lagged analyses of loneliness and depressive symptomatology in the Chicago Health, Aging, and Social Relations Study. Psychology and Aging. 2010;25:453–463. doi: 10.1037/a0017216. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clark R, Anderson NB, Clark VR, Williams DR. Racism as a stressor for African Americans: A biopsychosocial model. American Psychologist. 1999;54:805–816. doi: 10.1037//0003-066x.54.10.805. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cochran SD, Mays VM. Burden of psychiatric morbidity among lesbian, gay, and bisexual individuals in the California Quality of Life Survey. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2009;118:647–658. doi: 10.1037/a0016501. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dar-Nimrod I, Heine SJ. Genetic essentialism: On the deceptive determinism of DNA. Psychological Bulletin. 2011;137:800–818. doi: 10.1037/a0021860. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Diamond L, Lucas S. Sexual-minority and heterosexual youths’ peer relationships: Experiences, expectations, and implications for well-being. Journal of Research on Adolescence. 2004;14:313–340. [Google Scholar]
- Dibble SL, Roberts SA, Nussey B. Comparing breast cancer risk between lesbians and their heterosexual sisters. Women’s Health Issues. 2004;14:60–68. doi: 10.1016/j.whi.2004.03.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dodge B, Sandfort TG. A review of mental health research on bisexual individuals when compared to homosexual and heterosexual individuals. In: Firestein B, editor. Becoming visible: Counseling bisexuals across the lifespan. New York: Columbia University Press; 2007. pp. 28–51. [Google Scholar]
- Donald CA, Ware JE, Brook RH, Davies-Avery A. Conceptualization and measurement of health for adults in the health insurance study: Vol. IV, Social health. Santa Monica, CA: Rand Corporation; 1978. [Google Scholar]
- Doyle DM, Molix L. How does stigma spoil relationships? Evidence that perceived discrimination harms romantic relationship quality through impaired self-image. Journal of Applied Social Psychology. 2014a;44:600–610. doi: 10.1111/jasp.12252. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Doyle DM, Molix L. Love on the margins: The effects of social stigma and relationship length on romantic relationship quality. Social Psychological and Personality Science. 2014b;5:102–110. doi: 10.1177/1948550613486677. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Doyle DM, Molix L. Perceived discrimination as a stressor for close relationships: Identifying psychological and physiological pathways. Journal of Behavioral Medicine. 2014c;37:1134–1144. doi: 10.1007/s10865-014-9563-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Doyle DM, Molix L. Social stigma and sexual minorities’ romantic relationship functioning: A meta-analytic review. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 2015 doi: 10.1177/0146167215594592. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Durkheim E. Suicide: A study in sociology. Glencoe, IL: Free Press; 1951. [Google Scholar]
- Dush CMK, Amato PR. Consequences of relationship status and quality for subjective well-being. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships. 2005;22:607–627. [Google Scholar]
- Fokkema T, Kuyper L. The relation between social embeddedness and loneliness among older lesbian, gay, and bisexual adults in the Netherlands. Archives of Sexual Behavior. 2009;38:264–275. doi: 10.1007/s10508-007-9252-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fujiwara T, Kawachi I. A prospective study of individual-level social capital and major depression in the United States. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health. 2008;62:627–633. doi: 10.1136/jech.2007.064261. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fujiwara T, Kubzansky LD, Matsumoto K, Kawachi I. The association between oxytocin and social capital. PloS One. 2012;7:e52018. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0052018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goodman JK, Cryder CE, Cheema A. Data collection in a flat world: The strengths and weaknesses of Mechanical Turk samples. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making. 2013;26:213–224. [Google Scholar]
- Grootaert C, van Bastelaer T. Understanding and measuring social capital: A multidisciplinary tool for practitioners. Washington, DC: World Bank Publications; 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Hatzenbuehler ML, Phelan JC, Link BG. Stigma as a fundamental cause of population health inequalities. American Journal of Public Health. 2013;103:813–821. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2012.301069. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hawkley LC, Cacioppo JT. Loneliness matters: A theoretical and empirical review of consequences and mechanisms. Annals of Behavioral Medicine. 2010;40:218–227. doi: 10.1007/s12160-010-9210-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holt-Lunstad J, Birmingham W, Jones BQ. Is there something unique about marriage? The relative impact of marital status, relationship quality, and network social support on ambulatory blood pressure and mental health. Annals of Behavioral Medicine. 2008;35:239–244. doi: 10.1007/s12160-008-9018-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Holt-Lunstad J, Smith TB, Layton JB. Social relationships and mortality risk: A meta-analytic review. PLoS Medicine. 2010;7:1–20. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1000316. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hong JS, Espelage DL, Kral MJ. Understanding suicide among sexual minority youth in America: An ecological systems analysis. Journal of Adolescence. 2011;34:885–894. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2011.01.002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- House JS, Landis KR, Umberson D. Social relationships and health. Science. 1988;241:540–545. doi: 10.1126/science.3399889. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hughes ME, Waite LJ, Hawkley LC, Cacioppo JT. A short scale for measuring loneliness in large surveys: Results from two population-based studies. Research on Aging. 2004;26:655–672. doi: 10.1177/0164027504268574. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hurtado D, Kawachi I, Sudarsky J. Social capital and self-rated health in Colombia: The good, the bad and the ugly. Social Science and Medicine. 2011;72:584–590. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2010.11.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hyyppä MT, Mäki J, Impivaara O, Aromaa A. Individual-level measures of social capital as predictors of all-cause and cardiovascular mortality: A population-based prospective study of men and women in Finland. European Journal of Epidemiology. 2007;22:589–597. doi: 10.1007/s10654-007-9153-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Institute of Medicine. The health of lesbian, gay, bisexual, and transgender people: Building a foundation for better understanding. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press; 2011. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kamen C, Burns M, Beach SRH. Minority stress in same-sex male relationships: When does it impact relationship satisfaction? Journal of Homosexuality. 2011;58:1372–1390. doi: 10.1080/00918369.2011.614904. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kawachi I, Berkman LF. Social ties and mental health. Journal of Urban Health. 2001;78:458–467. doi: 10.1093/jurban/78.3.458. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kiecolt-Glaser JK, Fisher LD, Ogrocki P, Stout JC, Speicher CE, Glaser R. Marital quality, marital disruption, and immune function. Psychosomatic Medicine. 1987;49:13–34. doi: 10.1097/00006842-198701000-00002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kiecolt-Glaser JK, Newton TL. Marriage and health: His and hers. Psychological Bulletin. 2001;127:472–503. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.127.4.472. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- King M, Semlyen J, Tai SS, Killaspy H, Osborn D, Popelyuk D, Nazareth I. A systematic review of mental disorder, suicide, and deliberate self harm in lesbian, gay and bisexual people. BMC Psychiatry. 2008;8:70. doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-8-70. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Klonoff EA, Landrine H, Campbell R. Sexist discrimination may account for well-known gender differences in psychiatric symptoms. Psychology of Women Quarterly. 2000;24:93–99. [Google Scholar]
- Krieger N. Embodying inequality: A review of concepts, measures, and methods for studying health consequences of discrimination. International Journal of Health Services. 1999;29:295–352. doi: 10.2190/M11W-VWXE-KQM9-G97Q. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krieger N. Stormy weather: Race, gene expression, and the science of health disparities. American Journal of Public Health. 2005;95:2155–2160. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2005.067108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuyper L, Fokkema T. Loneliness among older lesbian, gay, and bisexual adults: The role of minority stress. Archives of Sexual Behavior. 2010;39:1171–1180. doi: 10.1007/s10508-009-9513-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kwon P. Resilience in lesbian, gay, and bisexual individuals. Personality and Social Psychology Review. 2013;17:371–383. doi: 10.1177/1088868313490248. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lick DJ, Durso LE, Johnson KL. Minority stress and physical health among sexual minorities. Perspectives on Psychological Science. 2013;8:521–548. doi: 10.1177/1745691613497965. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lin N, Ye X, Ensel WM. Social support and depressed mood: A structural analysis. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 1999;40:344–359. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Major B, Sawyer PJ. Attributions to discrimination: Antecedents and consequences. In: Nelson TD, editor. Handbook of prejudice, stereotyping, and discrimination. New York: Taylor & Francis Group; 2009. pp. 89–110. [Google Scholar]
- Masi CM, Chen HY, Hawkley LC, Cacioppo JT. A meta-analysis of interventions to reduce loneliness. Personality and Social Psychology Review. 2011;15:219–266. doi: 10.1177/1088868310377394. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mason W, Suri S. Conducting behavioral research on Amazon’s Mechanical Turk. Behavior Research Methods. 2012;44:1–23. doi: 10.3758/s13428-011-0124-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Matthews CR, Adams EM. Using a social justice approach to prevent the mental health consequences of heterosexism. Journal of Primary Prevention. 2009;30:11–26. doi: 10.1007/s10935-008-0166-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- McCubbin HI, Patterson JM. A family stress process: The double ABCX model of adjustment and adaptation. Marriage & Family Review. 1983;6:7–37. [Google Scholar]
- McGuire TG, Miranda J. New evidence regarding racial and ethnic disparities in mental health: Policy implications. Health Affairs. 2008;27:393–403. doi: 10.1377/hlthaff.27.2.393. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Merin Y. Equality for same-sex couples: The legal recognition of gay partnerships in Europe and the United States. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 2010. [Google Scholar]
- Meyer IH. Prejudice, social stress, and mental health in lesbian, gay, and bisexual populations: Conceptual issues and research evidence. Psychological Bulletin. 2003;129:674–697. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.129.5.674. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Meyer IH, Wilson PA. Sampling lesbian, gay, and bisexual populations. Journal of Counseling Psychology. 2009;56:23–31. [Google Scholar]
- Mustanski B, Newcomb ME, Garofalo R. Mental health of lesbian, gay, and bisexual youths: A developmental resiliency perspective. Journal of Gay & Lesbian Social Services. 2011;23:204–225. doi: 10.1080/10538720.2011.561474. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Otis MD, Rostosky SS, Riggle EDB, Hamrin R. Stress and relationship quality in same-sex couples. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships. 2006;23:81–99. [Google Scholar]
- Paolacci G, Chandler J. Inside the Turk: Understanding Mechanical Turk as a participant pool. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2014;23:184–188. [Google Scholar]
- Paolacci G, Chandler J, Ipeirotis PG. Running experiments on Amazon Mechanical Turk. Judgment and Decision Making. 2010;5:411–419. [Google Scholar]
- Paradies Y. A systematic review of empirical research on self-reported racism and health. International Journal of Epidemiology. 2006;35:888–901. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyl056. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pascoe EA, Richman LS. Perceived discrimination and health: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin. 2009;135:531–554. doi: 10.1037/a0016059. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pearson J, Wilkinson L. Family relationships and adolescent well-being: Are families equally protective for same-sex attracted youth? Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 2013;42:376–393. doi: 10.1007/s10964-012-9865-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Radkowsky M, Siegel LJ. The gay adolescent: Stressors, adaptations, and psychosocial interventions. Clinical Psychology Review. 1997;17:191–216. doi: 10.1016/s0272-7358(97)00007-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Randall AK, Bodenmann G. The role of stress on close relationships and marital satisfaction. Clinical Psychology Review. 2009;29:105–115. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2008.10.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reis HT, Collins WA, Berscheid E. The relationship context of human behavior and development. Psychological Bulletin. 2000;126:844–872. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.126.6.844. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Robles TF, Slatcher RB, Trombello JM, McGinn MM. Marital quality and health: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin. 2014;140:140–187. doi: 10.1037/a0031859. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rothblum E. From science fiction to computer-generated technology: Sampling lesbian, gay, and bisexual individuals. In: Meyer IH, Northridge ME, editors. The health of sexual minorities: Public health perspectives on lesbian, gay, bisexual and transgender populations. New York: Springer; 2007. pp. 441–454. [Google Scholar]
- Rothman KJ, Gallacher JE, Hatch EE. Why representativeness should be avoided. International Journal of Epidemiology. 2013;42:1012–1014. doi: 10.1093/ije/dys223. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Russell D, Peplau LA, Cutrona CE. The revised UCLA Loneliness Scale: Concurrent and discriminant validity evidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 1980;39:472–480. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.39.3.472. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ryff CD, Singer B. Interpersonal flourishing: A positive health agenda for the new millennium. Personality and Social Psychology Review. 2000;4:30–44. [Google Scholar]
- Shapiro DN, Chandler J, Mueller PA. Using Mechanical Turk to study clinical populations. Clinical Psychological Science. 2013;1:213–220. [Google Scholar]
- Trail TE, Goff PA, Bradbury TN, Karney BR. The costs of racism for marriage: How racial discrimination hurts, and ethnic identity protects, newlywed marriages among Latinos. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 2012;38:454–465. doi: 10.1177/0146167211429450. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Umberson D, Montez JK. Social relationships and health: A flashpoint for health policy. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 2010;51:S54–S66. doi: 10.1177/0022146510383501. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- United States Department of Health and Human Services, Healthy People 2020. Disparities. 2014 Feb 1; Retrieved from http://healthypeople.gov/2020/about/disparitiesAbout.aspx.
- Valanis BG, Bowen DJ, Bassford T, Whitlock E, Charney P, Carter RA. Sexual orientation and health: Comparisons in the Women’s Health Initiative sample. Archives of Family Medicine. 2000;9:843–853. doi: 10.1001/archfami.9.9.843. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vrangalova Z, Savin-Williams RC. Mostly heterosexual and mostly gay/lesbian: Evidence for new sexual orientation identities. Archives of Sexual Behavior. 2012;41:85–101. doi: 10.1007/s10508-012-9921-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Walen HR, Lachman ME. Social support and strain from partner, family, and friends: Costs and benefits for men and women in adulthood. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships. 2000;17:5–30. [Google Scholar]
- Wang J, Häusermann M, Vounatsou P, Aggleton P, Weiss MG. Health status, behavior, and care utilization in the Geneva Gay Men’s Health Survey. Preventive Medicine. 2007;44:70–75. doi: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2006.08.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Weiss JT. GL vs. BT: The archaeology of biphobia and transphobia within the US gay and lesbian community. Journal of Bisexuality. 2004;3:25–55. [Google Scholar]
- Williams DR, Mohammed SA. Discrimination and racial disparities in health: Evidence and needed research. Journal of Behavioral Medicine. 2009;32:20–47. doi: 10.1007/s10865-008-9185-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Williams DR, Yu Y, Jackson JS, Anderson NB. Racial differences in physical and mental health socio-economic status, stress and discrimination. Journal of Health Psychology. 1997;2:335–351. doi: 10.1177/135910539700200305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Williams T, Connolly J, Pepler D, Craig W. Peer victimization, social support, and psychosocial adjustment of sexual minority adolescents. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. 2005;34:471–482. [Google Scholar]
- Zou C, Andersen JP, Blosnich JR. The association between bullying and physical health among gay, lesbian, and bisexual individuals. Journal of the American Psychiatric Nurses Association. 2013;19:356–365. doi: 10.1177/1078390313510739. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]