Abstract
In somatosensory cortex, tactile stimulation within the neuronal receptive field (RF) typically evokes a transient excitatory response with or without postexcitatory inhibition. Here, we describe neuronal responses in which stimulation on the hand is followed by suppression of the ongoing discharge. With the use of 16-channel microelectrode arrays implanted in the hand representation of primary somatosensory cortex of New World monkeys and prosimian galagos, we recorded neuronal responses from single units and neuron clusters. In 66% of our sample, neuron activity tended to display suppression of firing when regions of skin outside of the excitatory RF were stimulated. In a small proportion of neurons, single-site indentations suppressed firing without initial increases in response to any of the tested sites on the hand. Latencies of suppressive responses to skin indentation (usually 12–34 ms) were similar to excitatory response latencies. The duration of inhibition varied across neurons. Although most observations were from anesthetized animals, we also found similar neuron response properties in one awake galago. Notably, suppression of ongoing neuronal activity did not require conditioning stimuli or multi-site stimulation. The suppressive effects were generally seen following single-site skin indentations outside of the neuron's minimal RF and typically on different digits and palm pads, which have not often been studied in this context. Overall, the characteristics of widespread suppressive or inhibitory response properties with and without initial facilitative or excitatory responses add to the growing evidence that neurons in primary somatosensory cortex provide essential processing for integrating sensory stimulation from across the hand.
Keywords: inhibitory period, latency, area 3b, area 3a, monkeys
among their common traits, primates, to varying degrees, rely on remarkable sensory and motor functions of the hand and arm. The cortical representations of the hand and associated neuron properties have long been studied to understand normal and impaired processing of somatosensory stimuli, with emphasis on developing strategies to promote recovery when processing is impaired. Examinations of widespread and multi-digit interactions in area 3b have added to our understanding of early stages of somatosensory processing to incorporate neurons with larger response fields that show reductions as well as increases in firing rate when discrete regions of skin are touched (DiCarlo et al. 1998; Fitzgerald et al. 2006b; Friedman et al. 2008; Kim et al. 2015; Reed et al. 2008, 2011; Thakur et al. 2006, 2012). Still, little information is available about the inhibitory components of the receptive field (RF) of neurons related to the hand and arm in primary somatosensory cortex in primates.
Here, we studied characteristics of response suppression in primate primary somatosensory cortex when the hand is touched, and we consider how the response patterns correlate with inhibitory processes in neural circuits across the hand representation. As quoted by Hsiao and colleagues in Thakur et al. (2012): “Previous studies in area 3b have revealed overlapping excitatory and inhibitory inputs throughout the RF of the neuron (Alloway and Burton 1991; Dykes et al. 1984). However, infield inhibition is rarely revealed by single punctate probes (Iwamura et al. 1983; Mountcastle and Powell 1959; Sur 1980; Sur et al. 1984), which show area 3b RFs as homogeneous excitatory regions (Sur 1980).” The term “infield inhibition” refers to the inhibitory field that spatially overlaps the excitatory response and can have some delay or duration that extends beyond excitatory transient responses (Gardner and Costanzo 1980a, b).
The main goal of the present study was to characterize firing suppression in response to tactile stimulation of a single location inside or outside of the minimum (threshold) excitatory RF in somatosensory cortex areas 3b and 3a of anesthetized and awake primates. The studies were conducted using chronically implanted electrode arrays (2 × 8) in galagos, squirrel monkeys, and one owl monkey, where the primary somatosensory cortical hand representation is exposed on the surface of the brain. We examined cortical neurons in area 3b of primates to explore the nature of information processing in primary somatosensory cortex with emphasis on how stimuli outside of the excitatory RF modulate neuronal activity. Previously, we have reported (Reed et al. 2010a, b, 2011) that area 3b neurons are modulated by stimuli presented outside of the minimal RFs (mRFs), such that stimulation on two different digit or palm sites can facilitate or suppress firing relative to single-site stimulation within the mRF. With the use of chronic microelectrode array recordings, here, we report suppression of baseline firing in response to single-site skin indentations and describe properties of responses related to types of suppression encountered in three different primate species. These latencies of suppression were relatively short and not statistically distinguishable from response facilitation latencies.
With the use of microelectrode arrays with known spacing between electrodes, we were able to identify firing suppression in response to single-site stimulation and determine somatotopic organization for a portion of primary somatosensory cortex. We found that passive single-site stimulation with skin indentations on the hand could evoke different response types (suppression of firing alone as well as excitation followed by suppression) in three different species and generally across awake and anesthetized conditions. We examined the latencies and durations of response suppressions with respect to stimulus location on the hand to detect relationships relevant to the somatotopic organization of cortex. Individual neurons responded to single-site stimulation at different hand locations with different response profiles that could include excitation (“E”), inhibition (“I”), or no response, depending on the location of the stimulus across the surface of the hand. The results further indicate that widespread spatial integration of information originating across different parts of the hand occurs as early as in areas 3b and 3a. Our evidence for long-range interactions between neurons within the hand representations of somatosensory cortex suggests that these neurons are involved in processing information from receptors across large portions of the hand, thus providing an early and necessary step toward the integration of somatosensory information that leads to object perception (Hsiao 2008; Hsiao and Bensmaia 2008; Saal and Bensmaia 2014).
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Experiments were conducted on two prosimian galagos (Otolemur garnettii), two squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus), and one owl monkey (Aotus trivirgatus). These animals were adult males. All procedures involving animals followed the U.S. National Institutes of Health Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals and were approved by the Vanderbilt University Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee.
To record chronically from somatosensory cortex, arrays of 16 electrodes were implanted in the hand representation of area 3b of one or both cerebral hemispheres in each animal. The electrode arrays consisted of Teflon-coated stainless-steel or tungsten microwires, 35 or 50 μm in diameter, spaced 0.25 mm apart in a 2 × 8 array. Each array was 2 mm long and 0.4 mm wide (Jain et al. 2001; Nicolelis et al. 1998, 2003).
Surgical Procedures
To implant the electrode arrays, each animal was anesthetized with ketamine hydrochloride (25 mg/kg im) and xylazine (0.4 mg/kg im). Supplemental injections were given as needed to maintain a surgical level of anesthesia. Dexamethasone was given (2 mg/kg im) to prevent or reduce brain swelling. Fluids were given subcutaneously, body temperature was maintained with a water-circulating heating pad, and heart rate and respiration were monitored. Two to four stainless screws were placed in the skull for securing ground wires, and two titanium bone screws were placed in the skull to stabilize the array. A small craniotomy was made over the part of area 3b that represents the hand in one or both cerebral hemispheres, and the dura was retracted. To guide the placement of the electrode array, a single microelectrode (1 MΩ impedance at 1 kHz) was used to record multi-unit (MU) activity evoked by light touch on the body and to identify the rostral and caudal borders of the hand representation in area 3b. Once the locations of interest were established, isoflurane (1.5% in 100% oxygen) gas was used for anesthesia. The microelectrode array was oriented so that all of the wires contacted the cortical surface before it was lowered either manually or with a burst-advancing motion by a microdrive (David Kopf Instruments, Tujunga, CA) to a depth of 700-1,000 μm below the pia surface, corresponding approximately to layers III–V. The arrays were secured to the skull with super glue and dental cement. The skin opening was closed with sutures. The animal was treated with antibiotics, carefully monitored, and returned to its home cage after fully recovering from anesthesia.
Data Acquisition
In each recording session, neuronal responses to hand stimulation were collected in animals that were lightly anesthetized with ketamine (10 mg/kg) to reduce any tendency for hand movements. Supplemental injections were given to maintain a light level of anesthesia as needed. The hand was pressed into a clay surface mold to keep it stable. One galago (PG-A) became so habituated to the procedures that stimulation was possible during the fully awake condition.
A multi-electrode array workstation (MEA; Plexon, Dallas, TX) was used to collect waveforms and timestamps with 40 kHz sampling (25 μs analog-to-digital conversion) at 12-bit resolution. Neurophysiological data were analyzed using NeuroExplorer (Nex Technologies, Madison, AL) and MATLAB software (MathWorks, Natick, MA). The preamplified analog neural signals were filtered (cut-off level 250 Hz–3 kHz) and amplified through the MEA. We used automatic and manual thresholding, such that the signals for both single units (SUs) and MUs must cross at least two times the SD of the average noise [see Nicolelis et al. (1997, 2003)]. Time-amplitude discrimination and principal component algorithms (PCAs) were used to isolate SU and MU responses in real time, which were also saved for offline sorting. Each neuronal spike was represented by a single data point in a PCA plot. Clusters of neuronal spikes that were clearly separated from the origin of the PCA plot and from other clusters were considered SUs. The identification of SUs was confirmed offline by autocorrelograms or interspike interval histograms using Nex software, as previously described (Jain et al. 2001; Nicolelis and Chapin 1994). Our classification of SUs included clusters of a few waveforms of similar shape, whereas we classified signals as MUs when several clusters of waveforms of differing shapes could not be isolated separately.
Mapping RFs.
Ten days after implantation of the electrode arrays and during subsequent recording sessions over the following months, mRFs were determined from all electrodes in the array for SUs and MUs that responded to tactile stimulation. mRFs (Jain et al. 2001; Merzenich et al. 1978; Nelson et al. 1980; Sur et al. 1982) were defined as the skin region, where light contact just above threshold, produced by a fine probe or a small brush, evoked maximal neural activity. The mRFs represent the center of a larger excitatory RF that can be driven by more intense stimuli. The extent of the mRFs was outlined on drawings of the hand and of other relevant skin surfaces. If tactile stimuli were ineffective, then digits and the hand were moved and tapped in an effort to elicit deep receptor and high threshold responses, which were also noted. Along with histological reconstruction, results of mRF mapping were used to aid estimates of the locations of the 16-electrode arrays, as illustrated, in part, by the example from galago PG-B in Fig. 1.
Tactile stimulation.
Quantitative data on the responses of single neurons and small clusters of neurons were acquired separately over a series of 4- to 6-h recording sessions that occurred once or twice/week over a period of many weeks. Cortical neurons were stimulated by punctate square-wave skin indentations generated by a computer-controlled Chubbuck electromechanical stimulating system (Chubbuck 1966). The stimulation probe had a cone shape that tapered to a 1-mm-diameter flat circular tip. For each experiment, the probe was positioned manually over a location of interest on the hand, and the neuronal activity was monitored through an oscilloscope and a speaker, while the probe was lowering to contact the skin. Once responses indicating contact of the skin were detected, a displacement value for the probe was selected (the indentation was usually at 200–500 μm). The Chubbuck probe completely retracted off the skin between pulses without a constant ramp. A constant force (60 g) was applied throughout the indentation on all of the experiments. Repetitive stimuli were delivered for 5 min in each recording stimulation site in the skin. A time and waveform generator (Tucker-Davis Technologies, Alachua, FL) was used to generate a square-wave pulse that controlled the Chubbuck stimulator. Time stamps were simultaneously sent to the multi-electrode array workstation to record the precise times of stimulation. The square-wave pulses produced either 20 ms indentations at 1–30 Hz or 500 ms indentations alternating with 1,500 ms probe retraction. We focused on results obtained with 20 ms skin indentations at low repetition rates (1–2 Hz).
Data Analysis
For each unit, the peri-stimulus time histogram (PSTH) was obtained from all trials per stimulation site. Responses to stimulus onset and stimulus removal were estimated as average firing rates (impulses/second) during two poststimulus time epochs of 10–25 and 25–40 ms using in-house MATLAB scripts. The mean firing rate for onset or offset responses was corrected for the baseline firing rate, corresponding to the activity recorded within a 50-ms period before the stimulus.
To estimate inhibitory response-onset latency, PSTHs were generated using bin sizes that ranged from 1 to 10 ms to optimize temporal resolution, depending on relative response strength and the level of baseline activity. Response latencies were calculated from these PSTHs using a window stepped every 1 ms, regardless of the PSTH bin size. Here, we report the latency and duration of periods of inhibition calculated by setting the threshold at 1.65 times the SD below the mean baseline firing rate, equivalent to a one-tailed significance level of 0.05 for a normal distribution. Only suppression that lasted for 10 ms or longer was classified as inhibition to avoid counting random firing fluctuations as inhibition.
The excitatory peak firing rates and latencies were not the focus of this study. However, the maximum firing-rate magnitude and the time at which the maximum firing occurred were collected from a PSTH with 1 ms bins and a smoothing filter width of five bins. The maximum firing rate was considered a significant response if the value exceeded the upper 95% confidence limit of the average firing rate. The excitatory response latency was defined as the time at which the firing rate exceeded the mean baseline firing at two times the SD of the baseline ongoing activity.
Firing rates, response latencies, unit classification, and identifying information were compiled in an electronic spreadsheet (Excel; Microsoft, Redmond, WA) and imported into SPSS software (version 22; IBM, Armonk, NY) for summary analyses. Nonparametric tests compared distributions and medians for measures, such as inhibition latency between two categories, with appropriate tests selected for number of comparisons (Kolmogorov-Smirnov test, Mann-Whitney test, median test). For comparing more than two categories, Kruskal-Wallis tests were used with post hoc correction applied for pair-wise comparisons. The nonparametric Spearman's ρ correlation coefficient was used to assess relationships of rank orders between two categories of data. For example, we assessed relationships between measures of inhibition latency and duration with measures of peak firing magnitude. For all tests, unless otherwise indicated, the significance level was set at 0.05 with two-tailed P values.
Note that we follow the convention of referring to firing-rate suppression as “inhibition” and “inhibitory response,” even though we are not recording the presence of inhibitory potentials (Sachdev et al. 2012). We also use firing-rate suppression, referred to as I here, as an indicator of the cortical and subcortical processes that may involve inhibition.
Stability of chronic array recordings.
The stability of chronic recordings can be evaluated, in part, by examining changes in RF properties (e.g., locations and sizes) over time. We found RFs that were largely stable for some neuron units, which is consistent with some reports [e.g., Jain et al. (2001)], whereas other neuronal RFs in our sample were variable (Fig. 1). For example (Fig. 1D), the RFs for neurons recorded from electrode six of galago PG-B remained on distal digit 5 (D5) from day 21 to 109 after array implantation in area 3b. However, in another example recorded from electrode four of the same array, the RFs shifted within D5 over a period of 3–4 mo.
The analysis of waveform shape is the standard way of identifying SUs, but because we characterized the neuronal responses over hours in a single recording session and over multiple days in a series of recording sessions, it is unclear whether the activity of a given unit over time corresponds to recordings from that same neuron over days. To compare neuronal responses (e.g., firing rates and latencies) with tactile stimulation on different locations of the hand, we needed to maintain recordings from the same neurons during all of the recording session within 1 experimental day. We used a technique for array implantations, in which the array is fixed relative to the skull (Jain et al. 2001; Nicolelis et al. 1998, 2003). Since the array was not floating with the brain, normal movements of the brain in the skull could affect the stability of recording over time. During a single recording session, unit waveforms generally appeared quite stable within the same day, irrespective of the number and location of the stimulation sites on the hand (Fig. 2A). In this investigation of waveform stability, we exported timestamps containing waveform information recorded in MEA to Excel using NeuroExplorer. For each recording file (usually during stimulation of a single location on the hand for 5 min), we averaged the waveforms of an SU from a given stimulation location and then superimposed all averaged waveforms from different stimulation locations on a line graph. Furthermore, we examined the signals over extended periods comprising multiple experimental sessions. We used the same averaging strategy and displayed all of the mean waveforms from a given unit as a function of time for stimulation at different sites in a single recording session. However, the mean waveforms tend to change gradually over a period of weeks, as shown in the example in Fig. 2B. These changes in neuronal recordings, based on characteristics of the extracellular recordings over weeks of time, are consistent with other examinations of the recording stability (Arce-McShane et al. 2014; Dickey et al. 2009).
Based on these findings, illustrated in Figs. 1 and 2, we treated an SU from a given electrode throughout each recording day as the same neuron. However, we considered units recorded from a given electrode on different days as different units due to the fluctuations in the unit properties in our samples. This practice could lead to a bias, because the same neuron could be represented multiple times as different unit responses; however, this bias is mitigated by our focus primarily on the ranges of values obtained in measurements for inhibition latency, duration, and spatial relationships within the stimulus presentation area on the hand. Note that the proportions of the occurrences of various response types are affected by this bias and should not be considered to represent populations of neurons.
Histology.
To visualize the positions of the electrodes in the electrode arrays in the histologically identified hand region of somatosensory area 3b, brain sections were processed either for cytochrome oxidase (Wong-Riley 1979) or Nissl substance. After the terminal experiment, the animals were given lethal injections (80 mg/kg or more) of pentobarbital. When areflexive, they were perfused through the heart with PBS (pH 7.4), successively followed by 4% paraformaldehyde in phosphate buffer and 4% paraformaldehyde with 10% sucrose in phosphate buffer. The brain was removed, and the thalamus was separated from the cortex. A block of frontoparietal cortex was separated from the rest of cortex, immersed in 30% sucrose in phosphate buffer, and refrigerated until the next day. The block containing the parietal cortex was cut parasagittally across the width of somatosensory cortex on a freezing microtome into 50 μm sections. The penetration depths of the electrodes in the arrays were determined from electrode tracts in the parasagittal brain sections in every other section processed for Nissl substance or cytochrome oxidase.
RESULTS
The results presented here represent a subset of the data of recorded units that displayed suppressive responses. Total recording sessions and unit responses for each case are shown in Table 1. All recordings were obtained from chronically implanted arrays of microelectrodes. A single 16-electrode array was implanted in somatosensory cortex of one New World owl monkey (OM-A) and one prosimian galago (PG-A), whereas an array was implanted in each hemisphere of one galago (PG-B) and two squirrel monkeys (SM-A, SM-B). In four cases, electrodes were in the hand region of area 3b of somatosensory cortex. Electrode tips were in layers IV and V. In one case (PG-A), the electrode array was in area 3a, and the electrode tips were in layers III and IV. The locations of the electrodes were histologically confirmed to be in the hand representation of area 3b or area 3a, as previously determined in microelectrode mapping studies (Carlson and Welt 1980; Sur et al. 1980; Wu and Kaas 2003). The locations of the RFs of neurons recorded in the two rows of each electrode array were consistent with the known somatotopy and the sizes of the mRFs of neurons or neuronal clusters determined in previous microelectrode mapping experiments. Thus progressions of mRFs for caudorostral rows of recording sites located in area 3b typically start on the wrist or hairy hand and progress to or near the glabrous fingertips (Fig. 1).
Table 1.
Case | Species | Recording Sessions | Unit Occurrence | SUs | MUs |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
SM-A | Squirrel monkey | 5 | 421 | 133 | 288 |
SM-B | Squirrel monkey | 4 | 217 | 93 | 124 |
PG-A (awake, 3a) | Galago | 6 | 98 | 51 | 47 |
PG-B | Galago | 11 | 369 | 143 | 226 |
OM-A | Owl monkey | 6 | 261 | 5 | 256 |
Total | 32 | 1,366 | 425 | 941 |
SUs, single units; MUs, multi-units.
Response Properties of Neurons in Area 3a in an Awake Galago
Unlike area 3b, many neurons in area 3a of anesthetized New World monkeys and prosimian galagos respond poorly to light tactile stimuli, as strong thalamic activation is relayed from muscle spindle receptors and probably other proprioceptive receptors (Huffman and Krubitzer 2001; Merzenich et al. 1983; Wu and Kaas 2003). However, responses to tactile stimuli can be recorded, as area 3b provides an input to area 3a (Krubitzer and Kaas 1990; Wu and Kaas 2003). For example, in lightly anesthetized baboons, a portion of neurons in area 3a was found to respond only to cutaneous stimulation, whereas others respond to both muscle and cutaneous or muscle stimulation alone (Heath et al. 1976). In one galago that was amenable to tactile stimulation in the awake condition and had an array implanted in area 3a (PG-A), we were able to record responses to skin indentations. In this and all other cases included in our study, we did not characterize responsiveness to deep pressure or movement.
In response to tactile stimulation on the hand, area 3a neurons in PG-A displayed response properties similar to those of neurons in area 3b. However, area 3a cutaneous RFs in the awake galago were usually larger than in area 3b. Area 3a neuronal RFs often covered several digits, the entire palm, or even the entire hand (Fig. 1E). Results from all cases are discussed together to follow, with exceptions noted when appropriate.
Typical Response Characteristics of Neurons in the Hand Representation of Areas 3b and 3a
In both New World monkeys and prosimian galagos, neurons in area 3b responded to tactile stimuli within the mRF in ways expected from previous descriptions (Jain et al. 2001; Reed et al. 2010b). Area 3a neurons responded to cutaneous stimulation in similar patterns compared with recordings from area 3b in anesthetized primates. Our results refer to response occurrences, for which neural activity recorded within a session was considered to originate from stable SUs and MUs, whereas activity from the same electrode across different days could vary in origin (Figs. 1 and 2). We focused our analysis on types of suppressive response occurrences (Fig. 3), in which neuronal activity was either suppressed by tactile stimulation without an initial firing increase (classified as inhibitory, or I responses) or presented a transient period of excitation, followed by a period of firing suppression (classified as excitatory-inhibitory, “E-I” responses).
With the use of chronically implanted electrodes, we tracked the neural activity recorded in response to tactile stimulation on different locations on the hand. Because we recorded simultaneously from 16 electrodes, we collected considerable amounts of data in which skin indentations were delivered outside regions evoking peak activation. Within the same recording day, most SUs and MUs included in this study were characterized first by identifying the region on the hand where skin indentations evoked the strongest neuronal discharge (peak activation location). After the identification of suppressive response occurrences from a given unit, firing in response to stimulation of the peak activation location and each additional stimulated location was classified as E, E-I, I, or “NS” (for no significant firing change). Note that a given unit could present different neuronal response types depending on the stimulus location on the hand (e.g., Fig. 4).
In our sample, most neurons responded to a brief skin indentation with a transient increase in firing rate, followed by a decrease in firing rate and then a return to baseline level or even below baseline rates (E-I response type). For a suprathreshold skin indentation, a minority of cortical neurons exhibited suppression of baseline activity that was established after the stimulus and then sustained for a short period (I response type) [also, see Sur et al. (1984)]. These responses of neurons in area 3b (S1) of galagos, squirrel monkeys, and owl monkey resemble those reported for other primates [e.g., Jain et al. (2001)] in this regard. The distributions of suppressive response types were the same for neurons in area 3b (anesthetized) and area 3a (unanesthetized galago) across the New World monkeys and galagos (Mann-Whitney U-test = 9.21 × 104, Z = −1.56, P = 0.119, n = 946 responses). Additionally, data from area 3a were collected from one case only (PG-A); thus we summarized values from all cases (areas 3a and 3b) for the results. Table 2 reports data from areas 3a and 3b separately. However, a small subset of data was collected from area 3a (PG-A; see Figs. 5B, 6B, and 7); therefore, statistical analyses are presented in Table 2 for area 3b alone, along with the significance test results from areas 3a and 3b combined to compare with values reported in the text.
Table 2.
Group | Measure or Subtype | Variables to Compare | Mean (SD) Area 3b | n Area 3b | Mean (SD) Area 3a | n Area 3a | Statistic Area 3b | P Area 3b | P Areas 3a + 3b |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
E-I | Suppression latency | SU | 33 (12) | 223 | 81 (30) | 10 | Z = −2.95 | 0.003* | 0.002* |
MU | 36 (13) | 682 | 70 (28) | 33 | |||||
E-I | Duration | SU | 49 (32) | 223 | 60 (28) | 10 | Z = −3.38 | 0.001* | 3.3 × 10−12* |
MU | 43 (33) | 682 | 28 (19) | 33 | |||||
I | Suppression latency | SU | 20 (14) | 150 | 18 (15) | 41 | Z = −5.64 | 1.7 × 10−8* | 8.6 × 10−5* |
MU | 25 (12) | 209 | 27 (18) | 14 | |||||
I | Duration | SU | 33 (24) | 150 | 83 (66) | 41 | Z = −0.57 | 0.57 | 0.083 |
MU | 42 (38) | 209 | 23 (14) | 14 | |||||
SU | Suppression latency | E-I | 33 (12) | 223 | 81 (30) | 10 | Z = −10.96 | 6.2 × 10−28* | 8.2 × 10−35* |
I | 20 (14) | 150 | 18 (15) | 41 | |||||
SU | Duration | E-I | 49 (32) | 223 | 45 (28) | 10 | Z = −5.46 | 4.7 × 10−8* | 3.1 × 10−4* |
I | 33 (24) | 150 | 83 (66) | 41 | |||||
SU | Excitation latency | E | 9 (15) | 295 | 18 (2) | 18 | Z = −0.71 | 0.48 | 0.16 |
E-I | 13 (29) | 223 | 19 (6) | 10 | |||||
SU | Latency of excitation/suppression | E | 18 (8) | 294 | 18 (2) | 18 | χ2 = 1.19 | 0.55 | 0.47 |
E-I | 17 (7) | 223 | 19 (6) | 10 | |||||
I | 20 (14) | 150 | 18 (15) | 41 | |||||
SU | Excitation peak firing | E | 64 (92) | 294 | 24 (21) | 18 | Z = −5.53 | 3.2 × 10−8* | 6.3 × 10−9* |
E-I | 79 (74) | 223 | 30 (18) | 10 | |||||
SU | E-I and I | Latency | – | 373 | – | 51 | ρ = 0.27 | 1.4 × 10−7* | 3.2 × 10−4* |
Duration | – | 373 | – | 51 | |||||
SU | E-I and I | Latency | – | 387 | – | 51 | ρ = 0.15 | 0.003* | 4.2 × 10−4* |
Peak firing | – | 387 | – | 51 | |||||
SU | E-I and I | Duration | – | 373 | – | 51 | ρ = 0.25 | 1.3 × 10−6* | 0.088 |
Peak firing | – | 387 | – | 51 | |||||
SU | E, E-I, and I | Latency | – | 339 | – | 51 | ρ = −0.18 | 0.001* | 0.001* |
Background firing | – | 757 | – | 51 | |||||
SU | E-I and I | Location | – | 379 | – | 51 | ρ = −0.20 | 6.0 × 10−4* | 0.001* |
Duration | – | 379 | – | 51 | |||||
SU | E-I and I | Location | – | 379 | – | 51 | ρ = −0.11 | 0.034* | 0.001* |
Latency | – | 379 | – | 51 | |||||
SU | E | Occurrences | – | 282 | – | 18 | ρ = −0.979 | 4.3 × 10−6* | 1.8 × 10−7* |
Location | – | 282 | – | 18 | |||||
SU | E-I | Occurrences | – | 220 | – | 10 | ρ = −0.937 | 1.9 × 10−4* | 1.5 × 10−8* |
Location | – | 220 | – | 10 | |||||
SU | I | Occurrences | – | 107 | – | 41 | ρ = −0.402 | 0.284 | 0.381 |
Location | – | 107 | – | 41 | |||||
SU | NS | Occurrences | – | 129 | – | 1 | ρ = −0.452 | 0.222 | 0.203 |
Location | – | 129 | – | 1 | |||||
SU | Response types (E, E-I, I, and NS) | Occurrences < median | – | 120 | – | 70 | χ2 = 46.49 | 4.5 × 10−10* | 1.5 × 10−7* |
Location | – | 120 | – | 70 |
Results from area 3b recordings in anesthetized primates summarized and compared with P values (
P < 0.05) reported in results (of areas 3a and 3b analyzed together). Data from area 3a in an unanesthetized galago are listed; statistics not calculated on this small subset. Mean and SD reported for pair-wise comparisons in units of milliseconds (latency, duration) or spikes/second (peak firing). Means are not reported for Spearman's ρ correlation analysis and the nonparametric median test. E-I, excitatory-inhibitory; I, inhibitory; E, excitatory; NS, no significant firing change.
Across cases, when we identified suppressive response occurrences of a given unit due to stimulation on a hand location, the majority of the responses to stimulation within the peak activation region included a transient excitatory response (E, 313/916, 34%) or transient excitatory response followed by a period of firing inhibition (E-I, 233/916, 25%). Almost 21% of neuronal responses (191/916) decreased the baseline firing rate in response to stimulation without a preceding excitatory response (I; Fig. 3). The remaining 19.5% (179/916) of neuronal responses did not deviate significantly from baseline firing during the tactile stimulation conditions delivered, classified as NS. Note that the E-I response type is similar to “replacing inhibition,” reported by Hsiao and colleagues [e.g., Sripati et al. (2006)], whereas E and I are similar to traditional excitatory and inhibitory response types, respectively.
For each response occurrence of SU and MU activity, we calculated firing rates, latencies, and duration of activity suppression, focusing on suppressive response components, as described below.
Analysis of suppression latencies and durations of neurons in areas 3b and 3a.
To summarize quantitatively response properties related to firing-rate suppression in our sample, we measured minimal latency and suppressive duration from units in all five cases, comprising eight hemispheres, regardless of stimulation location relative to the peak activation location. To compare the differences between SUs and MUs and between neurons with significant postexcitatory inhibition (E-I) or inhibition only (I, without preceding excitation), we divided the whole population of responses into several subgroups and plotted the frequency histograms in Fig. 5. The distributions of inhibitory latencies and durations of MUs (n = 941) and SUs (n = 424) were similar and overlapping, but the averages differed slightly. The mean inhibition latency of E-I responses was significantly different between SUs (n = 233 responses) with 35 ms and MUs (n = 718 responses) with 38 ms inhibition latencies (Z = −2.72, P = 0.007). Average inhibition latencies of I responses were significantly different between SUs (n = 191 responses) with 19 ms and MUs (n = 223 responses) with 25 ms (Z = −6.84, P = 7.9 × 10−12). The mean inhibition durations for E-I responses were significantly different between SUs with 49 ms durations and MUs with 42 ms (Z = −3.93, P = 8.57 × 10−5, n = 946). For I responses, no significant difference was detected in average inhibition durations between SUs with 43 ms and MUs with 41 ms durations (Z = −1.74, P = 0.083, n = 414). For the distributions of inhibition, with or without preceding excitatory responses, we found that inhibition latencies were shorter for SU responses than MU responses. The statistical differences may suggest that MU recordings include a somewhat different neuron population. However, MUs from many neurons firing at different excitatory (E) latencies may have caused firing suppression (I) to be detected at longer latencies. The E-I responses display, accordingly, significantly shorter response suppression durations for MUs than for SUs.
To evaluate relationships between inhibitory and excitatory response components further, we focused on the SU activity. As expected, the mean latency of the inhibitory component in the I response type (19 ms) was significantly shorter than when an excitatory component preceded the suppression in E-I (35 ms; Z = −12.56, P = 3.63 × 10−36, n = 424 neuronal responses). In our sample, the duration of the suppression was shorter for responses that showed suppression only (43 ms) compared with those that followed excitation (49 ms; Z = −3.69, P = 2.19 × 10−4, n = 424 neuronal responses). Thus for the responses in the E-I category, suppression occurred at longer latencies than suppression in the I category, suggesting that the excitatory response could have masked the earliest part of the inhibitory response. Similarly, the inhibitory component likely masked excitation in I responses but was not able to mask the excitatory component of the E-I responses. We did not detect a difference in latency between peak responses for E-I and E categories (Z = −1.02, P = 0.306). Notably, we evaluated the average latency of I responses (16.3 ms) compared with the excitatory latency of E responses (16.6 ms) and excitatory latency of E-I responses (15.6 ms) and found no significant differences [Kruskal-Wallis χ2 = 1.493, degrees of freedom (df) = 2, P = 0.474, n = 698 neuronal responses]. Thus the mean latencies for each category of our sample were similar, with an overall mean latency of 16.2 ms. We also found a weak but significant negative correlation between background firing rates and response latencies of all three response types (I, E-I, E). The results indicated that increased firing excitability decreased both the inhibitory and excitatory latencies or increased the detection of both types of responses (Spearman's ρ = −0.182, P = 0.001, n = 343 responses).
Whereas we focused on firing-rate suppression, we also compared the peak firing rates of SU responses categorized as E-I vs. E. The average magnitude of the excitatory component was larger for E-I responses (77.9 impulses/second, n = 246) vs. E responses (58.3 impulses/second, n = 310; Mann-Whitney U-test = 2.72 × 104, P = 6.33 × 10−9, total n = 556 responses). This suggests that the suppression component of an E-I response is part of in-field inhibition in the central RF, whereas E only occurs when stimulus is farther from the central mRF of the neuron.
We then analyzed the E-I responses and I responses to examine relationships between inhibitory and excitatory components using correlation and regression analyses. For all responses with suppression (E-I or I), suppression duration was very weakly correlated with the latency of the suppression (Spearman's ρ = 0.118, P = 3.215 × 10−4, n = 425 neuronal responses with suppression durations of 10 ms or more). For the subset of responses in which an excitatory response preceded the suppression (E-I), suppression latency was weakly correlated with excitatory response magnitude (Spearman's ρ = 0.229, P = 4.23 × 10−4, n = 234 neuronal responses with suppression durations of 10 ms or more). However, a relationship was not detected between excitatory peak magnitude with suppression duration (Spearman's ρ = 0.088, P = 0.181, n = 234 SU responses with suppression durations of 10 ms or more). Overall, we found weak correlations between excitatory and inhibitory response components, but other factors must contribute to the variation in the latency and duration of suppression components beyond the excitatory response magnitude.
Response types in relation to stimulus sites across digits vs. within digits.
We next tested how a neuron's response type changed in relation to the position of the stimulus on the hand. If peak neural responses are obtained for a site on a given part of a digit, then it is conceivable that comparisons of a neuron's responses to a given stimulus site located on an adjacent digit would differ from when the stimulus site was outside of the peak response site but within the same digit. Thus we examined neuron responses relative to different stimulus locations (e.g., across digits vs. within digits) using a classification ranking that we set (Fig. 6). For this ranking, the stimulus site that evoked the highest firing rate (“peak activation site” or “hot spot”) was identified and assigned a rank value of zero. Increasing values were assigned to the remaining stimulus sites on the hand in a given recording session. Next, we approximated the average distances between adjacent phalanges (which can vary for different digits) on the hand for each species (galago = 0.85 cm, owl monkey = 1.1 cm, squirrel monkey = 0.91 cm) to obtain a general value to relate to the separation between different locations on the primate hand. We used the average distance value (rounded up to 1 for visualization purposes) across and within digits and pads, and this value was summed for each step away from the peak activation site. This “uniform” ranking was based on indications that anatomical connections in primary somatosensory cortex are similarly dense within digits and across different digits (Liao et al. 2013; Negyessy et al. 2013) rather than faithfully representing anatomical distances in the actual hand. We tested a second classification scheme (not shown), in which we assigned a larger separation value or penalty when the stimulus was located on digits different than the one containing the hot spot compared with locations on the same digit as the hot spot. This “biased” ranking, which favors stimulus sites located on the same digit as the hot spot, mimics relationships of anatomical continuities on the hand or mimics potential for cortical connections to differ by location (Fang et al. 2002). We used correlations between the values assigned to each scheme with the measures of the response properties of excitatory and inhibitory firing (peak latency, peak excitatory firing rate, inhibitory latency, inhibitory duration) to examine how well the response properties correlated to the stimulus locations.
We found few differences in correlations between response property measures and each of the two classification-ranking schemes for stimulus location, with one exception. The peak E latency was significantly correlated with the uniform rank values, shown in Fig. 6, and was not significantly correlated with the biased rank values (Spearman's ρ = 0.088, P = 0.014; Spearman's ρ = 0.066, P = 0.066, respectively, n = 777). However, relationships, such as with peak firing rate, suppression duration, etc., were similarly correlated for the two ranking types, often with slightly higher (but still weak) correlations for the uniform ranking. Such correlations were found for suppression duration (Spearman's ρ = −0.164, P = 0.001; Spearman's ρ = −0.156, P = 0.001, respectively, n = 413) and suppression latency (Spearman's ρ = −0.167, P = 0.001; Spearman's ρ = −0.141, P = 0.004, respectively, n = 410). Overall, we found no evidence that the excitatory and inhibitory responses to tactile stimulation on the hand occurred with stronger magnitudes or were more often driven by sites within a digit rather than across different digits or palm pads. This suggests that the somatotopic organization of the cortical representation of the hand is the important factor compared with the separation between digits, which varies with hand positions and tasks. To reduce redundancy while exploring the spatial relationships of suppressive responses to hand stimulation, we therefore used only the uniform categorization of hand locations (Fig. 6) in subsequent analyses.
With the focus on the uniform ranking to relate the occurrences of E, E-I, or I responses to the stimulation of a given site that was not located at the peak activation region, we found a strong inverse relationship between the number of occurrences of E or E-I responses and the location value (Spearman's ρ = −0.992, P = 1.75 × 10−7, n = 305 responses; Spearman's ρ = −0.996, P = 1.54 × 10−8, n = 236 responses, respectively); however, we did not detect significant correlations between the assigned location values from the location-ranking values and the occurrences of I suppressive responses (Spearman's ρ = −0.333, P = 0.381, n = 116 responses) or the occurrences of stimulation producing no significant change in firing rate (NS; Spearman's ρ = −0.469, P = 0.203, n = 129 responses). The presence of strong negative correlations between E response components and stimulus locations indicates that E response components in our sample occur less frequently, as the stimulus site is more distant from the excitatory peak activation region. However, the lack of correlation between I response components and stimulus locations indicates that the I components may be more diffuse or may not be correlated with the location of the peak activation region.
We then examined the widespread nature of the I and E response components, using the uniform-ranking values to relate to the stimulus locations. For all response categories of E, E-I, and I vs. NS, fewer responses were collected when the stimulus was located at or beyond a separation equivalent of four phalanges away from the site evoking the maximum recorded firing rate (Fig. 6A; x-axis value = 4). This finding was supported by the nonparametric median test, which showed that the value of four was the first for which neuron response counts were significantly below the median (χ2 = 34.508, df = 3, P = 1.548 × 10−7, n = 244 responses). For the E category, no such responses were collected when the stimulus was located beyond the equivalent of seven phalanges away from the peak activation site (Fig. 6; x-axis value = 7). The response types that we found when the stimulus was the farthest away from the site of peak activation were the E-I and I categories.
Profiles of Excitation and Inhibition Tracked across Multiple Stimulus Locations
When we examine the responses of SUs, we use the term “response profiles” to designate the set of response types (E, E-I, I, and NS) that we collected from that unit when different hand sites are stimulated during the same recording session (see example in Fig. 4). An individual neuron response decreases or increases and sometimes changes in type (e.g., from E-I to I or NS) as the stimulus is moved to different locations on the hand. For inclusion in this analysis, SUs should have responded to touch with an E, E-I, or I response type on at least one location on the hand. Response profiles were classified as “All E,” “All I,” “All E-I,” or “Complex,” depending on the set of response types presented by the unit when different locations in the hand were stimulated (see below). Across all five animals, we tracked 113 SU response profiles to hand stimulation. In four out of the five animals, we determined profiles when we held the same unit for recordings across five or more stimulus sites within 1 recording day. In one animal (OM-A), profiles were determined for sets of four or more sites, due to small sample sizes.
Profiles of excitation with inhibition.
Most units responded to stimulation in different ways when we moved the probe to different hand sites. For >33% of the sample (38/113), the stimulation of a selected hand site resulted in initial excitatory responses, with or without subsequent inhibition, and then for the same unit, the stimulation of other hand sites resulted in firing-rate suppression. We termed this pattern a “complex response profile” type. Such profiles occurred in all species tested and in areas 3b and 3a (see Figs. 4, 7, and 8 to compare PG-B, PG-A, SM-A, respectively). We distinguished two subtypes of excitatory and inhibitory response profiles. For ∼22% of our total sample (25/113), this inhibition for I and E-I responses occurred when the stimulus was immediately adjacent to the stimulus site that evoked the highest peak firing rate (peak activation site; Fig. 6, x-axis value = 1), either on the adjacent phalange within the same digit or on the matching phalange on an adjacent digit (Fig. 7). We use the term “adjacent-I” to indicate the suppression in response to stimulation on sites near the excitatory regions. In the remaining 13 profiles (nearly 12% of the total sample), neuron activity was suppressed only when the stimulus was more distant from the peak activation site (Fig. 6, x-axis value ≥ 2, and Figs. 4 and 8). We use the term “nonadjacent-I” to indicate suppression in response to stimulation on sites far from the site that evoked the highest peak firing.
In nearly 33% of the sample (37/113), stimulating selected hand sites resulted in initial excitatory responses, with or without subsequent inhibition, but excitation always preceded suppression (response profile type All E-I; Fig. 9). We termed this an All E-I response profile, and we categorized adjacent-I and nonadjacent-I subtypes of this pattern. Adjacent-I is in-field inhibition, in which suppression followed the initial excitatory response when the stimulus site was adjacent to the peak activation site. This occurred in ∼19% of our total sample (22/113). In the remaining 15 profiles (>13% of the total sample), in-field inhibition only occurred when the stimulus was more distant from the peak activation site (nonadjacent-I).
Profiles of inhibition or excitation.
For a small number of neuron units, none of the tested hand locations and stimulus parameters evoked excitatory responses, even though these neurons were located in the hand representation in area 3b (21 SUs; 28 MUs) or area 3a (7 SUs; 1 MU for case PG-A). These neurons were detected by firing-rate suppression due to single-site skin indentation. The tactile response profiles for nearly 19% of our sample (21/113 response profiles) can be summarized as suppressed firing rates without an initial excitatory response to stimulation on all of the tested hand sites on the glabrous hand or did not evoke a significant increase from baseline firing rates. An example of such a response profile when all of the hand sites tested resulted in suppression or no significant response (All I profile) is shown in Fig. 10. For the remaining 15% of our sample (17/113), we found that stimulation at all tested hand sites significantly increased the neuronal firing rate over baseline firing in response to stimulation on all of the tested hand sites (All E profile). An example is shown in Fig. 11. For both the All E and All I response profile categories, it is possible that untested stimulation parameters would result in different response patterns, but this remains unknown.
Overall, the response profiles tracked from our recordings revealed E and I components of responses to single-site stimulation on the hand at different locations. The different response profiles found in our sample illustrate widespread unit responsiveness to stimulation delivered to individual sites located outside of the peak activation region (Fig. 12A). Counts of the different response profiles found in our sample revealed the predominance of response types that included both E and I components when sites beyond the peak activation location are stimulated (Fig. 12B). The variety of responses when stimulation occurs within and outside the peak activation location suggests that single-site stimulation evokes multiple suppression mechanisms that are likely important for stimulus representations in primary somatosensory cortex.
DISCUSSION
The goal of the present study was to report characteristics of the suppression of the ongoing baseline neuronal firing activity in somatosensory cortex of primates by tactile stimulation on a small (1 mm diameter) single site on the hand. Only a few studies have described similar surround suppression of ongoing spontaneous activity, driven by single-site tactile stimulation in the somatosensory cortex of primates (Gardner and Costanzo 1980a, b), rodents (Sachdev et al. 2000; Swadlow 1989), and star-nosed moles (Sachdev and Catania 2002). In star-nosed moles, Sachdev and Catania (2002) reported that 40% of neurons that responded to single-site mechanosensory stimulation of the RF center were inhibited by stimulation of surrounding areas of skin on the same appendage on the star. In our study of the hand representation in monkeys, we showed that surround suppression can be found when stimulating single sites on separate digits and palm pads, at sites away from a given neuron's excitatory mRF on the hand. These results revealed spatially broad effects of the stimulus on the hand in the representation in primary somatosensory cortex in a new way. Our findings are consistent with those from studies using a “conditioning” stimulus preceding a “test” stimulus [e.g., Chowdhury and Rasmusson (2003); Gardner and Costanzo (1980a, b)], dual-probe stimuli (Lipton et al. 2010; Reed et al. 2010b), and complex multi-probe stimuli, pioneered by Johnson, Hsiao, and colleagues (DiCarlo et al. 1998; Fitzgerald et al. 2006a, b; Thakur et al. 2012). Similar findings have been reported for neurons in the visual system (Allman et al. 1985; Hubel and Wiesel 1962, 1965; Lee et al. 2013; Li et al. 2001) and the auditory system (Davis et al. 2003; Nelken and Young 1994; Shofner and Young 1985; Spirou and Young 1991).
Note that although we collected simultaneous neuronal recordings using chronically implanted electrode arrays, this report focuses on tracking individual neuronal responses when different hand sites are stimulated. Examinations of the relationships of multiple neurons or pairs of neurons are beyond the scope of this report, but others have reported on how stimulus location is represented within the somatosensory system in rodents and nonhuman primates (Ghazanfar et al. 2000; Nicolelis et al. 1998; Petersen and Diamond 2000). Due to the bias in our chronic recordings, the percentages reported here should not be considered to represent the population responses in primary somatosensory cortex, because we sampled a limited area (2 rows of 8 electrodes) over time. However, it is noteworthy that Sripati et al. (2006) characterized the RFs of distal digit pads in macaque monkeys using random patterns of tactile pulses, and they described similar categories. They noted 6% showed “excitation only,” similar to type E; 42% with replacing inhibition, similar to type E-I; and 52% with “surround and replacing inhibition,” which may resemble the complex response profile we find when stimulating sites across the hand rather than within a distal digit pad. Sripati et al. (2006) did not describe purely inhibitory fields, but those data were obtained from a single phalanx in awake macaques, constructed in response to pulses from a 400-probe tactile stimulator.
In the present experiments, such I suppression, without a conditioning stimulus, was detected in response to single-site stimuli (instead of dual- or multi-site stimuli) with chronically embedded electrodes for at least two reasons. First, the primates in our experiments were either awake (PG-A) or only lightly anesthetized or sedated to prevent hand movements. The detection of suppression depends on the level of ongoing activity that can be suppressed, and these experimental conditions resulted in baseline firing at higher levels than when recording neuronal activity under surgical anesthesia. Second, the microelectrode arrays allowed simultaneous collection of recording neuronal activity from multiple sites in the hand representation, and any given single stimulus location on the hand would fall within the mRF of some neurons and fall outside the mRF of most other recorded neurons. This avoided problems of focusing on individual neurons and the stimuli that increase their firing rates without investigating firing-rate suppression. Thus we use the mRF as a guide for stimulus placement and reference, but our report using suprathreshold stimulation results in neuron activations in response to stimuli that are beyond the mRF.
Properties and Extent of Widespread Responses and Suppressive Components
In the present experiments, stimulus responses characterized by excitation followed by inhibition had significantly higher magnitudes than responses that showed excitatory response components alone. Yet, the significant differences in firing-rate magnitudes did not translate into differences in excitation latencies between E and E-I types. The E-I response type we describe is similar to replacing inhibition described by Sripati et al. (2006), who suggest that the suppression component may not be simply refractoriness but may also reflect active cortical interactions. At the circuit level, the strongly activated neurons are also the neurons that are detectably inhibited. An activation threshold must be reached in a particular circuit to suppress a given neuron or circuit effectively. Thus we also found that weaker response facilitation tended to be related to weak or nonexistent firing suppression, consistent with traditional models of RF organization and balanced cortical circuits (Douglas et al. 1995; Sachdev et al. 2012; Wehr and Zador 2003). Additionally, when the ongoing firing was suppressed without preceding excitation, the latencies were not detectably different from our sample of excitation latencies. These relatively early latencies suggest the existence of short latency, direct subcortical activation of suppressive response components, as discussed for the visual system (Carandini et al. 2002) and somatosensory system [see Sachdev et al. (2012) review]. Notably, we recorded suppressive response types to touch in area 3a, similar to previous reports [e.g., Heath et al. (1976)], including initial inhibition of firing [e.g., Xu et al. (2011)]. Some inhibition latencies may be longer in our sample from area 3a (see Figs. 5 and 7), possibly reflecting the route of tactile inputs from area 3b to 3a (Heath et al. 1976; Krubitzer and Kaas 1990; Wu and Kaas 2003). However, our sample from area 3a, in a case without sedation, is small, and we cannot interpret this data subset to generalize to area 3a across primates and across levels of sedation. Overall, we found neurons in areas 3b and 3a in different primate species and different levels of sedation that responded to skin indentations with suppression, particularly when the stimulated regions were distant from the site of peak excitatory firing.
We reported patterns of responses and response profiles that are widespread but not without limits. As the stimulus was presented at sites farther away from the peak activation site, the number of responses of any type that we collected for that unit decreased. We obtained I responses within one phalange or adjacent digit (1 step) but also farther away, and the response type collected at the farthest stimulus separation value was the I suppression (not including sites with no significant response). When comparing measured RF areas and overlap for measured cortical distances in area 3b of monkeys, Sur et al. (1980) used the concept of the hypercolumn for primary visual cortex from Hubel and Wiesel (1974). Sur et al. (1980) suggested that the primary somatosensory cortex hypercolumn would represent 1–1.5 mm diameter of cortex and that RFs are unlikely to overlap for neurons separated by a cortical distance of 500–600 μm in diameter. Whereas we did not calculate RF area or cortical distance, the multi-electrode arrays that we used here had an area of 0.4 × 2 mm2 for two rows of eight microwires, ∼250 μm apart within each row. Our excitatory mRF mapping data for the five primate cases correspond to the example data by Sur et al. (1980) (Fig. 8), because the RFs tend to overlap slightly across neurons recorded from separate electrodes, 250 μm or more apart (see Fig. 1 for PG-B). Our observations of the suppressive I response types in relation to stimulus location may indicate a somewhat more extensive reach of suppression, as I responses were collected from individual neurons when the stimulus was distant from the site that evoked the highest peak firing in the given neuron. Additional studies are required to relate the locations on the hand that suppress a neuron's firing rates with the cortical distances between the territories corresponding to the representations of those hand locations.
In some ways, our findings resemble the long-range interactions in visual cortex, where stimuli that are outside the classical RF of neurons affect responses to stimuli within the RF [see Hallum and Movshon (2014) review], and short- and long-range cortical connections can contribute to the suppressive lateral interactions. However, here, we stimulated one location at a time and examined the response profiles of neurons, similar to reports by Moxon and colleagues (Tutunculer et al. 2006) using extracellular recordings in the rat forelimb representation of primary somatosensory cortex. We focused on characterizing suppressive response components in primary somatosensory cortex of nonhuman primates, in contrast to focusing on excitatory response fields. Yet, similar to results for excitatory RFs, we revealed effects of single-site stimulation that occurred far from each neuron's excitatory mRF across distant digits or palm pads. As reported by Tutunculer et al. (2006), we also found evidence to suggest that the response types to tactile stimuli were more closely related to cortical separations within the hand representation than to separations between contiguous surfaces of the physical hand. Furthermore, neuronal activity recorded from a selected cortical electrode in response to stimulation of sites across the hand suggested that lateral interactions involving firing suppression occur not only along the representation of a single digit (in the rostral-caudal dimension in cortex) but also between digits (across the medial-lateral dimension in cortex).
The response types and profiles reported here for the representation of the hand in primary somatosensory cortex are consistent with intracellular recording studies. Intracellular recordings reveal that the amplitude of excitatory postsynaptic potentials (EPSPs) and inhibitory postsynaptic potentials (IPSPs) varies with stimulus location, such that stimulating points at the center of the RF evoking the largest EPSP also evoke the largest IPSPs. Both EPSP and IPSP amplitudes decay with increasing separation from the RF center, with more gradual decay of IPSP strength (Andersson 1965; Gabernet et al. 2005; Innocenti and Manzoni 1972; Whitehorn and Towe 1968). This distribution is similar to the response profiles shown in studies with awake macaques (Gardner and Costanzo 1980a, b; Laskin and Spencer 1979) and response profiles in the present report.
Possible Significance of Widespread Suppressive Response Components
Overall, our results of I suppression properties, including the latencies that do not differ from initial E responses, are consistent with reports of subcortical sources of suppression in primary somatosensory cortex in rodents by Higley and Contreras (2007) and others [see Sachdev et al. (2012) review], as well as in other sensory systems and species (Bolz and Gilbert 1986; Carandini et al. 2002; Cavanaugh et al. 2002; Mickey and Middlebrooks 2005; Ojima and Murakami 2002). Yet, we also found evidence consistent with cortical sources of inhibition (Davis et al. 2003; Fitzpatrick et al. 1999; Hubel and Wiesel 1965; Nelken and Young 1994; Shofner and Young 1985; Spirou and Young 1991; Walker et al. 1999; Webb et al. 2005; Wehr and Zador 2003). Thus our findings indirectly support recent views that suppressive response components arise from a combination of cortical and subcortical sources rather than from the traditional model of suppression from cortical sources only [e.g., Sachdev et al. (2012) review].
Finally, we suggest that these results also support the idea that cortical reactivation and reorganization after sensory loss involve reductions of inhibition that span essentially the entire hand representation, which in turn, allows previously subthreshold activations to elicit neuronal firing (Jain et al. 2008; Oliveira et al. 2014; Qi et al. 2011, 2014). The extent of suppressive response components when single sites were stimulated across hand locations adds properties of firing suppression to the overall picture of RF structure and supports findings related to widespread excitatory response fields reported in the somatosensory cortex of primates (Friedman et al. 2008; Lipton et al. 2010; Reed et al. 2008, 2010a, b, 2011, 2012; Thakur et al. 2012; Tutunculer et al. 2006) and rodents (Tutunculer et al. 2006). We believe that our findings reported here contribute to a better understanding of how primates process sensory stimuli on the hand for object perception and manipulation, which is fundamental for developing strategies to improve sensorimotor functions of the hand after impairments in patients with deficits due to nerve or spinal cord injury and disorders.
GRANTS
Support for this work was provided by the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke Grant NS16446 and James S. McDonnell Foundation (to J. H. Kaas); National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke Grant NS067017 (to H.-X. Qi); National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke Grant F31 NS053231 (to J. L. Reed); and Coordination for the Improvement of Higher Education Personnel (CAPES) grant (proc. 230/02-2; to J. G. Franca).
DISCLOSURES
No conflicts of interest, financial or otherwise, are declared.
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
Author contributions: H.-X.Q., J.L.R., J.G.F., N.J., Y.K., and J.H.K. conception and design of research; H.-X.Q., J.L.R., J.G.F., and N.J. performed experiments; H.-X.Q., J.L.R., and Y.K. analyzed data; H.-X.Q., J.L.R., and J.H.K. interpreted results of experiments; H.-X.Q. and J.L.R. prepared figures; H.-X.Q., J.L.R., Y.K., and J.H.K. drafted manuscript; H.-X.Q., J.L.R., J.G.F., N.J., Y.K., and J.H.K. edited and revised manuscript; H.-X.Q., J.L.R., J.G.F., N.J., Y.K., and J.H.K. approved final version of manuscript.
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
The authors thank Laura Trice for technical support and Dr. Troy Hackett for assisting array implantation surgeries. The authors appreciate this opportunity to contribute to this series in honor of Steven Hsiao, a wonderful scientist and a special friend to so many of us.
Present address of N. Jain: National Brain Research Centre, Manesar, India.
Present address of Y. Kajikawa: Cognitive Neuroscience and Schizophrenia Program, Nathan S. Kline Institute for Psychiatric Research, Orangeburg, New York 10962.
REFERENCES
- Allman J, Miezin F, McGuinness E. Stimulus specific responses from beyond the classical receptive field: neurophysiological mechanisms for local-global comparisons in visual neurons. Annu Rev Neurosci 8: 407–430, 1985. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alloway KD, Burton H. Differential effects of GABA and bicuculline on rapidly- and slowly-adapting neurons in primary somatosensory cortex of primates. Exp Brain Res 85: 598–610, 1991. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andersson SA. Intracellular postsynaptic potentials in the somatosensory cortex of the cat. Nature 205: 297–298, 1965. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Arce-McShane FI, Hatsopoulos NG, Lee JC, Ross CF, Sessle BJ. Modulation dynamics in the orofacial sensorimotor cortex during motor skill acquisition. J Neurosci 34: 5985–5997, 2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bolz J, Gilbert CD. Generation of end-inhibition in the visual cortex via interlaminar connections. Nature 320: 362–365, 1986. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carandini M, Heeger DJ, Senn W. A synaptic explanation of suppression in visual cortex. J Neurosci 22: 10053–10065, 2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carlson M, Welt C. Somatic sensory cortex (SmI) of the prosimian primate Galago crassicaudatus: organization of mechanoreceptive input from the hand in relation to cytoarchitecture. J Comp Neurol 189: 249–271, 1980. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cavanaugh JR, Bair W, Movshon JA. Selectivity and spatial distribution of signals from the receptive field surround in macaque V1 neurons. J Neurophysiol 88: 2547–2556, 2002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chowdhury SA, Rasmusson DD. Corticocortical inhibition of peripheral inputs within primary somatosensory cortex: the role of GABA(A) and GABA(B) receptors. J Neurophysiol 90: 851–856, 2003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chubbuck JG. Small-motion biological stimulator. Johns Hopkins APL Tech Digest 5: 18–23, 1966. [Google Scholar]
- Davis KA, Ramachandran R, May BJ. Auditory processing of spectral cues for sound localization in the inferior colliculus. J Assoc Res Otolaryngol 4: 148–163, 2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- DiCarlo JJ, Johnson KO, Hsiao SS. Structure of receptive fields in area 3b of primary somatosensory cortex in the alert monkey. J Neurosci 18: 2626–2645, 1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dickey AS, Suminski A, Amit Y, Hatsopoulos NG. Single-unit stability using chronically implanted multielectrode arrays. J Neurophysiol 102: 1331–1339, 2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Douglas RJ, Koch C, Mahowald M, Martin KA, Suarez HH. Recurrent excitation in neocortical circuits. Science 269: 981–985, 1995. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dykes RW, Landry P, Metherate R, Hicks TP. Functional role of GABA in cat primary somatosensory cortex: shaping receptive fields of cortical neurons. J Neurophysiol 52: 1066–1093, 1984. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fang PC, Jain N, Kaas JH. Few intrinsic connections cross the hand-face border of area 3b of New World monkeys. J Comp Neurol 454: 310–319, 2002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fitzgerald PJ, Lane JW, Thakur PH, Hsiao SS. Receptive field properties of the macaque second somatosensory cortex: representation of orientation on different finger pads. J Neurosci 26: 6473–6484, 2006a. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fitzgerald PJ, Lane JW, Thakur PH, Hsiao SS. Receptive field (RF) properties of the macaque second somatosensory cortex: RF size, shape, and somatotopic organization. J Neurosci 26: 6485–6495, 2006b. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fitzpatrick DC, Kuwada S, Kim DO, Parham K, Batra R. Responses of neurons to click-pairs as simulated echoes: auditory nerve to auditory cortex. J Acoust Soc Am 106: 3460–3472, 1999. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Friedman RM, Chen LM, Roe AW. Responses of areas 3b and 1 in anesthetized squirrel monkeys to single- and dual-site stimulation of the digits. J Neurophysiol 100: 3185–3196, 2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gabernet L, Jadhav SP, Feldman DE, Carandini M, Scanziani M. Somatosensory integration controlled by dynamic thalamocortical feed-forward inhibition. Neuron 48: 315–327, 2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gardner EP, Costanzo RM. Spatial integration of multiple-point stimuli in primary somatosensory cortical receptive fields of alert monkeys. J Neurophysiol 43: 420–443, 1980a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gardner EP, Costanzo RM. Temporal integration of multiple-point stimuli in primary somatosensory cortical receptive fields of alert monkeys. J Neurophysiol 43: 444–468, 1980b. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ghazanfar AA, Stambaugh CR, Nicolelis MA. Encoding of tactile stimulus location by somatosensory thalamocortical ensembles. J Neurosci 20: 3761–3775, 2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hallum LE, Movshon JA. Surround suppression supports second-order feature encoding by macaque V1 and V2 neurons. Vision Res 104: 24–35, 2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heath CJ, Hore J, Phillips CG. Inputs from low threshold muscle and cutaneous afferents of hand and forearm to areas 3a and 3b of baboon's cerebral cortex. J Physiol 257: 199–227, 1976. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Higley MJ, Contreras D. Cellular mechanisms of suppressive interactions between somatosensory responses in vivo. J Neurophysiol 97: 647–658, 2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hsiao S. Central mechanisms of tactile shape perception. Curr Opin Neurobiol 18: 418–424, 2008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hsiao SS, Bensmaia SJ. Coding of object shape and texture. In: The Senses: A Comprehensive Reference, edited by Gardner EP and Kaas JH. Waltham, MA: Elsevier, 2008, vol. 6, p. 55–66. [Google Scholar]
- Hubel DH, Wiesel TN. Receptive fields and functional architecture in two nonstriate visual sreas (18 and 19) of the cat. J Neurophysiol 28: 229–289, 1965. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hubel DH, Wiesel TN. Receptive fields, binocular interaction and functional architecture in the cat's visual cortex. J Physiol 160: 106–154, 1962. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hubel DH, Wiesel TN. Uniformity of monkey striate cortex: a parallel relationship between field size, scatter, and magnification factor. J Comp Neurol 158: 295–305, 1974. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Huffman KJ, Krubitzer L. Area 3a: topographic organization and cortical connections in marmoset monkeys. Cereb Cortex 11: 849–867, 2001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Innocenti GM, Manzoni T. Response patterns of somatosensory cortical neurones to peripheral stimuli. An intracellular study. Arch Ital Biol 110: 322–347, 1972. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Iwamura Y, Tanaka M, Sakamoto M, Hikosaka O. Functional subdivisions representing different finger regions in area-3 of the 1st somatosensory cortex of the conscious monkey. Exp Brain Res 51: 315–326, 1983. [Google Scholar]
- Jain N, Qi HX, Collins CE, Kaas JH. Large-scale reorganization in the somatosensory cortex and thalamus after sensory loss in macaque monkeys. J Neurosci 28: 11042–11060, 2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jain N, Qi HX, Kaas JH. Long-term chronic multichannel recordings from sensorimotor cortex and thalamus of primates. Prog Brain Res 130: 64–72, 2001. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kim SS, Gomez-Ramirez M, Thakur PH, Hsiao SS. Multimodal interactions between proprioceptive and cutaneous signals in primary somatosensory cortex. Neuron 86: 555–566, 2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krubitzer LA, Kaas JH. The organization and connections of somatosensory cortex in marmosets. J Neurosci 10: 952–974, 1990. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Laskin SE, Spencer WA. Cutaneous masking. II. Geometry of excitatory and inhibitory receptive fields of single units in somatosensory cortex of the cat. J Neurophysiol 42: 1061–1082, 1979. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee J, Kim K, Chung S, Lee C. Suppression of spontaneous activity before visual response in the primate V1 neurons during a visually guided saccade task. J Neurosci 33: 3760–3764, 2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li W, Thier P, Wehrhahn C. Neuronal responses from beyond the classic receptive field in VI of alert monkeys. Exp Brain Res 139: 359–371, 2001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Liao CC, Gharbawie OA, Qi H, Kaas JH. Cortical connections to single digit representations in area 3b of somatosensory cortex in squirrel monkeys and prosimian galagos. J Comp Neurol 521: 3768–3790, 2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lipton ML, Liszewski MC, O'Connell MN, Mills A, Smiley JF, Branch CA, Isler JR, Schroeder CE. Interactions within the hand representation in primary somatosensory cortex of primates. J Neurosci 30: 15895–15903, 2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Merzenich MM, Kaas JH, Sur M, Lin CS. Double representation of the body surface within cytoarchitectonic areas 3b and 1 in “SI” in the owl monkey (Aotus trivirgatus). J Comp Neurol 181: 41–73, 1978. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Merzenich MM, Kaas JH, Wall J, Nelson RJ, Sur M, Felleman D. Topographic reorganization of somatosensory cortical areas 3b and 1 in adult monkeys following restricted deafferentation. Neuroscience 8: 33–55, 1983. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mickey BJ, Middlebrooks JC. Sensitivity of auditory cortical neurons to the locations of leading and lagging sounds. J Neurophysiol 94: 979–989, 2005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mountcastle VB, Powell TP. Neural mechanisms subserving cutaneous sensibility, with special reference to the role of afferent inhibition in sensory perception and discrimination. Bull Johns Hopkins Hosp 105: 201–232, 1959. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Negyessy L, Palfi E, Ashaber M, Palmer C, Jakli B, Friedman RM, Chen LM, Roe AW. Intrinsic horizontal connections process global tactile features in the primary somatosensory cortex: neuroanatomical evidence. J Comp Neurol 521: 2798–2817, 2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nelken I, Young ED. Two separate inhibitory mechanisms shape the responses of dorsal cochlear nucleus type IV units to narrowband and wideband stimuli. J Neurophysiol 71: 2446–2462, 1994. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nelson RJ, Sur M, Felleman DJ, Kaas JH. Representations of the body surface in postcentral parietal cortex of Macaca fascicularis. J Comp Neurol 192: 611–643, 1980. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nicolelis MA, Chapin JK. Spatiotemporal structure of somatosensory responses of many-neuron ensembles in the rat ventral posterior medial nucleus of the thalamus. J Neurosci 14: 3511–3532, 1994. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nicolelis MA, Dimitrov D, Carmena JM, Crist R, Lehew G, Kralik JD, Wise SP. Chronic, multisite, multielectrode recordings in macaque monkeys. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 100: 11041–11046, 2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nicolelis MA, Ghazanfar AA, Faggin BM, Votaw S, Oliveira LM. Reconstructing the engram: simultaneous, multisite, many single neuron recordings. Neuron 18: 529–537, 1997. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Nicolelis MA, Ghazanfar AA, Stambaugh CR, Oliveira LM, Laubach M, Chapin JK, Nelson RJ, Kaas JH. Simultaneous encoding of tactile information by three primate cortical areas. Nat Neurosci 1: 621–630, 1998. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ojima H, Murakami K. Intracellular characterization of suppressive responses in supragranular pyramidal neurons of cat primary auditory cortex in vivo. Cereb Cortex 12: 1079–1091, 2002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Oliveira JT, Bittencourt-Navarrete RE, de Almeida FM, Tonda-Turo C, Martinez AM, Franca JG. Enhancement of median nerve regeneration by mesenchymal stem cells engraftment in an absorbable conduit: improvement of peripheral nerve morphology with enlargement of somatosensory cortical representation. Front Neuroanat 8: 111, 2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petersen RS, Diamond ME. Spatial-temporal distribution of whisker-evoked activity in rat somatosensory cortex and the coding of stimulus location. J Neurosci 20: 6135–6143, 2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Qi HX, Chen LM, Kaas JH. Reorganization of somatosensory cortical areas 3b and 1 after unilateral section of dorsal columns of the spinal cord in squirrel monkeys. J Neurosci 31: 13662–13675, 2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Qi HX, Reed JL, Gharbawie OA, Burish MJ, Kaas JH. Cortical neuron response properties are related to lesion extent and behavioral recovery after sensory loss from spinal cord injury in monkeys. J Neurosci 34: 4345–4363, 2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reed JL, Pouget P, Qi HX, Zhou Z, Bernard MR, Burish MJ, Haitas J, Bonds AB, Kaas JH. Widespread spatial integration in primary somatosensory cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 105: 10233–10237, 2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reed JL, Pouget P, Qi HX, Zhou Z, Bernard MR, Burish MJ, Kaas JH. Effects of spatiotemporal stimulus properties on spike timing correlations in owl monkey primary somatosensory cortex. J Neurophysiol 108: 3353–3369, 2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reed JL, Qi HX, Kaas JH. Spatiotemporal properties of neuron response suppression in owl monkey primary somatosensory cortex when stimuli are presented to both hands. J Neurosci 31: 3589–3601, 2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reed JL, Qi HX, Pouget P, Burish MJ, Bonds AB, Kaas JH. Modular processing in the hand representation of primate primary somatosensory cortex coexists with widespread activation. J Neurophysiol 104: 3136–3145, 2010a. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Reed JL, Qi HX, Zhou Z, Bernard MR, Burish MJ, Bonds AB, Kaas JH. Response properties of neurons in primary somatosensory cortex of owl monkeys reflect widespread spatiotemporal integration. J Neurophysiol 103: 2139–2157, 2010b. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Saal HP, Bensmaia SJ. Touch is a team effort: interplay of submodalities in cutaneous sensibility. Trends Neurosci 37: 689–697, 2014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sachdev RN, Catania KC. Receptive fields and response properties of neurons in the star-nosed mole's somatosensory fovea. J Neurophysiol 87: 2602–2611, 2002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sachdev RN, Krause MR, Mazer JA. Surround suppression and sparse coding in visual and barrel cortices. Front Neural Circuits 6: 43, 2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sachdev RN, Sellien H, Ebner FF. Direct inhibition evoked by whisker stimulation in somatic sensory (SI) barrel field cortex of the awake rat. J Neurophysiol 84: 1497–1504, 2000. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Shofner WP, Young ED. Excitatory/inhibitory response types in the cochlear nucleus: relationships to discharge patterns and responses to electrical stimulation of the auditory nerve. J Neurophysiol 54: 917–939, 1985. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spirou GA, Young ED. Organization of dorsal cochlear nucleus type IV unit response maps and their relationship to activation by bandlimited noise. J Neurophysiol 66: 1750–1768, 1991. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sripati AP, Yoshioka T, Denchev P, Hsiao SS, Johnson KO. Spatiotemporal receptive fields of peripheral afferents and cortical area 3b and 1 neurons in the primate somatosensory system. J Neurosci 26: 2101–2114, 2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sur M. Receptive fields of neurons in areas 3b and 1 of somatosensory cortex in monkeys. Brain Res 198: 465–471, 1980. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sur M, Merzenich MM, Kaas JH. Magnification, receptive-field area, and “hypercolumn” size in areas 3b and 1 of somatosensory cortex in owl monkeys. J Neurophysiol 44: 295–311, 1980. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sur M, Nelson RJ, Kaas JH. Representations of the body surface in cortical areas 3b and 1 of squirrel monkeys: comparisons with other primates. J Comp Neurol 211: 177–192, 1982. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sur M, Wall JT, Kaas JH. Modular distribution of neurons with slowly adapting and rapidly adapting responses in area 3b of somatosensory cortex in monkeys. J Neurophysiol 51: 724–744, 1984. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Swadlow HA. Efferent neurons and suspected interneurons in S-1 vibrissa cortex of the awake rabbit: receptive fields and axonal properties. J Neurophysiol 62: 288–308, 1989. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thakur PH, Fitzgerald PJ, Hsiao SS. Second-order receptive fields reveal multidigit interactions in area 3b of the macaque monkey. J Neurophysiol 108: 243–262, 2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thakur PH, Fitzgerald PJ, Lane JW, Hsiao SS. Receptive field properties of the macaque second somatosensory cortex: nonlinear mechanisms underlying the representation of orientation within a finger pad. J Neurosci 26: 13567–13575, 2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tutunculer B, Foffani G, Himes BT, Moxon KA. Structure of the excitatory receptive fields of infragranular forelimb neurons in the rat primary somatosensory cortex responding to touch. Cereb Cortex 16: 791–810, 2006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Walker GA, Ohzawa I, Freeman RD. Asymmetric suppression outside the classical receptive field of the visual cortex. J Neurosci 19: 10536–10553, 1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Webb BS, Dhruv NT, Solomon SG, Tailby C, Lennie P. Early and late mechanisms of surround suppression in striate cortex of macaque. J Neurosci 25: 11666–11675, 2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wehr M, Zador AM. Balanced inhibition underlies tuning and sharpens spike timing in auditory cortex. Nature 426: 442–446, 2003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whitehorn D, Towe AL. Postsynaptic potential patterns evoked upon cells in sensorimotor cortex of cat by stimulation at the periphery. Exp Neurol 22: 222–224, 1968. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wong-Riley M. Changes in the visual system of monocularly sutured or enucleated cats demonstrable with cytochrome oxidase histochemistry. Brain Res 171: 11–28, 1979. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wu CW, Kaas JH. Somatosensory cortex of prosimian galagos: physiological recording, cytoarchitecture, and corticocortical connections of anterior parietal cortex and cortex of the lateral sulcus. J Comp Neurol 457: 263–292, 2003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Xu Y, Wang X, Peck C, Goldberg ME. The time course of the tonic oculomotor proprioceptive signal in area 3a of somatosensory cortex. J Neurophysiol 106: 71–77, 2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]